Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Cross-modal representation of chewing food in posterior parietal and visual cortex

  • Tomohiro Ishii,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Resources, Validation, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Removable Prosthodontics and Geriatric Oral Health, Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo, Matsudo, Chiba, Japan

  • Noriyuki Narita ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    narita.noriyuki@nihon-u.ac.jp

    Affiliation Research Institute of Oral Science, Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo, Matsudo, Chiba, Japan

  • Sunao Iwaki,

    Roles Methodology, Visualization

    Affiliation Human Informatics and Interaction Research Institute, National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology (AIST), Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan

  • Kazunobu Kamiya,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Resources, Validation

    Affiliation Research Institute of Oral Science, Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo, Matsudo, Chiba, Japan

  • Michiharu Shimosaka,

    Roles Resources, Validation

    Affiliation Department of Anesthesiology, Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo, Matsudo, Chiba, Japan

  • Hidenori Yamaguchi,

    Roles Resources, Validation

    Affiliation Department of Anesthesiology, Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo, Matsudo, Chiba, Japan

  • Takeshi Uchida,

    Roles Resources, Validation

    Affiliation Dental Support Co. Ltd., Chiba, Chiba, Japan

  • Ikuo Kantake,

    Roles Funding acquisition, Supervision

    Affiliation Dental Support Co. Ltd., Chiba, Chiba, Japan

  • Koh Shibutani

    Roles Project administration, Supervision

    Affiliation Department of Anesthesiology, Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo, Matsudo, Chiba, Japan

Abstract

Even though the oral cavity is not visible, food chewing can be performed without damaging the tongue, oral mucosa, or other intraoral parts, with cross-modal perception of chewing possibly critical for appropriate recognition of its performance. This study was conducted to clarify the relationship of chewing food cross-modal perception with cortex activities based on examinations of the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and visual cortex during chewing in comparison with sham chewing without food, imaginary chewing, and rest using functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Additionally, the effects of a deafferent tongue dorsum on PPC/visual cortex activities during chewing performance were examined. The results showed that chewing food increased activity in the PPC/visual cortex as compared with imaginary chewing, sham chewing without food, and rest. Nevertheless, those activities were not significantly different during imaginary chewing or sham chewing without food as compared with rest. Moreover, subjects with a deafferent tongue dorsum showed reduced PPC/visual cortex activities during chewing food performance. These findings suggest that chewing of food involves cross-modal recognition, while an oral somatosensory deficit may modulate such cross-modal activities.

Introduction

Previous research has confirmed visual cortex activation in the human brain during chewing [1, 2], though the functional significance of the visual cortex while performing food chewing has not been fully elucidated.

The use of braille was found to induce visual cortex activity in blind subjects [35], while experimental vision loss in healthy subjects resulted in cross-modal visual cortex activation during Braille reading [68]. In consideration of results showing cross-modal somatosensory and visual cortex associations during finger discrimination in blind and blindfolded healthy subjects [38], it is speculated that oral food perception during chewing food performance may have cross-modal communication with visual cortex activity in the blinded oral cavity. Thus, cross-modal somatosensory and visual association may be advantageous for spatial recognition and memory related to chewing food [914].

We previously investigated PPC/visual cortex activation during oral shape discrimination as compared with sham task performance, and those findings suggested a cross-modal association between oral somatosensory and PPC/visual cortex activities during oral tactual shape discrimination [15]. Should a relationship of oral register discrimination with PPC/visual cortex activities exist, then masticatory function for forming a food mass without damaging the oral cavity during mastication may also require cross-modal communication of oral somatosensory and visual sensations, with those activities shown during mastication. Based on speculation that PPC/visual cortex activities are exhibited during mastication, the present study was conducted to further investigate the association between oral somatosensory and those activities during chewing food performance by contrasting chewing food with sham chewing without food, imaginary chewing, and rest conditions. Furthermore, examinations of the effects of blunting of food percepts with tongue dorsal anesthesia on PPC/visual cortex activities during chewing performance were also performed.

We previously reported an overview of oral somatosensory and visual associations during chewing at an international dental meeting sponsored by the International Association for Dental, Oral, and Craniofacial Research [16]. Presented here are detailed findings of cross-modal food percepts related to PPC/visual cortex activities induced by a deafferent tongue, not only during physiological but also pathological chewing. It is considered that findings indicating cross-modality of masticatory food masses would be helpful for clarification regarding the direct relationship between tactual and visual cross-modal food recognition of those food masses, as well as masticatory performance, in addition to the previously reported relationship of oral shape perception with masticatory performance. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) has been utilized to examine PPC/visual cortex activities [15, 1720]. In the present study, fNIRS was applied for several reasons, including 1) its previous use for evaluations of those activities in regard to oral discrimination, 2) availability for examination of mastication tasks in a normal setting, and 3) ease of establishing the experimental environment and use by the subjects. In the present study, fNIRS was employed to investigate manifestations of cross-modal associations of oral somatosensory-PPC/visual cortex activities during food chewing. This novel study then used those results to reveal the effects of chewing food perception on PPC/visual cortex activities during chewing performance. Additionally, discussion from the viewpoint of cross-modal food recognition while chewing is presented.

Materials and methods

Subjects

Nine right-handed healthy adult Asian males aged 23 to 47 years (30.8 ± 8.8 years, mean ± SD) participated in this study, with hand dominance confirmed using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [21]. All subjects were staff members of Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo. Sensitivity power analysis was conducted to confirm the validity of the sample size in this study. Those findings indicated that results of an F-test obtained with the G*power software package [22] (alpha = 0.05, power = 0.80, number of measurements = 5) and with the present number of samples would be sufficiently powerful to detect a minimum effect size of f = 0.391. Inclusion criteria included the following: 1) no symptoms indicating temporomandibular joint or masticatory muscle dysfunction in examinations conducted using diagnostic criteria for temporomandibular disorders [23]; 2) mentally healthy, as indicated by a score of <7 on the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale [24]; 3) no abnormalities found in a cranial examination; and 4) not currently taking any medication. Exclusion criteria for the subjects included: 1) presence of temporomandibular joint or masticatory muscle dysfunction; 2) mental impairment, as indicated by a score of ≥7 on the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale [24]; and 3) presently taking medication. Written informed consent was obtained from each enrolled subject and the study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo (EC 14–015 and EC19-14-015-1). This study was conducted in accordance with the provisions of the 1975 Declaration of Helsinki, revised in 2013.

