Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Feasibility of motor imagery and effects of activating and relaxing practice on autonomic functions in healthy young adults: A randomised, controlled, assessor-blinded, pilot trial

  • Turhan Kahraman,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Physiotherapy and Rehabilitation, Faculty of Health Sciences, Izmir Katip Celebi University, İzmir, Turkey

  • Derya Ozer Kaya,

    Roles Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Physiotherapy and Rehabilitation, Faculty of Health Sciences, Izmir Katip Celebi University, İzmir, Turkey

  • Tayfun Isik,

    Roles Investigation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Physiotherapy and Rehabilitation, Faculty of Health Sciences, Izmir Katip Celebi University, İzmir, Turkey

  • Sukriye Cansu Gultekin,

    Roles Investigation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Physiotherapy and Rehabilitation, Faculty of Health Sciences, Izmir Katip Celebi University, İzmir, Turkey

  • Barbara Seebacher

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Resources, Software, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    barbara.seebacher@i-med.ac.at

    Affiliations Clinical Department of Neurology, Medical University of Innsbruck, Innsbruck, Austria, Department of Rehabilitation Research, Rehab Centre Muenster, Muenster, Austria

Abstract

Introduction

Motor imagery (MI) is the mental rehearsal of a motor task. Between real and imagined movements, a functional equivalence has been described regarding timing and brain activation. The primary study aim was to investigate the feasibility of MI training focusing on the autonomic function in healthy young people. Further aims were to evaluate participants’ MI abilities and compare preliminary effects of activating and relaxing MI on autonomic function and against controls.

Methods

A single-blinded randomised controlled pilot trial was performed. Participants were randomised to the activating MI (1), relaxing MI (2), or control (3) group. Following a MI familiarisation, they practiced home-based kinaesthetic MI for 17 minutes, 5 times/week for 2 weeks. Participants were called once for support. The primary outcome was the feasibility of a full-scale randomised controlled trial using predefined criteria. Secondary outcomes were participants’ MI ability using the Movement Imagery Questionnaire-Revised, mental chronometry tests, hand laterality judgement and semi-structured interviews, autonomic function.

Results

A total of 35 participants completed the study. The feasibility of a larger study was confirmed, despite 35% attrition related to the COVID-19 pandemic. Excellent MI capabilities were seen in participants, and significant correlations between MI ability measures. Interview results showed that participants accepted or liked both interventions. Seven major themes and insider recommendations for MI interventions emerged. No significant differences and negligible to medium effects were observed in MI ability or autonomic function between baseline and post-intervention measures or between groups.

Conclusions

Results showed that neither activating nor relaxing MI seems to change autonomic function in healthy individuals. Further adequately powered studies are required to answer open questions remaining from this study. Future studies should investigate effects of different MI types over a longer period, to rule out habituation and assess autonomic function at several time points and simultaneously with MI.

Introduction

Most current physiotherapy approaches are based on the training of real movements to stimulate damaged motor neural connections through neuroplasticity-related mechanisms [1]. As a complementary strategy, motor imagery (MI) has increasingly been used in the physiotherapy field, defined as a mental rehearsal of movement without revealing a real movement [2]. MI is frequently categorised into visual and kinaesthetic imagery [3]. Kinaesthetic imagery would mean that an individual “senses” his/her own body moving. In contrast, visual imagery would be referred to when an individual “watches” the execution of an imagined motor action. Various studies have reported an activation of similar brain areas during movement execution and MI [46] and a similar duration of imagined and real movements (temporal congruence) [7]. Both the overlap in brain areas and comparable timing of real and imagined movements are referred to as their functional equivalence [8].

Several studies have found that MI training improves balance and mobility in older adults [9]. Accelerated functional recovery [10] and upper limb motor function gains have been observed after MI in individuals post stroke [11]. MI improves muscle strength in healthy adults [12] and facilitates motor function in people with Parkinson’s disease [13]. Moreover, RCTs have shown that MI is effective for people with multiple sclerosis [1418].

Numerous studies have investigated the mechanisms of MI [5, 6, 19, 20]. Some have suggested MI training-related benefits induced by improved motor learning, due to a strengthening of synaptic connections [6, 20]. Others have proposed changes in metabolic responses caused by MI training, comparable to those following real exercise training. A pioneering study observed similar acute effects on physiological parameters such as heart rate, oxygen consumption, blood pressure, respiratory rate, and metabolic rate after MI practice as after actual exercise training in healthy college students [21]. However, there is a lack of evidence about the effects of MI training over a prolonged time on autonomic functions [22]. The primary aim was to investigate the feasibility of MI training focusing on autonomic function in healthy young individuals. Further aims were to assess participants’ MI abilities and compare the preliminary effects of activating and relaxing MI on autonomic function against a control group.

Study hypotheses were that a full-scale RCT would be feasible, healthy young individuals would show high MI abilities, moderate to high correlations between MI ability measures would be seen, and trends for a change in autonomic function would be observed from baseline to post-intervention, with contrasting effects after activating and relaxing MI, as compared to the control group.

Materials and methods

Study design and setting

This pilot RCT was designed as an assessor-blinded trial with three parallel groups (1:1:1 allocation ratio) and carried out from 21.11.2019 to 31.3.2020. Assessments were performed at the Physiotherapy and Rehabilitation Laboratory, Izmir Katip Celebi University, Izmir, Turkey. Interventions were home-based and participants were supported by pre-recorded audio-visual files containing instructions and MI exercises.

The study protocol followed the CONSORT statement (S1 Checklist), was prospectively registered with ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT04171271), approved by Non-invasive Research Ethics Board of Izmir Katip Celebi University (reference 2019/399, 26.09.2019) and performed following the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent before they participated (see S1 Study protocol).

Participants

Healthy young individuals were recruited. This study was advertised at six classes (362 students) at Izmir Katip Celebi University, Izmir, Turkey. Inclusion criteria were: >18 years of age, apparently healthy and absence of any neurological or orthopaedic disease. Exclusion criteria were pregnancy and history of any previous disorder or surgery, altering the physical performance or physiological functions.

Eligible students interested were randomised into one of three groups: activating kinaesthetic MI training (group 1), relaxing kinaesthetic MI training (group 2), and controls (group 3).

Twelve participants per group are considered a minimum sample size [23], and 10% of the total sample size of a full-scale study is suggested for pilot studies [24]. Based on a power of 80%, a two-tailed alpha of 0.05, mean 1 of -2.13, mean 2 of 0.20 and pooled standard deviation of 4.67 from the group differences in respiratory rates of an MI training study that used physiological measures [21], a sample size of 189 participants for three groups resulted (http://powerandsamplesize.com/). Considering these recommendations, the sample size was determined as 15 participants per group (by addition of 20% attrition, a total of 54 participants).

