Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Sexual inhibition and sexual excitation in a sample of Polish women

  • Krzysztof Nowosielski ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Krzysztof Nowosielski, Jacek Kurpisz, Robert Kowalczyk

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    gabiner@drnowosielski.pl, knowosielski@sum.edu.pl

    Affiliations Faculty of Medical Sciences, Department of Gynecology and Obstetrics, Medical University of Silesia, Katowice, Poland, Institute of Medical Sciences, University of Opole, Opole, Poland

  • Jacek Kurpisz ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Krzysztof Nowosielski, Jacek Kurpisz, Robert Kowalczyk

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department and Clinic of Psychiatry, Pomeranian Medical University, Szczecin, Poland

  • Robert Kowalczyk

    Contributed equally to this work with: Krzysztof Nowosielski, Jacek Kurpisz, Robert Kowalczyk

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Sexology, Andrzej Frycz Modrzewski Krakow University, Cracow, Poland

Abstract

The contemporary concept of sexual counseling for women with sexual problems, distress, and female sexual dysfunction (FSD) includes tailored medical and/or psychological intervention. The dual control model and the Sexual Excitation/Sexual Inhibition Inventory for Women (SESII-W) are helpful for identifying risk factors and tailoring therapy for FSD. The current study aimed to (1) validate the Polish translation of the SESII-W in a sample of Polish women, and (2) verify the usefulness of the SESII-W in clinical practice. Five hundred nine white women age 18 to 55 years old (M ± SD age = 39.7 ± 11.3 years) were included in this cross-sectional study. Linguistic validation of the Polish translation of the SESII-W was first performed. A battery of tests was then used to evaluate reliability, convergent and discriminant validity, measurement invariances, and correlations between the SESII-W and other measures. Given that the original version of the SESII-W had unsatisfactory model fit, exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses were subsequently performed. Results showed a new final model that included 26 items with seven lower- and two higher-order factors and explained 58.9% of the variance in the data, with CFI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.05 and χ2 = 693.39, p < 0.001. Cronbach’s α was 0.77 for Sexual Excitation (SE) and 0.88 for Sexual Inhibition (SI) scales. A moderate negative association between SI and the presence of FSD according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition (DSM-5) criteria was noted. SE was positively associated with engaging in risky sexual behaviors, Extraversion and Openness to Experiences traits, and was negatively correlated with relationship quality. Finally, age was negatively correlated with all domains of the SESII-W except Arousal Contingency. SE and SI were both lower in older women as compared to younger once. These results demonstrate that the Polish version of SESII-W shows good psychometric properties. A higher propensity for SI was associated with the presence of sexual problems, distress, and FSD, whereas a higher propensity for SE was associated with greater engagement in risky sexual behaviors and personality type. However, future studies on larger and more diverse populations are required to confirm the replicability of the factor structure of the scale.

Introduction

The reason to engage or not in variety of sexual behaviors is regulated on different levels. One of the mechanisms that regulate sexual response is based on dual control model (DCM) theory. The basis of the DCM theory was presented by Jansen and Bancroft in 1999, who postulated the existence of two systems–inhibitory and excitatory [1]. At the trait level, the inhibitory and excitatory sexual systems are independent, but at the state level, they mutually moderate behavior. Both systems play a role in regulating sexual desire, wherein a high propensity for sexual inhibition may serve as a major factor in the etiology of sexual dysfunctions and a high propensity for sexual excitation may lead to engaging in risky sexual behaviors (RSB) [13].

The inhibition and excitation sexual systems should be considered in the broader context of Gray’s theory of behavioral inhibition and excitation systems that relate to dopaminergic and serotoninergic pathways in the brain, particularly in the limbic system [4]. Previous studies suggest that sexual inhibition and excitation is, at least in part, independent of the general behavioral control system in the brain. Furthermore, personality traits described by Eysenck, including extraversion (i.e., positive emotionality), neuroticism (i.e., negative emotionality), and psychoticism (i.e., constraint) alongside the dimension of anxiety and impulsivity, might contribute to the activation/inhibition system, also on the level of sexual control. Sexual excitation/inhibition might also be seen in the broader concept of risk avoidance/risk taking, when activity of inhibition system is high and low, respectively. DCM also assumes that the individual propensity for sexual inhibition/excitation results in either engaging or restricting from uncommitted and multiple sexual relationships, sexual risk taking, and different patterns in response to sexual cues (i.e., disgust vs. enjoyment and entertainment) [3,5].

To measure the aforementioned propensities, a sexual excitation/inhibition scales were developed for both men and women: the Sexual Inhibition (SI) and Sexual Excitation (SE) Scale–SIS/SES for men and women [5,6], the Sexual Excitation/Sexual Inhibition Inventory for Women–SESII-W [7], and the Sexual Excitation/Sexual Inhibition Inventory for Women and Men–SESII-W/M [8]. Short forms of the SESII-W have been recently developed [9].

The present study focused on the SESII-W scale, given that it is one of the most widely used scales to measure sexual inhibition/excitation. We also selected the SESII-W because it was specifically designed for women and enables more in-depth analysis of different dimensions of female sexual function. The SESII-W is a self-reported questionnaire that measures the propensity for sexual excitation and sexual inhibition. The original scale [7] consists of 36 question with 4 possible answers that range from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The scale has 8 subscales or domains that correspond to the following eight lower-order factors: (1) Arousability, (2) Partner Characteristics, (3) Sexual Power Dynamics, (4) Smell, (5) Setting (Unusual or Unconcealed), (6) Concerns about Sexual Function, (7) Arousal Contingency, and (8) Relationship Importance. Subscales 1–5 constitute the Sexual Excitation (SE) higher-order factor whereas subscales 6–8 constitute the Sexual Inhibition (SI) factor. The scale has been shown to have good internal consistency (except for Concerns about Sexual Function and SI; Cronbach’s α = 0.63 and 0.55, respectively), with the highest Cronbach’s α observed for Arousability (α = 0.80). Further, test-retest reliability of the original scale ranges from 0.51 (Arousal Contingency) to 0.82 (Sexual Inhibition).

