Figures
Abstract
Drosophila melanogaster is a popular model organism in the study of memory due to a wide arsenal of methods used to analyze neuronal activity. The most commonly used tests in research of behavioral plasticity are shock avoidance associated with chemosensory cues and courtship suppression after mating failure. Many authors emphasize the value of courtship suppression as a model of behavior most appropriate to natural conditions. However, researchers often investigate courtship suppression using immobilized and decapitated females as targets of courtship by males, which makes the data obtained from such flies less valuable. In our study, we evaluate courtship suppression towards immature mobile non-receptive females after training with mated or immature females combined with an aversive stimulus (quinine). We have shown that the previously described mechanisms of courtship suppression, as a result of the association of the courtship object with the repellent, as well as due to increased sensitivity to the anti-aphrodisiac cVA after mating failure, are not confirmed when immature mobile females are used. We discuss the reasons for the discrepancies between our results and literature data, define the conditions to be met in the courtship suppression test if the aim is to analyze the natural forms of behavioral plasticity, and present data on the test modifications to approximate conditions to natural ones.
Citation: Goncharova AA, Besedina NG, Bragina JV, Danilenkova LV, Kamysheva EA, Fedotov SA (2023) Courtship suppression in Drosophila melanogaster: The role of mating failure. PLoS ONE 18(8): e0290048. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0290048
Editor: Efthimios M. C. Skoulakis, Biomedical Sciences Research Center Alexander Fleming, GREECE
Received: May 31, 2023; Accepted: July 31, 2023; Published: August 10, 2023
Copyright: © 2023 Goncharova et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding: This study was supported by the State Program 47 GP "Scientific and Technological Development of the Russian Federation "(2019-2030), theme 0134-2019-0004 and St. Petersburg State University (project #94031363) (SAF). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
The courtship behavior of Drosophila melanogaster males towards females is an innate behavior that includes a series of actions after detecting and approaching a female. The male taps the female with a foreleg to sample her pheromones, starts to vibrate with one wing (courtship song) and pursuit her, afterwards licks the genitals and attempts to copulate [1, 2]. Courtship provides a male with auditory, mechanosensory, visual, and chemosensory signals from the target fly to assess its suitability (species, sex) and receptiveness [3, 4]. In response to courtship, the receptive female reduces locomotor speed and opens the vaginal plates promoting male copulation attempts [5, 6]. Mated and immature females are non-receptive and exhibit behaviors that prevent copulation including running away, flicking their wings, and other behaviors [7, 8]. Courtship activity is measured as the percentage of time spent in courtship out of the total observation time in a testing chamber (courtship index, CI) [9]. In addition to courtship, males can engage in preening, running, or resting [10]. The intensity of courtship towards virgin mature (receptive) and immature (non-receptive) females was reported to remain consistently high for at least an hour or until copulation [9, 11]. At the same time, the intensity of male courtship towards a fertilized (non-receptive) female gradually decreases over 30–60 minutes and copulation attempts fail [9]. 1–3 hours after isolation from a mated female males show courtship suppression when tested with another mated female for 10 minutes, compared to males without experience of prior mating failure experience [12, 13]. This short-term courtship suppression is considered a form of conditioning and is used to study the molecular mechanisms of behavioral plasticity [14, 15]. Protocols and facilities have been developed for mass testing of courtship suppression in flies [16]. Courtship conditioning is also used to test long-term memory lasting several days, in which males are kept with a female for 5–7 hours to perform intermittent multiple courtship attempts [17, 18]. If viewed as conditioning the long-term suppression of courtship in D. melanogaster males is similar to conditioned taste aversion [19] and the passive learning models in rodents [20]. In taste aversion, an animal acquires an aversion to the sweet taste of food that was paired with gastrointestinal malaise, and, in passive learning, the animal learns to avoid safe spaces (dark section or bottom area in the chamber) after shocks in them. It has been determined that taste aversion is mostly caused by a specific taste paired with illness [21] and passive avoidance in rats is the conditioned preference for the freezing reaction in an environment associated with electric shocks [22]. While many factors associated with the long-term suppression of courtship in Drosophila males have been revealed to date [23, 24], the specific conditioned stimuli for this form of learning have not been identified.
The mechanisms of short-term courtship suppression have been studied in more detail. It has been shown that short-term courtship suppression is caused by increased avoidance of cis-vaccenyl acetate (cVA), which is found on the body of mated females [13]. cVA is contained in the ejaculatory bulb of males [25] and is transferred to the reproductive organs and cuticle of females during copulation [26, 27]. Enhanced avoidance of cVA in courtship tests with mated females (testers) does not depend on the presence of this anti-aphrodisiac during training on a non-receptive female (trainer). Unsuccessful courtship of both mated and immature females has been reported to cause courtship suppression towards a mated female [28]. Changes in courtship behavior are determined by the activity of aSP13 dopaminergic neurons that modulate synaptic connections between Kenyon cells and mushroom body (MB) output neurons M6 in MB gamma lobe [15]. However, the mechanism of aSP13 activation in response to mating failure has not been determined.