Participants of this study were invited for semi-structured interviews, asking questions regarding their experiences of the MI and the acceptability of the intervention. The number of interviewees was based on the concept of saturation. Data saturation is reached when no additional information (themes) is expected from further interviews, sufficient information has been gathered to replicate the study and further coding is no longer viable [25]. Guest et al. have found that this was achieved with six to twelve interviews [26].

Randomisation, allocation concealment and blinding

Recruitment used convenience sampling and simple randomisation with allocation concealment. The randomisation and assignment of participants to groups were performed by a researcher who was not involved in the assessment or intervention procedures (BS). Randomisation was based on a single sequence of random assignments [27] using a random number list (https://www.randomizer.org/). Participants were enrolled by the study PI (TK). Assessors were blinded to the group allocation until study completion. Participants were asked not to disclose their group allocation to anyone until the study was completed.

Study intervention

In group 1, the intervention consisted of activating kinaesthetic MI training. In group 2, the intervention comprised relaxing kinaesthetic MI training. In both intervention groups, due to the piloting character, the duration was two weeks. Participants were asked to practice MI at home for 17 minutes per day and 5 times per week. The practice duration and frequency were chosen according to a previous study [28]. An MI script was written for the interventions. The video footage of the exercises used for MI tasks was created together with MI instructions. The PETTLEP [8] ideas were applied to support participants’ understanding of the intervention in their respective groups and their effectiveness (Position/Physical, Environment, Tasks, Timing, Learning, Emotion and Perspective).

Following the allocation and baseline assessments, participants in the intervention groups received one session of an MI introduction programme comprising MI theory and practice [29]. Additionally, participants were carefully instructed about their suggested physical positions during MI practice, choice of a suitable, quiet training environment, the tasks to be performed, the timing of imagined movements, how to enhance their MI experience, arousal related (group 1) or calmness and relaxation related (group 2) and motivational aspects of the MI tasks and perspective in their respective groups [8]. The instructor (TK) and creator (BS) of two audio-visual files, which included the respective exercises created specifically for this study, are physiotherapists with a profound knowledge and practical experience in MI. Training files could be accessed from a password-protected cloud platform via a shared link. The MI intervention was performed at home and the practice frequency was recorded in an exercise diary (see S1 File for intervention details).

The control group (group 3) received no specific intervention and was only assessed at baseline and 2 weeks later. At the end of week 1, intervention group members were called for support with their MI and as a reminder of the post-intervention assessment. For equivalency reasons and as a reminder of the second assessment, control group members were also called after week 1.

Data collection: Demographic measures and body composition

Age, gender, height, and weight of the participants were recorded. Body composition was evaluated using a body composition analyser (MC-780MA, TANITA Corporation of America, Inc. IL). Quantities of body fat, muscle, and water were reported in kg and percent. Additionally, using bioelectrical impedance analyses, the level of visceral fat was reported and ranged from 0 to 10, where higher scores indicated more visceral fat [30]. Adequate validity and reliability of the TANITA system have been demonstrated in college students [31]. The hand dominance (laterality) in everyday activities was assessed using the validated Turkish version of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (EHI) [3234]. Blinded assessors (two licensed physiotherapists) collected data at baseline and post-intervention.

Primary outcome

Feasibility.

The primary outcome was the feasibility of conducting a full-scale RCT. The criteria for feasibility success were a target recruitment rate of 10% (or 10 participants per month), a target retention rate of 80% and a target minimum adherence rate of 70% of the overall practice sessions (or 7 MI sessions out of 10 sessions). In addition, high acceptability of the intervention and no serious adverse events were expected. Participants’ adherence with and acceptability of the intervention were reported narratively. Both were recorded during phone calls at week 1 and at post-intervention assessments. A record sheet was prepared for any adverse events observed during the intervention sessions and tests, including information about the severity, duration, causality (i.e., whether they were attributable to the intervention), time and date, and clinical action taken. Raw count and percentage of missing data were also recorded.

Secondary outcomes

Motor imagery ability.

Four different MI ability assessments were employed because using at least two different approaches is recommended [35, 36].

Movement Imagery Questionnaire-Revised (MIQ-R). It assesses the visual and kinaesthetic MI ability and comprises 4 visual and 4 kinaesthetic items [37]. Each item involves performing a movement, followed by imaging that same movement using a visual or kinaesthetic mode. Participants are then asked to rate the ease or difficulty of generating that image on a 7-point Likert scale. Higher scores indicate greater visual or kinaesthetic MI ability. A cut-off point of 25 out of the maximum score of 56 has been suggested previously [38] and hence for each of the two subscales, a 12.5 cut-off point was used to signify good or poor MI ability. Acceptable validity and reliability of the Turkish version of the MIQ-R have been demonstrated [39].

Mental chronometry (MC) tests. They assess the temporal equivalence of actual and imagined movements [40]. MC was evaluated employing a walking and writing task [41]. For the walking task, participants imagined and executed walking a 6-metre distance at a comfortable speed. For the writing task, participants imagined and executed writing the following sentence in Turkish: "Türkiye’nin başkenti Ankara" meaning “Ankara is the capital of Turkey.” For time taking, participants kept an electronic stopwatch in their non-dominant hand. MI quality was evaluated using the formula 1-((actual movement duration-MI duration)/actual movement duration) [42], where ideal values would be close to 1.

Hand Laterality Judgement (HLJ). It was measured using the Recognise™ App (Neuro Orthopaedic Institute, NOI, Adelaide) on a tablet. This App has been found to be valid and reliable in healthy individuals, and minimum detectable changes are available [43, 44]. The accuracy and reaction time for the right and left sides were reported. High values in accuracy and low values in reaction time indicate high MI ability [45]. A meta-analysis has shown values of 2.08 (95% confidence interval (CI): 2.02, 2.15) seconds and 91.9% (95%CI: 91.3%, 92.5%) for the HLJ time and accuracy in healthy controls, respectively [46].

Motor imagery experiences and acceptability of the intervention. It was considered important that participants accepted the MI practice as otherwise, one could not expect people to maintain MI training for a prolonged period of time. So, at post-intervention, participants in the intervention groups were invited for semi-structured face-to-face interviews by the study PI. They were asked about their perceptions of the MI interventions, their experiences during the MI and their suggestions for a future MI intervention [47]. Interviews were designed according to recommendations on qualitative interviewing [48] (S2 Checklist) and conducted by two Master’s students in physiotherapy (SCG and TI, female and male). Details are presented in S2 File and S1 Table.