Validation studies for the German [10], Dutch [11], and Spanish [12] translations of the SESII-W have been recently published. However, these studies noted some differences in the measure as compared to the original validation study.

In particular, the original structure of the SESI-WW questionnaire was retained only in the German validation study [10], with 36 items grouped in eight lower-order factors and two higher-order factors. However, in that study, some items had low loading onto factors and some cross-loadings were observed [10]. Further, the model presented in the German validation study had unsatisfactory model fit, CFI = 0.85. Despite these limitations, the German SESII-W scale demonstrated adequate internal consistency, except for Partner Characteristics and Sexual Power Dynamics subscales (Cronbach’s α = 0.58 and 0.46, respectively). All factors demonstrated good to excellent test-retest reliability (Pearson’s r between 0.66 and 0.83).

Both the Dutch [11] and Spanish translations of the SESII-W [13] modified the original structure of the scale. The Dutch version included 35 items grouped in eight subscales and two higher-order factors, as in the original study. The Spanish version included 33 items with two higher-order factors; however, the structure of this scale differed from the original scale (i.e., Setting was moved from Sexual Excitation and included in Sexual Inhibition). The Dutch model demonstrated unsatisfactory fit indices (CFI = 0.87). While the Spanish model demonstrated satisfactory fit (RMSEA = 0.04, CFI = 0.92, TLI = 0.91), some cross-loadings between subscales were observed. For example, Sexual Power Dynamics and Concerns about Sexual Function loaded onto both SE and SI factors. Based on these three prior validation studies, we attempted to be stricter while analyzing the data to allow for items to load onto different lower order factors than in the original scale. Our goal was to obtain the highest model fit while minimizing cross-loadings.

Further validation studies on DCM are necessary, given the growing number of studies showing its utility in clinical practice. For example, results of Clifton et al. suggest that SE (measured by SESII-W) affects both genital and subjective arousal (Partner Characteristics and Settings positively, Sexual Power Dynamics–negatively), whereas SI affects only the subjective, more conscious components of arousal [14]. A study by Velten et al. confirmed that Setting positively predicts subjective arousal, whereas Concerns about Sexual Function negatively predicts objective arousal [15]. Unpublished data by Bradford show that the Arousal Contingency domain is positively correlated with trait anxiety, as measured by the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (r = 0.38) [16]. Similar results were obtained in a study by Carpenter et al., who used the SIS/SES scale for women and found that state anxiety (as measured by the STAI) was correlated with SI but not with other domains [6]. Based on the confirmed bidirectional relationship between depression and/or the presence of depressive symptoms and sexual function [17], Velten et al. showed that Arousability, Sexual Power Dynamics, Setting, Concern about Sexual Function, Arousal Contingency and depression predict sexual functioning [18]. Although the effects of depression on the inhibitory/excitatory system are not well established, lower levels of serotoninergic associated with depression and higher levels of catecholamine associated with anxiety may increase overall inhibitory tone while having only minor effects on excitatory tone [16]. A few recent studies [1828] showed a correlation between sexual functioning and SE/SI, such that women with sexual problems, sexual distress, or other sexual concerns had lower SE scores (primarily in Arousability) and higher SI scores as compared to women without sexual concerns. However, the aforementioned studies used screening questionnaires, such as the Female Sexual Function Index (FSFI) and Female Sexual Distress Scale (FSDS), to assess sexual problems. To our knowledge, only one study conducted in the Netherlands [11] used DSM-IV criteria to assess sexual dysfunction. Additionally, existing validation studies on the SESII-W [1012,26] or other SE/SI measurements [18,22,24] showed some discrepancies in scale structure and cultural and/or gender differences. Additionally, a study by Dang et al. [29] demonstrated ethnic differences in SE between white and Chinese undergraduate students in universities in Canada.

Given the temporal stability of SE/SI [30] and the utility of DCM in clinical practice [11,14,15], further studies in different cultural backgrounds and with current diagnostic criteria for sexual dysfunction are needed.

The present study aimed to fulfill that need for several reasons.

Firstly, it focuses on a cultural adaptation of SESII-W in a Central/Eastern European society. The new version of the SESII-W is expected to put the concept of DCM in the perspective of Eastern European countries, which will allow to examine the DCM from a new, less sexually liberal, perspective. The new Eastern European perceptive can be attributed to two main forces that shaped the concept of sexuality in Poland and other post-soviet countries: socialism and the Catholic Church [3133]. Given that conservative Catholicism was seen as the answer to socialistic tendencies, Catholicism shaped the contemporary restrictive model of sexual attitudes and behaviors in Polish society. Although many changes have been introduced, some of those restrictive tendencies are still at play and can alter the perception of excitatory cues and inhibitory factors. Given that religion may affect sexual behaviors and attitudes [34], the new version of the SESII-W may better capture individual differences in the propensity for excitation/inhibition in Central and Eastern European countries because it incorporates historical contexts and potential religious influences while retaining the general concept of the DCM. Therefore, the present validation study may prompt future studies in Central and Eastern Europe that may improve understanding of the DCM in a way that is consistent with historical background and conservative religious influences.