One possible trigger for dopaminergic activation may be changes in hormone level in response to unsuccessful courtship attempts. Knockdown of juvenile hormone receptors in dopaminergic neurons has been shown to reduce courtship intensity [29]. In adult D. melanogaster males, the level of juvenile hormone positively correlates with the level of 20-hydroxyecdysone (20E) [30], the active metabolite of ecdysone, which is also involved in courtship suppression. Ishimoto et al. [31] revealed that 20E levels increase after courtship conditioning and ecdysone signaling mutants exhibit defects in memory of mate failure. The intensity of courtship is also dependent on the level of ecdysis-triggering hormone (ETH), which provides post-copulation courtship inhibition [32]. Knockdown of ETH receptors in the corpus allatum, where the juvenile hormone is produced, as well as in sensory neurons OR67D and GR32A, which detect the male antiaphrodisiacs cVA and 7-tricosene (7T), respectively, abolishes courtship suppression after mating [32]. Therefore, courtship suppression induced by hormones depends on both the control centers in the fly brain and receptors in the sensory organs (antennae and labella). In addition, silencing ETH receptors in local interneurons of the antennal lobes also leads to disinhibition of courtship [33].
Since mating failure with a non-receptive female also causes hormonal changes, one can expect changes in sensitivity not only to cVA [13], but also to other antiaphrodisiacs, particularly to 7T, which is detected by GR32A receptors [34]. Moreover, other ligands that bind to GR32A should also increase their effects on fly behavior after unsuccessful courtship. In addition to 7T, GR32A receptors detect several other substances that the flies avoid, including quinine [35, 36]. Previously, Ackerman & Siegel [11] showed that male courtship towards an unreceptive immature female decreased within an hour when quinine was present in the observation chamber. The authors suggested that an association was formed between quinine and aphrodisiacs, leading to reduced courtship stimulation by aphrodisiacs. In their experiment, immature females were used as trainers and paired with quinine in a chamber, while mature females immobilized with ether were used as testers. Courtship suppression was observed in a delayed test without quinine in the chamber, conducted 5–120 minutes after training. The change in courtship targets raises questions about the influence of the specific pheromonal composition of mature females and the smell of ether on the results. In addition, it remains unknown whether courtship suppression would be observed if immature females were used as testers. On the other hand, if the decline in CI during one-hour training with immature females paired with quinine in the work of Ackerman & Siegel [11] is due to non-associative increased sensitivity to quinine, then courtship suppression can be demonstrated in a test with immature females paired with quinine after mating failure with the same type of female without quinine in the chamber.
To answer these questions, we evaluated the suppression of courtship towards non-receptive mobile immature females when quinine was added to the observation chamber. We found that mating failure in the absence of quinine did not increase the suppression of courtship behavior by quinine. To explain the inconsistency of results with our suggestion we compared courtship suppression in tests with mated females after mating failure with immature or mature mated females. Reduction in CI was observed only after training with mated females. Thus, mating failure alone is not sufficient to cause short-term courtship suppression. Furthermore, contrary to the conclusions of Ackerman & Siegel [11] we did not observe decreased CI in the test without quinine after mating failure in the presence of quinine. We discuss the need to revise views on the mechanism of courtship suppression in conditions close to the wildlife and propose a modified courtship suppression test protocol that is more consistent with the behavior of flies in the natural environment. It was revealed that males reared with females according to the protocol demonstrated long-term and short-term behavioral plasticity towards mated females similar to naïve males.
Materials and methods
Experimental flies
The wild-type strain Canton-S (CS) of D. melanogaster was obtained from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center. Flies were maintained on standard yeast-semolina-raisin-sugar medium in plastic vials (95 mm in height and 25 mm in diameter) at 25°C and under a 12-hour light/dark cycle.
Experimental conditions
For courtship testing, all flies were collected as virgins without anesthesia by aspiration during 3 hours after eclosion and reared under standard conditions. Experimental males were kept individually for 5 days before the courtship assay unless otherwise specified. Immature females were used within 14–20 hours after eclosion and kept in a group of 20 flies. Mature females for male training and males for female fertilization were kept in same-sex groups of 20–25 flies per vial. A day before the experiment, 4-day-old females were combined with males of the same age in one vial and left to mate for 18–22 hours.
Courtship assay
To assess courtship behavior a male and a mobile female were placed by aspiration into a transparent perspex experimental chamber (15 mm in diameter, 5 mm in height) lined with filter paper. An ethogram of male courtship was observed for 300 seconds by identifying and recording the onset of courtship behaviors (orientation, pursuing, vibration coupled with rest or running, tapping, licking, copulative attempts) and non-courtship behaviors (running, preening, and rest). This allowed for the calculation of the courtship index (CI, the percentage of observation time occupied by all courtship elements) and the percentage of time occupied by each behavioral element. Ethogram data were decoded and analyzed using the "Drosophila Courtship Lite 1.4" software developed by Nikolai Kamyshev. All behavioral assays were carried out at least twice on different days by at least two operators at 25°C between 10 a.m. and 4 p.m.