Autonomic functions.

Physiological cardiorespiratory and metabolic responses as indicators of autonomic function were assessed. The basal metabolic rate (BMR) was measured using a cardiopulmonary exercise test device (Cosmed Quark CPET, Cosmed, Rome, Italy). Participants were asked to refrain from eating, caffeine intake or smoking ≥4 hours before the BMR assessment. They were also instructed to avoid strenuous exercise 24 hours in advance of the assessment [49]. Measurements were performed with participants wearing comfortable clothing and lying supine on a bed. The room was ventilated for at least 1 hour before the measurement. Initially, the system was calibrated to allow for accurate O2 and CO2 gas concentration measurement. During the measurement, participants wore a mask designed to cover the mouth and nose; they were asked to lie quietly for 15 minutes. The system automatically recorded data related to the BMR. Reported variables were the resting metabolic rate (RMR) representing the daily caloric expenditure (kcal/day) [50], respiratory quotient (RQ), O2 uptake (VO2) (ml/min), CO2 production (VCO2) (ml/min), minute ventilation (VE) (l/min), concentrations of O2 (FEO2, %) and CO2 (FECO2, %) in the exhaled gases and respiratory frequency (RF). The RQ is the volume of the CO2 released over the volume of the O2 absorbed during respiration and indicates which macronutrients are being metabolised, as different energy pathways are used for fats (RQ = 0.7), proteins (RQ = 0.8), and carbohydrates (RQ = 1.0). Fat and carbohydrate substrates (%) used for the energy metabolism were also assessed.

Data analyses

Statistical analysis.

IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 26.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp) or GraphPad Prism (Version 8, La Jolla, CA) was used for all statistical analyses. Statistical significance was defined as a two-tailed p<0.05. Intention-to-treat analysis was performed for all cases with complete follow-up data, which were analysed by original assigned groups. Descriptive statistics were reported for all outcomes. Continuous data were checked for outliers and normality using the Shapiro-Wilk test, histograms and Q-Q plots. Age, 6-metre walk MC, right-left discrimination judgements, RQ, VCO2, FEO2 and most of the TANITA body composition measures did not satisfy the normality criteria. A reciprocal transformation (1/x) changed the distributions into normal. Due to the small sample size, means (95% confidence interval) and medians (25th-75th percentiles) were reported for continuous and ordinal data, respectively and raw count (frequency, percentage) for nominal data.

Correlational analyses were performed between the different MI ability measures to evaluate their relationship. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients of 0.3–0.49 were considered low, 0.5–0.69 moderate, and ≥0.7 strong [51]; they were calculated with their 95%CI and p-values corrected for multiple comparisons using a Bonferroni correction.

The recruitment rate was estimated by dividing the number of participants who consented by the number of people eligible, multiplied by 100. The retention rate was determined: (N who completed the study, divided by N of the total sample) ×100. The adherence rate was reported as the percentage of the planned MI practice sessions (5×/week) actually performed by the participants over the 2-week study period [52]. The recruitment, retention, and adherence rates were calculated with their 95% CI [53]; when the proportion was close to 0 or 1, a Poisson approximation was employed [54]. Little’s test was performed to evaluate whether the data were missing completely at random (MCAR). A p>0.05 would signify missingness completely at random i.e., independence of missing values from both the observed and the unobserved data [55] so that a listwise deletion of cases with missing values was safe [56]. Patterns of missing data were analysed (without performing multiple imputations) to identify the missing data percentage.

Based on the CONSORT statement, all statistical tests for baseline comparison between the groups are to be avoided and therefore, data were presented without p-values. For continuous data, two-factor mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) was employed, with groups as between-subjects factor and time (baseline, post-intervention) as within-subject factor. ANOVA effect size measures were estimated as partial eta squared values (ηp2). For all relevant analyses, significant violations of ANOVA were tested for and where appropriate, standard correction procedures were applied. For ordinal data, Kruskal–Wallis test was used across all groups (Gps 1–3) and time points (baseline, post-intervention), followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons test. Kruskal-Wallis effect size measures were calculated as eta squared values (η2H = (H–k + 1) / (n—k), where H is the Kruskal-Wallis H statistic, k is the number of groups and n is the total number of observations [57]. Partial eta squared effect sizes of 0.02 are regarded small, 0.13 medium and 0.26 large, and eta squared effect sizes of 0.01 are referred to as small, 0.06 medium and 0.14 large [58].

Qualitative data analysis.

Interviews were transcribed and analysed manually using Qualitative Content Analysis [59, 60]. Both a data-driven and a concept-driven approach were chosen for the analysis. Details are presented in S2 File. Throughout the analyses, rigour, credibility and reliability were maximised [61, 62] by following a systematic and consistent approach (S1 Checklist). In addition, the entire dataset was double coded by two researchers within two weeks after the initial coding (SCG, TK). After translation into English, the coding was checked by a third researcher (BS) and any discrepancies were solved. Finally, researchers were aware of their effect on the interview process and outcomes based on the concept of reflexivity [63].

Results

Recruitment was completed as planned. Due to the COVID-19 pandemic-related lockdown, 19 out of 54 participants (35.0%) dropped out of the study (Fig 1). There were 28 females and 7 males with a mean age of 20.3 years (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic characteristics and body composition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254666.t001

Participants’ MI ability and autonomic (metabolic and cardiorespiratory) function at baseline are shown in Table 2.

thumbnail
Table 2. Motor imagery ability and autonomic function at baseline.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254666.t002

Of eligible 359 students, 54 were enrolled in the study within 5 months, the recruitment rate was 14.9% (95%CI: 11.5, 19.1%). That is, per month, 10–11 participants could be recruited into the study. Prior to the COVID-19 outbreak, which caused an attrition rate of 35.0%, 100.0% of the participants remained in the study. Considering the 100.0% retention rate observed before the pandemic onset, it is likely that the 80.0% target retention rate would have been achieved. Participants reported the intervention as acceptable or even pleasurable (66.0%). They reported having practiced a median 10 (10.0–10.0 25th-75th percentile) times within the 2-week intervention period, thus, adherence was excellent. There were no reported adverse events related to this study. Little’s test of missing completely at random [56] showed a non-significant Chi-Square test (p = 0.711), indicating that the data were MCAR. This test was performed for the 35 participants who completed the study. Pattern analysis showed that 0.4% of the data were missing, which seemed negligible.

