Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Extended parental provisioning and variation in vertebrate brain sizes

  • Carel P. van Schaik ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    vschaik@aim.uzh.ch

    Affiliations Department of Ecology of Animal Societies, Max Planck Institute for Animal Behavior, Konstanz, Germany, Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland, Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

  • Zitan Song,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Ecology of Animal Societies, Max Planck Institute for Animal Behavior, Konstanz, Germany

  • Caroline Schuppli,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Development and Evolution of Cognition Research group, Max Planck Institute for Animal Behavior, Konstanz, Germany

  • Szymon M. Drobniak,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Evolution & Ecology Research Centre, School of Biological, Environmental & Earth Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia, Institute of Environmental Sciences; Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland

  • Sandra A. Heldstab,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland, Development and Evolution of Cognition Research group, Max Planck Institute for Animal Behavior, Konstanz, Germany

  • Michael Griesser

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany, Center for the Advanced Study of Collective Behavior, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany, Department of Collective Behavior, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Konstanz, Germany

Abstract

Large brains provide adaptive cognitive benefits but require unusually high, near-constant energy inputs and become fully functional well after their growth is completed. Consequently, young of most larger-brained endotherms should not be able to independently support the growth and development of their own brains. This paradox is solved if the evolution of extended parental provisioning facilitated brain size evolution. Comparative studies indeed show that extended parental provisioning coevolved with brain size and that it may improve immature survival. The major role of extended parental provisioning supports the idea that the ability to sustain the costs of brains limited brain size evolution.

Introduction: Expensive brains

The brain analyzes and integrates the inputs from our senses, regulates our physiology, and generates the motor commands for our movements. In addition, it is responsible for everything between perception and action, i.e., cognition. Relative to body size, brain size is extremely variable across vertebrates [1,2]. Mean brain sizes of ectothermic (fishes, amphibians, and reptiles) and endothermic (birds and mammals) lineages differ about 10-fold, but also vary considerably within each lineage (Fig 1). In addition, brains have tended to become larger over evolutionary time [1,2]. Understanding this striking variation and these evolutionary trends in relative brain size is a major task for comparative biology.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Brain size-body size envelopes of the major vertebrate lineages, to illustrate both intra-lineage and inter-lineage variation.

The long-dashed outlines represent the 2 endothermic lineages (birds and mammals), the dotted outlines represent fishes, and the solid outlines the 2 ectothermic tetrapod lineages (amphibians and reptiles). Redrawn after [97].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002016.g001

Brain size is generally positively correlated with the amount of sensory information a species processes (e.g., electrosensing in mormyroid fishes: [3]; stereoscopic vision in primates: [4,5]) or the precision of its motor control (e.g., number of legs in lizards: [6]; manipulation complexity in primates: [7]), suggesting that these enhanced sensorimotor functions alone may explain brain size changes without reference to greater cognitive abilities [8]. Nonetheless, comparative studies also show a clear link between relative brain size and more narrowly defined cognitive abilities, such as greater capacity for independent or social learning [912], and thus greater domain-general intelligence [13,14] and executive functions, such as self-control [15,16]. Because sensorimotor capacities do not vary systematically within species, intraspecific correlations between brain size and domain-general intelligence would provide even more convincing evidence for an effect of brain size on narrow-sense cognition. Indeed, in humans, brain size explains a modest, but robust proportion of variation in intelligence [17,18], a result now replicated in chimpanzees [19] and chestnut-headed thrushes [20].

These 3 sets of abilities (i.e., sensory input, cognitive processing, and motor output) are expected to coevolve. Having perfect information without sophisticated cognitive processing and advanced abilities to act upon the world would not be adaptive. Brain size should therefore also predict behavioral performance in fitness-enhancing activities. Indeed, larger-brained species are capable of extractive foraging [21] and tend to be more innovative in the foraging domain (primates: [22]; birds: [23]). They are also better at avoiding predators (mammals: [24]; birds: [25]) and more likely to survive when introduced into novel areas by humans (mammals: [26]; birds: [27]; reptiles and amphibians: [28]). These effects could ultimately lead to correlated evolution between brain size and maximum lifespan. Comparative studies have confirmed such a correlation for most mammals [2931], birds [32,33], and frogs [34], though not for reptiles [35]. An improved ability to form social bonds with conspecifics may also be linked to larger brain size [36] (but see [37]). A more indirect consequence of improved survival is that larger-brained species have more stable populations (primates: [38]; birds: [39]), and hence, a reduced risk of local extinction [40].

All these findings indicate that increasing brain size should very often be adaptive, as confirmed by the upward evolutionary trend in brain size [1]. One might therefore expect that, once controlled for body size differences, brain sizes would be similar across taxa. However, this is not the case: major differences between closely related lineages exist [41], as do differences between more distantly related lineages with similar cognitive demands, such as between social carnivores and anthropoid primates [42]. These differences imply that some brain-size related costs prevent the evolution of similar brain sizes in particular lineages, despite these various cognitive benefits [43]. Thus, a comprehensive explanation for the variation in brain size requires that we incorporate the fitness costs of increased brain (cf. [43]). This is what the expensive brain hypothesis [30] attempts to do.

Brains are unusually costly organs due to their high energy use per unit weight [4446] and especially because energy allocation to the brain cannot not be down-regulated during times of starvation (brain sparing: [47,48]). Interruption of this constant energy flow to the brain generally has lasting negative consequences for brain development and cognitive performance [4952]. As a result, brain size is presumably limited by the organism’s ability to sustain the energy turnover needed to grow or maintain the brain in response to cognitive opportunities in the ecological or social environment. Reduced energy inputs can arise due to ecologically imposed limitations on overall energy acquisition, in particular, the inability to adequately deal with periods of unavoidable food scarcity. Alternatively, reduced allocation to other competing costly functions, such as digestion [45] or production (i.e., growth and reproduction [2934,5355]), could enable increases in brain sizes. A recent review [56] found extensive empirical support for this hypothesis. In sum, the expensive brain hypothesis helps to explain why brain size does not always correspond to expectations based on the cognitive demands and opportunities offered by social systems or ecological niches. This conclusion holds even if controlled for taxonomic variation in neuron densities [57,58].

Because the expensive brain hypothesis focuses on the costs of brain size, it complements the various hypotheses postulating benefits to larger brain size (Fig 2). The 2 categories of hypotheses are therefore not exclusive, although the strength of each may vary taxonomically.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Categorization of the various hypotheses to explain evolutionary variation in relative brain size among vertebrates.