Courtship suppression
To assess courtship suppression males were trained with immature or mated mobile females for an hour in the chamber. Courtship in the first and last 5 minutes of one-hour training was recorded. Males that copulated during training were discarded. Sham control males were kept individually in the chamber for an hour instead of training. After training, males were returned to vials for a 30-minute rest and then tested with another non-receptive female (immature or mated) for 5 minutes in a clean chamber. Flies were transferred using shaking and aspiration. To analyze the effects of quinine on courtship suppression 2.0 mg of quinine sulfate (Sigma-Aldrich, 6119-70-6) was brushed onto a 0.5 x 4.2 cm strip of filter paper (Whatman Grade 1) placed around the edge of the chamber.
Statistics
The comparisons were performed using a randomization test [37]. The null hypothesis was rejected If the p-value was less than 0.01. The 95% confidence intervals were calculated using bootstrapping [38] in “Drosophila Courtship Lite 1.4” (10,000 iterations). Spearman rank-order correlation coefficient (Spearman’s correlation) was calculated using a t-test in IBM SPSS Statistics 20 (Armonk, USA) software.
Results
Courtship suppression by quinine is equal in naive and trained males
Courtship suppression was examined in four groups of 5-day-old males. In the first two groups, quinine was added to the chamber during training, and testing was carried out after 30 minutes in one group with quinine in the chamber and in another group without quinine. In the other two groups, testing was similarly carried out with quinine in one group and without in the other, but quinine was not added to the chamber during training.
During the one-hour training with immature females, the male courtship decreased by 32% (Fig 1A). However, in the test conducted 30 minutes after training, the CI returned to the initial value observed in naive males (Fig 1С). If quinine was added to the chamber, the CI in the first 5 minutes of training was almost twice as low compared to males in the chamber without quinine (Fig 1A). At the same time, training in a chamber with quinine did not lead to a significant decrease in courtship time (Fig 1A). These results contradict the dynamics of CI observed with immature females reported by Ackerman & Siegel [11], indicating the need for further study to clarify the methodological and other factors causing this discrepancy. Unfortunately, we did not find other studies that evaluated courtship dynamics with immature mobile females.
(A) The CIs are shown within the first 5 minutes (5) and the last 5 minutes (60) of one-hour training, conducted in either a clean chamber or a quinine chamber (5Q and 60Q). (B) The CIs are shown in the courtship suppression test conducted in a clean chamber after training in either a clean (-/-) or quinine chamber (Q/-), as well as in the test where quinine was added to the chamber after training in a clean (-/Q) or quinine chamber (Q/Q). (C) Comparisons of the CIs are provided in clean chambers between the first 5 minutes of training (5) and the tests conducted after training in a clean (-/-) or quinine chamber (Q/-), as well as in quinine chambers between the first 5 minutes of training (Q5) and the test conducted after training without quinine (-/Q). Mean values and 95% confidence intervals are shown. The p-values (two-tailed randomization test) are indicated above the respective columns in the comparison, and the number of flies in each group is shown below.
We also did not find a significant difference in courtship levels towards immature females in tests conducted after one-hour training in a clean or quinine chamber (Fig 1B). Our data do not support the conclusions of Ackerman & Siegel [11] regarding the formation of an association between female aphrodisiacs and quinine, resulting in courtship suppression. The discrepancies are likely due to differences in courtship objects used in the tests (mature and immature females) and experimental conditions. The courtship suppression observed with mature females after training in a chamber with quinine in the study of Ackerman & Siegel [11] may be influenced by female immobilization or the smell of ether (anesthesia). The use of mature females instead of immature ones in the courtship test seems to be an unlikely reason for the discrepancy between our results and Ackerman & Siegel’s [11] data since the authors also observed courtship suppression after training two males in a chamber with quinine and fly carcass coating with an extract of mature females. Therefore, altering the trainer and chemosensory cues did not affect the suppression of courtship towards an immobilized female. An important difference with Ackerman & Siegel’s [11] experiment is the different volume of the chamber. In Ackerman & Siegel’s [11] work, the chamber had a volume of 0.4 cm3, which is 2.2 times smaller than in the present study (0.9 cm3). The association of quinine with cues of a fly may occur in a more limited space. It is also worth noting that in the experiments of Ackerman & Siegel [11], females were collected under light ether anesthesia 0–10 hours after eclosion while in our experiments all flies were collected without anesthesia by aspiration during 3 hours after eclosion.
We could not confirm our hypothesis regarding the effect of mating failure on courtship suppression values in the test with quinine (Fig 1C). The CI in the test with quinine in the chamber was 43% after training with immature females, compared to 48% for males with no experience of mating failure (Fig 1C). Keleman et al. [13] demonstrated a non-specific increase in sensitivity to the anti-aphrodisiac cVA after mating failure. To train males, they used pseudomated mature females that behave like mated non-receptive females (remaining virgin) due to signal peptide transgene expression in the nervous system [39]. Therefore, we investigated whether mating failure with immature females would also lead to a decrease in CI in the test with a mated female. As a positive control, mated females were used for training.
