Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Reduced serotonergic transmission alters sensitivity to cost and reward via 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptors in monkeys

  • Yukiko Hori,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Functional Brain Imaging, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan

  • Koki Mimura,

    Roles Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Functional Brain Imaging, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan, Research Center for Medical and Health Data Science, The Institute of Statistical Mathematics, Tokyo, Japan

  • Yuji Nagai,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Functional Brain Imaging, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan

  • Yuki Hori,

    Roles Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Functional Brain Imaging, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan

  • Katsushi Kumata,

    Roles Resources

    Affiliation Department of Advanced Nuclear Medicine Sciences, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan

  • Ming-Rong Zhang,

    Roles Resources

    Affiliation Department of Advanced Nuclear Medicine Sciences, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan

  • Tetsuya Suhara,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Functional Brain Imaging, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan

  • Makoto Higuchi,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Functional Brain Imaging, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan

  • Takafumi Minamimoto

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    minamimoto.takafumi@qst.go.jp

    Affiliation Department of Functional Brain Imaging, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan

Abstract

Serotonin (5-HT) deficiency is a core biological pathology underlying depression and other psychiatric disorders whose key symptoms include decreased motivation. However, the exact role of 5-HT in motivation remains controversial and elusive. Here, we pharmacologically manipulated the 5-HT system in macaque monkeys and quantified the effects on motivation for goal-directed actions in terms of incentives and costs. Reversible inhibition of 5-HT synthesis increased errors and reaction times on goal-directed tasks, indicating reduced motivation. Analysis found incentive-dependent and cost-dependent components of this reduction. To identify the receptor subtypes that mediate cost and incentive, we systemically administered antagonists specific to 4 major 5-HT receptor subtypes: 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, 5-HT2A, and 5-HT4. Positron emission tomography (PET) visualized the unique distribution of each subtype in limbic brain regions and determined the systemic dosage for antagonists that would achieve approximately 30% occupancy. Only blockade of 5-HT1A decreased motivation through changes in both expected cost and incentive; sensitivity to future workload and time delay to reward increased (cost) and reward value decreased (incentive). Blocking the 5-HT1B receptor also reduced motivation through decreased incentive, although it did not affect expected cost. These results suggest that 5-HT deficiency disrupts 2 processes, the subjective valuation of costs and rewards, via 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptors, thus leading to reduced motivation.

Introduction

The motivation to do any voluntary goal-directed behavior is a trade-off between incentive and the expected cost. The subjective value of the reward expected upon achieving the goal determines the incentive (or incentive value), which has a positive influence on motivation. The expected cost is a subjective valuation of what must be paid to earn the reward (e.g., waiting, taking a risk, or putting forth effort) and has a negative influence [13]. Thus, motivation will be highest when high-value rewards can be obtained at low cost and vice versa. Deficiencies in the serotonin (5-HT) system are known to disturb incentive and expected cost, which in turn leads to abnormal motivation. Indeed, depression and other psychiatric disorders are associated with 5-HT deficiency, as 5-HT has become the target of numerous medications for treating these conditions. 5-HT deficiency is currently thought to reduce incentive (e.g., blunting/eliminating pleasure from rewards) and/or increase the expected cost (e.g., waiting becomes intolerable, resulting in impulsivity) [4,5]. In rodent studies, both 5-HT depletion and attenuation of 5-HT transmission via specific receptor antagonists increases impulsive choices [611] and reduces the frequency of effortful behavior [12]. In addition, pharmacological attenuation of dorsal raphe 5-HT neurons has been shown to impair the ability/desire to wait for long-delayed rewards [13]. Yet, despite this evidence, the specific mechanisms through which 5-HT contributes to motivation remain unclear, especially in primates.

Understanding the role of 5-HT in motivation is challenging, in part because incentives and costs have not been adequately considered in the 5-HT literature. Indeed, the behavioral effects of reducing 5-HT activity in the brain have never been quantitatively and independently examined in terms of incentives and costs. The fourteen 5-HT receptor subtypes in the central nervous system [14] and their heterogeneous localizations are another factor that complicates the issue. Therefore, characterizing the role of each receptor in modulating motivation via incentive and/or cost is critical, as is determining their locations in the brain.

In a previous study, we demonstrated dissociable roles of the 2 dopamine (DA) receptor subtypes in computing the cost-benefit trade-off in motivation. This was accomplished by combining positron emission tomography (PET), pharmacological manipulation of DA receptors, and quantitative measures of motivation in monkeys [15]. Here, we used the same methodology to examine the 5-HT system and determine how 5-HT neurotransmission affects the motivation for goal-directed behavior. First, we manipulated the 5-HT system by repeatedly administering para-chlorophenylalanine (pCPA), a reversible inhibitor of 5-HT synthesis, to macaque monkeys and examining its effects on goal-directed behavior. We then focused on four 5-HT receptor subtypes (5-HTRs; specifically, 5-HT1AR, 5-HT1BR, 5-HT2AR, and 5-HT4R) that are abundant in limbic (motivation-related) brain regions [16]. We mapped their distributions and manipulated 5-HT transmission by systemically administering receptor type-specific antagonists at doses predetermined by PET to achieve the same degree of receptor occupancy. Behavioral effects were assessed using 2 tasks designed to examine incentive and expected cost, respectively. Our results suggest that a reduction in 5-HT transmission leads to reduced motivation through 2 distinct processes: increased cost sensitivity (expected cost) via 5-HT1AR and reduced reward impact (incentive) via 5-HT1AR and 5-HT1BR.

Results

Effects of 5-HT depletion on incentive

We first determined how much 5-HT is depleted by preventing its synthesis with pCPA. We repeatedly injected pCPA (150 mg/kg, s.c.) over the course of 2 days, which resulted in a decrease in 5-HT metabolites (5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid, 5-HIAA) of at least 30% (N = 2, 33% and 64%) in the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), while the concentration of DA remained unchanged (Fig 1A and 1B). The 30% value was later used when determining the dosages for specific 5-HT receptor-type antagonists.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Effect of 5-HT depletion via pCPA on incentive.

(A) Schedule of pCPA treatment, behavioral testing, and CSF sampling. (B) Normalized concentration of 5-HT metabolite (5-HIAA) and DA before (baseline) and after pCPA treatment (depletion). (C) Reward-size task. Left: Sequence of events during 1 trial. A monkey initiated a trial by touching the bar in the chair. After 100 ms, a visual cue signaling the amount of reward (1, 2, 4, or 8 drops) that would be delivered was presented at the center of the monitor. After 500 ms, a red target also appeared at the center of the monitor. After a variable interval of 500–1,500 ms, the target turned green, indicating that the monkey could release the bar to receive the reward. If the monkey responded between 200 and 1,000 ms, the target turned blue indicating the trial had been completed correctly. On correct trials, water rewards were delivered immediately. An ITI of 1 s was enforced before the next trial could begin. If the monkey made an error by releasing the bar before the green target appeared, within 200 ms after it appeared, or failed to respond within 1 s, all visual stimuli disappeared, the trial was terminated immediately, and the trial was repeated after the 1-s ITI. After each correct trial, a new cue reward-size pair was picked from the set of 4 at random. Right: Relationship between visual cues and reward size. (D) Representative error rates as a function of reward size for monkey BO. Dotted curves are the best fit of inverse functions (Model #4, S3 Table). (E) Normalized error rate (percent of maximum error rate in 1 drop trial in the control session; mean ± SEM) as a function of reward size for n = 11 sessions collected from 4 monkeys (S2 Table). (F) Schematic illustration of increase in error rate as incentive a is reduced. (G) Box plot of normalized incentive (a) for each treatment condition (n = 11 for each). Each value was normalized to the value of the control condition. (H) Schematic explaining the increase in error rate by e, independent of reward size. (I) Box plot of parameter e (normalized as the ratio of maximum error rate in 1 drop trial in the control session; mean ± SEM) for each treatment condition (n = 11 for each). The data underlying this figure can be found in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10141750. 5-HIAA, 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid; CSF, cerebrospinal fluid; DA, dopamine; ITI, inter-trial interval; pCPA, para-chlorophenylalanine.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.g001

We next examined the effects of 5-HT depletion on incentive in 4 monkeys that were not used in the CSF study (S1 and S2 Tables). For this purpose, we used a reward-size task in which the amount of reward was manipulated across trials, but the task requirements (i.e., the costs) remained the same (Fig 1C). For each trial, the monkeys could receive a reward if they released a bar when a visual target changed from red to green. A visual cue at the beginning of each trial indicated the amount of reward they could get (1, 2, 4, or 8 drops). All monkeys had been trained to perform basic color discrimination trials on a cued multi-trial reward-schedule task [17] for more than 3 months. As in previous experiments using a single option task, the required action was very easy, and monkeys could not fail if they actually tried to release the bar at the proper time (the error rate is indeed much lower in the absence of information about costs and benefits) [2]. As in previous experiments in which costs and benefits were manipulated, errors (either releasing the bar too early or too late) were usually observed in small reward trials and/or close to the end of daily sessions [2,18,19]. Note that error trials were repeated with the same cue-reward condition, which prevented the monkeys from skipping unwanted trials. Therefore, bar “errors” were considered to have occurred when the monkeys were not sufficiently motivated to release the bar at a time that would lead to reward. The frequency of error trials is thus a reliable metric for quantifying the influence of motivation on behavior [15,18,19]. Furthermore, we have previously shown that the error rate (E) is inversely related to the reward size (R), which has been formulated with a single free parameter a [2] (Fig 1F), (1)