MI ability is a prerequisite for performing MI effectively, which is why MI abilities were scrutinised in detail at baseline. According to the MIQ-R cut-off point of 12.5 points, 1 out of 35 participants reported low kinaesthetic MI ability, and all participants reported good visual MI ability. There was a negligible to weak and non-significant correlation for some of the MI ability measures. A weak to moderate correlation was observed between the HLJ times for the left (non-dominant) hand and the visual and kinaesthetic subscales of the MIQ-R and also between the HLJ times and MIQ-R subscales and within these measures, all of which were significant at the 0.01 level. In the intervention groups, these relationships were slightly more pronounced, and there was a moderate correlation between the MC measures only in group 2. In addition, in group 2, a strong correlation between the HLJ time and accuracy for the right (dominant) hand was seen (p<0.01). Correlations between the visual and kinaesthetic MIQ-R subscales and of these with the HLJ time for the left hand were also strong (negative) and significant (p<0.01). Moderate relationships shown for the control group were significant (p<0.01) (Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Correlations between the motor imagery ability assessments at baseline.

The heatmap presents Spearman’s Rank correlation coefficients (ranging from-1 to +1) of MI ability measures, where darker blue and red fields signify stronger correlations. (A) All participants. (B) Activating MI group. (C) Relaxing MI group. (D) Control group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254666.g002

Between baseline and post-intervention, there were no statistically significant changes in the MI ability on any of the measures or between groups (p>0.05), but medium effect sizes were observed in visual and kinaesthetic MI abilities (Fig 3). Additionally, medium effect sizes in MC of walking were seen after relaxing MI whereas for the remaining measures of MI capability, effect sizes were negligible to small (S2 Table).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Effects of activating and relaxing motor imagery ability on motor imagery ability.

(A) Visual MI ability. (B) Kinaesthetic MI ability. (A and B) Medians at baseline (pre) and post-intervention (post) are represented by squares and interquartile ranges by error bars. Units of measurement are indicated on the y-axis. Motor Imagery Questionnaire-Revised (MIQ-R) data were analysed using Kruskal-Wallis test, followed by Dunn’s correction for multiple comparisons, with overall p-values and partial eta squared effect sizes shown on top of the figures. (C to H) Means at baseline (pre) and post-intervention (post) are represented by squares and lower and upper 95% confidence intervals by error bars. Units of measurement are indicated on the y-axis. Mental chronometry (MC) and hand laterality judgements (HLJ) were analysed using a two-factor mixed analysis of variance ANOVA, followed by a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, with overall p-values shown on top of the figures. (C) MC between imagined and real walking using a 6-Metre Walk Test, where the dotted line indicates identical durations of real and imagined walking. (D) Mental chronometry using a writing task, where the dotted line indicates identical durations of real and imagined writing. (E to F) HLJ. (E) Right-left discrimination (RLD) time (seconds) for the left hand. (F) RLD time (seconds) for the right hand. (G) RLD accuracy (percentage of correct responses) for the left hand. (H) RLD accuracy (percentage of correct responses) for the right hand.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254666.g003

Between baseline and post-intervention, no significant differences were found in any of the metabolic function measures between the groups (p>0.05) (Fig 4). Except medium effects of relaxing MI on BMR, negligible to small effect sizes were observed (S2 Table).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Effects of activating and relaxing motor imagery ability on metabolic function.

(A to D) Means at baseline (pre) and post-intervention (post) are represented by squares and lower and upper 95% confidence intervals by error bars. Units of measurement are indicated on the y-axis. Data were analysed using a two-factor mixed analysis of variance ANOVA, followed by a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, with overall p-values shown on top of the figures. (A) RMR resting metabolic rate—caloric expenditure (B) BMR basal metabolic rate. (C) Fat metabol fat substrates used for energy metabolism. (D) CHO metabol carbohydrate substrates used for energy metabolism.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254666.g004

Between baseline and post-intervention, there were no significant differences in any of the cardiorespiratory function measures between groups (p>0.05) (Fig 5). A majority of effects were negligible to small, but those for VO2 and FECO2 following relaxing MI were medium (S2 Table).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Effects of activating and relaxing motor imagery ability on cardiorespiratory function.

(A to G) Means at baseline (pre) and post-intervention (post) are represented by squares and lower and upper 95% confidence intervals by error bars. Units of measurement are indicated on the y-axis. Data were analysed using a two-factor mixed analysis of variance ANOVA, followed by a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, with overall p-values shown on top of the figures. (A) RQ respiratory quotient. (B) VO2 oxygen uptake. (C) VCO2 carbon dioxide production. (D) VE minute ventilation. (E) RF respiratory frequency. (F) FEO2 concentration of oxygen in the exhaled gases. (G) FECO2 concentration of carbon dioxide in the exhaled gases.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254666.g005

For assessing the acceptability of the intervention, five participants in the activating MI group and 4 in the relaxing MI group took part in the semi-structured interviews. The mean duration of the interviews was 23.9 ± 4.2 minutes. All participants reported to have closed their eyes during the MI. Overall, all participants accepted the intervention and 6 of 9 even regarded it pleasurable and valuable. Seven major themes emerged from the analysis, which are described in detail and together with relevant categories in S2 File. Participants also expressed their opinions on how the use of MI should be for them to continue after the study. Minor themes and their frequencies are shown in Fig 6.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Motor imagery recommendations from study participants.

Numbers on the radar graph represent the numbers of participants in the two intervention groups who expressed the respective opinions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254666.g006

Discussion

This randomised controlled pilot trial demonstrated the feasibility of a full-scale RCT of MI practice focusing on autonomic function in healthy young people. We hypothesised that young and healthy participants would be able for MI. Study findings showed slightly discrepant results. While the self-report visual and kinaesthetic MI capabilities indicated excellent abilities to imagine movements according to the MIQ-R cut-off, the writing and walking MC tests yielded contradictory results. Median MC scores for the walking test signified perfect temporal congruence in our participants. These results are in agreement with the literature showing that MC is well preserved in young adults as compared to older adults [64]. For the writing task, however, we observed temporal incongruence. Contrastingly, a previous study reported no differences between the MC of walking and writing tasks [41]. A comparison between the two studies showed that both were small, with 35 participants in our study and 16 participants in the previous study. We compared the duration of a single trial of executed and imagined walking and writing tasks; the other study however, employed a repeated trial design, with 50 trials (10 actual and 40 imagined) for both the walking and writing tasks. Considering the repeated trial design, a learning effect could have occurred [65], which would explain some of the discrepancies in results. Consistent with the literature, we also suggest that the timing of a cyclical motor task such as walking is easier than that of a complex task like writing [66]. Moreover, walking is a highly automatic motor activity whereas nowadays, writing usually involves typing on a computer keyboard, particularly in young people. As for the HLJ times, our results agree with those from a meta-analysis of 25 studies with found values of 2.08 seconds and 91.9% for the HLJ time and accuracy in healthy controls, respectively [46].