Mating failure with immature females does not suppress courtship in the test with mated females
Unlike training with mated females, training for one hour with immature females did not result in courtship suppression in the test with a mated female (Fig 2A). This finding indicates that mating failure alone is not sufficient to increase sensitivity to cVA on mated females as previously reported by Keleman et al. [13]. We speculate that courtship suppression after training with pseudomated females is likely object-specific. The absence of courtship suppression after mating failure with immature females may explain the negative result in revealing the suggested increase in sensitivity to quinine. Similar to cVA, this effect may only be found in pseudomated females.
(A) The CIs of males in tests with mated females are shown after training with immature and mated females. The comparison group consists of males placed in a chamber without a female during the training stage (sham). (B) Percentages of behaviors observed within the first 5 minutes during training with immature and mated females are displayed. See legend in Fig 1.
We compared the courtship behavior of males with immature and mated females during the first 5 minutes of training. Males trained with immature females were more likely to follow immature females with wing vibration or without vibration (following), less likely to run regardless of the movement of the female, and almost did not attempt copulation (Fig 2B). We suggested that the effectiveness of training depended on the magnitude of parameters that differed between courtship towards immature and mated females. Since these parameters have a high variance in the group with mating females, we estimated Spearman’s correlation between the parameters and CIs in the group. The analysis results showed no correlation, leaving the question of courtship suppression factors open. The specificity in rejecting of immature females in response to male courtship should also be considered, particularly the absence of ovipositor extrusion and a higher frequency of fluttering [7].
Thus, the study of courtship suppression using more natural conditions does not confirm findings obtained under conditions that are far from natural (e.g., immobilization of courtship objects, ectopic expression of behavioral factors) and significantly alter male behavior [40]. Since the main advantage of courtship suppression over other memory tests is its naturalness, our results emphasize the need for a more rigorous approach to methodological aspects when studying memory in courtship behavior. In addition to selecting the appropriate courtship object, it is crucial to conduct testing under conditions closer to natural ones. In particular, large observation chambers with the addition of yeast drops can be used, and the courtship behavior of several males towards several females can also be analyzed [7, 28]. However, these solutions require optimization, as they make experiments more expensive and pose challenges for automated analysis. Moreover, optimizing the conditions for fly maintenance before experiments is also necessary. For example, keeping males in a group significantly reduces courtship towards mated females [10]. Most studies isolate males for five days before the courtship suppression test, while females are kept in a group without males. Both conditions differ from the natural conditions of reproduction in mixed groups on a nutrient substrate, which is the case in both natural populations and laboratory stocks. Furthermore, the analysis of the evolutionary role of courtship suppression indicates that this form of learning is associated with the slowly recovering receptivity of mated females and is important for adaptation to high-density populations where females may mate a second time [41].
Males kept in a group with females learn to suppress courtship like naive males
To approximate fly maintenance for courtship suppression tests to natural conditions, we examined the possibility of using experienced males reared for three days in a mixed group with females. Courtship behavior testing was conducted immediately and with a delay after isolating the males from the group. Mated and virgin mature females were used. Males kept in a mixed group showed reduced courtship towards both mated and virgin mobile females compared to males of the same age kept alone (Fig 3A). The reduced courtship of experienced males towards virgin females was observed for three hours (Fig 3C) and returned to the values in naive males after 24-hour isolation (Fig 3B). At the same time, reduced CI in experienced males towards mated females persisted at least for 24 hours. This indicates the retention of long-term memory about the non-receptivity of mated females rather than the sexual satiety since the courtship level towards virgin females at that time is high. The nature of courtship suppression with virgin females in the first hours after isolation from the mixed group requires further investigation. It is noteworthy that in addition to a reduced CI, experienced males analyzed immediately after isolation also displayed a lower percentage of copulations with virgin females and a longer courtship time before copulation (Fig 3D). The reduced percentage of copulations may be a consequence of reproductive behavior satiety resulting from previous multiple copulations within the group, while the extended courtship time before copulation can reflect the experience of males in courting non-receptive females within the group.
CIs in males kept alone or isolated from the mixed group are shown in immediate (A) and 24-hour delayed (B) courtship assays conducted with virgin and mated females. (C) CIs towards virgin females by males kept alone or isolated from the mixed group at different time intervals (0, 1, and 3 hours) before the assay are shown. (D) The time of courtship towards virgin females before copulation in the immediate assay are shown and the proportion and number of males copulated during 300 seconds of observation are provided under the columns. (E) CIs towards virgin and mated females by experienced males trained with mated females 24 hours after isolation from the mixed group are shown. The comparison group consists of experienced males placed in a chamber without a female during the training stage (sham control). See legends in Figs 1 and 2.
Subsequently, we conducted a courtship suppression test 24 hours after isolating males from the mixed group. One-hour training with mated females resulted in courtship suppression towards mated females but not towards virgin females (Fig 3E). Therefore, our findings indicate that males reared in a mixed group can be used to study both long-term and short-term behavioral plasticity towards mated females under conditions that are closer to natural ones.