This inverse relationship was consistently observed in the control condition in all monkeys (e.g., CON in Fig 1D and 1E). After pCPA treatment, error rates increased independently from reward-size-related errors. For example, in monkey BO, the error rate became progressively higher in sessions that followed the first and second treatments, while differences that depended on reward size appeared to remain the same (Fig 1D, pCPA-day 1 and 2). A reward-independent increase in error rate was consistently found in all monkeys tested, as shown in the average plot of the normalized error rate (Fig 1E). We next quantified how much of the increase in errors was related to reduced incentive (i.e., devalued reward) and how much was reward-size independent. These factors can be captured by a decrease in parameter a (reward impact or incentive) of the inverse function and implementation of the intercept e, respectively (Fig 1F and 1H). To quantify the increase in error rate, we compared 5 models that considered these 2 factors as random effects: Model #1, random effect on a; Model #2, random effect on a fixed e; Model #3, independent random effects on both a and e; Model #4, a single normal distribution of random effect on a and e; and Model #5, random effect on e (see S3 Table). Model #4 was selected as the best model with the lowest Bayesian information criterion (BIC) value (S3 Table), indicating that the increase in error rate was explained by simultaneous changes in parameters a and e. Our model-based analysis revealed that, compared with the pre-treatment baseline, a was significantly lower on day 2 of pCPA treatment (one-way ANOVA, main effect of treatment, F(2, 20) = 4.6, p = 0.023; post hoc Tukey HSD, p = 0.025 for pCPA-day2 versus CON; Fig 1G). At the same time, parameter e was significantly higher on pCPA-day 2 (main effect of treatment, F(2, 20) = 4.1, p = 0.031; post hoc Tukey HSD, p = 0.025 for pCPA-day 2 versus CON; Fig 1I). These results suggest that the 5-HT depletion-induced increase in errors can be explained by 2 components: one is reduced incentive (a), and the other is a factor that appears orthogonal to the incentive value (increase in parameter e). We hypothesized that the value-independent component reflected expected cost and subsequent tests supported this interpretation (see Effects of 5-HTR blockade on cost-based motivation below).

Given that reward value (and thus incentive) decreases as animals become satiated [19], we further investigated how the error rate increased along with satiation. In Fig 2A, the average error rates from the normalized data (n = 11) are replotted as a function of normalized cumulative reward (see Materials and methods). As previously shown, overall error rates in the control condition increased for each reward size as the normalized cumulative reward increased. This satiation-dependent change in error rate was commonly observed among the 3 conditions, with the effect being stronger in the 2 posttreatment sessions (pCPA-day1 and -day2) and corresponding to reduced incentive due to 5-HT depletion (cf., reduced a, in Fig 1G). However, we also observed pronounced increases in posttreatment error rates that were independent of satiation level; namely, error rates became higher even during the early phase of the session, presumably when thirst drives were still high (Fig 2A). Indeed, fitting the data to an error-rate model that incorporates the satiation effect (Eq 4) showed that regardless of reward size or satiation level, the error rates were higher in posttreatment sessions (e = 3.3 and 6.6 for pCPA-day1 and -day2, respectively) than in the control session (e = 0.74). This again suggests that 5-HT depletion blunts motivation in a manner partially independent from reduced incentive.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Effect of 5-HT depletion on impact of satiation on error, RT, error pattern, and RT-error relationship.

(A) Error rate (mean ± SEM; n = 11 sessions) as a function of normalized cumulative reward for control and pCPA treatment conditions. Colors indicate reward size. Curves indicate the best-fit model of Eq 4. (B) Mean RT (mean ± SEM) as a function of reward size for control (CON) and pCPA treatment conditions in monkey BO. Colors indicate treatment condition. (C) Rate of early and late errors (mean ± SEM) for control and pCPA-day2 conditions. (D) Early release rate (mean ± SEM) for control and pCPA treatment conditions. (E) Relationship between error rate and mean RT for each reward size in the first and second half of each session under pCPA treatment in monkeys BO and TO, respectively. Colors indicate treatment condition. Colored lines represent the best-fitting linear regression models that explained the data (S4 Table). The data underlying this figure can be found in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10141750. pCPA, para-chlorophenylalanine; RT, reaction time.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.g002

We also examined changes in reaction time (RT) across trials as another behavioral measure of motivation. Consistent with previous studies [20,21], 5-HT depletion prolonged RTs (Fig 2B). Indeed, RTs were significantly longer with repeated injections [two-way ANOVA, main effect of treatment, F(2, 110) = 7.1, p < 0.01; post hoc Tukey’s honest significant difference (HSD), p < 0.01 for day1 versus CON and day2 versus CON] without significantly interacting with reward size (main effect of reward size, F(3, 110) = 6.0, p < 0.001 interaction, F(6, 110) = 0.40, p = 0.88). In addition to the slower responses, pCPA treatment also significantly decreased general motor activity in the home cage (4 sessions in 3 monkeys; one-way ANOVA, main effect of treatment, F(1,11) = 7.28, p = 0.021). Administering pCPA increased both early and late errors across all reward-size conditions (Fig 2C), with late errors tending to increase more (e.g., Fig 2D; one-way ANOVA, main effect of treatment, F(2, 20) = 11.9, p < 0.001; post hoc Tukey HSD, p < 0.005 for day1 versus CON and day2 versus CON). A simple explanation for these effects is that 5-HT depletion affects motivation, which is a common source of control over all behaviors related to performing the task. Thus, whether monkeys perform the action correctly (error rate) and how quickly they respond (RT) should be similarly affected. We reasoned that, if this were the case, the intersession variability in RT and error rate should be correlated and consistent across conditions. However, a session-by-session analysis revealed that the changes in these 2 behavioral measures did not follow the prediction; although there was a linear relationship between error rates and RTs in each treatment condition, the slopes of the linear relationship became steeper as pCPA treatment was repeated (Fig 2E and S4 Table). This finding suggests that additional factors beyond the normal motivational processes at work in goal-directed behavior may contribute to increased error.

Effects of 5-HTR blockade on incentive

Next, we sought to identify the receptor subtype(s) contributing to the incentive-dependent and incentive-independent (putative cost-dependent) decrease in motivation. We performed PET imaging with selective radioligands for 5-HT1A ([11C]WAY100635), 5-HT1B ([11C]AZ10419369), 5-HT2A ([18F]altanserin), and 5-HT4 ([11C]SB207145), and quantified specific radioligand binding using a simplified reference tissue model [22] with the cerebellum as the reference region. Consistent with previous human studies [16], different patterns of receptor distribution were observed for each subtype. For example, high levels of 5-HT1AR expression were observed in the anterior cingulate cortex, amygdala, and hippocampus, lateral prefrontal cortex, and posterior cingulate cortex, while its expression was relatively sparce in the basal ganglia (Fig 3A). High 5-HT1BR expression was observed in the occipital cortex, ventral pallidum, and substantia nigra (Fig 3B). High 5-HT2AR expression was observed in the mediodorsal prefrontal and occipital cortex, while subcortical expression was minimal (Fig 3C). Binding of 5-HT4 was mainly observed in the striatum (Fig 3D).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Heterogeneous distribution of 5-HTR subtypes as measured by PET.

Top: Average density maps for four 5-HTR subtypes (A–D; 5-HT1AR, 5-HT1BR, 5-HT2AR, and 5-HT4R, respectively) on the inflated common macaque surface. Lateral (top) and medial (bottom) views of the left hemisphere are shown. Bottom: Subcortical distribution of the four 5-HTRs shown on 2 representative coronal slices. Color scale represents 2nd–98th percentile of receptor density (i.e., radiotracer binding potential). AMY, amygdala; HP, hippocampus; VP, ventral pallidum; SN, substantia nigra; CD, caudate nucleus; PUT, putamen; PET, positron emission tomography. The data underlying this figure can be found in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10141750.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.g003

Selective antagonists are available for these 4 receptor subtypes, each of which is pharmacologically capable of attenuating or blocking 5-HT transmission. Because pharmacological profiles such as receptor affinity and bioavailability differ, the same level of inhibition cannot be achieved with identical dosages of these antagonists. Thus, to compare the effect of receptor blockade between subtypes, we first determined the appropriate dosages. We previously used receptor occupancy as an objective measure for this purpose [15]. To mimic a reduction in 5-HT transmission comparable to the serotonin depletion observed in the CSF after pCPA treatment (approximately 30%), we measured tracer binding at baseline and after systemic administration in 3 monkeys (S1 Table and S1 Fig) and established the dose of antagonist required to achieve approximately 30% to 40% blockade (receptor occupancy), except for 5-HT2A, which achieved over 50% (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Antagonist dosage and occupancy for each 5-HT receptor subtype.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.t001

We next examined the effects of separately blocking the receptor subtypes on performance in the reward-size task in 3 monkeys. We administered 3 of 4 different 5-HTR antagonists or vehicle as a control (S2 Table) to each monkey. Blockade of 5-HT1AR substantially increased the error rate (Fig 4A), while blockade of 5-HT1BR resulted in a moderate increase (Fig 4B). Model-based analysis revealed that blockade of 5-HT1AR significantly affected both incentive (a) and the reward-size independent factor (putative cost; e) (Model #29 in S5 Table)reducing a to 40% and increasing e by about 17% on average (Fig 4E and 4F). Blockade of 5-HT1BR significantly reduced a to 70% on average (Fig 4E; Model #30, S5 Table). In contrast, blockade of 5-HT2AR did not significantly affect either (Fig 4C; Model #25, S5 Table). This was not because the blockade level was too low, as another high dose of the antagonist (achieving approximately 84% occupancy) did not change the error rates (S2 Fig). Blocking 5-HT4R consistently decreased error rates (Fig 4D) and significantly increased a to approximately 150% (Fig 4E; Model #30, S5 Table). Thus, it is unlikely that 5-HT2AR or 5-HT4R directly mediates the behavioral changes observed with 5-HT depletion. Together, these results suggest that the incentive-dependent and incentive-independent components of the lower motivation (as quantified by increased error rate) that we observed upon 5-HT depletion were reproduced by blocking two 5-HTRs: incentive (reward impact, a) was reduced by blocking 5-HT1AR or 5-HT1BR, whereas a factor orthogonal to the incentive value (increased parameter e, putative cost) was increased exclusively by blocking 5-HT1AR.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Effects of 5-HTR blockade on incentive.

