Peer Review History

Original SubmissionFebruary 25, 2022
Decision Letter - Lyle J. Graham, Editor

Dear Dr. Crevecoeur,

Thank you very much for submitting your manuscript "Separability of Human Motor Memories during Reaching Adaptation with Force Cues" (PCOMPBIOL-D-22-00287) for consideration at PLOS Computational Biology. We apologize for the delay in responding.

As with all papers, your manuscript was reviewed by members of the editorial board. Based on our initial assessment, we regret that we will not be pursuing this manuscript for publication at PLOS Computational Biology. The experimental results provide new insights into sensorimotor adaptation dependent on context, and model seems appropriate. Nevertheless in its present form the model, already presented in your previous Crevecoeur etal eNeuro 2020, does not necessarily provide a novel advance vis-a-vis the computational analysis of a biological phenomena.

We are sorry that we cannot be more positive on this occasion. We very much appreciate your wish to present your work in one of PLOS's Open Access publications. Thank you for your support, and we hope that you will consider PLOS Computational Biology for other submissions in the future.

Sincerely,

Lyle Graham

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Revision 1

Attachments
Attachment
Submitted filename: response.docx
Decision Letter - Lyle J. Graham, Editor, Adrian M Haith, Editor

Dear Dr. Crevecoeur,

Thank you very much for submitting your manuscript "Separability of Human Motor Memories during Reaching Adaptation with Force Cues" for consideration at PLOS Computational Biology.

As with all papers reviewed by the journal, your manuscript was reviewed by members of the editorial board and by several independent reviewers. The reviewers were generally positive about the work, but highlighted a number of issues that need to be addressed.  In light of the reviews (below this email), we would like to invite the resubmission of a significantly-revised version that takes into account the reviewers' comments.

We cannot make any decision about publication until we have seen the revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments. Your revised manuscript is also likely to be sent to reviewers for further evaluation.

When you are ready to resubmit, please upload the following:

[1] A letter containing a detailed list of your responses to the review comments and a description of the changes you have made in the manuscript. Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

[2] Two versions of the revised manuscript: one with either highlights or tracked changes denoting where the text has been changed; the other a clean version (uploaded as the manuscript file).

Important additional instructions are given below your reviewer comments.

Please prepare and submit your revised manuscript within 60 days. If you anticipate any delay, please let us know the expected resubmission date by replying to this email. Please note that revised manuscripts received after the 60-day due date may require evaluation and peer review similar to newly submitted manuscripts.

Thank you again for your submission. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive so far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Adrian M Haith

Associate Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Lyle Graham

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Authors:

Please note here if the review is uploaded as an attachment.

Reviewer #1: This work investigates whether different background forces can, as cues, allow for separate, simultaneous adaptation to different force-fields applied during movement. In a set of experiments, this work finds evidence that first, oppositely-directed background forces can indeed cue opposing force-fields and, to a lesser extent, samely-directed background forces of different magnitudes can cue samely-directed force-fields of different magnitude. The authors use these data to demonstrate a model of adaptation with two timescales: one timescale representing online adaptation (within each movement) and another representing feedforward adaptation (from one trial to the next). The use of background forces as cues for different motor memories is a very interesting topic, the experiments are well-designed, the paper clear, the figures crisp. I have a number of questions regarding the framing/ interpretation of the results, and how the computational model relates to the experiment at hand.

Framing: As the manuscript discusses, previous work has shown that different force-fields can be cued by different prior or follow-up movements (e.g. refs 16,17). Here, the background forces are imposed before and remain on after the movement is executed. Could the present findings be understood within the concept of this previous work (or, maybe better, expanding the concept)? The common thread being, there is a motor plan before and after the movement that can act as a cue; whether this plan is to move from/to a different direction or to hold still against a different background load?

Lines 125-129 (and Figure 1d): because absolute angles are analyzed here, a smaller absolute angle could also be due to increased co-contraction (because of the added background load). How can we distinguish effects of actually learning the corresponding opposing fields vs. having smaller errors because of increased stiffness? (for this reason, I find the catch-trial findings eliciting oppositely-directed errors more convincing).

In similar lines: how can we distinguish between the part of the learning curves is due to stiffening vs. learning each force-field? In particular, the rapid component of the learning curve for path length (Figure 6c) could represent stiffening (especially later in movement) upon the introduction of the force-field. I suggest a brief discussion of these issues.