Experimental procedures

The experiments were performed in a quiet room, with a screen positioned around the subject to block extra visual information from entering the field of view and affecting visual perception. The subject was seated in a chair during the trial. Each received an explanation regarding the experimental procedures, which were conducted using fNIRS with probes attached to the back of the head. After the explanation, the probes were attached to the parietal and occipital lobe regions. Cerebral blood flow was measured using fNIRS during five different sessions, as follows: 1) rest during the task period (Rest), 2) chewing gum (Chewing), 3) imaginary chewing (Image), 4) sham chewing without gum (Sham), and 5) chewing with tongue surface anesthesia (Anesthesia) in each subject. The Rest, Chewing, Image, and Sham sessions were conducted in a random manner to prevent effects caused by the order of the sessions, while the Anesthesia session was performed last with all of the subjects. After finishing all five sessions, the three-dimensional locations of each probe and the landmark position (NZ, Iz, AI, A2, Cz) on the scalp were recorded. The experiments were conducted and data collected between July 20, 2014 and December 10, 2014.

Sessions and task trials

fNIRS data were collected during five sessions with the subject’s eyes open. Each session was composed of five different task performances or trials, as follows: 1) Rest, 2) Chewing, 3) Image, 4) Sham, and 5) Anesthesia. Each session was started with a pre-trial rest period of 40 seconds, then repeated five times for each trial period conducted for 50 seconds, and finished with a post-trial period of 40 seconds. One trial consisted of a 20-second pre-task period, a 10-second task period, and a 20-second post-task period, with each repeated five times during one session. The total measurement time for one session was 330 seconds (Fig 1A), with a five-minute interval between each session.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Experimental design, position of fNIRS probes, and anatomical identification of fNIRS channels.

A. Each session started with a pre-trial period of 40 seconds, then was repeated five times for each trial period of 50 seconds, and finished with a post-trial period of 40 seconds. One trial consisted of a 20 second pre-task period, a 10 second task period, and a 20 second post-task period. Each trial was repeated five times. The total measurement time for one session was 330 seconds. B. Each probe was fitted with a 4 × 4 thermoplastic shell and placed at the center of the bottom line of shell, which was positioned on the inion (Iz) according to the international 10–20 system. The distance between the tip of the probe and bottom of the shell was 10 mm. C. The coordinates for all probe and anatomical landmark positions (Nz, Iz, A1, A2, Cz) were obtained using a three-dimensional digitizer. Yellow indicates the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) [Brodmann area (BA) 7], green the visual association cortex (VAC) (BA19), blue the prestriate cortex (PSC) (BA18), and red the striate cortex (SC) (BA17). Each circle corresponds to a channel and the pie chart within each circle shows the percentage of areas in that channel.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0310513.g001

For the chewing tasks, the subject was asked to chew, imagine chewing, or simulate chewing gum on both sides equally for a period of 10 seconds. A single piece of chewing gum (1 g, hardness 2.3 × 105 Pa-S, tasteless, Lotte Co., Japan) was used as the test food as appropriate for the task. The examiner instructed each subject to remain quiet until given a verbal cue. After the examiner said “start”, the subject was asked to begin to chew the gum, imagine chewing gum, or simulate chewing gum until the examiner said “stop”. They were also instructed to avoid head movements during performance of the task.

Anesthesia

For the Anesthesia session, 0.2 g of Neozalocain paste (Neo Dental International, Inc, USA) was placed on the anterior two-thirds of the tongue dorsal surface, which provides sensory innervation by the lingual nerve, with a cotton swab for five minutes in order to investigate the effects of deprivation of sensory input from the tongue. The effects of anesthesia were assessed both objectively and subjectively. For the subjective assessment, the subject was asked about sense of numbness on the tongue [25]. For the objective assessment, loss of sensation in six regions of the dorsal tongue was determined using a pinprick test, during which a sharp dental probe was applied to the mucosa and moved in a pricking manner [26]. Pinprick stimulation has been shown to predominantly activate Aδ fibers [27].

Numbness on the tongue dorsal surface was assessed before and after anesthesia with a visual analogue scale (VAS), a 100 mm horizontal line with the left end representing no numbness and right end representing the worst imaginable numbness. Numbness is often evaluated using subjective methods such as the VAS. The anesthesia session was performed last for all subjects.

fNIRS measurements

fNIRS signals were recorded using a 24-channel fNIRS device (ETG-100, Hitachi Medical Co., Chiba, Japan), which utilizes near-infrared light at two wavelengths, 780 and 830 nm [28, 29]. The distance between each pair of detector probes was 30 mm and the device was set to measure at points associated with the surface of the cerebral cortices [30, 31]. The probes were fitted with 4 × 4 thermoplastic shells and placed at the center of the bottom line of the shell, which was positioned on the inion (Iz) according to the international 10–20 system [3234]. The distance between the tip of the probe and bottom of the shell was 10 mm (Fig 1B).

Anatomical localization of fNIRS channels

A 3-D magnetic spatial digitizer (3SPACE ISOTRACK2, Polhemus, USA) was used to record the position of each probe and landmark positions (NZ, Iz, AI, A2, Cz) in three dimensions on the scalp of each participant. Furthermore, estimation of the corresponding location of each channel in the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space [35, 36] was obtained with use of a probabilistic registration method [37, 38], with anatomical localization corresponding to the probe position identified using the Platform for Optical Topography Analysis Tools (POTATo) (Adv. Res. Lab., Hitachi Ltd. Japan), with reference to Automated Anatomical Labeling [39, 40]. POTATo, a plug-in-based analysis platform that runs on MATLAB (The MathWorks Inc. USA) [38, 41], processes concentration changes of oxygenated-hemoglobin ([oxy-Hb]), deoxygenated-hemoglobin ([deoxy-Hb]), and total hemoglobin ([total-Hb]) using differential absorption proportional values of near infrared light detected by an fNIRS device [42, 43], then converts each channel position into a normalized brain surface [44, 45].

