Peer Review History

Original SubmissionMay 17, 2023
Decision Letter - Adrian M Haith, Editor, Marieke Karlijn van Vugt, Editor

Dear Prof. Franklin,

Thank you very much for submitting your manuscript "Learning context shapes bimanual control strategy and generalization of novel dynamics" for consideration at PLOS Computational Biology.

As with all papers reviewed by the journal, your manuscript was reviewed by members of the editorial board and by several independent reviewers. The reviewers found that the study was well designed and well conducted, with mostly clear results and conclusions, but did raise some substantial concerns. In particular, the application of computational models is not as thorough as would normally be expected for papers published in PLOS Computational Biology. Given that the application of computational models is central to this journal we have to regard this as a significant weakness of the paper. We would, however, be willing to consider a revised version of the paper if you can address this, along with other weaknesses highlighted by the reviewers.

One suggestion for streamlining the computational part of the paper is that presenting and fitting a multitude of closely related models (Equations 3-7) is overly complicated when in fact all the models considered are special cases of a general model in which the predicted force is a linear combination of left and right hand velocities. It may be more straightforward to fit this two-parameter model (which is basically equivalent to the weight model) and interpret the estimated parameters, rather than comparing 5 distinct models, some of which are quite arbitrarily chosen (e.g. the average and subtract models). A clearer evaluation and presentation of how well this model accounts for behavior across the different generalization conditions (ideally on conditions outside those used to fit the model) would also help improve the modeling results.

I wanted to add a few other comments on top of those of the reviewers. First, before Jackson and Miall, there was also a paper from Bays and Wolpert (J Neurosci, 2006) which considered a similar paradigm and ought to be cited. You might also look at Krakauer et al., PLOS Biology, which examined generalization of shoulder/wrist movements, rather than left hand/ right hand, but contains a similar idea. Third, the attribution of the force to either the left or right hand is very reminiscent of compound conditioning paradigms in which e.g. a light and a tone are presented together, then one is subsequently removed. See e.g. Gershman 2015, PLOS Computational Biology for a review of models of this type of task, which might be pertinent. A model along these lines was also included in Krakauer et al., 2006, and somewhat related ideas are discussed in Avraham eLife, 2022. You may find that these are interesting parallels to at least discuss.

In light of the reviews (below this email), we would like to invite the resubmission of a significantly-revised version that takes into account the reviewers' comments. In particular, in this revision, we would expect the computational parts of the paper to be substantially improved, although all other reviewer concerns should also be addressed. We cannot make any decision about publication until we have seen the revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments. Your revised manuscript is also likely to be sent to reviewers for further evaluation.

Also note that, alternatively, if you feel that your results can stand alone without relying strongly on computational modeling approaches (which may well be the case), then you might prefer to submit your work to a different journal that places less emphasis on using computational methods.

When you resubmit, please upload the following:

[1] A letter containing a detailed list of your responses to the review comments and a description of the changes you have made in the manuscript. Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

[2] Two versions of the revised manuscript: one with either highlights or tracked changes denoting where the text has been changed; the other a clean version (uploaded as the manuscript file).

Important additional instructions are given below your reviewer comments.

Please prepare and submit your revised manuscript within 60 days. If you anticipate any delay, please let us know the expected resubmission date by replying to this email. Please note that revised manuscripts received after the 60-day due date may require evaluation and peer review similar to newly submitted manuscripts.

Thank you again for your submission. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive so far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Adrian M Haith

Academic Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Marieke van Vugt

Section Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Authors:

Please note here if the review is uploaded as an attachment.

Reviewer #1: Summary:

The authors presented clear evidence that during bimanual movement, one arm can refer to

the other arm's motion state when learning to resist the mechanical load induced by the

other arm. The authors also showed that this attribution to the arms’ motion states is

dependent on the degree how the motion states of two arms are correlated; If the motion

states of the arms are de-correlated, one can learn the correct association between the load

to the left arm and motion of the right arm. On the other hand, when the states are perfectly

correlated (i.e., both arms were moving in the same way), the load induced by the right arm

motion was partially attributed to the motion of the left arm. Although this finding is

somewhat expected, a more interesting finding is that despite such a difference in attribution,

the overall transfer of bimanual learning to the unimanual movement was less affected by

the training context. The results presented are informative for the motor learning community.