Two complementary clusters of hypotheses focus on either costs (expensive brain) or benefits (cognitive buffer). The brain size of a given species should reflect the balance between all relevant processes. In this review, we elaborate the developmental aspects of the expensive brain hypothesis. Key references are provided in superscripts: a: [124] (see also delayed benefits: [125]; b: [30]; 1: metabolic demands: [126,127]; 2: [45]; 3: [29, 30]; 4: [128,129]; 5: [55] (see also maturational constraints and brain malnutrition risks: [125]); 5: [83,88,108] (also: maternal energy); 7: [130,131]; 8: [132,133]; 9: [22]; 10: [134].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002016.g002

The expensive brain: Developmental aspects

Whereas most previous work on the expensive brain hypothesis focused on the consequences of the high energy costs for adults, here we focus on its developmental aspects (Fig 2). The high energy demands of growing brains create various problems.

First, brains are unusual organs in that they must acquire their cognitive and motor functions through practice and learning and therefore perform poorly before they are fully grown and differentiated. In most mammals, brain growth is largely completed around weaning [59,60], whereas adulthood is postponed until bodily growth is completed. Accordingly, many species tend to reach adult-level ecological skills such as the recognition of the values of specific food items and basic processing techniques around weaning [6163], while they master most complex skills later: extractive foraging [64] and especially tool use [65]. Birds differ greatly from mammals in that both brain and body growth are completed very early [66], well (sometimes years [67]) before reproduction starts. This suggests that the time needed for acquiring ecological skills (e.g., food processing [62,68] or predator recognition [69,70]) limits the age at which adulthood is reached. Overall, therefore, immatures in most birds and many mammals are ecologically less competent than adults, and some undergo a long phase of practice and learning before reaching adult skill levels (birds: [71,72]; mammals: [61,62]), even after brain growth has been completed.

Second, immature birds and especially mammals are in a phase of high ecological risk for 2 main reasons. They are less experienced and often smaller, which exposes them to higher risk of predation or disease [64,70]. They are also generally socially subordinate to adults, and thus may be peripheralized, either socially or in terms of habitat quality. They consequently face particularly high mortality risks, especially at higher population densities [73,74]. These 2 processes together indicate that the energy bottleneck gets worse as a species’ brain size increases.

Third, immatures have relatively higher brain maintenance costs than adults, at least in mammals. Not only are juvenile mammals smaller and less experienced, but also they are more encephalized than adults because brain growth is completed before somatic growth [59,60,75]. This forces them to allocate a larger proportion of their energy budget on maintaining the brain (see [76] for humans). In addition, they face extra costs. The creation and pruning of numerous synaptic connections means that differentiating brains are more costly per unit weight than mature brains [46,77].

Finally, in both birds and mammals, proper brain development requires play, which is often quite vigorous and therefore energetically expensive. Indeed, primate species with more postnatal brain growth (and thus larger adult brains) play more [78]. We are not aware of similarly extensive comparisons in other mammals or birds (but see [79]).

These various obstacles to brain growth suggest that immature endothermic vertebrates, with their relatively large adult brain size [1], would face a seemingly insurmountable energy crunch if they were to grow and differentiate their brains with the energy they can obtain independently by themselves. This bootstrapping problem becomes more severe as relative brain size increases (Fig 3). It can be solved if parents donate the energy needed to grow and develop larger brains.

thumbnail
Fig 3. The bootstrapping problem for developing brains.

In the absence of provisioning, immature animals developing their brains would likely face a long period of negative energy balance. Before the brain is fully grown and differentiated, it cannot provide adult-level cognitive benefits and the concomitant energy intake (green curve). The costs of growing, differentiating, and maintaining the brain (red curve) rise early and may even exceed adult values due to higher relative brain size of older immatures in mammals and costs of brain differentiation, before cognitive benefits, with their corresponding net energetic intake, stabilize at adult level. As a result, without parental provisioning the individual’s energy balance would be positive only after adulthood was reached.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002016.g003

Endotherms have both extended parental provisioning and much larger brains than ectotherms. Here, we define parental provisioning as the total energetic investment into the young, directly (in eggs, through gestation, lactation or provisioning of food), or indirectly (by carrying or huddling to keep warm). In most ectothermic vertebrates, parental provisioning is limited and brief: they simply release (usually small: [80]) eggs. This is the likely ancestral state in vertebrates ([81]). The immatures of such species thus face these various developmental obstacles on their own. Consequently, these species face limits on brain size evolution, because larger brains would go hand in hand with unrealistically prolonged developmental periods. The evolution of extended parental provisioning accompanying the evolution of endothermy ([80,82]), therefore likely facilitated the subsequent evolution of larger brains size. The goal of this essay is to examine the idea that the evolution of extended parental provisioning has enabled species to overcome the brain’s bootstrapping problem and that brain size and parental provisioning have subsequently coevolved.

The parental provisioning hypothesis

The parental provisioning hypothesis builds upon, yet greatly extends, Robert Martin’s [83] maternal energy hypothesis, which was initially based on a very different evolutionary logic, and perhaps because of this, failed to become widely adopted. It is instructive to trace the history of this hypothesis and how it gradually morphed into the parental provisioning hypothesis.

Martin [83] noted that the allometric scaling relationship with body size among placental mammals has the same exponent for both brain size and basal metabolic rate. This pattern suggested to him that “the resources channeled to the embryo from the mother” acted as a constraint on the brain size of a given species. The lower scaling exponent for brain size in birds and reptiles was attributed to their oviparity, and thus, consistent with this maternal energy effect. Initial attempts to test its predictions focusing on this allometric scaling were not favorable [84]. More direct tests were also not favorable. First, the precocial–altricial contrast in birds is inconsistent with this model. Precocial species, where young are well developed at birth and not provisioned after hatching, have smaller relative adult brain size but have much more developed brains at hatching than altricial ones, where young are poorly developed and need to be provisioned [85]. Second, maternal metabolic rate does not predict neonatal brain size or gestation length in a large sample of mammals [86] (but see [87] for a rebuttal).

These negative outcomes reduced the attention garnered by the maternal energy hypothesis, even though Martin [88] subsequently moved away from interspecific scaling. Focusing on placental mammals, he suggested that the pattern of correlations among “body size, brain size, basal metabolic rate, and gestation period indicates that the primary link is between maternal metabolic capacity and the developing brain of the offspring.” Thus, the hypothesis directly linked gestation length and maternal metabolic rate to neonatal brain size (cf. [89]). Perhaps the emphasis remained on gestation because Martin [83] had suggested that in primates, most brain growth is completed at birth. Although this may be correct for the number of neurons [90], neonatal brains in many species are less than half of adult size (e.g., [53,59]), especially in great apes and humans [91]. Moreover, brain differentiation (including myelination) is usually postnatal and among the most expensive aspects of brain development [46,77]. Thus, a proper test of the maternal energy hypothesis would require the inclusion of postnatal maternal investment in the form of lactation and (where relevant) provisioning.