Secondly, it uses Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition (DSM-5) criteria for sexual dysfunction to verify the correlation between DCM and female sexual dysfunction (FSD).

And finally, it characterizes the propensity for SE and SI in a large sample of women between 18 and 55 years of age.

For all that reasons the results of this study will increase the knowledge on DCM and help to improve the quality of care by introducing personalized sexual medicine [35] for couples with sexual problems

Based on previous research on the DCM and SESII-W [7,1012,36], we hypothesize that:

  1. The Polish model of the SESII-W will differ from the original model, as observed in prior studies. We predict that the Polish version of the SESII-W will require some modification due to sociocultural differences.
  2. There will be some degree of overlap between the SESII-W and the following measures: general propensity towards SE/SI, personality, sociosexual orientation, sexual adventurism, and sexual risk taking.
  3. Some questions in the Sexual Power Dynamics and Setting subscales concern sexual cues that may be perceived as socially inappropriate, e.g., “Having sex in a different setting than usual is a real turn on for me” or “It turns me on if my partner ‘talks dirty’ to me during sex”. We expect that social norms might influence responses to those questions, given the tendency to respond in socially desirable manner.
  4. A higher level of SI will be associated with the presence of sexual distress, sexual problems, sexual dysfunction, depression, and anxiety
  5. A higher level of SE will be associated with a greater frequency of masturbation, higher number of lifetime sexual partners, a tendency to engage in RSB, and a better relationship quality and satisfaction.
  6. SE will be negatively associated with age, relationship status, religious commitment, and religiosity, whereas positively with sexual activity. The directions are expected to be opposite for SE.
  7. The SESII-W will be measurement invariant across different groups (e.g., partnered vs. singles; higher vs. lower education; younger vs. older, online version vs. paper-pencil version), as seen in previous studies [36].

Methods

Participants

Nine hundred and seventy-nine white women (18–55 years) were eligible for this cross-sectional population-based study. The inclusion criteria were (1) age between 18 and 55 years and (2) agreement to participate in the study. The respondents were recruited between January 2017 and December 2018 using online social media advertisements (Facebook, local portals) or in outpatient gynecology clinic in Katowice, Poland, during routine yearly check-up visits. The questionnaire was prepared as an online application (posted on surveymonkey.com), as well as a printed paper-pencil version. Both forms (online, traditional printed) can be used in the future, enabling the clinicians to choose the most suitable form. On average, participants completed the study questionnaires in 40 minutes. The paper-pencil form of the questionnaires we as administered to participants during regular clinic visits, completed at home, and then returned (in person or by mail) to the office.

Out of the 979 eligible women, 674 were invited to take part in the online survey and 305 were handed the paper-pencil version of the survey. All participants read an informed consent statement and agreed to participate either verbally (paper-pencil version) or by clicking “YES” in the online survey. Forty-five women refused to participate in the study (11 from the online sample and 34 from the paper-pencil one). Another 374 and 51 respondents returned incomplete online and paper-pencil surveys, respectively. The final sample consisted of 509 participants with complete data (220 paper-pencil surveys and 289 online surveys). Out of the final 509 participants, 42 women from each group participated in the test-retest study which involved completing the questionnaire again after four weeks (see Fig 1). To identify participants for the test-retest procedure, an identification code was distributed. The response rate for the study was 54.5%. The minimum required sample size to perform Confirmatory Factorial Analysis (CFA) was found to be 498, calculated using a Monte Carlo approach [37].

The study protocol was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Silesian Chamber of Physicians and Dentists in Katowice, Poland (ŚIL/KB/756p/15).

Linguistic validation

The linguistic validation was performed according to 5 step test adaptation procedure recommended by Beaton et al. [38].

In the first step, we compared the translations (i.e., discrepancy resolution), which revealed some discrepancies in items 10, 13, 23, and 30. In item 10, “make me” was translated to “cause me to be”; in item 13, the word “really” was considered to be redundant; in item 23, “can be a turn on” was modified to “can turn me on”. In item 30, the word “certain” was also considered redundant. In the next steps, no major inconsistencies were noted, and the first version of SESII-W-PL (Polish version of the scale) was created. Finally, a field test was performed in the group of 25 female students from different departments in the University of Silesia. Difficulties in understanding or interpreting the scale items were recorded, which revealed some deviations in items 11 and 16. Based on these discrepancies, the experts changed “someone” to “partner” in item 11, and “relationship potential” to “material for partner” in item 16. The final version of the SESII-W-PL (Polish version of the scale) was then created.

Materials

The survey included questions regarding socioeconomic status, medical history, and sexual behaviors. Participants were also asked to report on their weight and height to calculate body mass index (BMI).

The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) was used to measure symptoms of depression and anxiety. HADS scores < 10 indicate a lack of depressive symptoms and/or anxiety symptoms whereas scores ≥ 11 points indicate a high risk of clinical depression and/or high levels of anxiety [39]. The HADS was previously validated in Poland and shown a good reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.88) [39].