Anatomical identification of the NIRS channels is shown in Fig 1C. Twenty-four measurement channels were divided into five regions of interest (ROIs) [4648], including the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), posterior parietal cortex/visual association cortex (PPC/VAC), VAC/prestriate cortex (VAC/PSC), PSC/striate cortex (PSC/SC), and SC. ROI-1 (channels 1, 2, 3) was located in the PPC [Brodmann area (BA) 7], ROI-2 (channels 4, 5, 6, 7) in the PPC (BA 7)/VAC (BA19), ROI-3 (channels 8, 9, 10, 11, 14) in the VAC (BA19)/PSC (BA18), ROI-4 (channels 12, 13, 18, 21, 22, 24) in the PSC (BA 18)/SC (BA17), and ROI-5 (channels 15, 16, 17, 19, 20, 23) in the SC (BA17). Subsequently, to reduce signal variations, the mean value for [oxy-Hb] for each ROI (averaged across channels) during the pre-task, task, and post-task periods was calculated for each experimental condition. (Fig 1C).

fNIRS data analysis

fNIRS data were preprocessed for addition averaging using POTATo. Oxy-Hb signal results have been widely reported in clinical research studies [49], with findings indicating better sensitivity to task-related hemodynamic changes [50] and excellent reliability for task-related activities [51]. Based on our previous results [5254], Oxy-Hb signals were focused on in the present study. The sampling interval was 0.1 seconds. Each trial was repeated five times and obtained values were averaged using the ‘integral mode’ of the ETG-100 software for the Rest, Chewing, Image, Sham, and Anesthesia sessions. Also, a linear fitting algorithm [55] was used for baseline corrections [56]. A moving average with a window width of 5 seconds was used to remove physiological noise such as cardiac artifacts [57] and short-term motion artifacts [58, 59] in the fNIRS signals.

Statistical analysis

Numbness VAS scale results after anesthesia were compared with those before anesthesia using a one-sample signed rank test to objectively confirm the effects of anesthesia [60, 61]. The value for [oxy-Hb] was calculated every 1 second and compared between the Chewing and Rest, Image and Rest, Sham and Rest, and Anesthesia and Rest sessions using paired t-tests, implemented with a plug-in-based analysis platform that runs on MATLAB (The MathWorks Inc.), with values showing p < 0.05 considered to be significantly different. A topographical representation of significant channels every 1 second was projected onto the occipital cortical surface of a Montreal Neurological Institute standard brain space [62, 63] using a three-dimensional composite display unit (version 2.41, Hitachi Medical Co. Chiba Japan) [64]. It has been shown that as the number of items being examined increases, so does the risk of type 1 errors [65]. Thus, in order to avoid such errors, two-way repeated measures ANOVA and multiple comparisons using a paired-t-test with Bonferroni correction were applied. The mean signal from the five ROIs was used for two-way repeated measures ANOVA.

Data for temporal changes of continuously averaged data for accumulated [oxy-Hb] every 1 section in each ROI did not support a normal distribution (Shapiro-Wilk, p < 0.05), thus non-parametric statistical analysis was performed using nonparametric two-way repeated measured analysis of variance with rank-transformed values (aligned ranked transformation, ART) [66]. ART is a modification of rank transformation [67, 68] that allows for accurate testing to determine interaction effects. By aligning the data to strip the interaction effect from the main effects, as well as the main effects from each other and the interaction, and then ranking it, mixed factorial ANOVA is possible [69]. Multiple comparisons using a paired t-test with Bonferroni correction were used as a post hoc test for comparisons of temporal changes of continuously averaged data of accumulated [oxy-Hb] for every 1 second in each ROI between the Chewing and Rest, Image and Rest, Sham and Rest, Anesthesia and Rest, Chewing and Sham, Chewing and Image, Chewing and Anesthesia, Image and Sham, Image and Anesthesia, and Sham and Anesthesia sessions. Effect size (f) values were accessed for ANOVA and also used to estimate the standardized degree of effect, including differences, and influence of the findings shown by the obtained data in this study. Eta squared (η2) values were used to estimate effect size, with a magnitude for effect (η2) of 0.01 defined as small, of 0.06 defined as medium, and of 0.14 defined as large [70]. The statistical software package SigmaPlot, ver. 14.5 (Systat Software, Inc., San Jose, CA, USA) was used for all analyses. P-values less than 0.05 were considered to indicate significance.

Results

Numbness on tongue dorsal surface before and after anesthesia

The numbness VAS scale value for all subjects before anesthesia was 0. A significant increase to 50.4 ± 0.4 (one-sample signed rank test, p = 0.004) was noted after starting anesthesia.

Grand averaged waveforms and topographical maps

The topography of changes in [oxy-Hb] during the pre-task, task, and post-task periods in the Rest, Chewing, Image, Sham, and Anesthesia sessions is shown in Fig 2. Grand averaged waveforms for the nine subjects for changes in [oxy-Hb] and [deoxy-Hb] during the Rest, Chewing, Image, Sham, and Anesthesia sessions are presented in Fig 2A–2E, respectively. During the Rest session, there was no apparent change in [oxy-Hb] in the occipital lobe (Fig 2A). As for the Chewing session, there was an increase in [oxy-Hb] in the PPC (BA7), VAC (BA19), PSC (BA18), and SC (BA17) (Fig 2B), while in the Image session there was an increase in [oxy-Hb] in the SC (BA17) (Fig 2C) during the task period. Furthermore, in the Sham session, there was an increase in [oxy-Hb] in the PPC (BA7) and SC (BA17) (Fig 2D), and in the Anesthesia session there was an increase in [oxy-Hb] in the PPC (BA7), VAC (BA19), PSC (BA18), and SC (BA17) (Fig 2E) during the task period.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Grand average waveforms and topographical maps.

Left: Grand average changes in oxygenated-hemoglobin concentration ([oxy-Hb], red line) and deoxygenated-hemoglobin concentration ([deoxy-Hb], blue line) during the Rest (A), Chewing (B), Image (C), Sham (D), and Anesthesia (E) sessions for each of the 24 measurement channels in the nine subjects. The x-axis indicates time (s) and y-axis hemodynamic change (mMmm). Grey vertical lines at 20 and 30 seconds indicate the start and end, respectively of the 10-second task period. Right: Topographical maps showing changes in [oxy-Hb] during the 10-second period preceding the task period (Pre-task), the 10-second task period (Task), and the 10-second period following the task period (Post-task) during the Rest (A), Chewing (B), Image (C), Sham (D), and Anesthesia (E) sessions. While there was no apparent change during Rest (A), there was a marked increase during the Chewing task period (B), a slight increase during the task period in the Image and Sham task periods (C, D), and a significant increase during the Anesthesia task and post-task periods (E).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0310513.g002

Temporal changes in [oxy-Hb] during rest, chewing, image, sham, and anesthesia

During the Rest, Chewing, Image, Sham, and Anesthesia sessions, parietal and occipital lobe activities showed significant interactions with time in the PPC (CH 1, 2, 3) [F = 1.399; p < 0.001, power = 0.943, η2 = 0.10, f = 0.418, effect size larger than minimum (f = 0.391) based on sensitivity analysis], PPC/VAC (CH 4, 5, 6, 7) [F = 1.428, p < 0.001, power = 0.962, η2 = 0.11, f = 0.423, effect size larger than minimum (f = 0.391) based on sensitivity analysis], VAC/PSC (CH 8, 9, 10, 11, 14) [F = 1.527, p < 0.001, power = 0.993, η2 = 0.11, f = 0.437, effect size larger than minimum (f = 0.391) based on sensitivity analysis], PSC/SC (CH 12, 13, 18, 21, 22, 24) [F = 2.779, p < 0.001, power = 1.00, η2 = 0.16, f = 0.539, effect size larger than minimum (f = 0.391) based on sensitivity analysis, and SC (CH 15, 16, 17, 19, 20, 23) [F = 3.312, p < 0.001, power = 1.00, η2 = 0.19, p = 0.643, effect size larger than minimum (f = 0.391) based on sensitivity analysis].