(A–D) Error rates (mean ± SEM) as a function of reward size in 3 monkeys after blocking 5-HT1AR, 5-HT1BR, 5-HT2AR, and 5-HT4R by systemic administration of specific antagonists (colored points) and saline control (black points) in 3 monkeys, respectively (S2 Table). Colored and black dashed curves indicate the best fit inverse functions summarized in S5 Table. Rescaled plots are shown in insets in B–D. (E, F) Summary of parameter changes for a (incentive) and e (value-independent error), respectively. Dots and bars indicate individual data and mean values, respectively. Dashed line at 1 indicates the value for the control. NA denotes that the parameter was not selected as a random effect in the best-fit model (S5 Table). The data underlying this figure can be found in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10141750.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.g004

Blockade of 5-HT1AR prolonged RTs independent of reward size (two-way ANOVA, main effect of treatment, F(1, 214) = 366, p < 10−16; treatment × reward size, F(1, 3) = 1.1, p = 0.33; S3A Fig). In contrast, RTs were not altered by blocking any of the other 5-HTRs (p > 0.05; S3B–S3D Fig). The error pattern did not change with any of the treatments (S4 Fig). A session-by-session analysis revealed that the linear relationship between error rates and RTs was altered exclusively by 5-HT1AR blockade (S5 Fig and S6 Table), again reproducing the behavioral changes observed after 5-HT depletion (Fig 2D). Thus, altering 5-HTR transmission via 5-HT1AR appeared to mediate the component of the motivational processes that was unrelated to reduced incentive, and which had been observed with 5-HT depletion, accurately mimicking changes in the error rate–incentive function and error rate–RT relationship.

Effects of 5-HTR blockade on cost-based motivation

Our results so far suggest that a reduction in 5-HT transmission via 5-HT1AR leads to a decrease in motivation that is partially independent of incentive (reward size/satiation). However, we have not yet established what this factor is or what it depends on. As motivation is the integration of incentive for rewards and the expected cost required to obtain them, and because motivation decreases with higher expected costs, we hypothesized that the emergence of the incentive-independent factor (parameter e in our model) represents an overestimation of expected cost. To test this hypothesis, we directly investigated the effect of selective 5-HTR blockade on cost-based motivation using a work/delay task (Fig 5A) that had the same basic features as the reward-size task, but instead of changing reward amounts, manipulated 2 types of expected costs separately on each trial [18]. In “work” trials, the monkeys had to perform 0, 1, or 2 additional instrumental trials (for details, see Materials and methods) to obtain a fixed amount of reward. In “delay” trials, after the monkeys correctly performed an instrumental trial, a reward was delivered after a 0- to 7-s delay. The number of extra trials or length of the delay was indicated by a visual cue presented throughout the trial. In the first trial after the reward, the visual cue indicated how much would have to be paid to get the next reward. Therefore, we assessed performance on the first trials to evaluate the impact of expected cost on motivation and decision-making. We have previously shown that monkeys exhibit linear relationships between error rate (E) and remaining cost (CU) for both work and delay trials, as follows: (2) where k is a coefficient and e is an intercept [18] (Fig 5B). Extending the inference and formulation of the reward-size task (Eq 1), this linear effect proposes that the reward value is hyperbolically discounted by the cost, with coefficient k corresponding to the discounting factors.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Effect of 5-HTR blockade on cost-based motivation.

(A) The work/delay task. Left: The sequence of events. A monkey initiated a trial by touching the bar in the chair. After 100 ms, a visual cue representing “cost required” was presented at the center of the monitor. After 500 ms, a red target also appeared at the center of the monitor. After a variable interval of 500–1,500 ms, the target turned green, indicating that the monkey could release the bar to receive a reward. If the monkey responded between 200 and 1,000 ms, the target turned blue indicating the trial had been completed correctly. On correct trials, 1 drop of water reward was delivered immediately or after a delay (3.6 s or 7.2 s). An ITI of 1 s was enforced before the next trial could begin. If the monkey made an error by releasing the bar before the green target appeared, within 200 ms after it appeared, or failed to respond within 1 s, all visual stimuli disappeared, the trial was terminated immediately, and the trial was repeated after the 1-s ITI. Right: Relationships between visual cues and trial timing in work trials (top 3 rows on the right) and delay duration in delay trials (bottom 3 rows on the left). Trials outlined by green boxes were used for the analysis. CU denotes the remaining (arbitrary) cost unit necessary to obtain a reward, i.e., either the remaining trials to perform (workload) or the remaining delay periods to wait through. (B) Schematic of model for increases in error rate related to increased cost sensitivity (k) or increased sensitivity to baseline cost (e). (C) Representative relationships between error rates (monkey ST; mean ± SEM) and remaining costs for delay (top) and work trials (bottom). Data for saline control (black line), 5-HT1AR, 5-HT1BR, 5-HT2AR, and 5-HT4R blockade (colored lines) are shown from left to right, respectively. Colored and black dashed lines indicate the best-fit linear models described in S7 Table. (D) Comparison of the effects of 5-HTR blockade on the workload-discounting parameter (kw), delay-discounting parameter (kd), and sensitivity to baseline cost (e). Dashed line at 1 indicates the value for the control. NA denotes that the parameter was not selected as a random effect in the best-fit model (S7 Table). The data underlying this figure can be found in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10141750. ITI, inter-trial interval.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.g005

We tested 3 monkeys (S1 and S2 Tables) and measured their error rates to infer delay and workload discounting. We confirmed that the error rates in the control condition increased as the remaining costs increased (e.g., Fig 5C, control). Fig 5B illustrates 2 possible mechanisms by which 5-HTR blockade might increase error rates. First, it could increase cost sensitivity (i.e., discounting factor, k; expected cost), which appears as an increase in error rate relative to the remaining cost (Fig 5B, left). Alternatively, errors might increase irrespective of the cost (i.e., increased e; Fig 5B, right). As when we analyzed effects on incentive, we compared the effect of 5-HTR antagonism on task performance when receptors were blocked to the same degree (i.e., approximately 30% occupancy; Table 1). Linear mixed model (LMM) analysis tested the assumption that 5-HTR blockade independently increased delay and workload discounting without considering the random effect of treatment condition (e.g., Figs 5C and S6 and S7 Table; see Materials and methods). We extracted the discounting factors and summarize their changes due to blockade in Fig 5D. Blockade of 5-HT1AR significantly increased delay discounting in 2 of 3 monkeys and workload discounting in all 3 cases, whereas it significantly increased cost-independent error in only 1 monkey (Figs 5D and S6B). In contrast, blockade of 5-HT1BR significantly decreased delay discounting in only 1 of 3 cases (S6B Fig). Blockade of either 5-HT2AR or 5-HT4R did not significantly impact the discounting factors.

In the control condition, RT was longer as the remaining cost increased. Blockade of 5-HT1AR significantly enhanced the effect of expected cost on RT regardless of cost type (three-way ANOVA, main effect of treatment, F(1, 358) = 92.2, p < 10−16; treatment × remaining cost, F(1, 2) = 6.1, p = 0.002; treatment × trial type, F(1, 1) = 3.6, p = 0.057; S7 Fig) in all but one case (monkey ST). In contrast, blocking the other subtypes did not prolong RT (p > 0.05; S7 Fig). None of the treatments changed the error pattern (p > 0.05; S8 Fig). Thus, increased expected cost can consistently account for the behavioral manifestation of reduced motivation due to 5-HT1AR antagonism, i.e., both prolonged RT and increased error rates.

Discussion

5-HT deficiency has been shown to decrease motivation and alter cost sensitivity in humans. Although studies in rodents have provided insights into how 5-HT signaling impacts various aspects of motivational behavior, a knowledge gap remains between these basic findings and behavioral disorders in humans. Using macaque monkeys, the present study aimed to fill this gap and identify specific roles of 5-HTR subtypes in reducing motivation. We found that reduced 5-HT transmission leads to reduced motivation through 2 distinct processes: increased cost sensitivity exclusively via 5-HT1AR and reduced incentive via 5-HT1AR and 5-HT1BR. Our findings shed light on the mechanistic role of 5-HT in different aspects of motivational regulation and their relevance to different aspects of medications used to treat depression.

Two factors decrease motivation for goal-directed behavior

Previous behavioral pharmacological studies demonstrated that 5-HT depletion slows action and/or reduces the likelihood of engaging in behavior [20,21,23], suggesting decreased motivation. However, these previous studies did not directly examine the effect of 5-HT depletion on incentive by using multiple incentive conditions. Thus, data describing the quantitative relationships among 5-HT, reward, and motivation are unavailable. In the present study, we formulated and quantified the relationship using a behavioral paradigm (reward-size task), in which the behavioral measure, error rates, reflects the incentive portion of motivation. Using the same paradigm, we previously demonstrated that DA receptor blockade (either D1- or D2-like receptors) increased the rate of error and was explained by a decrease in incentive (i.e., lower subjective reward, indicated by a smaller parameter a) [15]. In the current study, although 5-HT depletion also increased the error rate, the rate was only partially explained by reduced incentive. We also observed a factor orthogonal to the incentive value (increased parameter e). In addition, error rates increased constantly regardless of the satiation level. Thus, our results suggest that 5-HT depletion results in both incentive-dependent and incentive-independent reductions in motivation. Subsequent experiments using another behavioral paradigm (work/delay task) revealed that the incentive-independent factor is likely the cost.

Importantly, our pharmacological studies replicated and disentangled incentive-dependent and cost-dependent factors; the former was observed exclusively after 5-HT1AR and 5-HT1BR blockade and the latter after 5-HT1AR antagonism. These behavioral effects can be directly compared among the 4 major 5-HTR subtypes because we used PET to fix the antagonist occupancy level across subtypes. Thus, our findings suggest 2 independent mechanisms for 5-HT depletion-induced loss of motivation that are mediated by 2 receptor subtypes (5-HT1AR and 5-HT1BR), which PET indicated are distributed in different brain regions. As discussed below, our results highlight a new view of the 5-HT system at the receptor level with respect to differential aspects of motivational control.