Discussion
Our results do not support the existence of associative forms of plasticity in courtship behavior towards mobile immature flies. Previous studies by Ackerman & Siegel [11] and other authors [42, 43] aimed to confirm the hypothesis that courtship suppression was a result of the association between aphrodisiacs and antiaphrodisiacs on the body of mated females [9]. However, these studies used immobilized females to test conditioned avoidance. The females were either decapitated or anesthetized with ether, even if they were non-receptive. Immobilizing the females negated the main advantage of courtship suppression. As a model for the study of learning, courtship was considered a natural form of behavior compared to other tests for memory such as shock-associated odor conditioning [44]. Whether suppression of courtship towards immobilized (e.g., dead) females is a natural form of behavior remains an open question. The authors noted the great variation among trials as the reason for the rejection of mobile receptive females [9, 45]. More recent studies have shown that mating failure does not suppress courtship towards mobile receptive females [13, 28], although opposite results have also been reported [46]. The reasons for these discrepancies remain unclear, but it is important to note that courtship conditioning is highly dependent on the testing conditions. For example, suppression of courtship towards immobilized receptive females is absent when training with an immature female is conducted in the light, but can be observed when training is conducted in the dark [42]. Similarly, we were unable to confirm courtship suppression towards mated females after training with immature females as previously reported by Dukas [28] who conducted tests in large vials with two males and two females simultaneously.
Ejima et al. [47, 48] investigated courtship suppression in the dark using immobilized immature, virgin, and mated females as trainers and testers in various combinations. Based on the results of these tests, they proposed the formation of an association between mating failure and several undefined conditioned stimuli. Two ways of learning were described: conditioning to a trainer-specific pheromonal profile and generalized cVA-related conditioning (for review, see [49]). In the first way, courtship suppression was observed in tests with immobilized immature or virgin females but only if the training was conducted with the same type of females. The second way is characterized by the suppression of courtship towards all three types of females after training with a mated female. However, attempts to identify the pheromonal profile mediating courtship conditioning have been unsuccessful. Siwicki et al. [43] found a correlation between conditioning with mated trainers and the levels of 9-pentacosene. However, in tests with females lacking cuticular hydrocarbons, adding 9-pentacosene to the chamber was not sufficient to induce courtship suppression in males after mating failure. Therefore, the mechanism of courtship suppression using immobilized females remains undetermined, partly because this form of behavioral plasticity is not natural and involves multiple adaptation mechanisms.
It should be noted that considering mating failure as an aversive stimulus in the work of Ejima et al. [47] does not meet the criteria of associative learning. An association is evaluated based on the reaction to a conditioned stimulus, which is combined in time with the reaction in training and previously did not trigger this response or triggered it extremely weakly [50]. Therefore, considering mating failure as a stimulus is incorrect, as failure itself is not a sensory signal triggering a reaction. The nonspecific sensitization to anti-aphrodisiac and hormonal shifts observed by Keleman et al. [13] and Ishimoto et al. [31] after mating failure indicate a change in the functional state of the organism, in which the same stimuli elicit different innate reactions (avoidance) that are not the result of association. Similarly, inserting a nipple into an infant’s mouth triggers a sucking reflex if the infant is hungry, but after being satiated, the infant exhibits nipple spitting and other innate reactions to avoid feeding [51]. Interestingly, short-term suppression of courtship towards mobile immature males is also a form of non-associative learning resulting from habituation to aphrodisiacs on the cuticle of immature individuals [52]. Thus, the mechanisms of short-term courtship suppression with mobile flies such as pseudomated females and immature males have been described. On the other hand, the explanation for the courtship suppression with immobilized flies remains at the level of hypotheses. In Fig 4 we have illustrated published data on the presence or absence of courtship suppression in various combinations of trainers and testers, as well as on the mechanisms of suppression.
(A) Combinations described in the literature [9, 12, 13, 17, 43, 45, 47, 48, 53, 54] are illustrated where the trainer and/or tester flies are immobilized. (B) Combinations described in the literature [12, 13, 28, 52] and all other possible combinations are illustrated where both the trainer and tester flies are mobile. A review of the objects, conditions, and conclusions of the mentioned studies can be found in a previous review [49].
It is important that the associative suppression of courtship can, in principle, be developed, for example, in response to external cues from non-receptive females, such as eye color [55]. At the same time, this form of conditioning is achieved by approaching the conditions of training and testing as close as possible to natural ones. Males are placed for training in a group with receptive and non-receptive mobile females with different external characteristics and are tested in a day with two receptive mobile females that are different in these characteristics.