Comparison between chewing and rest.

When Chewing was compared to Rest, the values for [oxy-Hb] were significantly (p < 0.05) increased during the task periods in PPC (Fig 3A), PPC/VAC (Fig 3B), VAC/PSC (Fig 3C), PSC/SC (Fig 3D), and SC (Fig 3E).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Temporal changes in [oxy-Hb] during rest, chewing, image, sham, anesthesia.

Changes in [oxy-Hb] in the PPC, VAC, PSC, and SC during the Rest, Chewing, Image, Sham, and Anesthesia sessions are shown. Significant differences were clearly present between Chewing and Rest (red bar) in the PPC, PPC/VAC, VAC/PSC, PSC/SC, and SC; between Chewing and Image (green bar) in the PPC, PPC/VAC, VAC/PSC, PSC/SC, and SC; between Chewing and Sham (orange bar) in the PPC, VAC/PSC, PSC/SC, and SC; and between Chewing and Anesthesia (blue bar) in the VAC/PSC, PSC/SC, and SC. Light blue shading indicates the 10-second task period.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0310513.g003

Comparison between image and rest.

The values for [oxy-Hb] were not significantly different between Image and Rest during the task periods.

Comparison between sham and rest.

The values for [oxy-Hb] were not significantly different between Sham and Rest during the task periods.

Comparison between anesthesia and rest.

The values for [oxy-Hb] were not significantly different between Anesthesia and Rest during the task periods.

Comparison between chewing and image.

When Chewing was compared to Image, the values for [oxy-Hb] were significantly (p < 0.05) increased during the task periods in PPC (Fig 3A), PPC/VAC (Fig 3B), VAC/PSC (Fig 3C), PSC/SC (Fig 3D), and SC (Fig 3E).

Comparison between chewing and sham.

When Chewing was compared to Sham, the values for [oxy-Hb] were significantly (p < 0.05) increased during the task periods in PPC (Fig 3A), VAC/PSC (Fig 3B), PSC/SC (Fig 3D), and SC (Fig 3E).

Comparison between chewing and anesthesia.

When Chewing was compared to Anesthesia, the values for [oxy-Hb] were significantly (p < 0.05) increased during the task periods in VAC/PSC (Fig 3C), PSC/SC (Fig 3D), and SC (Fig 3E).

Discussion

Visual cortex activities during chewing, imaginary chewing, and sham chewing without food, and during rest

This study was performed as an investigation of parietal and occipital cortex activities during chewing performance. Significant increases in PPC/visual cortex activities were found while chewing food as compared with imaginary chewing, sham chewing without food, and rest settings, whereas there were no significant differences in those activities when imaginary chewing and sham chewing without food were compared with rest.

Previous studies have reported a cross-modal association of finger somatosensory with visual cortex activities during Braille reading in blind subjects [35], while blindfolded healthy subjects have also been found to have induced visual cortex activities during Braille reading [68]. Kagawa et al. [15] presented findings regarding oral somatosensory and visual cross-modality that showed PPC/visual cortex activities during oral shape tactual discrimination when visual receptivity was eliminated. Moreover, Ptito et al. [71], Kupers et al. [72], and Vuillerme et al. [73] each reported details related to visual cortex activities induced by tongue dorsal electrical stimulation in healthy controls. Based on these findings, it is considered that indications of PPC/visual cortex activities during chewing food performance suggest a cross-modal association of oral somatosensory with PPC/visual cortex activities involved in chewing food recognition during chewing performance in a non-visualized mouth. The present study also found no significant PPC/visual cortex activities during imaginary chewing or sham chewing without food, as compared to a rest condition, though it has been suggested the visual cortex activity occurs by means of mental imaginary performance [7477]. Regarding the differences between these imaginary effects on PPC/visual cortex activities, it is considered that the vividity of the target might be different in mental imagery. Therefore, it is possible that the properties of the food bolus may be quite unclear during chewing in the invisible oral space [7882]. In the present subjects, chewing food performance was shown to induce significant PPC/visual cortex activities as compared with the imaginary chewing, sham chewing, and rest conditions, thus suggesting a cross-modal oral somatosensory and PPC/visual cortex association while chewing food.

Association of oral sensory deprivation with modulatory PPC/visual cortex activity during chewing performance

Results obtained in this study also show the effects of anesthetized oral sensory deprivation on reduced PPC/visual cortex activities during chewing performance. A decrease in those activities caused by oral anesthesia may also paradoxically indicate that cross-modal PPC/visual cortex activities are due to oral somatosensory food perception. Furthermore, such a pathological cross-modal communication between somatosensory deficits and decreased PPC/visual cortex activities suggests discriminative deficits in food recognition while chewing and an association with awareness of chewing disability, as seen in aged individuals with tooth loss [8386]. It is also possible that such cross-modal effects may extend to multisensory chewing food recognition, such as the relationships of the somatosensory system with the taste cortex [87, 88] and auditory cortex [89, 90], in addition to the association of the somatosensory system with the PPC/visual cortex [9193]. The present results showed that oral deprivation while chewing resulted in decreased PPC/visual cortex activity, which suggests that an oral somatosensory deficit can induce cross-modal effects on food recognition during chewing performance. It is thus speculated that PPC/visual cortex activities while chewing have a relationship with subjective chewing food ability in partially edentulous aged patients. Examinations of PPC/visual cortex activities while chewing may be applicable for evaluation of oral food recognition ability for the field of dentistry.