We also emphasize that our results suggest that multiple factors and related brain processes may drive a single behavioral measure of motivation, namely, an increase in error rate. This may be important from a clinical perspective, as there is a tendency to use general terms such as “decreased motivation” or “anhedonia” to subsume all aspects of motivational impairment into 1 broad construct. Our results challenge this oversimplified view of motivational impairment and propose a more nuanced view in which multiple behavioral factors and neural processes underlie it.

Reduced 5-HT transmission via 5-HT1BR leads to decreased motivation in a value-dependent manner

The incentive-dependent component of decreased motivation was reproduced by both 5-HT1AR and 5-HT1BR blockade, with the latter showing a relatively stronger effect. Blockade of 5-HT1AR produced both value-dependent and -independent decreases in motivation, highlighting a central role in decreased motivation. However, unlike 5-HT1AR, 5-HT1BR blockade did not affect cost sensitivity. Similar value-dependent reductions in motivation were observed following DA receptor antagonism with either D1R or D2R blockade [15]. Given the mutual interactions between 5-HT and DA systems [24,25], the behavioral changes observed here may be mediated, at least in part, by DA. 5-HT1BR is abundant in the basal ganglia, particularly in the output structures (globus pallidus and substantia nigra) (Fig 3B). Moreover, activation of 5-HT1BR increases DA release in the mesocorticolimbic system in rats [26,27]. Blockade of 5-HT1BR attenuates cocaine-seeking, but not natural food-intake [28], suggesting that 5-HT transmission via 5-H1BR is related to outcome-based motivational control.

Primate visual cortex is rich in 5-HT1BR [29], which has been implicated in contrast-based edge/contour detection [30], a potential mediator of the observed behavioral changes. However, the use of high-contrast stimuli (i.e., white/black cues and red/green signals) may prevent potential effects due to contrast sensitivity. In fact, the behavioral effects of 5-HT1BR blockade on the current study were reward-size dependent, which is contrary to what would happen if it was related to changes in the visual system; analyzing data by reward size would result in uniform effects, not reward-size-dependent ones.

Our findings extend the current view of 5-HT1BR function, derived primarily from rodent studies, to primates and emphasize that its contribution to motivation is reward-related rather than cost-related [31]. This has important implications for understanding how 5-HT regulates motivational behavior and its relationship to the pathophysiology and pharmacotherapy of depression and other disorders. A previous study suggested that up-regulation of postsynaptic 5-HT1BR in the nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum may be involved in the antidepressant effect of ketamine [32]. Alteration of 5-HT1BR binding has also been reported in some psychiatric disorders, such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), alcohol dependence, pathological gambling, and drug abuse [3337]. It is thus important to conduct future studies focusing on the relationship between altered 5-HT1BR function and reward sensitivity in humans.

Reduced 5-HT transmission via 5-HT1AR leads to decreased motivation due to overestimation of future costs

One of the key findings of this study was that blockade of 5-HT1AR resulted in lower motivation by altering both incentive and non-incentive-related factors. Although errors can occur through a variety of mechanisms such as impulsivity and decreased attention, our results suggest that the incentive-independent changes after blockade may be related to an overestimation of the impending cost; when we manipulated costs (workload or a delay before reward) independently of incentive value, the impact of cost on motivation increased following 5-HT1AR blockade (Fig 5). These observations led us to conclude that reduced 5-HT transmission via 5-HT1AR results in increased cost sensitivity and decreased incentive, both of which act to reduce motivation.

5-HT has been implicated in the control of temporal discounting, which is the part of the cost-benefit evaluation process that regulates motivation. In humans, a decrease in 5-HT caused by tryptophan depletion has been shown to increase the rate of delayed reward discounting [9,38]. Conversely, 5-HT reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs, presumably by up-regulating 5-HT) decrease delay discounting [39]. Similarly, 5-HT depletion induced by pCPA administration tended to cause rats to choose immediate small rewards over long-delayed large rewards [7], whereas administration of an SSRI was associated with a decrease in impulsive choices [6]. In addition to 5-HT depletion, reduced 5-HT transmission mediated by 5-HT1AR results in increased cost sensitivity to delayed reward; specifically, administration of a 5-HT1AR antagonist (WAY100635) tended to cause rats to choose immediate small rewards over long-delayed large rewards [6]. In monkeys, 5-HT1AR blockade decreased correct responses as the delay to reward increased [12]. However, other studies suggest mixed results, with relatively weak or no effect of 5-HT1AR antagonism on delayed discounting [4042]. This may be because those studies confounded incentive and cost factors in each option.

In contrast to delay sensitivity, the involvement of 5-HT in valuation of other types of costs, such as effort, is less clear or controversial. A rodent study of cost-benefit trade-offs showed that SSRIs reduced effort expenditure [43]. Another study also reported similar effects of SSRIs, including suppression of higher effort activities such as lever pressing and wheel running [44]. Other rodent studies have shown that 5-HT depletion with pCPA had no effect on the tendency of rats to make cost-based choices [7,45]. One human study showed that patients taking SSRIs produced more effort for monetary incentives, which was mediated by a reduction in the cost of effort, but no change in the weights of incentives [5]. In contrast, several human studies have reported that SSRIs are relatively ineffective in treating motivational dysfunctions such as fatigue and anergia [44,4650].

As our PET data show (Fig 3), 5-HT1ARs are predominantly expressed in the limbic system, such as the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), amygdala, and hippocampus, which is in good agreement with human data [16]. The binding potential of 5-HT1AR, including in these regions, is diminished in patients with major depressive disorder [51,52] and monkeys exhibiting depression-like behavior [53], suggesting that alteration of 5-HT transmission is a pathophysiology associated with depression and probably its behavioral phenotype, including effortful information processing. However, additional studies are needed to identify the brain circuitry contributing to increased cost sensitivity via 5-HT1AR.

5-HT1AR blockade induced increases in error rate during the reward-size task, with individual variation—the increase was high in 2 monkeys and relatively small in another (Fig 4). Despite the quantitative differences, these effects were qualitatively consistent across individuals in the sense that they could be decomposed into 2 factors: lower incentive and another factor orthogonal to incentive values. Because 1 monkey exhibited relatively weaker effects for both factors, the variation may be due to pharmacological effects, including differences in 5-HT1AR expression level and/or antagonist pharmacokinetics.

In the current study, other receptors were found to have limited or no involvement in the increased cost sensitivity. However, several previous rodent studies suggest a contribution of 5-HT2AR in the mPFC to impulsivity [5456]. Another rodent study showed that antagonism of 5-HT2CR enhanced effort-based motivation [57], suggesting that it mediates decreases rather than increases in motivation. Similarly, the current study showed that blockade of 5-HT4R significantly increased motivation, suggesting that normal 5-HT transition via 5-HT4R decreases incentive (Fig 4E). Importantly, this implies that 5-HT4R is a potential target for treating loss of motivation. Our results, together with other studies, indicate that different 5-HTR subtypes likely modulate motivation in different directions, suggesting a more complicated view of what SSRIs do in humans, as they are assumed to enhance 5-HT transmission equally at multiple receptor subtypes.

Studies examining the activity of 5-HT neurons and their optogenetic manipulation also encourage us to consider motivation-independent factors beyond cost sensitivity. 5-HT neurons convey information not only about reward but also about aversive events [58,59]. In addition, another example of complexity is the involvement of 5-HT in orchestrating behavioral inhibition [60]. Future investigation will be needed to examine the consequence of manipulating 5-HT transmission in nonhuman primates in the context of reward and punishment.

Limitations of this study

First, because we administered antagonists systemically, the current study could not determine which brain areas are responsible for antagonist-induced changes in incentive and expected cost. While our findings, particularly the differential localization of receptor subtypes (Fig 3), support the idea that different limbic structures are involved in incentive and cost sensitivity, further research (such as local infusion of 5-HTR antagonists) is needed to identify key loci and determined the circuitry and molecular mechanisms underlying 5-HT’s role in motivation.

Second, because our receptor blockade was partial and incomplete, the motivational involvement of other receptor subtypes may have been overlooked. For example, previous studies reported the involvement of 5-HT2AR in temporal discounting; specifically, systemic administration of the mixed 5-HT2AR agonist DOI dose-dependently impaired the ability to wait, whereas the 5-HT2AR antagonist ketanserin blocked the impulsivity effect of DOI [61]. In the present study, relatively high blockade of 5-HT2AR (approximately 50% and 80% occupancy) did not alter incentive motivation or cost sensitivity. However, this does not rule out the possible involvement of 5-HT2AR in these functions, as has been previously suggested.

Third, it should be noted that the present study was conducted only in male monkeys. Given that sex differences in 5-HT function are known [62], future studies should examine whether the present results are applicable to female monkeys.

Finally, because 5-HT1AR and 5-HT1BR are located in the cell soma and presynaptic terminals of 5-HT neurons and serve as autoreceptors that regulate 5-HT neuronal activity and release [6365], systemically administering their antagonists may affect serotonin transmission in multiple ways, not only reducing postsynaptic receptor activation but also increasing overall 5-HT signaling. Despite this complexity, behavioral changes resulting from blockade of these receptors partially mimicked those observed after 5-HT depletion, suggesting that the reduction in motivation was mainly due to decreased 5-HT signaling via 5-HT1AR and 5-HT1BR. Clear dissection of presynaptic regulatory mechanisms, especially in primate models, is challenging with currently available technology. Therefore, further investigation using advanced/innovative methods will provide deeper insights into the intricate interplay between 5-HT signaling and receptor subtypes.