DMS-5 criteria [40] were used to test for FSD. For the purpose of this study, we used five questions regarding sexual function in the context of DSM-5: (1) “During the last 6 months, how often did you feel like having sex, had sexual fantasies, felt sexually excited and/or felt lubricated in response to sexual stimuli (touch, voice, when seeing a partner, smell, erotic materials)?”; (2) “During the last 6 months, how often during sexual activities did you experience orgasm or felt sexually fulfilled, that was satisfying enough in the context of time of occurrence, duration, or intensity?”; (3) “During the last 6 months, how often have you been satisfied with your sexual life (emotionally and physically)?”; (4) “During the last 6 months, how often have you experienced problems with penetration (inserting penis to your vagina), anxiety, fear, or unpleasant increased pelvic muscle tension when thinking of, during, or after sexual intercourse?”; (5) “If you were happy with your sexual activity (questions 1–3) during less than 25% of sexual contacts, or if you felt pain or/and anxiety when thinking of, during, or after sex recurrently and permanently in sexual contacts, did it cause distress?” For questions 1–4, the possible answers were as follows: (a) No sexual activity, (b) During less than 25% of sexual contacts, (c) During 25%-50% of sexual contacts, (d) During half of sexual contacts, (e) During 50–75% of sexual contacts, (f) During more that 75% of sexual contacts. For question 5, participants responded either “Yes” or “No”. These responses were used for FSD diagnosis according to DSM-5 criteria in both version of the survey. Female Sexual Interest/Arousal Disorder (FSIAD) was diagnosed if sexual interest and/or arousal was present in less than 25% of sexual activities what was associated with sexual distress. Female Orgasm Disorder (FOD) was diagnosed if orgasm and/or satisfaction was experienced in less than 25% of sexual encounters causing distress. Genito-Pelvic Pain/Penetration Disorder (GPPPD) was diagnosed if pain and/or anxiety was present recurrently and permanently during sexual contact what was associated with sexual distress.

The Polish versions of the Female Sexual Function Index (FSFI) and the Female Sexual Distress Scale-Revised (FSDS-R) were used to assess sexual functioning and the presence of sexual problems (FSFS) and sexual distress (FSDS-R), respectively. Higher FSFI scores reflect better sexual functioning whereas higher FSDS scores reflect a higher level of sexual distress. Additionally, women with FSFI scores ≤ 27.55 and FSDS-R ≥ 13 were classified as being “at risk” of FSD, i.e., distressing sexual concerns. This threshold was based on published recommendations for cut-offs for the population of Polish women [41,42]. Both scales have been validated in Poland and have been shown to have excellent reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.96 and 0.86 for FSFI-PL and FSDS-R, respectively) [41,42].

Respondents were asked to report on their number of lifetime sexual partners, frequency of masturbation (“How often did you masturbate during last the 4 weeks?”), number of any sexual activities in the last 4 weeks (defined as any of the following: vaginal, anal and oral contacts, caressing/cuddling, sexual foreplay, and masturbation), satisfaction from a partner as a lover (5-point Likert scale ranging from 1–5 wherein 1 = very dissatisfied and 5 = very satisfied), relationship status (partnered vs. single), relationship satisfaction (5-point Likert scale), and psychosexual orientation. The latter was measured using the following question: “How would you describe your sexual orientation defined as sexual attraction, emotions, fantasies, behavior towards specified gender, or/and self-labelling in the sense of identity based on those feelings and related behaviors?”. Possible answers were as follows: heterosexual, homosexual, bisexual, asexual, homoerotic, cannot define [43]. Respondents were also asked if they are engaging in RSB, defined as one or more of the following: “sexual contacts with more than one sexual partner at the same time, engaging in sexual activity with a casual person (one-night stand), frequent change of sexual partners, having intercourse with a person living with HIV, inconsistent use of condoms in oral, anal, and vaginal contacts (except for steady partner), prostitution or using the services of an escort agency, sexual contacts under influence of psychoactive substances other than alcohol and marijuana (‘chemsex’), or drug injection with shared needles leading to sex, within the last 6 months” [44].

The general activation/inhibition properties of the individuals were assessed by the Behavioral Inhibition/Behavioral Activation Scale (BIS/BAS) [45]. The BAS scale is comprised of three subscales: reward responsiveness (BAS-RR), drive (BAS-D), and fun seeking (BAS-FS). The BIS/BAS is a widely used measure of general inhibition and activation tendencies, as functional systems in the brain, with higher scores in each domain representing higher tendencies. In the present study, we used a validated Polish version of the BIS/BAS, that has been shown to have satisfactory reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.69–0.74) [45].

We used the 11-item Polish version of the Sexual Sensation Seeking Scale (SSSS) to assess sexual adventurism and sexual risk taking. Possible answers on the SSSS range from 1 (not at all like me) to 4 (very much like me), and the scale has been shown to have acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.83). Higher scores on the SSSS correspond with a greater tendency to sexual risk taking [46].

The 9-item Sociosexual Orientation Inventory-Revised (SOI-R) was used to measure individual restrictiveness towards a relationship. A lower level of restrictiveness is associated with a willingness to engage in uncommitted and multiple sexual relationships, whereas a higher level is associated with a predisposition towards long-term relationships [47]. The Polish version of the SOI-R has three subscales–(1) Attitude (SOI-AT), (2) Behavior (SOI-B), and (3) Desire (SOI-D). The scale has been shown to have good internal consistency with Cronbach’s α = 0.89 [47]. Lower SOI-R scores indicate higher restrictiveness.

The Polish version of the 5-item Sexual Opinion Survey–Short Form (SOS-SF) was used to measure affective and evaluative responses to sexual stimuli, i.e., level of erotophobia/erotophilia. The Polish version of the SOS-SF has been shown to have a good reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.82). Higher SOS-SF scores indicate a greater erotophobic tendencies [46].

The 29-item Social Desirability Questionnaire (SDQ) was used to assess the tendency to respond in a socially desirable manner. The SDQ was based on The Marlowe-Crown Social Desirability Scale (MCSD), and validated in a Polish population with Cronbach’s α = 0.81 [48]. Higher SDQ score reflects greater conformity to social rules and conventions.