Martin [88] also argued that the rate of maternal investment acts as a constraint on brain size, which, he suggested, leaves no room for variation in investment that produces adaptive variation in adult brain size (which would be achievable through variation in interbirth intervals or litter size). He argued that any links between a species’ brain size and ecology or social organization would be “a secondary consequence,” so that “there may be no very tight relationship between relative brain size and specific behavioral capacities.” Subsequent research has shown that adaptive explanations are supported for both the links with ecology [37,92], social organization [36], and cognitive performance [13,14]. This stance effectively reduced the appeal of the hypothesis.

Martin [87] later expanded the hypothesis’ scope by including lactation, and Martin and Isler [93] also considered of the overall duration of investment independent of metabolic turnover by the mother, reinforcing the conclusion that “development of the brain is heavily dependent on resources provided by the mother” ([87], p. 54). Unfortunately, these extensions garnered little attention.

Numerous comparative analyses have examined the link between adult or neonate brain size and life-history parameters in various groups, especially mammals (e.g., [29,30,54]; see also [32] for birds). Many studies found that larger-brained species take longer to reach adulthood (e.g., [34,53,55]). Although this points to competition between the growth of the brain and that of the body [94], consistent with the expensive brain hypothesis, such competition would also arise in the absence of extended parental provisioning and apparently can only be reduced by it. Therefore, this negative correlation in itself does not confirm the maternal energy hypothesis, although Barton and Capellini [55] could relate their findings to the maternal energy hypothesis, because “evolutionary changes in pre- and postnatal brain growth correlate specifically with duration of the relevant phases of maternal investment (gestation and lactation, respectively)” (see also [95]).

Note that the maternal energy hypothesis, after moving away from allometries, also reduced its stress on metabolic rates and began to cover the full period of parental investment to explain interspecific variation in brain size. The parental provisioning hypothesis expands it by including all forms of energetic investment and their rate and duration by both mothers, fathers, and helpers, and by regarding the process as an adaptive strategy (and not a constraint) to achieve the species’ optimum brain size. It also provides an explicit rationale for the need for extended parental provisioning.

Testing a major assumption: Provisioning and brain growth rates

The parental provisioning hypothesis is consistent with fundamental brain growth patterns (Fig 4). Across vertebrates, brain growth rates often show a sharp slowdown after a period of rapid growth [2,96,97], and this point coincides with the transition from parental provisioning to independence, i.e., self-sustained growth. In mammals, the initial period of rapid growth of the brain is generally isometric with that of the body [2,96,98]. In precocial species, born with relatively large brains [59], its growth slows down after birth, whereas in altricial mammals, growth continues to be high after birth [99]. In both precocial and altricial mammals, brain growth is completed by the end of parental provisioning, i.e., weaning [59,90,98], although subsequent differentiation may continue.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Schematic depiction of brain growth relative to body growth in different vertebrates as a function of parental provisioning.

The first phase (parental provisioning) shows the same, steep slope (virtually isometric: 1.0). After the end of parental provisioning, the slope becomes very low (ca 0.2) in mammals and altricial birds, whereas it become intermediate in precocial birds and ectotherms (ca 0.5) until adulthood is reached, and in most of the latter continues at the same relative rate after that due to indeterminate growth (sources are provided in the main text).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002016.g004

In birds, altricial and precocial species show different patterns of brain growth [85]. In altricials, brain growth is completed by the time offspring fledge [100,101], and thus entirely paid for by parental provisioning. Precocial birds face more of a bootstrapping problem, because there is little or no post-hatching provisioning and young must therefore find their own food. This explains why they have slower post-hatching brain growth than altricial species and achieve smaller relative brain size among adults (Fig 4).

In most ectothermic vertebrates, parental provisioning is far more limited. In most fishes, provisioning is entirely through eggs [102], and brain growth is high only during the very brief period before reserves in the egg are depleted and slows down soon after hatching [97]. However, since so much of the brain still needs to be developed, the brain growth trajectory remains steeper than for the endothermic vertebrates, as illustrated in Fig 4, and species with indeterminate growth retain the same slopes throughout life. The overall pattern is therefore consistent with the prediction that rates of brain growth are steep during the parental provisioning phase but clearly reduced thereafter.

Testing the predictions of the parental provisioning hypothesis

Two important predictions follow from the hypothesis. First, we expect positive correlated evolution between extended parental provisioning and relative brain size, given that parents must be able to muster the energy to pay for their offspring’s brain growth. Second, we expect that this correlated evolution has made it possible for lineages with extended parental provisioning to evolve larger brains. Here, we examine the evidence for these predictions.

Provisioning and brain size: Comparative tests

Comparative tests can assess the prediction that variation in the intensity and duration of parental provisioning shows correlated evolution with adult brain size. To start with birds, precocial and altricial species differ in brain size, with altricial species having larger brains for their body size than precocial species [85]. While long known [103], this difference has never been satisfactorily explained. The parental provisioning hypothesis links it to the amount of provisioning beyond egg size. In a study of 1,176 bird species, Griesser and colleagues [104] confirmed that the duration of parental provisioning showed strong correlated evolution with adult brain size.

Around 90% of bird species show biparental provisioning [105], and the modest variation in the number of caretakers is not correlated with relative brain size [104]. Among mammals, although over 80% of species have uniparental provisioning by the mother [106], allomaternal care (provisioning or carrying) is positively correlated with relative brain size, with the effect of male care being stronger than that of helpers [107], arguably because the male always helps whereas the number of helpers is highly variable and thus unreliable. These findings are therefore fully consistent with the parental provisioning hypothesis.

Turning to the ectotherm–endotherm contrast, extended parental provisioning may contribute to the explanation of the gap in relative brain size that separates them (Fig 1). In most ectotherms, provisioning stops at egg deposition of their (usually tiny [80]) eggs. In mammals, it only stops when offspring are weaned at roughly one third of adult size. Altricial birds fledge their young at close to adult size, whereas in precocial birds, although they do not provision young post-hatching, the eggs are large relative to those of ectotherms. Among ectotherms, cartilaginous fishes (Chondrichthyes: sharks, rays, skates, and sawfish) have brain sizes approaching those of endotherms [1]. Studies of chondrichthyes showed that species with matrotrophy, i.e., where young are supported beyond the yolk inside the egg, show larger relative brain size than those without it, at least for species up to 100 kg [108,109]. Although highly suggestive, the authors consider this support preliminary because the effect does not hold for the largest species. Lacking so far, are similar studies in the few radiations in ray-finned fishes (Actinopterygii), amphibians, and reptiles that show sufficient variation in parental provisioning. Overall, though, the existing studies of correlated evolution between extended parental provisioning and relative brain size overall support the parental provisioning hypothesis.

Parental provisioning and the potential for encephalization

Interspecific brain–body allometries have long been explained as reflecting one major process, such as somatosensory needs or metabolic turnover [1,110], as artifacts of non-adaptive genetic correlations [111], or even as statistical artifacts [84]. However, none of these explanations is strongly supported [112,113]. Thus, the taxonomic variation in allometric slopes requires a new explanation, couched in terms of variable selective responses to new challenges by brain, body, or both.