We further hypothesised that there would be moderate to high correlations between the MI ability measures. Results confirmed our study hypotheses partly, with moderate to high and significant correlations between some of the measures. Another study assessed MI abilities in 12 swimmers using electrodermal (skin resistance) responses, subjective ratings (Visual Imagery of Movement Questionnaire-2), and an MC test (swimming turn sequence) [67]. Results showed significant moderate correlations between MI vividness and autonomic responses during MI, while all other correlations were non-significant [67]. Our findings are in line with those findings.

Results from the qualitative interview analyses showed that participants accepted or liked the interventions in both study groups. This is crucial indeed because one cannot expect people maintaining any practice of real or imagined exercises which they do not appreciate. Findings also indicated that the participants did experience MI; however, using a kinaesthetic or visual or mixed-mode and an internal or external perspective. Some of the participants explained that it was more natural for them to visually imagine themselves moving. Based on that, we speculate that the use of an alternative mode or perspective enabled them for MI or enhanced their MI capability. This was supported by their descriptions of their body perceptions, for example to having felt the muscles quite intensely. Further, their narratives on perceived barriers to MI such as fatigue and loss of concentration were in line with the literature [68, 69]. Facilitators were reported such as a quiet environment and being alone, which would naturally enhance concentration. A learning effect was also mentioned as some participants described initial difficulties with imagining the tasks which resolved with practice. Interestingly, participants in the activating MI group expressed some problems with MI of unfamiliar movements, particularly if they could not perform these motor tasks in real e.g., due to a lack of muscle strength. A potential lack in fitness had not been considered during the study development, since all participants were young and healthy physiotherapy students. In addition, we observed medium effects in mental chronometry of walking after relaxing MI only. Although walking was none of the trained tasks, the relaxed MI training of various movements with a focus on body perception could have contributed to this change. However, the findings were in agreement with the existing literature in people with neurological disorders such as stroke [69]. Additionally, a study in young dancers has found that with complex and unfamiliar movements e.g., complex jumps, primarily visual MI was used whereas kinaesthetic MI and real movement durations were significantly longer [70].

However, one could question whether a temporal congruence between both the visual and kinaesthetic MI modes and executed motor actions was required in all cases. Considering the theoretical tenets of MI, with the notion of functional equivalence [8], MC may indeed be a prerequisite. It is established that, prior to the execution of a motor action, the brain has already generated a motor representation, which is thought to include all movement phases from the movement plan until the intended result [71]. It seems logical that for a completely unknown motor action, motor representations are not yet available [72]. So, a reasonable level of familiarity and expertise gained by the training of a movement appears essential for the functional equivalence between physical and imagined movements. Evidence suggests that it may not be possible to imagine a motor task appropriately if one cannot execute the task physically [72]. This proposition has been supported by a functional magnetic resonance imaging study from Olsson et al. (2008) conducted in expert and novice high jumpers [73]. Results showed that MI of the complex high jumps led to activations of motor areas only in expert jumpers whereas in novices, recruitment of the visual and parietal areas was seen.

In our study, activating and relaxing MI training did not lead to statistically significant preliminary effects on autonomic function. This was opposed to our hypothesis that at least trends for a change in autonomic function would be seen post-intervention, with contrasting effects after activating and relaxing MI and as compared to the control group. Our hypothesis was based on the functional equivalence between real motor actions and imagined motor tasks. Functional equivalence provided, a high-intensity and relaxing MI training would yield similar but weaker effects than a real training of the same motor activities [74]. Concerning actual training, numerous studies have shown that both high-intensity training and relaxation practice [75] induce significant changes in autonomic function [76]. It is acknowledged that the present study was small due to the COVID-19 pandemic, as mentioned above. Therefore, there may have been a lack of power to detect significant differences. This suggestion is strengthened by medium effect sizes found for differences in VO2 and FECO2 after relaxing MI versus activating and controls. Nonetheless, other factors may be responsible for the absence of relevant changes post-intervention, which are yet to be explored.

A study compared the effects of MI alone with MI plus action observation against a control group on autonomic function in 45 healthy people and showed differences in the heart and respiratory rates and some of the measures of electrodermal activities [77]. Although different autonomic function measures were used in our study, one would expect similar results. However, that study investigated changes after a single intervention session. Conceivably, there may have been a habituation effect in our study, associated with the MI practice of five times per week and over two weeks. The autonomic nervous system is sensitive to novel stimuli and hence produces stronger responses such as heart rate increases alongside physical reactions [78]. If the same stimuli were presented repeatedly, like MI tasks in our study, elicited autonomic responses would be weaker or even absent because of habituation [79]. Moreover, in contrast to a previous study [80], we did not assess autonomic functions during the MI. Autonomic responses are often short-lived, a notion supported by another MI study in elite athletes, where steep increments in heart rates were seen when the MI commenced [80]. However, long-term changes in the resting heart rate after strength and endurance training, yoga, and tai chi have been reported by various studies [81], and hence, we regarded it reasonable to anticipate changes in autonomic responses after the intervention in our study.

There are some limitations to our study. Firstly, in this study, convenience sampling was used because it is straightforward, low-cost and efficient. For a larger study in a normal sample, considering the lack of generalisability of convenience samples, it seems useful however, taking into account a higher dropout rate of 30%. Secondly, although the number of participants enrolled was based on a sample size calculation including a 20% attrition rate, due to the COVID-19 pandemic-related shut-down, a substantial attrition rate of 35% was observed. Thus, the study remained with a small sample size. We accounted for that examining the type and patterns of missing data. Thirdly, no data were collected during the MI practice. This would have enabled us to compare our results to those from other studies directly. However, we expected to see some trends for changes in autonomic functions after the two-week intervention beyond the MI sessions. In addition, as our study intervention was home-based and involved 10 sessions, such measurements would have been impracticable. Finally, the intervention period was two weeks due to the exploratory nature of this study. Using a longer intervention duration, the chances for long-term effects may have been greater. The intervention was probably too long for short-term responses in the autonomic function to be elicited and too short for long-term effects to occur.