Conclusion

The present study demonstrates a differential contribution of receptor subtypes to reduced motivation caused by decreased 5-HT transmission: 5-HT1AR mediates increased cost sensitivity and decreased incentive, whereas 5-HT1BR only mediates reduced incentive. Because these 2 receptor subtypes are differentially distributed in limbic brain areas, their differential contribution might indicate that separate 5-HT circuitry underlies distinct aspects of the valuation processes that regulate motivation for action. Taken together, our findings increase our understanding of how 5-HT signaling impacts motivation in terms of cost-benefit trade-offs, thus providing an advanced framework for understanding the pathophysiology and medications used to treat psychiatric disorders.

Materials and methods

Ethics statement

All experimental procedures involving animals were conducted in accordance with the Guide for the Care and Use of Nonhuman Primates in Neuroscience Research (The Japan Neuroscience Society; https://www.jnss.org/en/animal_primates) and approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of the National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology (#09–1035).

Subjects

A total of 15 adult male macaque monkeys (13 Rhesus and 2 Japanese; 4.5 to 7.9 kg; aged 4 to 12 years at the start of the experiment; see S1 Table for a summary of subjects) were used in this study. Food was available ad libitum, and motivation was controlled by restricting access to fluid to experimental sessions in which water was provided as a reward for task performance. Animals received water supplementation whenever necessary (e.g., when they were unable to obtain sufficient water through experimentation) and had free access to water whenever testing was interrupted for more than 1 week. For environmental enrichment, play objects and/or small foods (fruits, nuts, and vegetables) were provided daily in the home cages.

Drug treatment

For 5-HT depletion, monkeys were injected intraperitoneally with pCPA solution (C3635, Sigma-Aldrich, 0.9% in saline) at a dose of 150 mg/kg/day for 2 consecutive days. We examined the effect of 5-HT depletion on behavior by analyzing the behavioral parameters in the 2 consecutive days before pCPA treatment as control (CON) and those in the days following the first and second treatments (pCPA-day1 and -day2, respectively; Fig 1A). Although we did not perform a control test in the same cohort, we looked at the saline injection data and confirmed that the act of injection itself did not affect error rates in the 2 days after injection [two-way ANOVA, main effect of days, F(2, 138) = 2.3, p = 0.10]. Total motor activity in the home cage was also analyzed after the behavioral test until room lights were turned off each day (16:00 to 21:00 h) after the control and post-pCPA sessions. Motor activity was recorded using motion-sensitive accelerometers (ActiCal, Motion Biosensors, PHILIPS) attached to the monkeys’ collars.

For 5-HTR blockade, the following 5-HTR antagonists were used: WAY100635 (W108, Sigma-Aldrich; for 5-HT1AR), GR55562 (cat# 1054, Tocris; for 5-HT1BR), MDL100907 (M3324, Sigma-Aldrich; for 5-HT2AR), and GR125487 (cat# 1658, Tocris; for 5-HT4R). WAY100635, GR55562, and GR125487 were dissolved in 0.9% saline, while MDL100907 was suspended in a drop of hydrochloric acid and the final volume was adjusted with saline. The dose for each antagonist is listed in Table 1. Monkeys were pretreated with one of the antagonists intramuscularly 15 min before the start of the behavioral test or PET scan. For behavioral testing, saline was injected as a control using the same procedure. The volume administered was set at 1 mL for all experiments.

5-HTR blockade was performed 4 times per individual antagonist in the behavioral test. Each vehicle or antagonist was administered once per week, with days of the week counterbalanced. After completing an antagonist test, the monkeys’ baseline task performances and conditions were assessed, including daily activity in the home cage, body weight, and water and food consumption. If there were no abnormalities, the next sequence of behavioral tests was initiated with a different antagonist. The order of treatment of the 4 antagonists was counterbalanced for each monkey.

Measuring 5-HT metabolites and DA in the CSF

Acute CSF samples were collected by lumbar puncture with a 23-gauge needle under ketamine–xylazine anesthesia. CSF was collected twice (0.5 μl/sample per monkey): once a week before pCPA administration and again the day after 2 consecutive days of pCPA administration. The supernatant was passed through a 0.22-μm filter (Millex-GV, Merck, Germany), centrifuged at 2,190 × g for 20 min, and aliquoted into another microcentrifuge tube. For analysis of 5-HIAA and DA, high-performance liquid chromatography (HTEC-500, EICOM Co., Japan) was used with a monoamine separation column (SC-5ODS, EICOM). CSF data were recorded and analyzed using Power Chrom software (version 2.5, eDAQ, United States of America). The mobile phase was a mixture of 0.1 M phosphate buffer (pH 6.0) and methanol at a ratio of 83:17, including EDTA-2Na (5 mg/L). The amount of monoamine was calculated quantitatively as an absolute amount relative to the monoamine standard solution (MA11-STD, EICOM).

PET procedure and occupancy measurement

Four monkeys were used for PET measurements, which were performed with 4 PET ligands: [11C]WAY100635 (for 5-HT1AR), [11C]AZ10419369 (for 5-HT1BR), [18F]altanserin (for 5-HT2AR), and [11C]SB207145 (for 5-HT4R). PET scans were performed using an SHR-7700 PET scanner (Hamamatsu Photonics, Japan) or microPET Focus220 scanner (Siemens Medical Solutions USA) on conscious monkeys that were seated in a chair. The injected amount of radioactivity and its molar activity at the time of injection were between 88.4 to 360.4 MBq and 14.8 to 296.3 GBq/μmol, respectively. After transmission scans for correcting attenuation, a dynamic emission scan was performed for 90 min, except for scans with [11C]SB207145 (120 min). Ligands were injected as a single bolus via the crural vein at the start of the scan. All emission data were reconstructed by means of filtered back projection using a Colsher or Hanning filter. Radioactive concentrations in tissue were obtained from volumes of interest (VOIs) placed on the global cortex, visual cortex, and cerebellum (as a reference region). Each VOI was defined on individual structural magnetic resonance (MR) images (EXCELART/VG Pianissimo at 1.0 Tesla, Toshiba, Japan) that were co-registered with PET images using PMOD image analysis software (PMOD Technologies, Switzerland). The regional radioactivity of each VOI was calculated for each frame and plotted against time. Regional binding potentials relative to non-displaceable radioligands (BPND) of 5-HTRs were estimated using a simplified reference tissue model. Monkeys were scanned with and without drug treatment on different days.

Parametric images of the BPND were constructed using the original multilinear reference tissue model [66]. Individual structural MR images were registered to the Yerkes19 macaque template [67,68] using FMRIB’s linear registration tool (FLIRT) and FMRIB’s nonlinear registration tool (FNIRT) implemented in FSL software (FMRIB’s Software Library, http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl) [69]. BPND images were then normalized to the template using structural MRI-to-template matrices. Average BP maps across monkeys were plotted on the surface map [70] using the Connectome Workbench [71]. Because BPND is assumed to linearly reflect the spatial distribution and availability of receptors throughout the brain region, we rescaled each BP map by its minimum and maximum values to allow comparison of relative receptor distribution.

Occupancy levels were determined from the degree of BPND reduction by antagonists [72]. 5-HT receptor occupancy was estimated as follows: (3) where BPND Baseline and BPND Treatment are BPND measured without (baseline) and with an antagonist, respectively. The target VOI was the visual cortex for 5-HT1BR and the global cortex for other subtypes.

Behavioral tasks and testing procedures

A total of 8 monkeys were used for the behavioral study (see S1 Table). For all behavioral training and testing, each monkey sat in a primate chair inside a sound-attenuated dark room. Visual stimuli were presented on a computer monitor in front of the monkey. Behavioral control and data acquisition were performed using a QNX-based Real-time Experimentation data acquisition system (REX; Laboratory of Sensorimotor Research, National Eye Institute) and commercially available software (Presentation, Neurobehavioral Systems). We used 2 types of behavioral tasks, reward-size and work/delay, as previously described [2,18]. Before the behavioral experiments, all monkeys were trained to perform color discrimination trials on a cued multi-trial reward schedule task [17] for more than 3 months.

In the reward-size task, a monkey initiated a trial by touching the bar in the chair; 100 ms later, a visual cue (13° on a side), which will be described below, was presented at the center of the monitor. After 500 ms, a red target (0.5° on a side) also appeared at the center of the monitor. After a variable interval of 500, 750, 1,000, 1,250, or 1,500 ms, the target turned green, indicating that the monkey could release the bar to earn a liquid reward. If the monkey responded within 200 to 1,000 ms, the target turned blue, indicating that the trial had been completed correctly. In correct trials, a reward of 1, 2, 4, or 8 drops of water (1 drop = ~0.1 ml) was delivered immediately after the blue signal. Each reward size was selected randomly with equal probability. The visual cue presented at the beginning of the trial indicated the number of reward drops that the monkey would receive (Fig 2A). An inter-trial interval (ITI) of 1 s was enforced before the next trial began.

In the work/delay task, the basic task consisted of a series of color discrimination trials that was the same as in the reward-size task (see Fig 5A). The main difference was that, unlike the reward-size task, we manipulated task requirements but fixed the amount of reward. There were 2 types of trial: work trials and delay trials. In work trials, the monkeys had to perform 0, 1, or 2 additional instrumental trials to obtain the reward, and a visual cue indicated how many trials remained until a water reward (approximately 0.25 ml) was delivered. In delay trials, the monkeys performed 1 color discrimination and a water reward (approximately 0.25 ml) was delivered immediately after each correct signal or after a delay period. The visual cue indicated both the trial type and cost to obtain a reward (Fig 5A). Pattern cues indicated delay trials and the timing of reward delivery after a correct performance: immediately (0.3 s, 0.2 to 0.4 s; mean, range), after a short delay (3.6 s, 3.0 to 4.2 s), or after a long delay (7.2 s, 6.0 to 8.4 s). Grayscale cues indicated work trials and the number of extra trials the monkey would have to perform to obtain a reward. We set delay durations to be equivalent to the duration of 1 or 2 trials of color discrimination so that we could directly compare the cost of 1 or 2 arbitrary units (cost unit; CU) [18]. All other aspects of the task were the same as those in the reward-size task.