The parental provisioning hypothesis may make a contribution to this debate. Its logic suggests that lineages with extended parental provisioning may more readily satisfy the preconditions for major evolutionary increases in brain size (encephalization) than those with limited and briefer parental provisioning, which may therefore remain caught in rather low-cognition niches. Marsh’s rule, which states that over evolutionary time species tend to become more encephalized (i.e., brains becoming larger relative to body size [1]), may therefore apply more strongly to lineages with more extended parental provisioning. Where this process is accompanied by enough adaptive variation in body size within a given lineage, this could produce steeper slopes of the brain–body relationship (where both are log-transformed) at higher taxonomic levels among extant species, a phenomenon known as the taxon-level effect [112].

One obvious way to test this prediction is to compare the slope of the brain–body allometry in precocial and altricial bird lineages. In precocial species, which do not provision their young beyond the resources provided in the egg, the bootstrapping problem may dampen selection on increased brain size, whereas those in altricial lineages have the opportunity to respond to it by increasing their provisioning. As a result, we would expect steeper slopes for the brain–body allometry among altricial birds than among precocial ones.

Earlier results, produced for other purposes, provide a preliminary test. Nealen and Ricklefs [110] estimated the exponents of the brain–body allometry (i.e., the slopes of the log[brain]-log[body] regression) at multiple taxonomic levels. Their results revealed steeper slopes at the level of orders, families, and even genera among altricial taxa than among precocial ones. A more recent study [104] replicated this result with a modern phylogeny and a larger sample: A highly significant interaction effect between body weight and development mode on brain size revealed that altricial taxa have a far steeper slope. To visualize this effect, Fig 5 (data taken from [94]) shows the slope differences between altricial and precocial bird orders and families (based on ordinary least-squares regression).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Brain–body allometries among altricial and precocial birds.

Violin plots of slopes of the brain–body allometries of altricial (gray bars) and precocial (white bars) orders and families of birds. Data are taken from [104]. Orders or families were included when >5 species were available. Horizontal bars represent the median, red diamonds the mean, and boxes enclose the central 50% percentile range. The difference at the family level is significant (t = 2.60, DF = 26.46, p < 0.02). Sample sizes for altricial birds: 15 orders and 38 families; for precocial birds: 8 orders and 17 families.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002016.g005

An even more promising testing ground may be ectothermic vertebrates, which largely lack any post-hatching parental provisioning, even if some species guard young. Tsuboi and colleagues [97] reported that (phylogenetically corrected) brain–body allometry slopes at higher taxonomic levels are indeed clearly higher for birds (0.57) and mammals (0.59) than for fishes (0.50 for Actinopterygii and 0.41 for Chondrichthyes) and amphibians (0.46). Reptiles are closer to endotherms (0.56), but are better able than other ectotherms to maintain high body temperatures during activity through behavioral thermoregulation [114]. Tests at lower taxonomic levels have not been done yet. While these will no doubt soon emerge, this preliminary survey supports the proposition that lineages with extended parental provisioning are more likely to experience stronger encephalization, as expected under Marsh’s rule.

Implications

Extending the maternal energy hypothesis, the parental provisioning hypothesis argues that brain size is not just limited by the ability of adults to avoid starvation, predation, and disease through cognitive means, but also by their cognitively supported ability to garner the time and energy to provide their young with the energy needed to construct the brains needed for this. Comparative work shows a strong correlation between total parental provisioning and brain size and also suggests that where extensive parental provisioning did not evolve, the evolutionary potential for greater encephalization is reduced. It thus attributes the ectotherm–endotherm gap in relative brain sizes partly to the evolution of systematic extended parental provisioning in early endotherms.

If we accept these conclusions, 2 important implications deserve attention. First, the parental provisioning hypothesis may also plug another gap in our understanding of brain size evolution, linked to immature survival. Second, the importance of parental provisioning for brain size also invites us to rethink the relationship between the cognitive abilities that produce fitness benefits and those that reduce fitness costs in selection on brain size.

Parental provisioning and immature survival

The comparative tests reviewed above show that increased brain size tends to reduce reproductive rates and slows down development, which increases generation time. Even though it is unlikely that a species’ brain size is not adaptive, it may nonetheless be questioned whether the recorded increase in adult survival outweighs this dual fitness cost. We suggest that extended parental provisioning, and the concomitant continued protection of young, also provides another, previously overlooked adaptive advantage to larger brains.

There are currently no published comparative analyses of immature survival in relation to brain size. However, we recently conducted a preliminary analysis for primates, using published information on 18 species in 13 genera for which the relevant information from populations in undisturbed natural habitats has been published. We found that relative brain size improves survival until the age at first reproduction, in spite of the longer time needed to reach this point (unpublished results). Given the delay in the emergence of the various ecological skills produced by brains, it is difficult to imagine any other mechanism responsible for this remarkable pattern than parental provisioning and the associated protection.

Provided future work shows this result generalizes beyond primates, it supports the following evolutionary scenario for the evolution of parenting and its role in brain size evolution. Several forms of postnatal parental care or protection improve offspring survival [81] (and may also independently correlate with brain size [115]). Parental care may facilitate provisioning since this speeds up growth and thus reduces the time in the most vulnerable stage. Once parental provisioning beyond the egg has evolved, it alleviates the bootstrapping problem and provides reliable opportunities for practice and learning. These effects facilitate selection on increased brain size.

Selection and brain size

The importance of parental provisioning for brain size also invites us to rethink how to integrate the various costs and benefits in selection on brain size. We can in principle recognize 4 sets of cognitive abilities, here defined broadly to also include sensory and motor abilities. A first set of abilities acts to maintain the adult brain (box A in Fig 6) by guaranteeing the stability of its energy supply. A second set enables adequate parental provisioning, and so serves to construct the adult brain (box B). The third and fourth set of cognitive abilities produce the cognitive performance that is responsible for the immature and adult survival as well as reproduction of its bearer (boxes C and D). Selection will favor an optimum brain size at which fitness is maximized. This optimum size depends on details of the ecological and social environment, the species’ bauplan, but also critically on the extent to which these 4 sets of cognitive skills overlap.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Brain size and the nature of cognition.