Study limitations and future research directions

In the present study, neural activity in the parietal and occipital lobes indicated a statistically significant interaction between task and time. Chewing food trials have found significant neural activation in the parietal and occipital lobes as compared to during rest, which were based on p-values and effect sizes used to evaluate the significance of the data [94]. Effect size represents the magnitude of change in outcome and is often more important than relying on p-value alone when interpreting study results [95]. Furthermore, effect size is independent of sample size [94]. Both p-value and effect size were larger than the minimum effect size value (0.391) obtained from sensitivity test power analysis in the present study, which is considered to confirm the significance of the results even though the sample size of 9 subjects is small. The results also confirmed that tactile-visual cross-modal substrates in the parietal and occipital cortex indicate shape perception in the oral cavity for chewing food perception during mastication, as also noted in a previous study [15]. Additionally, they indicate that oral sensory deprivation results in significantly decreased neural activity in the parietal and occipital lobes during food chewing. Thus, the requirement of representation of somatosensory-visual cross-modal food perception during chewing was confirmed. Nevertheless, in consideration of the variety of individual normal occlusion conditions encountered [96] as well as the influence of surface anesthesia in the mouth [97], the number of measurements in healthy subjects should be increased in future clinical trials in order to detect subtle differences between conditions, which may be a limitation of the present study.

Previous studies functionally evaluated chewing ability considered as on the particle size distribution in comminuted test food [98, 99], findings following glucose extraction from chewed food [100, 101], and color changes in chewing gum used for testing [102, 103]. In addition, the relationship [83, 84] of chewing efficiency with oral stereognosis ability has also been examined in young healthy and edentulous subjects. In future clinical studies, measurements of PPC/visual cortex activities during chewing can be applied to evaluate chewing ability from the viewpoints of tactile and visual cross-modal representations of chewing food in the dentistry patients with chewing difficulties.

Conclusions

The functional associations of oral somatosensory food percepts with PPC/visual cortex activities during chewing were investigated in healthy subjects. Food chewing produced significant PPC/visual cortex activities as compared to imaginary chewing, sham chewing without food, and rest conditions. On the other hand, there were no significant differences between imaginary and sham chewing without food as compared with rest. In addition, oral somatosensory deprivation induced significant decreases in PPC/visual cortex activities during chewing, which suggests a pathological cross-modal representation of oral somatosensory-PPC/visual cortex activities. Based on these findings, it is considered that there are physiological and pathological cross-modal links between oral somatosensory and PPC/visual cortex activities in the process of chewing food recognition during chewing performance.

Supporting information

S1 File. Oxy-Hb raw data tables obtained under rest, chewing, sham, image, anesthesia condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0310513.s001

(XLSX)