It is still unknown whether long-term courtship suppression in a standard assay using mated females [17] results from non-associative or associative avoidance. In the passive avoidance model in rats, an association is formed between environmental cues and electric shocks, which inhibit relocation to a safe place and increase freezing response [22]. However, the specific cues that are crucial for long-term courtship suppression have not yet been identified (Fig 4B). It is important to note, that in experiments on long-term courtship suppression, researchers often use immobilized flies [23, 24]. Considering that there is no data on the courtship towards immobilized females in nature, this approach may be ineffective for studying memory mechanisms, as was the case with short-term memory. For example, inconsistencies in courtship assay results were observed in studies involving juvenile hormone-deficient males, which differed in mobility and maturity. Wijesekera et al. [56], using immature females, showed that males with reduced levels of juvenile hormone acid O-methyl transferase have a reduced CI, whereas Lee et al. [57] found no decrease in CI in males tested with decapitated mature virgin females.
Conclusion
In the present study, using immature mobile females, we examined the mechanisms of courtship suppression described in the literature. Male courtship activity was tested after a training session with the addition of a repellent and after mating failure. Our data do not support the formation of an association between the aversive stimulus (quinine) and the aphrodisiacs of immature females, resulting in courtship suppression. Similarly, mating failure with immature females did not lead to courtship suppression with the same type of females. Our results highlight the dependence of behavioral plasticity mechanisms on various factors, including the mobility and maturity of females. Researchers studying courtship suppression as a natural model of memory should strive to use testing conditions that closely resemble the natural breeding environment of flies.
Supporting information
S1 Appendix. Experimental data.
All data used to generate the results for this study.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0290048.s001
(XLSX)
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to acknowledge Nikolai Kamyshev for his valuable comments and the Resource Center "Chromas" at St. Petersburg State University for their technical support.
References
- 1. Kohatsu S, Koganezawa M, Yamamoto D. Female Contact Activates Male-Specific Interneurons that Trigger Stereotypic Courtship Behavior in Drosophila. Neuron. 2011 Feb;69(3):498–508. pmid:21315260
- 2. Spieth HT. Courtship Behavior in Drosophila. Annu Rev Entomol. 1974 Jan;19(1):385–405.
- 3. Fan P, Manoli DS, Ahmed OM, Chen Y, Agarwal N, Kwong S, et al. Genetic and Neural Mechanisms that Inhibit Drosophila from Mating with Other Species. Cell. 2013 Jul;154(1):89–102. pmid:23810192
- 4. Griffith LC, Ejima A. Multimodal Sensory Integration of Courtship Stimulating Cues in Drosophila melanogaster: Contextual Effects on Chemosensory Cues. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2009 Jul;1170(1):394–8.
- 5. Clemens J, Coen P, Roemschied FA, Pereira TD, Mazumder D, Aldarondo DE, et al. Discovery of a New Song Mode in Drosophila Reveals Hidden Structure in the Sensory and Neural Drivers of Behavior. Current Biology. 2018 Aug;28(15):2400–2412.e6. pmid:30057309
- 6. Mezzera C, Brotas M, Gaspar M, Pavlou HJ, Goodwin SF, Vasconcelos ML. Ovipositor Extrusion Promotes the Transition from Courtship to Copulation and Signals Female Acceptance in Drosophila melanogaster. Current Biology. 2020 Oct;30(19):3736–3748.e5. pmid:32795437
- 7. Dukas R, Scott A. Fruit fly courtship: The female perspective. Current Zoology. 2015 Dec 1;61(6):1008–14. pmid:32256538
- 8. von Philipsborn AC. Neuroscience: The Female Art of Saying No. Current Biology. 2020 Oct;30(19):R1080–3. pmid:33022239
- 9. Siegel RW, Hall JC. Conditioned responses in courtship behavior of normal and mutant Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1979 Jul;76(7):3430–4.
- 10. Goncharova AA, Bragina YuV, Fedotov SA, Kamyshev NG. Influence of group rearing on sexual behavior of Drosophila melanogaster males. J Evol Biochem Phys. 2016 Nov;52(6):454–62.
- 11. Ackerman SL, Siegel RW. Chemically Reinforced Conditioned Courtship in Drosophila: Responses of Wild-Type and the dunce, amnesiac and don giovanni Mutants. Journal of Neurogenetics. 1986 Jan;3(2):111–23.
- 12. Kamyshev NG, Iliadi KG, Bragina JV. Drosophila Conditioned Courtship: Two Ways of Testing Memory. Learn Mem. 1999 Jan 1;6(1):1–20.
- 13. Keleman K, Vrontou E, Krüttner S, Yu JY, Kurtovic-Kozaric A, Dickson BJ. Dopamine neurons modulate pheromone responses in Drosophila courtship learning. Nature. 2012 Sep;489(7414):145–9. pmid:22902500
- 14. Kamyshev NG, Iliadi KG, Bragina JV, Kamysheva EA, Tokmatcheva EV, Preat T, et al. Novel memory mutants in Drosophila: Behavioral characteristics of the mutant nemyP153. BMC Neuroscience. 2002. pmid:12149133
- 15. Zhao X, Lenek D, Dag U, Dickson BJ, Keleman K. Persistent activity in a recurrent circuit underlies courtship memory in Drosophila. eLife. 2018 Jan 11;7:e31425. pmid:29322941
- 16. Gil-Martí B, Barredo CG, Pina-Flores S, Poza-Rodriguez A, Treves G, Rodriguez-Navas C, et al. A simplified courtship conditioning protocol to test learning and memory in Drosophila. STAR Protocols. 2023 Mar;4(1):101572. pmid:36633946
- 17. Keleman K, Krüttner S, Alenius M, Dickson BJ. Function of the Drosophila CPEB protein Orb2 in long-term courtship memory. Nat Neurosci. 2007 Dec;10(12):1587–93. pmid:17965711
- 18. Zhuravlev AV, Ivanova PN, Makaveeva KA, Zakharov GA, Nikitina EA, Savvateeva-Popova EV. cd1 Mutation in Drosophila Affects Phenoxazinone Synthase Catalytic Site and Impairs Long-Term Memory. IJMS. 2022 Oct 15;23(20):12356. pmid:36293213
- 19.