Further studies are required to figure out underlying mechanisms of activating and relaxing MI, particularly concerning the functional equivalence theory, and potentially including functional imaging measures. Future studies should also investigate the effects of MI over a longer period. These studies may assess autonomic function at several time points and simultaneously with activating (high intensity) and relaxing MI. Additional measures of autonomic functions could be useful, such as electrodermal activity.

Conclusions

Both activating and relaxing MI are acceptable interventions and a larger RCT is feasible. Healthy young people showed excellent MI abilities where the different MI ability measures corresponded with each other. After a two-week intervention consisting of activating or relaxing MI, no changes in the autonomic function were observed compared to non-intervention controls. These findings could be related to a habituation effect after the prolonged MI practice. The absence of measures performed during MI sessions could contribute to the outcomes. Adequately powered studies are needed to unravel the underlying mechanisms of activating and relaxing MI concerning the functional equivalence theory.

Supporting information

S1 File. PETTLEP principles of the motor imagery interventions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254666.s003

(PDF)

S1 Table. Questions asked in the semi-structured interviews.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254666.s006

(PDF)

S2 Table. Effect sizes of activating and relaxing motor imagery related to all outcomes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254666.s007

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We would like to warmly thank all the participants in this study, Professor Markus Reindl for his help with the graphics, Marilena Politaki for helping with the videos and Dr Buse Ozcan Kahraman for her support during the preparation and vocalisation of Turkish scripts.