References

  1. 1. Quintero A, Ichesco E, Myers C, Schutt R, Gerstner GE. Brain activity and human unilateral chewing: an FMRI study. J Dent Res. 2013; 92: 136–142. pmid:23103631
  2. 2. Quintero A, Ichesco E, Schutt R, Myers C, Peltier S, Gerstner GE. Functional connectivity of human chewing: an fcMRI study. J Dent Res. 2013; 92: 272–278. pmid:23355525
  3. 3. Sadato N, Pascual-Leone A, Grafman J, Ibañez V, Deiber MP, Dold G, et al. Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind subjects. Nature. 1996; 380: 526–528. pmid:8606771
  4. 4. Sadato N, Okada T, Kubota K, Yonekura Y. Tactile discrimination activates the visual cortex of the recently blind naive to Braille: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in humans. Neurosci Lett. 2004; 359: 49–52. pmid:15050709
  5. 5. Qin W, Yu C. Neural pathways conveying novisual information to the visual cortex. Neural Plast. 2013; 2013: 864920. pmid:23840972
  6. 6. Saito DN, Okada T, Honda M, Yonekura Y, Sadato N. Practice makes perfect: the neural substrates of tactile discrimination by Mah-Jong experts include the primary visual cortex. BMC Neurosci. 2006; 7: 79. pmid:17144928
  7. 7. Merabet LB, Hamilton R, Schlaug G, Swisher JD, Kiriakopoulos ET, Pitskel NB, et al. Rapid and reversible recruitment of early visual cortex for touch. PLoS One. 2008; 3: e3046. pmid:18728773
  8. 8. Nakashita S, Saito DN, Kochiyama T, Honda M, Tanabe HC, Sadato N. Tactile-visual integration in the posterior parietal cortex: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Brain Res Bull. 2008; 75: 513–525. pmid:18355627
  9. 9. Macaluso E, Driver J. Spatial attention and crossmodal interactions between vision and touch. Neuropsychologia. 2001; 39: 1304–1316. pmid:11566313
  10. 10. Zhang D, Zhang X, Sun X, Li Z, Wang Z, He S, et al. Cross-modal temporal order memory for auditory digits and visual locations: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2004; 22: 280–289. pmid:15202106
  11. 11. Demattè ML, Sanabria D, Sugarman R, Spence C. Cross-modal interactions between olfaction and touch. Chem Senses. 2006; 31: 291–300. pmid:16452454
  12. 12. Verhagen JV, Engelen L. The neurocognitive bases of human multimodal food perception: sensory integration. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006; 30: 613–650. pmid:16457886
  13. 13. Spence C, Piqueras-Fiszman B. Oral-Somatosensory Contributions to Flavor Perception and the Appreciation of Food and Drink. In Piqueras-Fiszman B, Spence C, editors, Multisensory Flavor Perception: From Fundamental Neuroscience Through to the Marketplace. Elsevier. 2016. p. 59–79. (Woodhead Publishing Series in Food Science, Technology and Nutrition; 298). https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-100350-3.00004–3
  14. 14. Sinding C, Saint–Eve A, Thomas-Danguin T. Multimodal sensory interactions. In E. Guichard & C. Salles (Eds.), Flavor from food to behaviors, wellbeing and health. Elsevier. 2022. p. 205–231. (Woodhead Publishing Series in Food Science, Technology and Nutrition).
  15. 15. Kagawa T, Narita N, Iwaki S, Kawasaki S, Kamiya K, Minakuchi S. Does shape discrimination by the mouth activate the parietal and occipital lobes? ‐ near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS One. 2014; 9: e108685. pmid:25299397
  16. 16. Ishii T, Narita N, Kamiya K. Visual cortex activities while chewing and oral sensory participation. Proceedings of the 2017 IADR/AADR/CADR General Session; 2017 Mar 20–24; San Francisco, California.
  17. 17. Kashou NH, Xu R, Roberts CJ, Leguire LE. Using FMRI and FNIRS for localization and monitoring of visual cortex activities. Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2007; 2007: 2634–2638. pmid:18002536
  18. 18. Wijeakumar S, Shahani U, Simpson WA, McCulloch DL. Localization of hemodynamic responses to simple visual stimulation: an fNIRS study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012; 53: 2266–2273. pmid:22427541
  19. 19. Wiggins IM, Hartley DE. A synchrony-dependent influence of sounds on activity in visual cortex measured using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). PLoS One. 2015; 10: e0122862. pmid:25826284
  20. 20. Lin CC, Barker JW, Sparto PJ, Furman JM, Huppert TJ. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) brain imaging of multi-sensory integration during computerized dynamic posturography in middle-aged and older adults. Exp Brain Res. 2017; 235: 1247–1256. pmid:28197672
  21. 21. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971; 9: 97–113. pmid:5146491
  22. 22. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007; 39: 175–191. pmid:17695343
  23. 23. Schiffman E, Ohrbach R, Truelove E, Look J, Anderson G, Goulet JP, et al. Diagnostic criteria for temporomandibular disorders (DC/TMD) for clinical and research applications: recommendations of the international RDC/TMD consortium network* and Orofacial pain special interest group†. International RDC/TMD Consortium Network, International association for Dental Research; Orofacial Pain Special Interest Group, International Association for the Study of Pain. J Oral Facial Pain Headache. 2014; 28: 6–27. pmid:24482784
  24. 24. Snaith RP, Zigmond AS. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed). 1986; 292: 344. pmid:3080166
  25. 25. Afsal MM, Khatri A, Kalra N, Tyagi R, Khandelwal D. Pain perception and efficacy of local analgesia using 2% lignocaine, buffered lignocaine, and 4% articaine in pediatric dental procedures. J Dent Anesth Pain Med. 2019; 19: 101–109. pmid:31065592
  26. 26. Phyo HE, Chaiyasamut T, Kiattavorncharoen S, Pairuchvej V, Bhattarai BP, Wongsirichat N. Single buccal infiltration of high concentration lignocaine versus articaine in maxillary third molar surgery. J Dent Anesth Pain Med. 2020; 20: 203–212. pmid:32934986
  27. 27. Curatolo M, Petersen-Felix S, Arendt-Nielsen L. Sensory assessment of regional analgesia in humans: a review of methods and applications. Anesthesiology. 2000; 93: 1517–1530. pmid:11149448
  28. 28. Ehlis AC, Bähne CG, Jacob CP, Herrmann MJ, Fallgatter AJ. Reduced lateral prefrontal activation in adult patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) during a working memory task: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) study. J Psychiatr Res. 2008; 42: 1060–1067. pmid:18226818
  29. 29. Narita N, Kamiya K, Yamamura K, Kawasaki S, Matsumoto T, Tanaka N. Chewing-related prefrontal cortex activation while wearing partial denture prosthesis: pilot study. J Prosthodont Res. 2009; 53: 126–135. pmid:19345661
  30. 30. Okada E, Firbank M, Schweiger M, Arridge SR, Cope M, Delpy DT. Theoretical and experimental investigation of near-infrared light propagation in a model of the adult head. Appl Opt. 1997; 36: 21–31. pmid:18250644
  31. 31. Tamura M, Hoshi Y, Okada F. Localized near-infrared spectroscopy and functional optical imaging of brain activity. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1997; 352: 737–742. pmid:9232862
  32. 32. Klem GH, Lüders HO, Jasper HH, Elger C. The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl. 1999; 52: 3–6. pmid:10590970
  33. 33. Jurcak V, Okamoto M, Singh A, Dan I. Virtual 10–20 measurement on MR images for inter-modal linking of transcranial and tomographic neuroimaging methods. Neuroimage. 2005; 26: 1184–1192. pmid:15961052