Overall, the manuscript is well-written, and the results are clear. The authors did a

good job addressing the concerns raised in the previous round. I only have several minor

points that can be easily addressed.

Minor Comments:

Line#157-158 and line# 266-267: Given the results of unimanual conditions (Fig. 7E), it looks

ambiguous group shows modulation by the speed in the early exposure phase (Fig. 4G).

Though this may be simply due to the use of different ANOVA, a brief mention of this could

be helpful.

Line# 176-260: ANOVA results are missing. Referring to appropriate figure panels (e.g., Fig.

5A, B) would largely help readers to understand the results of bimanual conditions.

Especially, Figures 5B and 5D seem not referred to in the Results section.

Line#192-193: B is omitted here.

Figure 5C and D: If I correctly understood, the models were fitted to training condition data

and then tested on the data for generalization conditions. It is unclear whether the MSEs

presented here include that for the training data. In the context of cross-validation, the MSEs

for the fitting (i.e., MSEs for training condition data) should be excluded from the evaluation.

Figure 5D: Missing x-labels

The presentation of Figure 5 could be improved. For instance, the data for bimanual and

unimanual conditions could go into separate figures, as the current version is too visually

crowded. Also, using lines in Fig. 5C and D is not very informative. Simply using dots

arranged horizontally can improve visibility in the same space.

Panel D can be only for unimanual conditions and probably go to separate figures with

unimanual peak force data (from panels A and B).

As there are separated panels for the two groups, actual force values do not need different

colors. Currently, it is visually hard to detect lines in similar colors (e.g., red/orange lines over

brown shade). Gray shade could be nice for getting good contrast with all the colors used.

Line#609: Referring to wrong Eq. # (should be seven instead of 5).

Line#594-619 (regression analysis): Were the alpha values also constrained to [0,1]? If so, it

might be unfair to the subtraction coding model (perhaps the presence of B requires the

constraint on alpha, but the B in the models looks redundant). Please clarify.

Please also specify how unimanual conditions were simulated (by simply setting x_dot_right

to 0?).

Reviewer #2: In this work, the authors probe the influence of speed or movement length as a factor in adopting a bimanual control strategy. They perform an experiment wherein the speed of right hand movement determined the lateral force applied to the left hand. Two experimental groups were employed, both utilizing the velocity of right hand movement to determine the left-hand forces while the right hand always moved in a null field. In the “ambiguous” group, both hands moved at identical speeds and distances, creating ambiguity for the sensorimotor system in utilizing the sensory state of either hand to plan subsequent movements. In contrast, for the “evident” group, there was a distinction in speed between the hands, making it evident to the sensorimotor system that right hand speed was more relevant for predicting the force-field applied to the left hand. Generalization trials, incorporating interspersed error clamp catch trials, were used to assess the encoding scheme utilized in forming the bimanual memory. The authors report that the evident group employed a right hand encoding scheme, whereas the ambiguous group adopted an average or weighted average encoding scheme for bimanual memory formation.

1. How the motor system adapts to perturbations applied during bimanual movements is not well understood, and the literature on this topic is also limited. The study therefore addresses an important issue, but the findings are hardly profound or ground-breaking. The larger conclusion that training conditions strongly influence the encoding of a motor memory, and hence also shape subsequent generalization, is well-known from work in unimanual movements. The current study extends this to bimanual action conditions, but does not break new ground in terms of deepening our understanding about the influence of training conditions.

2. I also think that the authors have over-interpreted their results in favour of what has been ultimately reported. Specifically, it is not at all clear to me why the average encoding isn’t the best predictor of how people adapt left arm dynamics in either group. The authors do consider this possibility for the ambiguous group (and this makes sense), but I am not convinced that the right hand encoding does better than the average encoding for the evident group. All the plots of figure 5 and the statistics also point to average encoding doing better even for the evident group. The authors are requested to present key metrics that will convince a reader that

3. Control groups with force fields applied to the left arm under unimanual conditions are sorely missing from the study. While it may be obvious that predictive force development will be associated only with left arm kinematics in this case, it is important to know how the left arm contribution during bimanual actions, particularly in the ambiguous case, emerges/changes relative to the unimanual condition.

4. The modelling Results are very under-reported. The authors have made somewhat sweeping statements about one encoding model being better or worse than another. However, no statistical comparison across different models is available. The authors must provide a solid comparison across models (using AIC, etc.) so that one can make more informed and reliable conclusions about which encoding method works best in the two cases.