Personality traits were measured with the Ten-Item-Personality-Inventory (TIPI). This 10-item questionnaire measures how specified personal traits apply to an individual using a 7-point scale that ranges from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”. The following five traits are measured on the TIPI: Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness. A Polish version of the TIPI was used, which, similar to the original scale, was shown to have an unsatisfactory Cronbach’s α = 0.41–0.67 (the lowest for Agreeableness and Openness to Experience and the highest for Conscientiousness) [49].

The Well-Matched Relationship Questionnaire (WMRQ) was used to evaluate relationship satisfaction. The WMRQ consists of 32 questions grouped into four relationship dimensions: intimacy, disappointment, personal fulfilment, and similarity. Participants are asked to assess their relationship using a 5-point Likert scale, and higher WMRQ scores indicate a greater level of each dimension. The WMRQ has been validated and standardized in a Polish population and has been shown to have good psychometric properties (Cronbach’s α = 0.81) [50].

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using Statistica 12.0 Pl computer software (StatSoft. Cracow, Poland), IBM SPSS 20 computer software with AMOS version 25 (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Armonk, NY: IBM Corp; 2012), R version 4.0.2 statistics (R project accessed at: https://www.r-project.org/) with Iavaan and Semtools package (accessed at https://lavaan.ugent.be). Missing values was assessed for all variables (less than 5%). Cases with missing data were deleted when performing confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and exploratory factor analysis (EFA). Skewness and kurtosis were assessed to check for univariate and multivariate distribution normality. Values larger than 3 for skewness or larger than 10 for kurtosis were considered to indicate nonnormality. Mardia’s coefficient, a measure of multivariate kurtosis and skewness, was used for multivariate distributions. Mardia’s coefficients of ≤ 5 were considered to be indicative of normality [37]. P-values of < 0.05 were considered to be statistically significant.

Factor analysis

In the first step, CFA was performed using AMOS on the entire sample of women (N = 509) to analyze the fit indices in the existing model [7]. The following values of indices were used to indicate acceptable fit: the probability of a close fit (p Close Fit) = > 0.05; Comparative Fit Index (CFI) ≥ 0.95, and root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) ≤ 0.06 [37]. Modification indices were inspected to identify non-fitting items. The original models did not reach satisfactory fit indices (CFI = 0.77, RMSEA = 0.065). Given the lack of multivariate normality in the data, we used EFA to investigate the structure of the model. EFA was performed using the principal axis with Promax rotation preceded by Monte-Carlo parallel analysis, as recommended by Swami et al. [37]. We also used CFA to examine model fit, using maximum likelihood method with bootstrapping. To apply EFA and CFA, we first randomly divided the study sample into two roughly equal groups using SPPS software: group 1 (N = 255) and group 2 (N = 254). EFA was performed on group 1, and CFA of the new model was performed on group 2 [37,51]. Items with factor loadings ≥ 0.40 were retained in the model [12,37,52]. Factorability was measured using inter-item correlations (≥ 0.50), the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy (≥ 0.80), and Bartlett’s test of sphericity (p > 0.05). An eight-factor model was developed. As in original model, Monte Carlo analysis was performed on the extracted lower-order factors [7] and the principal axis analysis was again executed. Similarly, factors with loadings ≥ 0.40 were retained in the model. The final model consisted of eight factors and two higher order factors. CFA was performed on the final model with modification indices analysis to identify the best model fit.

Measurement invariances

Comparisons among models were performed using AMOS plugins [53], to determine whether the SISII-W-PL can be used in different populations. Similar to an analysis performed by Velten et al. [36], we divided the study sample into subgroups according to the following variables: (1) age (≤ 45 years, younger; > 45 years, older); (2) education level (higher, university; lower, primary/secondary); (3) relationship status (partnered, singles); and (4) type of questionnaire (online; paper-pencil). Configural data and metrics were tested [10]. If the differences were significant, z-scores and p-values of the standardized subscale scores were reported, and effect size was estimated using Cohen’s d. Small effects were defined as d > 0.20; medium, > 0.50; and large, > 0.80 [10].

Reliability

Intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) was used to assess reliability [37] and the Cronbach’s α was used to determine internal consistency [37]. ICC values of > 0.40 reflect poor to fair agreement; 0.41–0.60, moderate agreement; 0.61–0.80, good agreement; and > 0.80, excellent agreement between two measurements [37]. Cronbach’s α values of 0.5–0.75 indicate moderate reliability; 0.76–0.9, good reliability; and > 0.90, excellent reliability [54]. McDonald’s omega was also calculated, with values ≥ 0.70 indicative of satisfactory reliability [55].

Construct validity

As in the original validation study [7], convergent and discriminant validity of the SESII-W-PL was assessed using correlations with other variables that measure proximal and distal constructs. Correlations were performed using Pearson’s r, wherein r values of ≥ 0.10 indicate a weak effect size; r ≥ 0.30, moderate; and r ≥ 0.50, large effect size [56]. For dichotomous variables, the point-biserial correlation coefficient (r pb) was calculated [57]. One-way ANOVA controlling for age with Bonferroni correction (if possible) was used to assess the relationship between SESII-W-PL and select socioeconomical and sexual behaviors variables. Partial eta square (ƞ2) values of ≥ 0.01 were considered to be small effect sizes; ƞ2 ≥. 006, medium effects; and ƞ2 ≥ 0.14, large effects [58].