Natural selection is expected to optimize brain size, by finding the optimum balance between the cognitive abilities (in the broad sense) required to pay for the costs of maintaining the adult brain (A) and constructing it during development (B) on the one hand, and the brain-size-dependent cognitive abilities that are translated into adult performance (C) and immature performance (survival: D) on the other hand. The 4 sets of cognitive abilities no doubt show high overlap, but their nature remains poorly studied. For birds, set A would presumably contain abilities such as migratory habits, food storing, extractive foraging, and communal roosting; set B abilities like predation avoidance (especially of nest contents), efficient foraging, habitat and nest site selection, flexibility, coordination ability; set C many of the same abilities as A and B, but also avoidance of predation on adults, post-independence skill learning, optimal mate choice, and social skills; and set D also nest site selection, nest building, and predation-sensitive provisioning.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002016.g006

The cognitive abilities in A and B merely exist to maintain and build the brain, respectively. In the absence of the cognitive benefits produced by C and D, they would be futile. Thus, if the cognitive abilities in sets A and B are very different from those in C and D, selection on increased brain size would face a very high hurdle. This consideration therefore suggests strong overlap between them and that selection on brain size will be easier when cognitive abilities are not strictly domain specific, such as general intelligence and executive functions [16,22], because strictly domain-specific cognitive adaptations are less likely to enhance both the A-B and the C-D sets.

A recent analysis of birds [104] found that, once parental provisioning was controlled for, the correlations between brain size and the commonly measured indices of cognitive demand, such as group size, duration of social bonds, or ecological niches practically disappeared from the model, apart from ecological behaviors directly affecting energy balance, such as long-distance migration [116]. This suggests that the 2 sets of variables rely on the same cognitive abilities but parental provisioning is measured more precisely. Alternatively, some of the variables traditionally thought to affect brain size are perhaps not the main selective pressures, but feature in analyses merely because they happen to be available for many species.

The fact that the same cognitive abilities may serve to pay for energetic costs and produce direct fitness benefits raises a methodological problem. Most conventional methods for analyzing comparative data assume a unidirectional flow of causality from various variables representing fitness costs or benefits to the trait of interest. In the present case, depending on the stage of lineage evolution, brain size will be involved in a number of feedback loops (cf. Fig 6), and thus both respond to and drive the surrounding landscape of eco-social and life-history traits. Modeling evolutionary brain size trajectories that include such feedback loops will require new methods. These may include models for more robust and accurate estimation of shifts in the rate of change in variables across large phylogenies [117119]. Likewise, we need models that allow for more accurate placement of variables as causes or effects in multivariate networks of traits, such as structural equation modeling or d-separation path analysis [120].

Promising insights into the evolution of brain sizes will also likely emerge from the ongoing re-evaluation of the importance of variation in comparative analyses: Methods focusing both on average patterns as well as the drivers of variance around trends (e.g., heteroscedasticity of brain–body size allometries observed across vertebrate taxa) are now able to incorporate phylogenetic relationships between species [121,122], providing new tools to disentangle the evolutionary history of brain sizes.

Finally, the parental provisioning hypothesis raises the broader question of which cognitive processes are the target of selection. It suggests that eco-cognitive and parenting skills have played a major role, with the fitness benefits of social bonds perhaps being derived from the abilities that evolved to enable parental provisioning, especially in lineages where more individuals coordinate parental activities [123].

Acknowledgments

We benefited from discussion with numerous colleagues, but especially Judith Burkart, Karin Isler, Marcelo Sanchez-Villagra, and Maria van Noordwijk.