In both tasks, if the monkey released the bar before the green target appeared, within 200 ms after it appeared, or failed to respond within 1 s, we regarded the trial as an “error trial.” At this point, all visual stimuli disappeared, the trial was terminated immediately, and after a 1-s ITI, the trial was repeated with the same cue-reward/cost condition. Our behavioral measurement of motivation was the proportion of error trials. Before each testing session, monkeys were subjected to approximately 22 h of water restriction in their home cage. Each session continued until the monkey would no longer initiate a new trial (usually less than 100 min). The monkeys were tested with the work/delay task for 1 or 2 daily sessions as training to become familiar with the cueing condition. Each monkey was tested from Monday to Friday. Treatment with 5-HTR antagonist or saline (as a control) was performed 1 day per week.

Behavioral data analysis

All data and statistical analyses were performed using the R statistical computing environment. The average error rate for each trial type was calculated for each daily session, with error rates for each trial type being defined as the number of error trials divided by the total number of trials of that given type. We did not distinguish between the 2 types of errors (early or late release) and used their sum except for the error pattern analysis. We performed repeated-measures ANOVAs to determine the effect of treatment × reward size (for data in the reward-size task) or treatment × cost type × remaining cost (for data in the work/delay task) on RT and error pattern. Post hoc comparisons were performed using Tukey’s HSD test, and a priori statistical significance was set at p = 0.05.

We used error rates to estimate the level of motivation, because the error rates of these tasks (E) are inversely related to the value for action [2]. In the reward-size task, we used the inverse function (Eq 1). We fit the data to LMMs [73] in which the random effects across 5-HT depletion conditions (i.e., cCPA-day1 and -day2) on parameter a and/or intercept e (Fig 1) were nested. Model selection was based on the BIC, an estimator of in-sample prediction error for nested models (S3 Table). Using the selected model, parameter a and intercept e were estimated simultaneously within each subject. Parameter a was then normalized to the value in the nontreated condition (control, CON). LMMs were also applied for correlation analysis between error rate and RT. For the 5-HT depletion study, mean error rates and RTs were calculated for each reward size in the first and second halves of each session to increase the number of data sets (Fig 2E). Four statistical models were nested to account for the presence or absence of random effects of monkey and treatment conditions and the best-fit model was selected based on BIC (S4 Table).

To examine the effects of satiation, each session was divided into 3 parts based on normalized cumulative reward, Rcum. Mean error rates in the reward size task across 11 sessions were then fit to the following model: (4) where the satiation effect, F(Rcum), as the reward value was exponentially decaying in Rcum at a constant λ [18]: (5)

To estimate the effect of 5-HTR blockade on the parameters, we also used LMMs. Models were nested to account for the presence or absence of random effects, random effects of treatment conditions, and subjects (see S5 Table). The best model was selected based on the BIC for the entire dataset, which is the sum of the results for the regression of each unit nested by individual and/or treatment condition. We confirmed the robustness of the parameter(s) by performing the Bayesian inference analysis, which indicates how reliable 2 parameters are in fitting the data (see S9 Fig).

In the work/delay task, we used linear models to estimate the effect of remaining cost, i.e., workloads and delay, as previously described [15,19]: (6) (7) where Ew and Ed are the error rates, and kw and kd are cost factors for work and delay trials, respectively. CU is the number of remaining cost units, and e is the intercept. We simultaneously fitted a pair of these linear models to the data by sum-of-squares minimization without weighting. We also used LMMs to estimate the effect of 5-HTR blockade on discounting parameters. We imposed the constraint that the intercept (e) has the same value between cost types and assumed a basic statistical model in which the random effects of blocking condition independently affect the regression coefficients. Other methods were the same as those used for data in the reward-size task. Models and results are reported in S7 Table.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Summary of subjects used in this study.

A tick indicates the monkey used the experiment. Species (J, Japanese; R, Resus); Sex (F, Female; M, Male); BW, body weight; CSF, cerebrospinal fluid; RS task, reward-size task; W/D task, work/delay task.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Summary of drug treatment for each monkey.

Numbers indicate the number of treatments.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Model comparison for the effect of 5-HT depletion on error rate (for Fig 1E).

a(cond) and e(cond) indicate the random effects of conditions with 5-HT depletion on parameters a and e, respectively. The random effect of Model #3 was assumed to be normally distributed on a and e, independently. The random effect of Model #4 was assumed to be a 2D normal distribution on a and e: (1/acond, econd) ~ biNorm(0, ∑cond). BIC is a relative measure of quality for the models (#1–5). ΔBIC denotes the difference from the minimum BIC.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s003

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Model comparison for the effect of 5-HT depletion on the relationship between error rate and RT in the reward-size task (for Fig 2D).

a(mk), a(cond), e(mk), and e(cond) indicate the random effects of 5-HT depletion conditions on parameters a and e, respectively. The probability distribution of the random effects were as follows: amk and acond were ~N(0,σ2mk) and ~N(0,σ2cond), respectively. (a’mk, e’mk) and (a’cond, e’cond) were ~biNorm(0, ∑mk) and ~biNorm(0, ∑cond), respectively. E, error rate; RT, reaction time; mk, monkey; cond, treatment condition (pCPA-day1, pCPA-day2, or control). BIC is a relative measure of quality for the models (#1–4). ΔBIC denotes the difference from the minimum BIC.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s004

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Model comparison for the effect of 5-HTR blockade on error rate in the reward-size task (for Fig 4).

a(cond) a(mk), a(cond_mk), e(cond), e(mk), and e(cond_mk) indicate the random effects of blocking 5-HTR on parameters a and e, respectively. The probability distribution of the random effects were as follows: acond, amk, and acond_mk were ~N(0,σ2cond), ~N(0,σ2mk), and ~N(0,σ2cond_mk), respectively. (a’cond, e’cond), (a’mk, e’mk), and (a’cond_mk, e’cond_mk) were ~biNorm(0, ∑cond), ~biNorm(0, ∑mk), and ~biNorm(0, ∑cond_mk), respectively. E, error rate; cond, treatment condition (antagonist or control); mk, monkey; cond_mk, interaction of treatment condition and monkey. BIC is a relative measure of quality for the models (#1–30). ΔBIC denotes the difference from the minimum BIC.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s005

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Model comparison for the effect of 5-HTR blockade on the relationship between error rate and RT in the reward-size task (for S4 Fig).

a(mk), a(cond), e(mk), and e(cond) indicate the random effects of 5-HT depletion on parameters a and e, respectively. The probability distribution of the random effects were as follows: acond and amk were ~N(0,σ2cond) and ~N(0,σ2mk), respectively. (a’cond, e’cond) and (a’mk, e’mk) were ~biNorm(0, ∑cond) and ~biNorm(0, ∑mk), respectively. E, error rate; RT, reaction time; mk, monkey; cond, treatment condition (antagonist or control). BIC is a relative measure of quality for the models. ΔBIC denotes the difference from the minimum BIC.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s006

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Model comparison for the effect of 5-HTR blockade on error rate in the work/delay task (for Fig 5C).

CU and E0 indicate the remaining cost and the intercept, respectively. k(treat), k(type), e(treat), and e(type) denote the random effects of blocking 5-HT on parameters k and e, respectively. The probability distribution of the random effects were as follows: ktreat and ktype were ~N(0,σ2treat) and ~N(0,σ2type), respectively. (k’treat, e’treat) and (k’type, e’type) were ~biNorm(0, ∑treat) and ~biNorm(0, ∑type), respectively. E, error rate; treat, treatment condition (antagonist or control): type, trial type (delay or work). BIC is a relative measure of quality for the models (#1–25). ΔBIC denotes the difference from the minimum BIC.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s007

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Occupancy estimation.

Example of occupancy estimation based on reduction in specific tracer binding. (A) Representative coronal section from an MR image showing the region of interest for binding measurements (ROI, drawn by purple line). (B, C) Representative coronal sections from parametric PET images showing the specific binding (BPND) of [11C]WAY100635 in monkey CH during baseline (B) and blocking (C) conditions (pretreated with cold WAY100635 at a dose of 0.3 mg/kg, i.m.). Occupancy was determined as the proportion of reduced specific binding relative to baseline [i.e., (BPND baseline–BPND blocking)/BPND baseline]. In this case, the reduction of specific binding was 36.7%.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s008

(EPS)

S2 Fig. No discernible effect of high-dose 5-HT2AR blockade on error rates in the reward-size task.

Error rates (mean ± SEM) as a function of reward size for control (black) and 5-HTR blockade at high 5-HT2AR occupancy (84%) (MDL100907 at 0.03 mg/kg, i.m.; dark green). There was no significant difference in error rates between CON and 5-HT2AR blockade (two-way ANOVA, main effect of treatment, F(1, 72) = 0.54, P = 0.46). Dotted curves indicate the best fit of inverse functions (Model #25 in S5 Table). Data obtained from monkey KN. The data underlying this figure can be found in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10141750.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s009

(EPS)

S3 Fig. Effect of 5-HTR blockade on reaction time in the reward-size task.

Mean reaction time as a function of reward size for control (black) and 5-HTR blockade conditions (color). (A) 5-HT1A blockade, (B) 5-HT1BR blockade, (C) 5-HT2AR blockade, and (D) 5-HT4R blockade. The data underlying this figure can be found in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10141750.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s010

(EPS)

S4 Fig. Effect of 5-HTR blockade on error pattern in the reward-size task.

Early release rate (mean ± SEM) as a function of reward size for control (black) and 5-HTR blockade conditions (color). (A) 5-HT1A blockade, (B) 5-HT1BR blockade, (C) 5-HT2AR blockade, and (D) 5-HT4R blockade. The data underlying this figure can be found in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10141750.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s011

(EPS)

S5 Fig. Effect of 5-HTR blockade on the relationship between error rate and reaction time in the reward-size task.