Results

General characteristics

The mean age of the studied women was 39.7 ± 11.3 years (range = 18–55). The majority of women were heterosexual (87.4%; n = 445), and the remaining 12.6% described themselves as homosexual (3.3%; n = 17), bisexual (8.3%; n = 4 2), asexual (0.40%; n = 2), or homoerotic (0.60%; n = 3). The majority of respondents identified as being Catholic (74.3%; n = 378), although only 30.3% (n = 154) reported regular church attendance. Most respondents (80.7%; n = 411) lived in cities and had a secondary education (44.6%; n = 227). Sixty-eight percent of women in the sample (n = 346) reported at least one pregnancy. Based on HADS scores, symptoms of depression and anxiety based were reported in 5.7% (n = 29) and 13.4% (n = 68) of respondents, respectively.

The analysis of sexual behavior revealed that 12.1% (n = 60) of women were single, 82.9% (n = 422) reported having a sexual partner, and 91.2% were sexually active in the last 4 weeks. Surprisingly, 14.9% (n = 76) of women reported recently engaging in RSB. Based on DSM-5 criteria, distress was present in 24.4% (n = 124) of women, and FSD was reported in 14.7% (n = 75) of women. In contrast, based on the FSFI, sexual problems were present in 32.2% of women, sexual distress (FSDS-R) in 39.7%, and distressing sexual concerns (FSFI and FSDS-R) in 24% of women. There were no differences in the examined variables between groups 1 and 2 with the exception of SOI-Desire, which was higher in group 1 as compared to group 2 (2.42 vs. 2.13, respectively, p = 0.04; see Table 1). There were also no differences between the groups that completed the survey online vs. paper-pencils with the exception of BMI, which was higher in the online group as compared to the paper-pencil group (23.6 vs. 22.3, respectively, p = 0.001; see Table 1).

Factor analysis

The CFA conducted on a total sample of 509 women on the original model showed poor model fit (Table 2). As reported in the Method section, EFA was performed on group 1. Monte Carlo analysis showed an eight lower-factor solution, which accounted for 56.2% of the variance in the data. The KMO showed sampling adequacy for further analysis (KMO = 0.81, χ2 = 3161.1; Bartlett’s test of sphericity, df = 630, p < 0.001). However, some items (items 8, 30, 32) were excluded from the model based on low loadings. In addition, items 13 and 29 presented loading overlap (similar loadings for Settings and Arousability, and for Concern about Sexual function and Arousal Contingency, respectively), and were subsequently excluded from the model. Of note, some items loaded onto a different subscale than in the original scale. In particular, items 24–26 moved from Arousability to Partner Characteristics, item 10 moved from Partner Characteristics to Arousability, item 28 moved from Sexual Power Dynamics to Arousal Contingency, item 3 was moved from Setting to Arousability, and items 4 and 7 did not have negative loadings.

thumbnail
Table 2. Model fit indices for different model and different groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249560.t002

In the next step, EFA was executed on the eight-factor model to extract higher-order factors, following the original validation study [7]. Two higher-order factors–sexual excitation and sexual inhibition–were subsequently extracted. However, the Setting subscale did not reach adequate loading (≥ 0.40) and was subsequently removed from the model. The model had sampling adequacy for further analysis (KMO = 0.72, χ2 = 635.7; Bartlett’s test of sphericity, df = 28, p < 0.001) and explained 58.95% of the variance in the data. The CFA was executed then on this model. During the analysis, the following changes were made to reach sufficient model fit: (1) item 1 from the Relationship Importance subscale had low loading and was removed; (2) items 27 and 28 from the Sexual Power Dynamics subscale had low loading (< 0.40) and were thus removed. Modification indices were also checked and corrected. The final model consisted of 26 item with 7 domains and 2 higher-order factors. Please see Table 3 and Fig 2 for details, and Table 2 for fit indices. As expected, all subscales were correlated on the low or moderate level (Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 3. Factor loadings and wording of the original version of the SESIIW and the Polish translation in group 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249560.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Interrelation between SESII-W-PL subscales (Pearson’s r correlations).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249560.t004

Multigroup comparison

The multigroup comparison (Table 2) showed structural invariance of the model across survey versions (i.e., paper-pencil vs. online; p = 0.29), relationship groups (single vs. partnered, p = 0.14), and age groups (p = 0.06). The model was not invariant across education groups (higher vs lower, p = 0.03). As compared to respondents with lower education, those with higher education had higher scores in Arousability (p = 0.001, d = -0.28), Partner Characteristics (p = 0.001, d = -0.42), Smell (p = 0.001, d = -0.32), Sexual Power Dynamics (p = 0.04, d = -0.23), and had overall higher SE scores (p = 0.001, d = -0.39). However, the effect sizes for these educational differences were in the medium range. Furthermore, the multi-group CFA comparison revealed fit indices and a drop of model fit between models that was below a ΔCFI 0.010 and a ΔRMSEA 0.015, that are indicative for models structural invariance [36]. Thus, the obtained result proved that the model was threshold measurement invariant across paper-pencil and online versions, single and partnered women, older and younger participants, as well as individuals with and without a university degree (Table 2).

Reliability

Cronbach’s α for each subscale and for the two higher-order factors were in the moderate to good range, with α = 0.62 for Concern about Sexual Function scale and α = 0.88 for the Smell subscale (Table 5), indicating good internal consistency. However, McDonald’s omega for Concern about Sexual Function was unsatisfactory.

thumbnail
Table 5. General statistics for the lower- and higher-order factors of the SESII-W-PL.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249560.t005

Eighty-four women participated in the retest procedure. The mean age of the retest subgroup was 30.3 ± 8.24 years (range = 18–44 years). All respondents in the retest subgroup were heterosexual. The mean time between the two measurements was 4.6 ± 1.7 weeks (range = 2–8 weeks). The test-retest analysis (Table 5) showed good to excellent agreement between measurements, with statistically significant correlations.