garcia1955-Conditioned Aversion to Saccharin Resulting from Exposure to Gamma Radiation.pdf.
- 20. Lorenzini C, Bucherelli C, Giachetti A. Passive and active avoidance behavior in the light-dark box test. Physiology & Behavior. 1984 Apr;32(4):687–9.
- 21. Kislal S, Blizard DA. Acquisition and retention of conditioned aversions to context and taste in laboratory mice. Learn Behav. 2018 Jun;46(2):198–212. pmid:29124570
- 22. Kiernan MJ, Westbrook RF, Cranney J. Immediate shock, passive avoidance, and potentiated startle: Implications for the unconditioned response to shock. Animal Learning & Behavior. 1995 Mar;23(1):22–30.
- 23. Lee SS, Adams ME. Regulation of Drosophila Long-Term Courtship Memory by Ecdysis Triggering Hormone. Front Neurosci. 2021 Apr 22;15:670322. pmid:33967686
- 24. Inami S, Sato T, Kurata Y, Suzuki Y, Kitamoto T, Sakai T. Consolidation and maintenance of long-term memory involve dual functions of the developmental regulator Apterous in clock neurons and mushroom bodies in the Drosophila brain. Shaw PJ, editor. PLoS Biol. 2021 Dec 3;19(12):e3001459. pmid:34860826
- 25.
butterworth1969-Lipids of Drosophila A Newly Detected Lipid in the Male.pdf.
- 26. Everaerts C, Farine JP, Cobb M, Ferveur JF. Drosophila Cuticular Hydrocarbons Revisited: Mating Status Alters Cuticular Profiles. Marion-Poll F, editor. PLoS ONE. 2010 Mar 9;5(3):e9607. pmid:20231905
- 27. Laturney M, Billeter JC. Drosophila melanogaster females restore their attractiveness after mating by removing male anti-aphrodisiac pheromones. Nat Commun. 2016 Aug 3;7(1):12322. pmid:27484362
- 28. Dukas R. Experience improves courtship in male fruit flies. Animal Behaviour. 2005 May;69(5):1203–9.
- 29. Hormonal control of motivational circuitry orchestrates the transition to sexuality in Drosophila. SCIENCE ADVANCES. 2021. pmid:34134981
- 30. Rauschenbach IYu Gruntenko NE, Chentsova NA Adonyeva NV, Alekseev AA. Role of ecdysone 20-monooxygenase in regulation of 20-hydroxyecdysone levels by juvenile hormone and biogenic amines in Drosophila. J Comp Physiol B. 2008 Jan;178(1):27–32.
- 31. Ishimoto H, Sakai T, Kitamoto T. Ecdysone signaling regulates the formation of long-term courtship memory in adult Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009 Apr 14;106(15):6381–6.
- 32. Meiselman MR, Ganguly A, Dahanukar A, Adams ME. Endocrine modulation of primary chemosensory neurons regulates Drosophila courtship behavior. Riddiford LM, editor. PLoS Genet. 2022 Aug 23;18(8):e1010357. pmid:35998183
- 33. Deshpande SA, Meiselman M, Hice RH, Arensburger P, Rivera-Perez C, Kim DH, et al. Ecdysis triggering hormone receptors regulate male courtship behavior via antennal lobe interneurons in Drosophila. General and Comparative Endocrinology. 2019 Jul;278:79–88. pmid:30543770
- 34. Wang L, Han X, Mehren J, Hiroi M, Billeter JC, Miyamoto T, et al. Hierarchical chemosensory regulation of male-male social interactions in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci. 2011 Jun;14(6):757–62. pmid:21516101
- 35. Kim H, Choi MS, Kang K, Kwon JY. Behavioral Analysis of Bitter Taste Perception in Drosophila Larvae. CHEMSE. 2016 Jan;41(1):85–94.
- 36. Ahmed OM, Avila-Herrera A, Tun KM, Serpa PH, Peng J, Parthasarathy S, et al. Evolution of Mechanisms that Control Mating in Drosophila Males. Cell Reports. 2019 May;27(9):2527–2536.e4. pmid:31141679
- 37. Nuzzo RL. Randomization Test: An Alternative Analysis for the Difference of Two Means. PM&R. 2017 Mar;9(3):306–10. pmid:28237692
- 38. Mokhtar SF, Md Yusof Z, Sapiri H. Confidence Intervals by Bootstrapping Approach: A Significance Review. Mal J Fund Appl Sci. 2023 Feb 25;19(1):30–42.