Session-by-session relationships between error rate and average reaction time for each reward size during control and after 5-HTR blockade for each monkey. Colors indicate treatment condition. Lines represent the best-fit linear regression models that explain the data. The data underlying this figure can be found in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10141750.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s012

(EPS)

S6 Fig. Effect of 5-HTR blockade on reaction time in the work/delay task.

Relationships between error rates (mean ± SEM) and remaining costs for delay (top) and work trials (bottom) for monkeys KY (A) and MP (B), respectively. Data for saline control (black line), 5-HT1AR, 5-HT1BR, 5-HT2AR, and 5-HT4R blockade (colored lines) are shown from left to right, respectively. Colored and black dashed lines indicate the best-fit linear models described in S7 Table. The data underlying this figure can be found in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10141750.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s013

(EPS)

S7 Fig. Effect of 5-HTR blockade on reaction time in the work/delay task.

Mean reaction time as a function of reward size for control (black) and 5-HTR blockade conditions (color) in delay and work trials. The data underlying this figure can be found in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10141750.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s014

(EPS)

S8 Fig. Effect of 5-HTR blockade on error pattern in the work/delay task.

Late release rate (mean ± SEM) as a function of reward size for control (black) and 5-HTR blockade conditions (color). The data underlying this figure can be found in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10141750.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s015

(EPS)

S9 Fig. Parameter estimation of the data for 5-HT1AR blockade and control in monkey KN.

The red and green dots represent the posterior distribution of the model parameters from an Hamiltonian Monte Carlo simulation (iter_warmup = 2000, iter_sampling = 4000, and chains = 4) for 5-HT1AR blockade and control conditions, respectively. The red and green circles indicate the 95th percentile for each condition. In both conditions, the parameter obtained from the best-fit LMM model (+) are very close to the maximum a posteriori (MAP) estimate from the simulation (black dots), suggesting a high reliability of the parameter estimates. All R-hat values were below 1.02, indicating that this Bayesian simulation was statistically converged. All random effects were statistically significant as the 95% confidence intervals deviated from 0. The data underlying this figure can be found in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10141750.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002445.s016

(EPS)

Acknowledgments

We thank R. Suma, T. Sugii, R. Yamaguchi, Y. Matsuda, and J. Kamei for their technical assistance. We also thank S. Bouret for his comments on the earlier version of the manuscript. The 2 Japanese monkeys used in this study were provided by National Bio-Resource Project “Japanese Monkeys” of MEXT, Japan.