Construct validity

To measure construct validity, Pearson r correlations were calculated between the SESII-W-PL scales and other variables (see Table 6 in S1 Table in the Supplemental Material). As expected, total BAS scores–reflecting general activation propensity–was positively correlated with SE scores. Conversely, total scores on the BIS–reflecting inhibitory tone–were positively correlated with SI scores. Similarly, and as expected, SOS-SF–a marker of erotophobic tendencies–was negatively correlated with SE and positively correlated with SI. In contrast, SOI-R–a measure of individual restrictiveness to relationship–was positively correlated with SE and negatively correlated with SI. That is, women who reported being less restrictive in engaging in uncommitted relationship (i.e., lower SOI-R scores) had higher SE scores. Similarly, and as expected, SSSS scores and engaging in RSB were positively correlated with SE (but not with SI). Finally, SE was positively correlated with Extraversion and Openness to Experiences traits, and SI was negatively correlated with Neuroticism, Openness to Experiences, and Extraversion. In particular, women showing more extraverted tendencies were more likely scored higher on SI. All correlations were weak to medium.

SE subscales were not correlated with SDQ scores, indicating that responses were not altered by conformity to social rules and conventions. Thus, these results suggest that the SESII-W-PL is not influenced by social desirability. SE was not correlated with the presence of sexual distress (FSDS-R), sexual problems (FSFI), distressing sexual concerns (FSFI + FSDS-R), nor with FSD (DSM-5 criteria). However, SI was positively correlated with these distress-related variables, as well as with Concerns about Sexual Function and Arousal Contingency. SI was negatively correlated with Arousability, such that women with higher Arousability scores were more likely to develop sexual problems, distressing sexual concerns, and sexual dysfunction as compared to women with lower Arousability scores. Additionally, and as expected, better sexual functioning (i.e., higher FSFI scores) was positively correlated with SE and negatively correlated with SI scores. All correlations were weak.

As expected, anxiety and depression symptoms (as measured by HADS scores) were positively correlated with SI. However, the presence of depressive symptoms was not correlated with any subscale nor with the SE/SI scale. In contrast, the presence of anxiety was positively correlated with higher Arousal Contingency and SI scores.

There was a weak correlation between SE and the analyzed sexual behaviors (see Table 6 in S1 Table in the Supplemental Material), wherein higher SE scores were associated with more frequent and diverse sexual activities. Interestingly, a moderate negative correlation was noted between SE and relationship quality (assessed by WMRQ), except for in the Disappointment domain of the WMRQ.

Associations between SESII-W-PL and demographic factors

Age was negatively correlated with all higher- and lower-order factors except Arousal Contingency (low to moderate effect). Residency, education level, and regular church attendance were not related to SE nor SI when controlling for age. Although there was a negative correlation between being currently in a relationship and Partner Characteristics, and a positive correlation between Relationship Importance and SI, these correlations did not reach statistical significance when controlling for age. When controlling for age, being sexually active in the last 4 weeks was positively correlated with Smell (F(1, 218) = 4.39, p = 0.04, ƞ2 = 0.02), Relationship Importance (F(1, 218) = 5.56, p = 0.02, ƞ2 = 0.02), and Sexual Excitation (F(1, 218) = 4.02, p = 0.04, ƞ2 = 0.02). These effects were medium. Similarly, being Roman Catholic was negatively correlated with Arousability (F(12, 42) = 1.69, p = 0.02, ƞ2 = 0.23) and positively with Concerns about Sexual Function (F(33, 185) = 1.75, p = 0.01, ƞ2 = 0.24). These effects were large.

Discussion

This is the first study, to our knowledge, to evaluate the psychometric properties of the SESII-W in an Eastern and Central European sample, and to demonstrate sexual concerns in the context of the DCM. These findings have important implications for clinical practice given that, for the first time, we altered the original structure of the model to obtain a satisfying model fit. In the three prior SESII-W validation studies [10,11,13], the original structure of the model was retained, some cross-loadings between SE and SI were allowed, or unsatisfactory model fit was achieved. Based on our prior experience in cultural adaptations of questionnaires [41,42,59], we performed EFA to establish a new structure. Our approach was based on the assumption that some questions might be understood differently and contribute to other dimensions (i.e., lower order factors) of the DCM, what was described in detail in the Introduction section.

The present study included 509 women between the ages of 18 and 55 years from different socioeconomical backgrounds, applied a battery of different tests and included multigroup comparison to evaluate structural invariance. Results confirmed good psychometric validity of the new model, which included 7 lower-order and 2 higher-order factors. Given that all the examined variables were only weakly or moderately correlated with SESII-W-PL higher and lower order factors, convergent and discriminant validity of the scale was confirmed. These data indicate that the SESII-W-PL adequately measures the propensity for SE/SI. More importantly, we demonstrated a new data on correlations between SESII-W-PL and its domain, which suggests that this scale may be useful in clinical practice.

The results of the present study showed that the Polish version of SESII-W differs from the original validation study. The Polish SESII-W has 26 items grouped into seven lower-order factors that comprise SE and SI scales. Although some items were allocated to different subscales as compared to the original study (for e.g., some items from the Arousability subscale were moved to the Partner Characteristics subscale and vice versa), these differences did not impact construction of higher-order factors. Of note, only the German version of the SESII-W retained both the original model structure and original number of items [10]. Indeed, the Spanish version had 34 items and a modified structure [12] and the Dutch version had 35 items and retained the original structure [11].