- 39. Nakayama S, Kaiser K, Aigaki T. Ectopic expression of sex-peptide in a variety of tissues in Drosophila females using the P[GAL4] enhancer-trap system. Mol Gen Genet. 1997 Apr;254(4):449–55. pmid:9180699
- 40. Tompkins L, Gross AC, Hall JC, Gailey DA, Siegel RW. The role of female movement in the sexual behavior ofDrosophila melanogaster. Behav Genet. 1982 May;12(3):295–307. pmid:6812562
- 41. Reif M, Linsenmair KE, Heisenberg M. Evolutionary significance of courtship conditioning in Drosophila melanogaster. Animal Behaviour. 2002 Jan;63(1):143–55.
- 42. Tompkins L, Siegel RW, Gailey DA, Hall JC. Conditioned courtship inDrosophila and its mediation by association of chemical cues. Behav Genet. 1983 Nov;13(6):565–78. pmid:6422921
- 43. Siwicki KK, Riccio P, Ladewski L, Marcillac F, Dartevelle L, Cross SA, et al. The role of cuticular pheromones in courtship conditioning of Drosophila males. Learn Mem. 2005 Nov;12(6):636–45.
- 44. Quinn WG, Harris WA, Benzer S. Conditioned Behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1974 Mar;71(3):708–12.
- 45. Gailey DA, Jackson FR, Siegel RW. CONDITIONING MUTATIONS IN DROSOPHILA MELANOGASTER AFFECT AN EXPERIENCE-DEPENDENT BEHAVIORAL MODIFICATION IN COURTING MALES. Genetics. 1984 Apr 1;106(4):613–23.
- 46. McBride SMJ, Giuliani G, Choi C, Krause P, Correale D, Watson K, et al. Mushroom Body Ablation Impairs Short-Term Memory and Long-Term Memory of Courtship Conditioning in Drosophila melanogaster. Neuron. 1999 Dec;24(4):967–77. pmid:10624959
- 47. Ejima A, Smith BPC, Lucas C, Levine JD, Griffith LC. Sequential Learning of Pheromonal Cues Modulates Memory Consolidation in Trainer-Specific Associative Courtship Conditioning. Current Biology. 2005 Feb;15(3):194–206. pmid:15694302
- 48. Ejima A, Smith BPC, Lucas C, van der Goes van Naters W, Miller CJ, Carlson JR, et al. Generalization of Courtship Learning in Drosophila Is Mediated by cis-Vaccenyl Acetate. Current Biology. 2007 Apr;17(7):599–605. pmid:17363250
- 49. Raun N, Jones S, Kramer JM. Conditioned courtship suppression in Drosophila melanogaster. Journal of Neurogenetics. 2021 Jul 3;35(3):154–67.
- 50. Hawkins RD, Byrne JH. Associative Learning in Invertebrates. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol. 2015 May;7(5):a021709. pmid:25877219
- 51. Ventura AK, Inamdar LB, Mennella JA. Consistency in infants’ behavioural signalling of satiation during bottle-feeding: Infants’ behavioural signalling of satiation. Pediatric Obesity. 2015 Jun;10(3):180–7.
- 52. Vaias LJ, Napolitano LM, Tompkins L. Identification of stimuli that mediate experience-dependent modification of homosexual courtship inDrosophila melanogaster. Behav Genet. 1993 Jan;23(1):91–7. pmid:8476396
- 53. Gailey DA, Jackson FR, Siegel RW. MALE COURTSHIP IN DROSOPHILA: THE CONDITIONED RESPONSE TO IMMATURE MALES AND ITS GENETIC CONTROL. Genetics. 1982 Dec 1;102(4):771–82. pmid:17246097
- 54. Montague SA, Baker BS. Memory Elicited by Courtship Conditioning Requires Mushroom Body Neuronal Subsets Similar to Those Utilized in Appetitive Memory. Broughton SJ, editor. PLoS ONE. 2016 Oct 20;11(10):e0164516. pmid:27764141
- 55. Verzijden MN, Abbott JK, Philipsborn ACV, Loeschcke V. Male Drosophila melanogaster learn to prefer an arbitrary trait associated with female mating status. Current Zoology. 2015 Dec 1;61(6):1036–42. pmid:32256540
- 56. Wijesekera TP, Saurabh S, Dauwalder B. Juvenile Hormone Is Required in Adult Males for Drosophila Courtship. Skoulakis EMC, editor. PLoS ONE. 2016 Mar 22;11(3):e0151912. pmid:27003411
- 57. Lee SS, Ding Y, Karapetians N, Rivera-Perez C, Noriega FG, Adams ME. Hormonal Signaling Cascade during an Early-Adult Critical Period Required for Courtship Memory Retention in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2017 Sep;27(18):2798–2809.e3. pmid:28918947