References

  1. 1. Jerison H. Evolution of the brain and intelligence. New York: Academic Press; 1973.
  2. 2. Halley AC, Deacon TW. The developmental basis of evolutionary trends in primate encephalization. In: Kaas J, editor. Evolution of nervous systems. 2nd ed. Oxford: Elsevier; 2017. p. 149–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-804042-3.00135–4
  3. 3. Sukhum KV, Shen J, Carlson BA. Extreme enlargement of the cerebellum in a clade of teleost fishes that evolved a novel active sensory system. Curr Biol. 2018;28:3857–3863. pmid:30449664
  4. 4. Barton RA. Binocularity and brain evolution in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:10113–10115. pmid:15199183
  5. 5. Kirk EC. Visual influences on primate encephalization. J Hum Evol. 2006;51:76–90. pmid:16564563
  6. 6. de Meester G, Huyghe K, van Damme R. Brain size, ecology and sociality: a reptilian perspective. Biol J Linn Soc Lond. 2019;126:381–391.
  7. 7. Heldstab SA, Kosonen ZK, Koski SE, Burkart JM, van Schaik CP, Isler K. Manipulation complexity in primates coevolved with brain size and terrestriality. Sci Rep. 2016;6:1–9. pmid:27075921
  8. 8. Barton RA. Embodied cognitive evolution and the cerebellum. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2012;367:2097–2107. pmid:22734053
  9. 9. Rensch B. Increase of learning capability with increase of brain-size. Am Nat. 1956;90:81–95.
  10. 10. Riddell WI, Corl KG. Comparative investigation of the relationship between cerebral indices and learning abilities. Brain Behav Evol. 1977;14:385–398. pmid:412561
  11. 11. Benson-Amram S, Dantzer B, Stricker G, Swanson EM, Holekamp KE. Brain size predicts problem-solving ability in mammalian carnivores. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113:2532–2537. pmid:26811470
  12. 12. Street SE, Navarrete AF, Reader SM, Laland KN. Coevolution of cultural intelligence, extended life history, sociality, and brain size in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017;114:7908–7914. pmid:28739950
  13. 13. Deaner RO, Isler K, Burkart J, van Schaik C. Overall brain size, and not encephalization quotient, best predicts cognitive ability across non-human primates. Brain Behav Evol. 2007;70:115–124. pmid:17510549
  14. 14. Reader SM, Hager Y, Laland KN. The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011;366:1017–1027. pmid:21357224
  15. 15. MacLean EL, Hare B, Nunn CL, Addessi E, Amici F, Anderson RC, et al. The evolution of self-control. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014:111. pmid:24753565
  16. 16. Burkart JM, Schubiger MN, van Schaik CP. The evolution of general intelligence. Behav Brain Sci. 2017;40:e195. pmid:27464851
  17. 17. Pietschnig J, Penke L, Wicherts JM, Zeiler M, Voracek M. Meta-analysis of associations between human brain volume and intelligence differences: How strong are they and what do they mean? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;57:411–432. pmid:26449760
  18. 18. Lee JJ, McGue M, Iacono WG, Michael AM, Chabris CF. The causal influence of brain size on human intelligence: Evidence from within-family phenotypic associations and GWAS modeling. Dermatol Int. 2019;75:48–58. pmid:32831433
  19. 19. Hopkins WD, Li X, Roberts N. More intelligent chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) have larger brains and increased cortical thickness. Dermatol Int. 2019;74:18–24.
  20. 20. Lou Y, Zou Y, Fang Y, Swenson JE, Pape Møller A, Sun Y. Individuals with larger head volume have better learning ability in wild chestnut thrushes. Behav Ecol. 2022.
  21. 21. Parker ST. Re-evaluating the extractive foraging hypothesis. New Ideas Psychol. 2015;37:1–12.
  22. 22. Reader SM, Laland KN. Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:4436–4441. pmid:11891325
  23. 23. Overington SE, Morand-Ferron J, Boogert NJ, Lefebvre L. Technical innovations drive the relationship between innovativeness and residual brain size in birds. Anim Behav. 2009;78:1001–1010.
  24. 24. Shultz S, Finlayson LV. Large body and small brain and group sizes are associated with predator preferences for mammalian prey. Behav Ecol. 2010;21:1073–1079.
  25. 25. Møller AP, Erritzøe J. Brain size and the risk of getting shot. Biol Lett. 2016;12:20160647. pmid:27807251
  26. 26. Sol D, Bacher S, Reader SM, Lefebvre L. Brain size predicts the success of mammal species introduced into novel environments. Am Nat. 2008;172:S63–S71. pmid:18554145
  27. 27. Sol D, Duncan RP, Blackburn TM, Cassey P, Lefebvre L. Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:5460–5465. pmid:15784743
  28. 28. Amiel JJ, Tingley R, Shine R. Smart moves: effects of relative brain size on establishment success of invasive amphibians and reptiles. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e18277. pmid:21494328
  29. 29. Sol D. Revisiting the cognitive buffer hypothesis for the evolution of large brains. Biol Lett. 2009;5:130–133. pmid:19049952
  30. 30. Isler K, van Schaik CP. The expensive brain: a framework for explaining evolutionary changes in brain size. J Hum Evol. 2009;57:392–400. pmid:19732937
  31. 31. DeCasien AR, Thompson NA, Williams SA, Shattuck MR. Encephalization and longevity evolved in a correlated fashion in Euarchontoglires but not in other mammals. Evolution (N Y). 2018;72:2617–2631. pmid:30370648
  32. 32. Minias P, Podlaszczuk P. Longevity is associated with relative brain size in birds. Ecol Evol. 2017;7:3558–3566. pmid:28515891
  33. 33. Jiménez-Ortega D, Kolm N, Immler S, Maklakov AA, Gonzalez-Voyer A. Long life evolves in large-brained bird lineages. Evolution. 2020;74:2617–2628. pmid:32840865
  34. 34. Yu X, Zhong MJ, Li DY, Jin L, Liao WB, Kotrschal A. Large-brained frogs mature later and live longer. Evolution. 2018;72:1174–1183. pmid:29611630
  35. 35. Stark G, Pincheira-Donoso D. The evolution of brain size in ectothermic tetrapods: large brain mass trades-off with lifespan in reptiles. Evol Biol. 2022.
  36. 36. Dunbar RIM, Shultz S. Evolution in the Social Brain. Science. 1979;2007(317):1344–1347. pmid:17823343
  37. 37. DeCasien AR, Williams SA, Higham JP. Primate brain size is predicted by diet but not sociality. Nat Ecol Evol. 2017;1:0112. pmid:28812699
  38. 38. Morris WF, Altmann J, Brockman DK, Cords M, Fedigan LM, Pusey AE, et al. Low demographic variability in wild primate populations: fitness impacts of variation, covariation, and serial correlation in vital rates. Am Nat. 2011;177:E14–E28. pmid:21117962
  39. 39. Fristoe TS, Iwaniuk AN, Botero CA. Big brains stabilize populations and facilitate colonization of variable habitats in birds. Nat Ecol Evol. 2017;1:1706–1715. pmid:28963479
  40. 40. Shultz S, Bradbury RB, Evans KL, Gregory RD, Blackburn TM. Brain size and resource specialization predict long-term population trends in British birds. Proc Biol Sci. 2005;272:2305–2311. pmid:16191644
  41. 41. Isler K. Energetic trade-offs between brain size and offspring production: Marsupials confirm a general mammalian pattern. Bioessays. 2011;33:173–179. pmid:21254150
  42. 42. Holekamp KE. Questioning the social intelligence hypothesis. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:65–69. pmid:17188553
  43. 43. Dunbar RIM, Shultz S. Why are there so many explanations for primate brain evolution?. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2017;372:20160244. pmid:28673920
  44. 44. Mink JW, Blumenschine RJ, Adams DB. Ratio of central nervous system to body metabolism in vertebrates: its constancy and functional basis. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 1981;241:R203–R212. pmid:7282965
  45. 45. Aiello LC, Wheeler P. The expensive-tissue hypothesis: the brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Curr Anthropol. 1995;36:199–221.