5. This study has a very limited sample size – only 8 subjects per group. Our field of work has gained some disrepute due to small sample sizes and most labs are striving to do better. The authors are therefore requested to provide a justification for why such a small sample size is good enough for making sound conclusions.

Minor:

1. Is the right arm encoding line missing from the bff condition in Figure 5B? I cant seem to find the red line.

2. line 609: "Initial ω in (5) was set to 0.5". There is no ω in eq. 5.

**********

Have the authors made all data and (if applicable) computational code underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data and code underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data and code should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data or code —e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No: The authors state the data will be made available on Figshare before publication.

Reviewer #2: No: I could not find a statement on data and code availability.

**********

PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Figure Files:

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org.

Data Requirements:

Please note that, as a condition of publication, PLOS' data policy requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions outlined in your manuscript. Data must be deposited in an appropriate repository, included within the body of the manuscript, or uploaded as supporting information. This includes all numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.. For an example in PLOS Biology see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5.

Reproducibility:

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option to publish peer-reviewed clinical study protocols. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Revision 1

Attachments
Attachment
Submitted filename: Response to the reviewers_PloS_CompBiol.pdf
Decision Letter - Adrian M Haith, Editor, Marieke Karlijn van Vugt, Editor

Dear Prof. Franklin,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript 'Learning context shapes bimanual control strategy and generalization of novel dynamics' has been provisionally accepted for publication in PLOS Computational Biology.

Before your manuscript can be formally accepted you will need to complete some formatting changes, which you will receive in a follow up email. A member of our team will be in touch with a set of requests.

Please note that your manuscript will not be scheduled for publication until you have made the required changes, so a swift response is appreciated.

IMPORTANT: The editorial review process is now complete. PLOS will only permit corrections to spelling, formatting or significant scientific errors from this point onwards. Requests for major changes, or any which affect the scientific understanding of your work, will cause delays to the publication date of your manuscript.

Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, you will automatically be opted out of early publication. We ask that you notify us now if you or your institution is planning to press release the article. All press must be co-ordinated with PLOS.

Thank you again for supporting Open Access publishing; we are looking forward to publishing your work in PLOS Computational Biology. 

Best regards,

Adrian M Haith

Academic Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Marieke van Vugt

Section Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************************************************

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Authors:

Please note here if the review is uploaded as an attachment.

Reviewer #1: The authors have nicely addressed my previous comments. I have no further comment to add.

Reviewer #2: I think the authors have a done a good job addressing the concerns raised. I would recommend accepting the manuscript after a few typos in the document are fixed.

**********

Have the authors made all data and (if applicable) computational code underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data and code underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data and code should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data or code —e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Formally Accepted
Acceptance Letter - Adrian M Haith, Editor, Marieke Karlijn van Vugt, Editor

PCOMPBIOL-D-23-00784R1

Learning context shapes bimanual control strategy and generalization of novel dynamics

Dear Dr Franklin,

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been formally accepted for publication in PLOS Computational Biology. Your manuscript is now with our production department and you will be notified of the publication date in due course.

The corresponding author will soon be receiving a typeset proof for review, to ensure errors have not been introduced during production. Please review the PDF proof of your manuscript carefully, as this is the last chance to correct any errors. Please note that major changes, or those which affect the scientific understanding of the work, will likely cause delays to the publication date of your manuscript.

Soon after your final files are uploaded, unless you have opted out, the early version of your manuscript will be published online. The date of the early version will be your article's publication date. The final article will be published to the same URL, and all versions of the paper will be accessible to readers.

Thank you again for supporting PLOS Computational Biology and open-access publishing. We are looking forward to publishing your work!

With kind regards,

Lilla Horvath

PLOS Computational Biology | Carlyle House, Carlyle Road, Cambridge CB4 3DN | United Kingdom ploscompbiol@plos.org | Phone +44 (0) 1223-442824 | ploscompbiol.org | @PLOSCompBiol

Open letter on the publication of peer review reports

PLOS recognizes the benefits of transparency in the peer review process. Therefore, we enable the publication of all of the content of peer review and author responses alongside final, published articles. Reviewers remain anonymous, unless they choose to reveal their names.

We encourage other journals to join us in this initiative. We hope that our action inspires the community, including researchers, research funders, and research institutions, to recognize the benefits of published peer review reports for all parts of the research system.

Learn more at ASAPbio .