  34. 34. Jurcak V, Tsuzuki D, Dan I. 10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 systems revisited: their validity as relative head-surface-based positioning systems. Neuroimage. 2007; 34: 1600–1611. pmid:17207640
  35. 35. Collins DL, Neelin P, Peters TM, Evans AC. Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space. J Comput Assist Tomogr. 1994; 18: 192–205. pmid:8126267
  36. 36. Brett M, Johnsrude IS, Owen AM. The problem of functional localization in the human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002; 3: 243–249. pmid:11994756
  37. 37. Okamoto M, Dan I. Automated cortical projection of head-surface locations for transcranial functional brain mapping. Neuroimage. 2005; 26: 18–28. pmid:15862201
  38. 38. Singh AK, Okamoto M, Dan H, Jurcak V, Dan I. Spatial registration of multichannel multi-subject fNIRS data to MNI space without MRI. Neuroimage. 2005; 27: 842–851. pmid:15979346
  39. 39. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002; 15: 273–289. pmid:11771995
  40. 40. Tsuzuki D, Dan I. Spatial registration for functional near-infrared spectroscopy: from channel position on the scalp to cortical location in individual and group analyses. Neuroimage. 2014; 85: 92–103. pmid:23891905
  41. 41. Dan I, Okamoto M, Tsuzuki D, Singh AK. Toward standardizing spatial analysis for optical topography. IEEE / ICME Int conference on Complex Medical Engineering. 2007; 2012–2018.
  42. 42. Boas DA, Gaudette T, Strangman G, Cheng X, Marota JJ, Mandeville JB. The accuracy of near infrared spectroscopy and imaging during focal changes in cerebral hemodynamics. Neuroimage. 2001; 13: 76–90. pmid:11133311
  43. 43. Sutoko S, Sato H, Maki A, Kiguchi M, Hirabayashi Y, Atsumori H, et al. Tutorial on platform for optical topography analysis tools. Neurophotonics. 2016; 3: 010801. pmid:26788547
  44. 44. Sugiura L, Ojima S, Matsuba-Kurita H, Dan I, Tsuzuki D, Katura T, et al. Sound to language: different cortical processing for first and second languages in elementary school children as revealed by a large-scale study using fNIRS. Cereb Cortex. 2011; 21: 2374–2393. pmid:21350046
  45. 45. Tachibana A, Noah JA, Bronner S, Ono Y, Onozuka M. Parietal and temporal activity during a multimodal dance video game: an fNIRS study. Neurosci Lett. 2011; 503: 125–130. pmid:21875646
  46. 46. Sui Y, Kan C, Zhu S, Zhang T, Wang J, Xu S, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity for determining outcomes in upper extremity function after stroke: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Front Neurol. 2022; 13: 965856. pmid:36438935
  47. 47. Zhang S, Zhu T, Tian Y, Jiang W, Li D, Wang D. Early screening model for mild cognitive impairment based on resting-state functional connectivity: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neurophotonics. 2022; 9: 045010. pmid:36483024
  48. 48. Li H, Fu X, Lu L, Guo H, Yang W, Guo K, et al. Upper limb intelligent feedback robot training significantly activates the cerebral cortex and promotes the functional connectivity of the cerebral cortex in patients with stroke: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Front Neurol. 2023; 14: 1042254. pmid:36814999
  49. 49. Ozawa S. Application of Near-Infrared Spectroscopy for Evidence-Based Psychotherapy. Front Psychol. 2021;12:527335–527335. pmid:34366946
  50. 50. Yeung MK, Lin J. Probing depression, schizophrenia, and other psychiatric disorders using fNIRS and the verbal fluency test: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Psychiatr Res. 2021;140:416–35. pmid:34146793
  51. 51. Huang Y, Mao M, Zhang Z, Zhou H, Zhao Y, Duan L. Test-retest reliability of the prefrontal response to affective pictures based on functional nearinfrared spectroscopy. J Biomed Opt. 2017;22(1):16011–23. pmid:28114450
  52. 52. Narita N, Kamiya K, Iwaki S, Ishii T, Endo H, Shimosaka M, et al. Activation of Prefrontal Cortex in Process of Oral and Finger Shape Discrimination: fNIRS Study. Front Neurosci. 2021 Feb 5;15:588593. pmid:33633532
  53. 53. Narita N, Ishii T, Iwaki S, Kamiya K, Okubo M, Uchida T, et al. Prefrontal Consolidation and Compensation as a Function of Wearing Denture in Partially Edentulous Elderly Patients. Front Aging Neurosci. 2020 Jan 31;11:375. pmid:32082135
  54. 54. Narita N, Kamiya K, Makiyama Y, Iwaki S, Komiyama O, Ishii T, et al. Prefrontal modulation during chewing performance in occlusal dysesthesia patients: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Clin Oral Investig. 2019 Mar;23(3):1181–1196. pmid:29967973
  55. 55. Naseer N, Hong KS. fNIRS-based brain-computer interfaces: a review. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015; 9: 3. pmid:25674060
  56. 56. Ehlis AC, Herrmann MJ, Plichta MM, Fallgatter AJ. Cortical activation during two verbal fluency tasks in schizophrenic patients and healthy controls as assessed by multi-channel near-infrared spectroscopy. Psychiatry Res. 2007; 156: 1–13. pmid:17587553
  57. 57. Yang D, Huang R, Yoo SH, Shin MJ, Yoon JA, Shin YI, et al. Detection of mild cognitive impairment using convolutional neural network: Temporal-feature maps of functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Front Aging Neurosci. 2020; 12: 141. pmid:32508627
  58. 58. Suto T, Fukuda M, Ito M, Uehara T, Mikuni M. Multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy in depression and schizophrenia: cognitive brain activation study. Biol Psychiatry. 2004; 55: 501–511. pmid:15023578
  59. 59. Wang W, Qiu C, Ota T, Sawada M, Kishimoto N, Kishimoto T. Effects of TAI CHI exercise on attention in healthy elderly subjects as measured by near-infrared spectroscopy during the stroop task. J. Nara Med. Assoc. 2013; 64: 79–86. http://hdl.handle.net/10564/2738.
  60. 60. Cressie NA, Sheffield LJ, Whitford HJ. Use of the one sample t-test in the real world. J Chronic Dis. 1984; 37:107–114. pmid:6693529
  61. 61. Atalay K, Gezer Savur F, Kirgiz A, Erdogan Kaldirim H, Zengi O. Serum vitamin D levels in different morphologic forms of age related cataract. Acta Endocrinol (Buchar). 2020; 16: 178–182. pmid:33029234
  62. 62. Tian F, Liu H. Depth-compensated diffuse optical tomography enhanced by general linear model analysis and an anatomical atlas of human head. Neuroimage. 2014; 85: 166–180. pmid:23859922
  63. 63. Okamoto M, Dan H, Sakamoto K, Takeo K, Shimizu K, Kohno S, et al. Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10–20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping. Neuroimage. 2004; 21: 99–111. pmid:14741647
  64. 64. Ichikawa N, Kaga M, Fujiwara M, Kawasaki S, Kawaguchi F. Development of optical topography system ETG-l00. MEDIX. 2001; 34: 47–52.
  65. 65. Marumo K, Takizawa R, Kinou M, Kawasaki S, Kawakubo Y, Fukuda M, et al. Functional abnormalities in the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex during a semantic fluency task, and their association with thought disorder in patients with schizophrenia. Neuroimage. 2014; 85: 518–526. pmid:23624170