References

  1. 1. Kleim JA. Neural plasticity and neurorehabilitation: teaching the new brain old tricks. Journal of communication disorders. 2011;44(5):521–8. Epub 2011/05/24. pmid:21600589.
  2. 2. Decety J, Jeannerod M. Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: does Fitts’s law hold in motor imagery? Behavioural brain research. 1995;72(1–2):127–34. Epub 1995/12/14. pmid:8788865.
  3. 3. Filgueiras A, Quintas Conde EF, Hall CR. The neural basis of kinesthetic and visual imagery in sports: an ALE meta - analysis. Brain Imaging Behav. 2018;12(5):1513–23. Epub 2017/12/21. pmid:29260381.
  4. 4. Sharma N, Pomeroy VM, Baron JC. Motor imagery: a backdoor to the motor system after stroke? Stroke. 2006;37(7):1941–52. pmid:16741183.
  5. 5. Gerardin E, Sirigu A, Lehericy S, Poline JB, Gaymard B, Marsault C, et al. Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10(11):1093–104. pmid:11053230.
  6. 6. Kraeutner S, Gionfriddo A, Bardouille T, Boe S. Motor imagery-based brain activity parallels that of motor execution: evidence from magnetic source imaging of cortical oscillations. Brain research. 2014;1588:81–91. pmid:25251592.
  7. 7. Decety J, Jeannerod M, Prablanc C. The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioural brain research. 1989;34(1–2):35–42. pmid:2765170.
  8. 8. Holmes PS, Collins DJ. The PETTLEP approach to motor imagery: A functional equivalence model for sport psychologists. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology. 2001;13(1):60–83.
  9. 9. Nicholson V, Watts N, Chani Y, Keogh JW. Motor imagery training improves balance and mobility outcomes in older adults: a systematic review. J Physiother. 2019;65(4):200–7. Epub 2019/09/16. pmid:31521556.
  10. 10. López ND, Monge Pereira E, Centeno EJ, Miangolarra Page JC. Motor imagery as a complementary technique for functional recovery after stroke: a systematic review. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2019;26(8):576–87. Epub 2019/07/28. pmid:31347992.
  11. 11. Machado TC, Carregosa AA, Santos MS, Ribeiro N, Melo A. Efficacy of motor imagery additional to motor-based therapy in the recovery of motor function of the upper limb in post-stroke individuals: a systematic review. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2019;26(7):548–53. Epub 2019/07/03. pmid:31264520.
  12. 12. Paravlic AH, Slimani M, Tod D, Marusic U, Milanovic Z, Pisot R. Effects and Dose-Response Relationships of Motor Imagery Practice on Strength Development in Healthy Adult Populations: a Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Sports medicine (Auckland, NZ). 2018;48(5):1165–87. Epub 2018/03/16. pmid:29541965.
  13. 13. Caligiore D, Mustile M, Spalletta G, Baldassarre G. Action observation and motor imagery for rehabilitation in Parkinson’s disease: A systematic review and an integrative hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;72:210–22. Epub 2016/11/21. pmid:27865800.
  14. 14. Kahraman T, Savci S, Ozdogar AT, Gedik Z, Idiman E. Physical, cognitive and psychosocial effects of telerehabilitation-based motor imagery training in people with multiple sclerosis: A randomized controlled pilot trial. Journal of telemedicine and telecare. 2020;26(5):251–60. Epub 2019/02/13. pmid:30744491.
  15. 15. Seebacher B, Kuisma R, Glynn A, Berger T. Effects and mechanisms of differently cued and non-cued motor imagery in people with multiple sclerosis: A randomised controlled trial. Mult Scler. 2019;25(12):1593–604. Epub 2018/08/15. pmid:30106328.
  16. 16. Seebacher B, Kuisma R, Glynn A, Berger T. Exploring cued and non-cued motor imagery interventions in people with multiple sclerosis: a randomised feasibility trial and reliability study. Arch Physiother. 2018;8:6. Epub 2018/03/07. pmid:29507773; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5833073.
  17. 17. Seebacher B, Kuisma R, Glynn A, Berger T. The effect of rhythmic-cued motor imagery on walking, fatigue and quality of life in people with multiple sclerosis: A randomised controlled trial. Multiple sclerosis (Houndmills, Basingstoke, England). 2017;23(2):286–96. Epub 2016/04/09. pmid:27055804.
  18. 18. Seebacher B, Kuisma R, Glynn A, Berger T. Rhythmic cued motor imagery and walking in people with multiple sclerosis: a randomised controlled feasibility study. Pilot and feasibility studies. 2015;1:25. Epub 2015/07/11. pmid:27965804; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5154106.
  19. 19. Schieber MH. Mirror neurons: reflecting on the motor cortex and spinal cord. Curr Biol. 2013;23(4):R151–2. Epub 2013/02/23. pmid:23428324.
  20. 20. Mulder T. Motor imagery and action observation: cognitive tools for rehabilitation. Journal of neural transmission. 2007;114(10):1265–78. Epub 2007/06/21. pmid:17579805; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2797860.
  21. 21. Wang Y, Morgan WP. The effect of imagery perspectives on the psychophysiological responses to imagined exercise. Behavioural brain research. 1992;52(2):167–74. pmid:1294196.
  22. 22. Collet C, Di Rienzo F, El Hoyek N, Guillot A. Autonomic nervous system correlates in movement observation and motor imagery. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2013;7:415. pmid:23908623.
  23. 23. Julious SA. Sample size of 12 per group rule of thumb for a pilot study. Pharmaceutical statistics. 2005;4(4):287–91. https://doi.org/10.1002/pst.185.
  24. 24. Treece EW, Treece JW. Elements of research in nursing. 3rd ed. St. Louis, MO: Mosby; 1982.
  25. 25. Fusch PI, Ness LR. Are We There Yet? Data Saturation in Qualitative Research. The Qualitative Report. 2015;20(9):1408–16.
  26. 26. Guest G, Bunce A, Johnson L. How Many Interviews Are Enough?: An Experiment with Data Saturation and Variability. Field Methods. 2006;18(1):59–82.
  27. 27. Altman DG, Bland JM. Statistics notes. Treatment allocation in controlled trials: why randomise? Bmj. 1999;318(7192):1209. pmid:10221955; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1115595.
  28. 28. Schuster C, Hilfiker R, Amft O, Scheidhauer A, Andrews B, Butler J, et al. Best practice for motor imagery: a systematic literature review on motor imagery training elements in five different disciplines. BMC medicine. 2011;9:75. Epub 2011/06/21. pmid:21682867; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3141540.
  29. 29. Wondrusch C, Schuster-Amft C. A standardized motor imagery introduction program (MIIP) for neuro-rehabilitation: development and evaluation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7:477. pmid:23986676; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3749428.
  30. 30. Verney J, Schwartz C, Amiche S, Pereira B, Thivel D. Comparisons of a Multi-Frequency Bioelectrical Impedance Analysis to the Dual-Energy X-Ray Absorptiometry Scan in Healthy Young Adults Depending on their Physical Activity Level. J Hum Kinet. 2015;47:73–80. Epub 2015/11/12. pmid:26557191; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4633269.
  31. 31. Vasold KL, Parks AC, Phelan DML, Pontifex MB, Pivarnik JM. Reliability and Validity of Commercially Available Low-Cost Bioelectrical Impedance Analysis. International journal of sport nutrition and exercise metabolism. 2019;29(4):406–10. Epub 2018/12/07. pmid:30507268.
  32. 32. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113. pmid:5146491.
  33. 33. Büsch D, Hagemann N, Bender N. The dimensionality of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory: An analysis with models of the item response theory. Laterality. 2010;15(6):610–28. pmid:19662566
  34. 34. Uysal SA, EKİNCİ Y, Çoban F, Yakut Y. Edinburgh El Tercihi Anketi Türkçe güvenirliğinin araştırılması. Journal of Exercise Therapy and Rehabilitation. 2019;6(2):112–8.
  35. 35. Malouin F, Richards CL, Durand A, Doyon J. Clinical assessment of motor imagery after stroke. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 2008;22(4):330–40. pmid:18326057.
  36. 36. Guillot A, Collet C. The neurophysiological foundations of mental and motor imagery. New York: Oxford University Press; 2010.
  37. 37. Butler AJ, Cazeaux J, Fidler A, Jansen J, Lefkove N, Gregg M, et al. The Movement Imagery Questionnaire-Revised, Second Edition (MIQ-RS) Is a Reliable and Valid Tool for Evaluating Motor Imagery in Stroke Populations. Evidence-based complementary and alternative medicine: eCAM. 2012;2012:497289–. Epub 2012/02/28. pmid:22474504.
  38. 38. De Vries S, Tepper M, Feenstra W, Oosterveld H, Boonstra AM, Otten B. Motor imagery ability in stroke patients: the relationship between implicit and explicit motor imagery measures. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2013;7:790. pmid:24312044; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3832786.