  46. 46. Bauernfeind AL, Barks SK, Duka T, Grossman LI, Hof PR, Sherwood CC. Aerobic glycolysis in the primate brain: reconsidering the implications for growth and maintenance. Brain Struct Funct. 2014;219:1149–1167. pmid:24185460
  47. 47. Wells JCK. The evolutionary biology of human body fatness: thrift and control. Cambridge University Press; 2010.
  48. 48. Peters A. The selfish brain: competition for energy resources. Am J Hum Biol. 2011;23:29–34. pmid:21080380
  49. 49. Levitsky DA, Strupp BJ. Malnutrition and the Brain: Changing Concepts. Changing Concerns J Nutr. 1995;125:2212S–2220S. pmid:7542703
  50. 50. Mackes NK, Golm D, Sarkar S, Kumsta R, Rutter M, Fairchild G, et al. Early childhood deprivation is associated with alterations in adult brain structure despite subsequent environmental enrichment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020;117:641–649. pmid:31907309
  51. 51. Winick M, Noble A. Cellular Response in Rats during Malnutrition at Various Ages. J Nutr. 1966;89:300–306. pmid:5913937
  52. 52. Nowicki S, Searcy W, Peters S. Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a review and experimental test of the “nutritional stress hypothesis.” J Comp Physiol A. 2002;188:1003–1014. pmid:12471497
  53. 53. Barrickman NL, Bastian ML, Isler K, van Schaik CP. Life history costs and benefits of encephalization: a comparative test using data from long-term studies of primates in the wild. J Hum Evol. 2008;54:568–590. pmid:18068214
  54. 54. Gonzalez-Lagos C, Sol D, Reader SM. Large-brained mammals live longer. J Evol Biol. 2010;23:1064–1074. pmid:20345813
  55. 55. Barton RA, Capellini I. Maternal investment, life histories, and the costs of brain growth in mammals. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:6169–6174. pmid:21444808
  56. 56. Heldstab SA, Isler K, Graber SM, Schuppli C, van Schaik CP. The economics of brain size evolution in vertebrates. Curr Biol. 2022; in Press. pmid:35728555
  57. 57. Herculano-Houzel S. Numbers of neurons as biological correlates of cognitive capability. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2017;16:1–7.
  58. 58. Kverková K, Marhounová L, Polonyiová A, Kocourek M, Zhang Y, Olkowicz S, et al. The evolution of brain neuron numbers in amniotes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2022:119. pmid:35254911
  59. 59. Martin RD. Primate origins and evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1990.
  60. 60. Orr ME, Garbarino VR, Salinas A, Buffenstein R. Extended Postnatal Brain Development in the Longest-Lived Rodent: Prolonged Maintenance of Neotenous Traits in the Naked Mole-Rat Brain. Front Neurosci. 2016:10. pmid:27877105
  61. 61. Heldstab SA, Isler K, Schuppli C, van Schaik CP. When ontogeny recapitulates phylogeny: Fixed neurodevelopmental sequence of manipulative skills among primates. Sci Adv. 2020:6. pmid:32754638
  62. 62. Schuppli C, Isler K, van Schaik CP. How to explain the unusually late age at skill competence among humans. J Hum Evol. 2012;63:843–850. pmid:23141772
  63. 63. Carvajal L, Schuppli C. Learning and skill development in wild primates: toward a better understanding of cognitive evolution. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2022;46:101155.
  64. 64. Janson CH, van Schaik CP. Ecological risk aversion in juvenile primates: slow and steady wins the race. In: Pereira ME, Fairbanks LA, editors. Juvenile primates: Life history, development, and behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1993. p. 57–74.
  65. 65. Meulman EJM, Seed AM, Mann J. If at first you don’t succeed… Studies of ontogeny shed light on the cognitive demands of habitual tool use. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2013;368:20130050. pmid:24101632
  66. 66. Starck JM, Ricklefs RE. Avian growth and development: evolution within the altricial-precocial spectrum. Oxford University Press; 1998.
  67. 67. Mourocq E, Bize P, Bouwhuis S, Bradley R, Charmantier A, de la Cruz C, et al. Life span and reproductive cost explain interspecific variation in the optimal onset of reproduction. Evolution. 2016;70:296–313. pmid:26763090
  68. 68. MacLean AAE. Age-specific foraging ability and the evolution of deferred breeding in three species of gulls. Wilson Bull. 1986:267–279.
  69. 69. Griesser M, Suzuki TN. Naïve juveniles are more likely to become breeders after witnessing predator mobbing. Am Nat. 2017;189:58–66. pmid:28035889
  70. 70. Griesser M, Drobniak SM, Nakagawa S, Botero CA. Family living sets the stage for cooperative breeding and ecological resilience in birds. PLoS Biol. 21.6.2017;15:e2000483. pmid:28636615
  71. 71. Skutch AF. Helpers among birds. Condor. 1961;63:198–226.
  72. 72. Langen TA. Skill acquisition and the timing of natal dispersal in the white-throated magpie-jay. Calocitta formosa Anim Behav. 1996;51:575–588.
  73. 73. Saether BE. Survival rates in relation to body weight in European birds. Ornis Scandinavica. 1989;20:13.
  74. 74. Bonenfant C, Gaillard J, Coulson T, Festa-Bianchet M, Loison A, Garel M, et al. Empirical evidence of density-dependence in populations of large herbivores. Adv Ecol Res. 2009;41:313–357.
  75. 75. Worthman CM. Biocultural interactions in human development. In: Pereira ME, Fairbanks LA, editors. Juvenile primates: life history, development and behavior. New York: Oxford University Press; 1993. p. 339–358. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.20463
  76. 76. Pontzer H, Yamada Y, Sagayama H, Ainslie PN, Andersen LF, Anderson LJ, et al. Daily energy expenditure through the human life course. Science. 1979;2021(373):808–812. pmid:34385400
  77. 77. Chugani HT, Phelps ME, Mazziotta JC. Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development. Ann Neurol. 1987;22:487–497. pmid:3501693
  78. 78. Montgomery SH. The relationship between play, brain growth and behavioural flexibility in primates. Anim Behav. 2014;90:281–286.
  79. 79. Bond A, Diamond J. A comparative analysis of social play in birds. Behaviour. 2003;140:1091–1115.
  80. 80. Beekman M, Thompson M, Jusup M. Thermodynamic constraints and the evolution of parental provisioning in vertebrates. Behav Ecol. 2019;30:583–591.
  81. 81. Clutton-Brock TH. The evolution of parental care. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1991.
  82. 82. Farmer CG. Reproduction: The Adaptive Significance of Endothermy. Am Nat. 2003;162:826–840. pmid:14737720
  83. 83. Martin RD. Relative brain size and basal metabolic rate in terrestrial vertebrates. Nature. 1981;293:57–60. pmid:7266659
  84. 84. Pagel MD, Harvey PH. The taxon-level problem in the evolution of mammalian brain size: facts and artifacts. Am Nat. 1988;132:344–359.
  85. 85. Bennett PM, Harvey PH. Relative brain size and ecology in birds. J Zool. 1985;207:151–169.
  86. 86. Pagel MD, Harvey PH. How mammals produce large-brained offspring. Evolution. 1988;42:948–957. pmid:28581173
  87. 87. Martin RD. Comparative aspects of human brain evolution: scaling, energy costs and confounding variables. In: Jablonski NG, Aiello LC, editors. The origin and diversification of language. San Francisco: University of California Press; 1998. p. 35–68.
  88. 88. Martin RD. Scaling of the mammalian brain: the maternal energy hypothesis. J Physiol. 1996;11:149–156.
  89. 89. Sacher GA, Staffeldt EF. Relation of gestation time to brain weight for placental mammals: implications for the theory of vertebrate growth. Am Nat. 1974;108:593–615.
  90. 90. Deacon TW. Fallacies of progression in theories of brain-size evolution. Int J Primatol. 1990;11:193–236.