Figure 1 can benefit from the addition of a schematic illustrating the different phases of the experiment. While details can be inferred by a careful reading of the methods, a schematic could make this much easier.

Was the washout block (described in lines 428-431) analyzed? It would be particularly interesting to see if the different background forces during that period elicit performance reflecting the learning of one force-field or the other.

Testing both conditions in all individuals is very reasonable. Were there any order effects however?

Modeling: I don’t totally get how the two parts of this paper – different force-fields cued by different background loads on one hand, vs. a (very interesting) model encompassing both an online and offline component – blend together. The paper mentions that the data in Experiment 1 give a “novel opportunity to illustrate the model”. Can the authors elaborate on this? The model has been shown and tested in dedicated experiments in the authors’ recent work (ref. 33) – what regarding the present experiment design allows us to understand the model better? Is there something regarding background force cues that makes this dataset better suited to demonstrate the model than a garden-variety force-field adaptation task? From the other side, are there specific insights about learning multiple force-fields at the same time using force cues that are demonstrated through the model?

Minor comments/questions

Abstract, Lines 25-28. This makes it sound like there was no interference in the opposite-direction case – however, to make that claim one needs to compare how quickly the two opposite force-fields are learned separately – instead, it might be better to say “increased interference”.

Not an important issue, but I was thinking the word “offline” to represent trial-to-trial adaptation might be confusing to some readers, as it may cue them to think of offline gains in learning/consolidation.

The findings in Experiment 2 are definitely interesting – my first thought was that participants learn the average force-field, but the discussion addressed this well. Could participants be learning one field but scale their response by the force cue? Figure 2c suggests that the adaptation to the stronger force-field is privileged over adaptation to the smaller one – smaller errors – why would that be?

Lines 472-473: is it necessary to truncate traces at 600ms to calculate path length? What fraction of trials was completed by that time?

Line 586: which data were used for that purpose?

Reviewer #2: This study investigates whether distinct background forces are sufficient cues to allow for dual adaptation to opposing forces fields. The authors found partial dual adaptation, errors reduce across training and expected catch trials are produced, when the background forces are in opposing directions although this is less the case when the background forces are in the same direction but different magnitudes, likely due to interference. The papers also demonstrates that the time courses of dual adaptation to FF with opposing backgrounds appears to be made of a combination of offline and online adaptation. The results are very interesting and a nice contribution to our understanding of the contextual cues that brain needs to concurrently create distinct motor memories. I particularly like Experiment 3; which is a somewhat novel way (as far as I know) of exploring interference and dual adaptation.

I was informed that this is a revision, but I wasn’t one of the original reviewers and didn’t see the reviewers or replies. But I don’t have much to add but only a few comments.

The figures are great, and I appreciate the individual data for the catch-trials. But I think the paper would benefit from seeing individual data for training phases, especially for Experiment 2, where the effect could be driven by a subset of participants especially since the sample size is bordering on too small. I know individual curves may be too messy, so maybe fitting an exponent decay with an asymptote and plot the rate of change and asymptote values. Such fits are more sensitive for looking at differences in in learning rates (across groups, as a function of interference etc) than just averaging the first block of trials (which these authors don’t do here but is fairly common).

I can imagine that how early the background force is experienced prior to movement onset could make a difference to how useful the cue is, especially when differentiating between a small and large background forces, although this is not mentioned. In fact, I had no idea when or what event triggered the background force until I read the methods at the end and I’m still not 100% sure. The methods indicate the background force was applied (across 500 ms ramp) prior to target onset but before participants stabilized their hand onto the home to start the movements. What does stabilisation entail? My understanding is that people acquire the start target, and that then the background force ramps up but is there an additional requirement that they had to keep their hand within a window of the start position (make sense) which would have been a “challenge” since they have to overcome the background force. How long did it take for participants to stabilize (given that the background force should have made it more challenging)? And likewise, what was the average amount of time people experience the full force of the background prior to movement onset. Once the background force ramped up, how long until the target appears. Did the amount of time that background force was experience differ for the different magnitudes of forces given that it would been easier to stabilise the hand at the start position for the light background force than the heavy one.