  66. 66. Wobbrock JO, Findlater L, Gergle D, Higgins JJ. The aligned rank transform for nonparametric factorial analyses using only anova procedures. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems; 2011 May 7–12; Vancouver BC, Canada. (CHI ‘11). Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, 143–146.
  67. 67. Conover WJ, Iman RL. Rank transformations as a bridge between parametric and nonparametric statistics. The American Statistician. 1981; 35: 124–129.
  68. 68. Akritas MG. The rank transform method in some two-factor designs. J Am Stat Assoc. 1990; 85: 73–78.
  69. 69. Higgins JJ, Blair RC, Tashtoush S. The aligned rank transform procedure. Proceedings of the Conference on Applied Statistics in Agriculture. Manhattan, Kansas: Kansas State University; 1990. p. 185–195.
  70. 70. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988.
  71. 71. Ptito M, Moesgaard SM, Gjedde A, Kupers R. Cross-modal plasticity revealed by electrotactile stimulation of the tongue in the congenitally blind. Brain. 2005; 128: 606–614. pmid:15634727
  72. 72. Kupers R, Fumal A, de Noordhout AM, Gjedde A, Schoenen J, Ptito M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the visual cortex induces somatotopically organized qualia in blind subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006; 103: 13256–13260. pmid:16916936
  73. 73. Vuillerme N, Pinsault N, Fleury A, Chenu O, Demongeot J, Payan Y, et al. Effectiveness of an electro-tactile vestibular substitution system in improving upright postural control in unilateral vestibular-defective patients. Gait Posture. 2008; 28: 711–715. pmid:18632272
  74. 74. Miyashita Y. How the brain creates imagery: projection to primary visual cortex. Science. 1995; 268: 1719–1720. pmid:7792596
  75. 75. Sparing R, Mottaghy FM, Ganis G, Thompson WL, Töpper R, Kosslyn SM, et al. Visual cortex excitability increases during visual mental imagery—a TMS study in healthy human subjects. Brain Res. 2002; 938: 92–97. pmid:12031540
  76. 76. Spence C, Deroy O. Crossmodal mental imagery. In S. Lacey & R. Lawson (Eds.), Multisensory imagery. Springer Science + Business Media. pp. 157–183. 2013.
  77. 77. Pearson J. The human imagination: the cognitive neuroscience of visual mental imagery. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2019; 20: 624–634. pmid:31384033
  78. 78. Dijkstra N, Bosch SE, van Gerven MA. Vividness of visual imagery depends on the neural overlap with perception in visual areas. J Neurosci. 2017; 37: 1367–1373. pmid:28073940
  79. 79. Fulford J, Milton F, Salas D, Smith A, Simler A, Winlove C, et al. The neural correlates of visual imagery vividness ‐ An fMRI study and literature review. Cortex. 2018; 105: 26–40. pmid:29079342
  80. 80. Keogh R, Bergmann J, Pearson J. Cortical excitability controls the strength of mental imagery. Elife. 2020; 9: e50232. pmid:32369016
  81. 81. Gulyás E, Gombos F, Sütöri S, Lovas A, Ziman G, Kovács I. Visual imagery vividness declines across the lifespan. Cortex. 2022; 154: 365–374. pmid:35921690
  82. 82. O’ Dowd A, Cooney SM, Newell FN. Self-reported vividness of tactile imagery for object properties and body regions: An exploratory study. Conscious Cogn. 2022; 103: 103376. pmid:35849942
  83. 83. Kumamoto Y, Kaiba Y, Imamura S, Minakuchi S. Influence of palatal coverage on oral function ‐ oral stereognostic ability and masticatory efficiency. J Prosthodont Res. 2010; 54: 92–96. pmid:20083447
  84. 84. Mary KM, Cherian B. Evaluation of oral stereognosis, masticatory efficiency, and salivary flow rate in complete denture wearers. J Indian Prosthodont Soc. 2020; 20: 290–296. pmid:33223699
  85. 85. Hirano K, Hirano S, Hayakawa I. The role of oral sensorimotor function in masticatory ability. J Oral Rehabil. 2004; 31: 199–205. pmid:15025651
  86. 86. Bhandari A, Hegde C, Prasad DK. Relation between oral stereognosis and masticatory efficiency in complete denture wearers: an in vivo study. Brazilian Journal of Oral Sciences. 2015; 9: 358–361.
  87. 87. Cerf-Ducastel B, Van de Moortele PF, MacLeod P, Le Bihan D, Faurion A. Interaction of gustatory and lingual somatosensory perceptions at the cortical level in the human: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Chem Senses. 2001; 26: 371–383. pmid:11369672
  88. 88. de Araujo IE, Simon SA. The gustatory cortex and multisensory integration. Int J Obes (Lond). 2009; 33 Suppl 2: S34–43. pmid:19528978
  89. 89. Fu KMG, Johnston TA, Shah AS, Arnold L, Smiley J, Hackett TA, et al. Auditory cortical neurons respond to somatosensory stimulation. J. Neurosci. 2003; 23: 7510–7515. pmid:12930789
  90. 90. Godenzini L, Alwis D, Guzulaitis R, Honnuraiah S, Stuart GJ, Palmer LM. Auditory input enhances somatosensory encoding and tactile goal-directed behavior. Nat Commun. 2021; 12: 4509. pmid:34301949
  91. 91. Frasnelli J, Collignon O, Voss P, Lepore F. Crossmodal plasticity in sensory loss. Prog Brain Res. 2011; 191: 233–249. pmid:21741555
  92. 92. Gilissen SR, Arckens L. Posterior parietal cortex contributions to cross-modal brain plasticity upon sensory loss. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2021; 67: 16–25. pmid:32777707
  93. 93. Ewall G, Parkins S, Lin A, Jaoui Y, Lee HK. Cortical and subcortical circuits for cross-modal plasticity induced by loss of vision. Front Neural Circuits. 2021; 15: 665009. pmid:34113240
  94. 94. Sullivan GM, Feinn R. Using Effect Size-or Why the P Value Is Not Enough. J Grad Med Educ. 2012; 4: 279–282. pmid:23997866
  95. 95. Peterson SJ, Foley S. Clinician’s Guide to Understanding Effect Size, Alpha Level, Power, and Sample Size. Nutr Clin Pract. 2021; 36: 598–605. pmid:33956359
  96. 96. Julien KC, Buschang PH, Throckmorton GS, Dechow PC. Normal masticatory performance in young adults and children. Arch Oral Biol. 1996; 41: 69–75. pmid:8833593
  97. 97. Månsson I, Sandberg N. Effects of surface anesthesia on deglutition in man. Laryngoscope. 1974; 84: 427–437. pmid:4814413
  98. 98. van der Bilt A, van der Glas HW, Mowlana F, Heath MR. A comparison between sieving and optical scanning for the determination of particle size distributions obtained by mastication in man. Arch Oral Biol. 1993; 38:159–162. pmid:8476345
  99. 99. Buschang PH, Throckmorton GS, Travers KH, Johnson G. The effects of bolus size and chewing rate on masticatory performance with artificial test foods. J Oral Rehabil. 1997; 24: 522–526. pmid:9250840
  100. 100. Aiyar A, Shimada A, Svensson P. Assessment of masticatory efficiency based on glucose concentration in orthodontic patients: A methodological study. J Oral Rehabil. 2022; 49:954–960. pmid:35899420
  101. 101. Shiga H, Nakajima K, Uesugi H, Komino M, Sano M, Arai S. Reference value of masticatory performance by measuring the amount of glucose extraction from chewing gummy jelly. J Prosthodont Res. 2022; 66: 618–622. pmid:34880167
  102. 102. Kamiyama M, Kanazawa M, Fujinami Y, Minakuchi S. Validity and reliability of a Self-Implementable method to evaluate masticatory performance: use of color-changeable chewing gum and a color scale. J Prosthodont Res. 2010; 54: 24–28. pmid:19837023
  103. 103. Takahara M, Shiraiwa T, Maeno Y, Yamamoto K, Shiraiwa Y, Yoshida Y, et al. Screening for a decreased masticatory function by a color-changeable chewing gum test in patients with metabolic disease. Intern Med. 2022; 61: 781–787. pmid:35296621