  91. 91. DeSilva JM, Lesnik JJ. Brain size at birth throughout human evolution: A new method for estimating neonatal brain size in hominins. J Hum Evol. 2008;55:1064–1074. pmid:18789811
  92. 92. Powell LE, Isler K, Barton RA. Re-evaluating the link between brain size and behavioural ecology in primates. Proc Biol Sci. 2017;284:20171765. pmid:29046380
  93. 93. Martin RD, Isler K. The maternal energy hypothesis of brain evolution: an update. Broadfield D, Yuan M, Toth N, Schick K, editors. The Human Brain Evolving: Papers in Honor of Ralph L Holloway. Bloomington: Stone Age Institute Press; 2010. p. 15–35.
  94. 94. Kuzawa CW, Chugani HT, Grossman LI, Lipovich L, Muzik O, Hof PR, et al. Metabolic costs and evolutionary implications of human brain development. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111:13010–13015. pmid:25157149
  95. 95. Powell LE, Barton RA, Street SE. Maternal investment, life histories and the evolution of brain structure in primates. Proc Biol Sci. 2019;286:20191608. pmid:31530145
  96. 96. Hofman MA. Energy metabolism, brain size and longevity in mammals. Q Rev Biol. 1983;58:495–512. pmid:6665118
  97. 97. Tsuboi M, van der Bijl W, Kopperud BT, Erritzøe J, Voje KL, Kotrschal A, et al. Breakdown of brain–body allometry and the encephalization of birds and mammals. Nat Ecol Evol. 2018;2:1492–1500. pmid:30104752
  98. 98. Martin RD. Human brain evolution in an ecological context. James Arthur Lecture on the Evolution of the Human Brain, no 52. New York: American Museum of Natural History; 1983.
  99. 99. Dobbing J, Sands J. Comparative aspects of the brain growth spurt. Early Hum Dev. 1979;3:79–83. pmid:118862
  100. 100. Portmann A. Cerebralisation und Ontogenese. Bauer KF, editor. Medizinische Grundlagenforschung. 1962;4:1–62.
  101. 101. Iwaniuk AN, Nelson JE. Developmental differences are correlated with relative brain size in birds: a comparative analysis. Can J Zool. 2003;81:1913–1928.
  102. 102. Balshine S. Patterns of parental care in vertebrates. Royle NJ, Smiseth PT, Kölliker M, editors. The evolution of parental care. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012. p. 62–80.
  103. 103. Portmann A. Etudes sur la cérébralisation chez les oiseaux. cérébralisation et mode ontogénétique. Alauda. 1947;15:161–171.
  104. 104. Griesser M, Drobniak SM, Graber SM, van Schaik C. Parental provisioning drives brain size in birds. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2023;120. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2121467120 pmid:36608292
  105. 105. Cockburn A. Prevalence of different modes of parental care in birds. Proc Biol Sci. 2006;273:1375–1383. pmid:16777726
  106. 106. Woodroffe R, Vincent A. Mother’s little helpers: Patterns of male care in mammals. Trends Ecol Evol. 1994;9:294–297. pmid:21236858
  107. 107. Heldstab SA, Isler K, Burkart JM, van Schaik CP. Allomaternal care, brains and fertility in mammals: who cares matters. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2019;73:71.
  108. 108. Mull CG, Yopak KE, Dulvy NK. Does more maternal investment mean a larger brain? Evolutionary relationships between reproductive mode and brain size in chondrichthyans. Mar Freshw Res. 2011;62:567.
  109. 109. Mull CG, Yopak KE, Dulvy NK. Maternal investment, ecological lifestyle, and brain evolution in sharks and rays. Am Nat. 2020;195:1056–1069. pmid:32469656
  110. 110. Nealen PM, Ricklefs RE. Early diversification of the avian brain: body relationship. J Zool. 2001;253:391–404.
  111. 111. Riska B, Atchley WR. Genetics of growth predict patterns of brain-size evolution. Science. 1979;1985(229):668–671. pmid:17739380
  112. 112. van Schaik CP, Triki Z, Bshary R, Heldstab SA. A farewell to the encephalization quotient: a new brain size measure for comparative primate cognition. Brain Behav Evol. 2021;96:1–12. pmid:34247154
  113. 113. Triki Z, Aellen M, van Schaik CP, Bshary R. Relative brain size and cognitive equivalence in fishes. Brain Behav Evol. 2021;96:124–136. pmid:34753141
  114. 114. Shine R. Life-History Evolution in Reptiles. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 2005;36:23–46.
  115. 115. Gonzalez-Voyer A, Winberg S, Kolm N. Social fishes and single mothers: brain evolution in African cichlids. Proc Biol Sci. 2009;276:161–167. pmid:18796397
  116. 116. Vincze O. Light enough to travel or wise enough to stay? Brain size evolution and migratory behavior in birds. Evolution. 2016;70:2123–2133. pmid:27436482
  117. 117. O’Meara BC, Ané C, Sanderson MJ, Wainwright PC. Testing for different rates of continuous trait evolution using likelihood. Evolution. 2006;60:922–933. pmid:16817533
  118. 118. Eastman JM, Alfaro ME, Joyce P, Hipp AL, Harmon LJ. A novel comparative method for identifying shifts in the rate of character evolution on trees. Evolution. 2011;65:3578–3589. pmid:22133227
  119. 119. Sansalone G, Allen K, Ledogar JA, Ledogar S, Mitchell DR, Profico A, et al. Variation in the strength of allometry drives rates of evolution in primate brain shape. Proc Biol Sci. 2020;287:20200807. pmid:32635870
  120. 120. von Hardenberg A, Gonzalez-Voyer A. Disentangling evolutionary cause-effect relationships with phylogenetic confirmatory path analysis. Evolution. 2013;67:378–387. pmid:23356611
  121. 121. Hadfield JD, Nakagawa S. General quantitative genetic methods for comparative biology: phylogenies, taxonomies and multi-trait models for continuous and categorical characters. J Evol Biol. 2010;23:494–508. pmid:20070460
  122. 122. Nakagawa S, Poulin R, Mengersen K, Reinhold K, Engqvist L, Lagisz M, et al. Meta-analysis of variation: ecological and evolutionary applications and beyond. Methods Ecol Evol. 2015;6:143–152.
  123. 123. Uomini N, Fairlie J, Gray RD, Griesser M. Extended parenting and the evolution of cognition. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2020. pmid:32475334
  124. 124. Allman JM. Evolving Brains. New York, NY: Scientific American Library; 1999.
  125. 125. Deaner RO, Barton RA, van Schaik CP. Primate brains and life histories: renewing the connection. In: Kappeler PM, Pereira ME, editors. Primate life histories and socioecology. Chicago: Chicago University Press; 2003. p. 233–265.
  126. 126. Armstrong E. Relative Brain Size and Metabolism in Mammals. Science. 1979;1983(220):1302–1304. pmid:6407108
  127. 127. Genoud M, Isler K, Martin RD. Comparative analyses of basal rate of metabolism in mammals: data selection does matter. Biol Rev. 2018;93:404–438. pmid:28752629
  128. 128. van Woerden JT, van Schaik CP, Isler K. Brief communication: seasonality of diet composition is related to brain size in new world monkeys. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2014;154:628–632. pmid:24888896
  129. 129. van Woerden JT, van Schaik CP, Isler K. Effects of Seasonality on Brain Size Evolution: Evidence from Strepsirrhine Primates. Am Nat. 2010;176:758–767. pmid:21043783
  130. 130. Parker ST, Gibson KR. Object manipulation, tool use and sensorimotor intelligence as feeding adaptations in cebus monkeys and great apes. J Hum Evol. 1977;6:623–641.
  131. 131. Byrne RW. The Technical Intelligence hypothesis: An additional evolutionary stimulus to intelligence? In: Whiten A, Byrne RW, editors. Machiavellian Intelligence II. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1997. p. 289–311. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511525636.012
  132. 132. Dunbar RIM. The social brain hypothesis. Evol Anthropol. 1998;6:178–190.
  133. 133. Byrne RW, Whiten A, editors. Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1988.
  134. 134. van Schaik CP, Burkart JM. Social learning and evolution: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011;366:1008–1016. pmid:21357223