It may be useful to describe background force timing a bit earlier than the methods or make use of Fig 1 and its legend to provide a bit information about this timing relative to target onset and start stabilization. The timecourse for b and c looks more like a schematic, so not sure whether to rely on it to indicate that actual time that the full background force was applied prior to movement onset (0).

Line 385, the term “interrogations” seems odd. I understand what you mean but its not match the context and has a negative connotation.

Line 26. The “t” in their is missing

**********

Have the authors made all data and (if applicable) computational code underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data and code underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data and code should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data or code —e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No: Not yet -they will wait until publication. But ideally shouldn't be necessary to wait until publication.

**********

PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Denise

Figure Files:

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org.

Data Requirements:

Please note that, as a condition of publication, PLOS' data policy requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions outlined in your manuscript. Data must be deposited in an appropriate repository, included within the body of the manuscript, or uploaded as supporting information. This includes all numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.. For an example in PLOS Biology see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5.

Reproducibility:

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option to publish peer-reviewed clinical study protocols. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Revision 2

Attachments
Attachment
Submitted filename: response_final.docx
Decision Letter - Lyle J. Graham, Editor, Adrian M Haith, Editor

Dear Dr. Crevecoeur,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript 'Separability of Human Motor Memories during Reaching Adaptation with Force Cues' has been provisionally accepted for publication in PLOS Computational Biology.

Before your manuscript can be formally accepted you will need to complete some formatting changes, which you will receive in a follow up email. A member of our team will be in touch with a set of requests.

Please note that your manuscript will not be scheduled for publication until you have made the required changes, so a swift response is appreciated.

IMPORTANT: The editorial review process is now complete. PLOS will only permit corrections to spelling, formatting or significant scientific errors from this point onwards. Requests for major changes, or any which affect the scientific understanding of your work, will cause delays to the publication date of your manuscript.

Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, you will automatically be opted out of early publication. We ask that you notify us now if you or your institution is planning to press release the article. All press must be co-ordinated with PLOS.

Thank you again for supporting Open Access publishing; we are looking forward to publishing your work in PLOS Computational Biology. 

Best regards,

Adrian M Haith

Academic Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Lyle Graham

Section Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************************************************

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Authors:

Please note here if the review is uploaded as an attachment.

Reviewer #1: Thank you for your clarifications and adjustments to the paper. I particularly appreciate explaining and motivating how the present data are uniquely suited to illustrate your model. I have no other comments.

Reviewer #2: I'm satisfied with the authors' replies to my concerns.

**********

Have the authors made all data and (if applicable) computational code underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data and code underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data and code should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data or code —e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: None

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Formally Accepted
Acceptance Letter - Lyle J. Graham, Editor, Adrian M Haith, Editor

PCOMPBIOL-D-22-00287R2

Separability of Human Motor Memories during Reaching Adaptation with Force Cues

Dear Dr Crevecoeur,

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been formally accepted for publication in PLOS Computational Biology. Your manuscript is now with our production department and you will be notified of the publication date in due course.

The corresponding author will soon be receiving a typeset proof for review, to ensure errors have not been introduced during production. Please review the PDF proof of your manuscript carefully, as this is the last chance to correct any errors. Please note that major changes, or those which affect the scientific understanding of the work, will likely cause delays to the publication date of your manuscript.

Soon after your final files are uploaded, unless you have opted out, the early version of your manuscript will be published online. The date of the early version will be your article's publication date. The final article will be published to the same URL, and all versions of the paper will be accessible to readers.

Thank you again for supporting PLOS Computational Biology and open-access publishing. We are looking forward to publishing your work!

With kind regards,

Olena Szabo

PLOS Computational Biology | Carlyle House, Carlyle Road, Cambridge CB4 3DN | United Kingdom ploscompbiol@plos.org | Phone +44 (0) 1223-442824 | ploscompbiol.org | @PLOSCompBiol

Open letter on the publication of peer review reports

PLOS recognizes the benefits of transparency in the peer review process. Therefore, we enable the publication of all of the content of peer review and author responses alongside final, published articles. Reviewers remain anonymous, unless they choose to reveal their names.

We encourage other journals to join us in this initiative. We hope that our action inspires the community, including researchers, research funders, and research institutions, to recognize the benefits of published peer review reports for all parts of the research system.

Learn more at ASAPbio .