References

  1. 1. Berridge KC. Motivation concepts in behavioral neuroscience. Physiol Behav. 2004;81(2):179–209. pmid:15159167
  2. 2. Minamimoto T, La Camera G, Richmond BJ. Measuring and modeling the interaction among reward size, delay to reward, and satiation level on motivation in monkeys. J Neurophysiol. 2009;101(1):437–447. pmid:18987119
  3. 3. Pessiglione M, Vinckier F, Bouret S, Daunizeau J, Le Bouc R. Why not try harder? Computational approach to motivation deficits in neuro-psychiatric diseases. Brain. 2018;141(3):629–650. pmid:29194534
  4. 4. Husain M, Roiser JP. Neuroscience of apathy and anhedonia: a transdiagnostic approach. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19(8):470–484. pmid:29946157
  5. 5. Meyniel F, Goodwin GM, Deakin JW, Klinge C, MacFadyen C, Milligan H, et al. A specific role for serotonin in overcoming effort cost. Elife. 2016:5. pmid:27824554
  6. 6. Bizot J, Le Bihan C, Puech AJ, Hamon M, Thiebot M. Serotonin and tolerance to delay of reward in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1999;146(4):400–412. pmid:10550490
  7. 7. Denk F, Walton ME, Jennings KA, Sharp T, Rushworth MF, Bannerman DM. Differential involvement of serotonin and dopamine systems in cost-benefit decisions about delay or effort. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2005;179(3):587–596. pmid:15864561
  8. 8. Mobini S, Chiang TJ, Ho MY, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E. Effects of central 5-hydroxytryptamine depletion on sensitivity to delayed and probabilistic reinforcement. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2000;152(4):390–397. pmid:11140331
  9. 9. Schweighofer N, Bertin M, Shishida K, Okamoto Y, Tanaka SC, Yamawaki S, et al. Low-serotonin levels increase delayed reward discounting in humans. J Neurosci. 2008;28(17):4528–4532. pmid:18434531
  10. 10. Wogar MA, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E. Does the effect of central 5-hydroxytryptamine depletion on timing depend on motivational change? Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1993;112(1):86–92. pmid:7871014
  11. 11. Wogar MA, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E. Effect of lesions of the ascending 5-hydroxytryptaminergic pathways on choice between delayed reinforcers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1993;111(2):239–243. pmid:7870958
  12. 12. Akizawa F, Mizuhiki T, Setogawa T, Takafuji M, Shidara M. The effect of 5-HT(1A) receptor antagonist on reward-based decision-making. J Physiol Sci. 2019;69(6):1057–1069. pmid:31705485
  13. 13. Miyazaki KW, Miyazaki K, Doya K. Activation of dorsal raphe serotonin neurons is necessary for waiting for delayed rewards. J Neurosci. 2012;32(31):10451–10457. pmid:22855794
  14. 14. Barnes NM, Ahern GP, Becamel C, Bockaert J, Camilleri M, Chaumont-Dubel S, et al. International Union of Basic and Clinical Pharmacology. CX. Classification of Receptors for 5-hydroxytryptamine; Pharmacology and Function. Pharmacol Rev. 2021;73(1):310–520. pmid:33370241
  15. 15. Hori Y, Nagai Y, Mimura K, Suhara T, Higuchi M, Bouret S, et al. D1- and D2-like receptors differentially mediate the effects of dopaminergic transmission on cost-benefit evaluation and motivation in monkeys. PLoS Biol. 2021;19(7):e3001055. pmid:34197448
  16. 16. Beliveau V, Ganz M, Feng L, Ozenne B, Hojgaard L, Fisher PM, et al. A High-Resolution In Vivo Atlas of the Human Brain’s Serotonin System. J Neurosci. 2017;37(1):120–128. pmid:28053035
  17. 17. Bowman EM, Aigner TG, Richmond BJ. Neural signals in the monkey ventral striatum related to motivation for juice and cocaine rewards. J Neurophysiol. 1996;75(3):1061–1073. pmid:8867118
  18. 18. Minamimoto T, Hori Y, Richmond BJ. Is working more costly than waiting in monkeys? PLoS ONE. 2012;7(11):e48434. pmid:23144881
  19. 19. Minamimoto T, Yamada H, Hori Y, Suhara T. Hydration level is an internal variable for computing motivation to obtain water rewards in monkeys. Exp Brain Res. 2012;218(4):609–618. pmid:22411583
  20. 20. Cools R, Blackwell A, Clark L, Menzies L, Cox S, Robbins TW. Tryptophan depletion disrupts the motivational guidance of goal-directed behavior as a function of trait impulsivity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30(7):1362–1373. pmid:15770237
  21. 21. Roiser JP, Blackwell AD, Cools R, Clark L, Rubinsztein DC, Robbins TW, et al. Serotonin transporter polymorphism mediates vulnerability to loss of incentive motivation following acute tryptophan depletion. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(10):2264–2272. pmid:16541086
  22. 22. Lammertsma AA, Hume SP. Simplified reference tissue model for PET receptor studies. Neuroimage. 1996;4(3 Pt 1):153–158. pmid:9345505
  23. 23. Winstanley CA, Dalley JW, Theobald DE, Robbins TW. Fractionating impulsivity: contrasting effects of central 5-HT depletion on different measures of impulsive behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29(7):1331–1343. pmid:15054475
  24. 24. Cools R, Nakamura K, Daw ND. Serotonin and dopamine: unifying affective, activational, and decision functions. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(1):98–113. pmid:20736991
  25. 25. Fischer AG, Jocham G, Ullsperger M. Dual serotonergic signals: a key to understanding paradoxical effects? Trends Cogn Sci. 2014. pmid:25532701
  26. 26. Boulenguez P, Rawlins JN, Chauveau J, Joseph MH, Mitchell SN, Gray JA. Modulation of dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens by 5-HT1B agonists: involvement of the hippocampo-accumbens pathway. Neuropharmacology. 1996;35(11):1521–1529. pmid:9025099
  27. 27. Yan QS, Zheng SZ, Yan SE. Involvement of 5-HT1B receptors within the ventral tegmental area in regulation of mesolimbic dopaminergic neuronal activity via GABA mechanisms: a study with dual-probe microdialysis. Brain Res. 2004;1021(1):82–91. pmid:15328035
  28. 28. Przegalinski E, Golda A, Filip M. Effects of serotonin (5-HT)(1B) receptor ligands on cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Pharmacol Rep. 2008;60(6):798–810. pmid:19211971
  29. 29. Watakabe A, Komatsu Y, Sadakane O, Shimegi S, Takahata T, Higo N, et al. Enriched expression of serotonin 1B and 2A receptor genes in macaque visual cortex and their bidirectional modulatory effects on neuronal responses. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(8):1915–1928. pmid:19056862
  30. 30. Shimegi S, Kimura A, Sato A, Aoyama C, Mizuyama R, Tsunoda K, et al. Cholinergic and serotonergic modulation of visual information processing in monkey V1. J Physiol Paris. 2016;110(1–2):44–51. pmid:27619519
  31. 31. Desrochers SS, Lesko EK, Magalong VM, Balsam PD, Nautiyal KM. A role for reward valuation in the serotonergic modulation of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2021;238(11):3293–3309. pmid:34390360
  32. 32. Yamanaka H, Yokoyama C, Mizuma H, Kurai S, Finnema SJ, Halldin C, et al. A possible mechanism of the nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum 5-HT1B receptors underlying the antidepressant action of ketamine: a PET study with macaques. Transl Psychiatry. 2014;4(1):e342. pmid:24399045
  33. 33. Clark L, Boileau I, Zack M. Neuroimaging of reward mechanisms in Gambling disorder: an integrative review. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;24(5):674–693. pmid:30214041
  34. 34. Hu J, Henry S, Gallezot JD, Ropchan J, Neumaier JF, Potenza MN, et al. Serotonin 1B receptor imaging in alcohol dependence. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(9):800–803. pmid:20172504
  35. 35. Li Y, Simmler LD, Van Zessen R, Flakowski J, Wan JX, Deng F, et al. Synaptic mechanism underlying serotonin modulation of transition to cocaine addiction. Science. 2021;373(6560):1252–1256. pmid:34516792
  36. 36. Matuskey D, Bhagwagar Z, Planeta B, Pittman B, Gallezot JD, Chen J, et al. Reductions in brain 5-HT1B receptor availability in primarily cocaine-dependent humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;76(10):816–822. pmid:24433854
  37. 37. Pietrzak RH, Henry S, Southwick SM, Krystal JH, Neumeister A. Linking in vivo brain serotonin type 1B receptor density to phenotypic heterogeneity of posttraumatic stress symptomatology. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18(4):399–401. pmid:22584868
  38. 38. Crockett MJ, Clark L, Smillie LD, Robbins TW. The effects of acute tryptophan depletion on costly information sampling: impulsivity or aversive processing? Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2012;219(2):587–597. pmid:22094531
  39. 39. Homberg JR. Serotonin and decision making processes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36(1):218–236. pmid:21693132
  40. 40. Evenden JL. Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1999;146(4):348–361. pmid:10550486
  41. 41. Evenden JL, Ryan CN. The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats VI: the effects of ethanol and selective serotonergic drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1999;146(4):413–421. pmid:10550491
  42. 42. Yates JR, Perry JL, Meyer AC, Gipson CD, Charnigo R, Bardo MT. Role of medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal monoamine transporters and receptors in performance in an adjusting delay discounting procedure. Brain Res. 2014;1574:26–36. pmid:24928616
  43. 43. Yohn SE, Lopez-Cruz L, Hutson PH, Correa M, Salamone JD. Effects of lisdexamfetamine and s-citalopram, alone and in combination, on effort-related choice behavior in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2016;233(6):949–960. pmid:26694811
  44. 44. Presby RE, Rotolo RA, Hurley EM, Ferrigno SM, Murphy CE, McMullen HP, et al. Sex differences in lever pressing and running wheel tasks of effort-based choice behavior in rats: Suppression of high effort activity by the serotonin transport inhibitor fluoxetine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2021;202:173115. pmid:33493546
  45. 45. Izquierdo A, Carlos K, Ostrander S, Rodriguez D, McCall-Craddolph A, Yagnik G, et al. Impaired reward learning and intact motivation after serotonin depletion in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2012;233(2):494–499. pmid:22652392
  46. 46. Papakostas GI, Nutt DJ, Hallett LA, Tucker VL, Krishen A, Fava M. Resolution of sleepiness and fatigue in major depressive disorder: A comparison of bupropion and the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(12):1350–1355. pmid:16934768
  47. 47. Fava M, Ball S, Nelson JC, Sparks J, Konechnik T, Classi P, et al. Clinical relevance of fatigue as a residual symptom in major depressive disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2014;31(3):250–257. pmid:24115209
  48. 48. Cooper JA, Tucker VL, Papakostas GI. Resolution of sleepiness and fatigue: a comparison of bupropion and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in subjects with major depressive disorder achieving remission at doses approved in the European Union. J Psychopharmacol. 2014;28(2):118–124. pmid:24352716
  49. 49. Rothschild AJ, Raskin J, Wang CN, Marangell LB, Fava M. The relationship between change in apathy and changes in cognition and functional outcomes in currently non-depressed SSRI-treated patients with major depressive disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2014;55(1):1–10. pmid:24268564
  50. 50. Popovic D, Vieta E, Fornaro M, Perugi G. Cognitive tolerability following successful long term treatment of major depression and anxiety disorders with SSRi antidepressants. J Affect Disord. 2015;173:211–215. pmid:25462418
  51. 51. Drevets WC, Frank E, Price JC, Kupfer DJ, Greer PJ, Mathis C. Serotonin type-1A receptor imaging in depression. Nucl Med Biol. 2000;27(5):499–507. pmid:10962258
  52. 52. Hirvonen J, Karlsson H, Kajander J, Lepola A, Markkula J, Rasi-Hakala H, et al. Decreased brain serotonin 5-HT1A receptor availability in medication-naive patients with major depressive disorder: an in-vivo imaging study using PET and [carbonyl-11C]WAY-100635. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;11(4):465–476. pmid:17971260
  53. 53. Shively CA, Friedman DP, Gage HD, Bounds MC, Brown-Proctor C, Blair JB, et al. Behavioral depression and positron emission tomography-determined serotonin 1A receptor binding potential in cynomolgus monkeys. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(4):396–403. pmid:16585468
  54. 54. Carli M, Baviera M, Invernizzi RW, Balducci C. Dissociable contribution of 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptors in the medial prefrontal cortex to different aspects of executive control such as impulsivity and compulsive perseveration in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(4):757–767. pmid:16192987
  55. 55. Talpos JC, Wilkinson LS, Robbins TW. A comparison of multiple 5-HT receptors in two tasks measuring impulsivity. J Psychopharmacol. 2006;20(1):47–58. pmid:16204332
  56. 56. Winstanley CA, Dalley JW, Theobald DEH, Robbins TW. Global 5-HT depletion attenuates the ability of amphetamine to decrease impulsive choice on a delay-discounting task in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2003;170(3):320–331. pmid:12955303
  57. 57. Bailey MR, Goldman O, Bello EP, Chohan MO, Jeong N, Winiger V, et al. An Interaction between Serotonin Receptor Signaling and Dopamine Enhances Goal-Directed Vigor and Persistence in Mice. J Neurosci. 2018;38(9):2149–2162. pmid:29367407
  58. 58. Cohen JY, Amoroso MW, Uchida N. Serotonergic neurons signal reward and punishment on multiple timescales. Elife. 2015:4. pmid:25714923
  59. 59. Paquelet GE, Carrion K, Lacefield CO, Zhou P, Hen R, Miller BR. Single-cell activity and network properties of dorsal raphe nucleus serotonin neurons during emotionally salient behaviors. Neuron. 2022;110(16):2664–79 e8. pmid:35700737
  60. 60. Berwian IM, Wenzel JG, Collins AGE, Seifritz E, Stephan KE, Walter H, et al. Computational Mechanisms of Effort and Reward Decisions in Patients With Depression and Their Association With Relapse After Antidepressant Discontinuation. JAMA Psychiatry. 2020;77(5):513–522. pmid:32074255
  61. 61. Hadamitzky M, Feja M, Becker T, Koch M. Effects of acute systemic administration of serotonin2A/C receptor ligands in a delay-based decision-making task in rats. Behav Pharmacol. 2009;20(5–6):415–423. pmid:19730369
  62. 62. Nishizawa S, Benkelfat C, Young SN, Leyton M, Mzengeza S, de Montigny C, et al. Differences between males and females in rates of serotonin synthesis in human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997;94(10):5308–5313. pmid:9144233
  63. 63. Boschert U, Amara DA, Segu L, Hen R. The mouse 5-hydroxytryptamine1B receptor is localized predominantly on axon terminals. Neuroscience. 1994;58(1):167–182. pmid:8159291
  64. 64. Hannon J, Hoyer D. Molecular biology of 5-HT receptors. Behav Brain Res. 2008;195(1):198–213. pmid:18571247
  65. 65. Pazos A, Palacios JM. Quantitative autoradiographic mapping of serotonin receptors in the rat brain. I. Serotonin-1 receptors. Brain Res. 1985;346(2):205–230. pmid:4052776
  66. 66. Ichise M, Ballinger JR, Golan H, Vines D, Luong A, Tsai S, et al. Noninvasive quantification of dopamine D2 receptors with iodine-123-IBF SPECT. J Nucl Med. 1996;37(3):513–520. pmid:8772658
  67. 67. Donahue CJ, Glasser MF, Preuss TM, Rilling JK, Van Essen DC. Quantitative assessment of prefrontal cortex in humans relative to nonhuman primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115(22):E5183–E5192. pmid:29739891
  68. 68. Donahue CJ, Sotiropoulos SN, Jbabdi S, Hernandez-Fernandez M, Behrens TE, Dyrby TB, et al. Using Diffusion Tractography to Predict Cortical Connection Strength and Distance: A Quantitative Comparison with Tracers in the Monkey. J Neurosci. 2016;36(25):6758–6770. pmid:27335406
  69. 69. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. 2004;23(Suppl 1):S208–S219. pmid:15501092
  70. 70. Autio JA, Glasser MF, Ose T, Donahue CJ, Bastiani M, Ohno M, et al. Towards HCP-Style macaque connectomes: 24-Channel 3T multi-array coil, MRI sequences and preprocessing. Neuroimage. 2020;215:116800. pmid:32276072
  71. 71. Marcus DS, Harwell J, Olsen T, Hodge M, Glasser MF, Prior F, et al. Informatics and data mining tools and strategies for the human connectome project. Front Neuroinform. 2011;5:4. pmid:21743807
  72. 72. Cunningham VJ, Rabiner EA, Slifstein M, Laruelle M, Gunn RN. Measuring drug occupancy in the absence of a reference region: the Lassen plot re-visited. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2010;30(1):46–50. pmid:19738632
  73. 73. Rajala AZ, Jenison RL, Populin LC. Decision making: effects of methylphenidate on temporal discounting in nonhuman primates. J Neurophysiol. 2015;114(1):70–79. pmid:25972584