  39. 39. Akkarpat I. The Effects of Imagery Intervention on Basketball Free-Throw Performance, Self-Confidence and Anxiety Across Age Groups. Hacettepe University Institute of Health Science, Sport Science and Technology Program, M.Sc. Thesis, Ankara, Turkey, 2014.
  40. 40. Jeannerod M. The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1994;17(2):187–245.
  41. 41. Papaxanthis C, Pozzo T, Skoura X, Schieppati M. Does order and timing in performance of imagined and actual movements affect the motor imagery process? The duration of walking and writing task. Behavioural brain research. 2002;134(1–2):209–15. Epub 2002/08/23. pmid:12191807.
  42. 42. Collet C, Guillot A, Lebon F, MacIntyre T, Moran A. Measuring motor imagery using psychometric, behavioral, and psychophysiological tools. Exercise and sport sciences reviews. 2011;39(2):85–92. pmid:21206282.
  43. 43. Zimney KJ, Wassinger CA, Goranson J, Kingsbury T, Kuhn T, Morgan S. The reliability of card-based and tablet-based left/right judgment measurements. Musculoskelet Sci Pract. 2018;33:105–9. Epub 2017/09/20. pmid:28923696.
  44. 44. Williams LJ, Braithwaite FA, Leake HB, McDonnell MN, Peto DK, Lorimer Moseley G, et al. Reliability and validity of a mobile tablet for assessing left/right judgements. Musculoskelet Sci Pract. 2019;40:45–52. Epub 2019/02/01. pmid:30703633.
  45. 45. Boonstra AM, de Vries SJ, Veenstra E, Tepper M, Feenstra W, Otten E. Using the Hand Laterality Judgement Task to assess motor imagery: a study of practice effects in repeated measurements. International journal of rehabilitation research Internationale Zeitschrift fur Rehabilitationsforschung Revue internationale de recherches de readaptation. 2012;35(3):278–80. Epub 2012/08/09. pmid:22872302.
  46. 46. Breckenridge JD, Ginn KA, Wallwork SB, McAuley JH. Do People With Chronic Musculoskeletal Pain Have Impaired Motor Imagery? A Meta-analytical Systematic Review of the Left/Right Judgment Task. J Pain. 2019;20(2):119–32. Epub 2018/08/12. pmid:30098404.
  47. 47. Lamnek S, Krell C. Qualitative Sozialforschung. 6th ed. Basel, CH: Beltz; 2016.
  48. 48. Helfferich C. Leitfaden- und Experteninterviews. In: Baur N, Blasius J, editors. Handbuch Methoden der empirischen Sozialforschung. Wiesbaden, D: Springer VS; 2014.
  49. 49. Vandarakis D, Salacinski AJ, Broeder CE. A comparison of COSMED metabolic systems for the determination of resting metabolic rate. Research in sports medicine. 2013;21(2):187–94. pmid:23541105.
  50. 50. Ravussin E, Burnand B, Schutz Y, Jéquier E. Twenty-four-hour energy expenditure and resting metabolic rate in obese, moderately obese, and control subjects. Am J Clin Nutr. 1982;35(3):566–73. Epub 1982/03/01. pmid:6801963.
  51. 51. Hinkle DE, Wiersma W, Jurs SG. Applied Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. 5th ed. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; 2003.
  52. 52. Osterberg L, Blaschke T. Adherence to medication. The New England journal of medicine. 2005;353(5):487–97. pmid:16079372.
  53. 53. Newcombe RG. Two-sided confidence intervals for the single proportion: comparison of seven methods. Statistics in Medicine. 1998;17(8):857–72. pmid:9595616.
  54. 54. Brown L, Cai T, DasGupta A. Interval estimation for a binomial proportion. Statistical Science. 2001;16(2):101–33.
  55. 55. Li C. Little’s test of missing completely at random. The Stata Journal. 2013;13(4):795–809.
  56. 56. Little RJA. A Test of Missing Completely at Random for Multivariate Data with Missing Values. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1988;83(404):1198–202.
  57. 57. Tomczak M, Tomczak E. The need to report effect size estimates revisited. An overview of some recommended measures of effect size. Trends in Sport Sciences. 2014;21(1):19–25.
  58. 58. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed ed. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1988.
  59. 59. Schreier M. Qualitative Content Analysis in Practice. London, UK: SAGE Publications; 2012.
  60. 60. Bryman A. Integrating quantitative and qualitative research: how is it done? Qualitative Research. 2006;6(1):97–113.
  61. 61. Cypress BS. Rigor or Reliability and Validity in Qualitative Research: Perspectives, Strategies, Reconceptualization, and Recommendations. Dimensions of critical care nursing: DCCN. 2017;36(4):253–63. pmid:28570380.
  62. 62. Noble H, Smith J. Issues of validity and reliability in qualitative research. Evidence-based nursing. 2015;18(2):34–5. pmid:25653237.
  63. 63. Berger R. Now I see it, now I don’t: researcher’s position and reflexivity in qualitative research. Qualitative Research. 2013;15(2):219–34.
  64. 64. Personnier P, Kubicki A, Laroche D, Papaxanthis C. Temporal features of imagined locomotion in normal aging. Neuroscience letters. 2010;476(3):146–9. Epub 2010/04/20. pmid:20399251.
  65. 65. Mulder T, Zijlstra S, Zijlstra W, Hochstenbach J. The role of motor imagery in learning a totally novel movement. Exp Brain Res. 2004;154(2):211–7. Epub 2003/09/26. pmid:14508635.
  66. 66. Thaut MH. Rhythm, music and the brain. Scientific foundations and clinical applications. Leman M, editor. Oxon: Routledge; 2005.
  67. 67. Di Rienzo F, Collet C, Hoyek N, Guillot A. Selective effect of physical fatigue on motor imagery accuracy. PloS one. 2012;7(10):e47207. pmid:23082148; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3474822.
  68. 68. Demougeot L, Papaxanthis C. Muscle fatigue affects mental simulation of action. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2011;31(29):10712–20. pmid:21775614.
  69. 69. Schuster C, Glassel A, Scheidhauer A, Ettlin T, Butler J. Motor imagery experiences and use: asking patients after stroke where, when, what, why, and how they use imagery: a qualitative investigation. Stroke research and treatment. 2012;2012:503190. pmid:22593834; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3347754.
  70. 70. Paris-Alemany A, La Touche R, Gadea-Mateos L, Cuenca-Martínez F, Suso-Martí L. Familiarity and complexity of a movement influences motor imagery in dancers: A cross-sectional study. Scandinavian journal of medicine & science in sports. 2019;29(6):897–906. Epub 2019/02/05. pmid:30714228.
  71. 71. Principles of neural science. 4th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill; 2000. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.287.5451.273 pmid:10634775
  72. 72. Olsson CJ, Nyberg L. Motor imagery: if you can’t do it, you won’t think it. Scandinavian journal of medicine & science in sports. 2010;20(5):711–5. Epub 2010/03/27. pmid:20338003.
  73. 73. Olsson CJ, Jonsson B, Larsson A, Nyberg L. Motor representations and practice affect brain systems underlying imagery: an fMRI study of internal imagery in novices and active high jumpers. The open neuroimaging journal. 2008;2:5–13. pmid:19018312; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2577943.
  74. 74. Lebon F, Collet C, Guillot A. Benefits of motor imagery training on muscle strength. Journal of strength and conditioning research / National Strength & Conditioning Association. 2010;24(6):1680–7. Epub 2010/05/29. pmid:20508474.
  75. 75. Zaccaro A, Piarulli A, Laurino M, Garbella E, Menicucci D, Neri B, et al. How Breath-Control Can Change Your Life: A Systematic Review on Psycho-Physiological Correlates of Slow Breathing. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2018;12:353. Epub 2018/09/25. pmid:30245619; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6137615.
  76. 76. Abreu RMd, Rehder-Santos P, Simões RP, Catai AM. Can high-intensity interval training change cardiac autonomic control? A systematic review. Brazilian journal of physical therapy. 2019;23(4):279–89. Epub 2018/10/02. pmid:30293954.
  77. 77. Cuenca-Martínez F, Suso-Martí L, Grande-Alonso M, Paris-Alemany A, La Touche R. Combining motor imagery with action observation training does not lead to a greater autonomic nervous system response than motor imagery alone during simple and functional movements: a randomized controlled trial. PeerJ. 2018;6:e5142–e. pmid:30002975.
  78. 78. LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual review of neuroscience. 2000;23:155–84. Epub 2000/06/09. pmid:10845062.
  79. 79. Bradley MM. Natural selective attention: orienting and emotion. Psychophysiology. 2009;46(1):1–11. Epub 2008/09/10. pmid:18778317; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3645482.
  80. 80. Oishi K, Maeshima T. Autonomic nervous system activities during motor imagery in elite athletes. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2004;21(3):170–9. Epub 2004/09/18. pmid:15375347.
  81. 81. Reimers AK, Knapp G, Reimers CD. Effects of Exercise on the Resting Heart Rate: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Interventional Studies. J Clin Med. 2018;7(12). Epub 2018/12/06. pmid:30513777; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6306777.