Ten items did not enter the Polish version of the SESII-W (i.e., items 1, 4, 7, 8, 13, 27, 28, 29, 30, and 32). In other validation studies, item 30 was either removed [12] or deemed to be problematic [10,11]. Items 28 [10] and 27 [11] were deemed to be problematic in other validation studies, but retained in the final versions of the scale. In the present validation study, items 1 (“If I think that a partner might hurt me emotionally, I put the brakes on sexually”), 13 (“I get really turned on if I think I may get caught while having sex”), and 32 (“Sometimes I am so attracted to someone, I cannot stop myself from becoming sexually aroused”) were eliminated. Here, the Setting subscale was also removed, which consisted of items 4 (“Having sex in a different setting than usual is a real turn on for me”) and 7 (“If it is possible someone might see or hear us having sex, it is more difficult for me to get aroused”). As reported in previous studies [3133,59], the place and context of sexual behaviors might be more restrictive in Eastern European countries as compared to Northern European countries. In the present study, the majority of Polish women reported that they would not be willing to engage in any sexual activity in an unusual setting, or when somebody is or might even “be around”. Rather, women preferred more traditional settings for sexual activities (e.g., a quiet place, bedroom rather than kitchen, locked bedroom doors, lights off). These patterns may, in part, explain the exclusion of items 13, 4, and 7 from the model. Furthermore, despite the shift from a more conservative model of relationships with a dominating male partner to equality in sexual relationship [60], many Polish women may feel uncomfortable with expressing sexuality or concerns about sexual encounters. This may be related to a lack of emotional comfort with a partner, which may be attributed to masculinity dominance in sexual education or, to be more precise, the relationship model [61]. These factors may explain, in part, the exclusion of items 1 and 32. Items 8 (“If I see a partner interacting well with others, I am more easily sexually aroused”) and 29 (“Sometimes I feel so ‘shy’ or self-conscious during sex that I cannot become fully aroused”) were also removed, which may also be due to the aforementioned cultural differences. A partner’s capacity for social interaction may not be perceived as sexual cues. In contrast, performance anxiety might work as an “all or nothing” phenomenon. According to Basson’s model of sexual response [62], if a woman decides to engage in sexual activities, the level of anxiety preceding the sexual act does not block her from being fully satisfied [63,64]. However, that is only one of possible explanations.

Despite the differences in the structure of the scale and the comparison of standardized scores from the four previous validation studies [7,1012], we did not observe major differences. We also did not observe major differences between the present results and result of other studies utilizing the SESII-W in both heterosexual [18,27,29,65,66] and homosexual women [26], and between online and paper-pencil surveys. Further, Cronbach’s α showed similar satisfactory values for SE, SI, Partner Characteristics, and Smell in all versions except the original validation (α for SE and SI = 0.55 and 0.70, respectively) and lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender (LBGT) women (α for SE and SI = 0.59 and 0.60, respectively). Similar to the present results, satisfactory α was not reached for Sexual Power Dynamics, Relationship Importance, and Concerns about Sexual Function subscales in prior studies [7,1012,26], and Arousability was also unsatisfactory in the German validation study [10]. However, Arousal Contingency reached satisfactory reliability in all studies except the current study. Further, our test-retest analysis demonstrated good to excellent agreement between measurements with statistically significant correlations, which is consistent with prior studies [7,1012,26].

Age was negatively correlated with all higher- and lower-order factors except Arousal Contingency. A few studies have examined the correlation between age of respondents and SE/SI. Graham et al. showed an age-related decrease in SE (but not SI) scores [7]. However, Velten et al. reported age-related decreases in both SI and SE in a German sample [15]. Interestingly, a study on a representative sample from Flanders et al. [67] used a multiple regression model to demonstrate a nonlinear association between age and SE. In that study, SE increased with age until around 25 to 40 years of age, and then steadily declined after around age 40. SI, in contrast, increased between 41 and 54 years of age and stabilized later in life [67]. A later study by the same authors extended those findings and found that SE increased with age until approximately 30 years, and subsequently decreased [68]. In contrast, SI demonstrated a U-curve shape with age, with peak SI scores reported in the 40’s [68]. The SIS/SES scale was used in both studies. The relationship between SE and age might be partially explained by age-related decline in Arousability. However, inhibitory tone may also decline with age–a hypothesis that requires further investigation.

Women with sexual problems, sexual distress, and/or distressing sexual concerns demonstrated lower scores in Arousability and higher scores in SI, Arousal Contingency, and Concerns about Sexual Function. Women with sexual dysfunction also reported higher scores in SI and Concerns about Sexual Function. In contrast, women with better sexual functioning (i.e., higher FSFI scores) reported higher scores in Arousability and SE, and reported lower scores in Concerns about Sexual Function, Arousal Contingency, and SI. Similar to prior studies, the present study demonstrates that FSFI is positively correlated with SE, and negatively correlated with SI. In particular, Arousal Contingency was negatively correlated with Desire, Arousal, Lubrication, and Satisfaction subscales [16]. A study by Sanders et al. demonstrated that Arousal Contingency and Concerns about Sexual Function were positively correlated with the presence of sexual problems in general, arousal problems, orgasm problems, and with low lifetime sexual interest, evaluated by a Likert-style questionnaire [20]. A recent study by Velten et al. examined 2,275 couples in Germany using the SESII-W/M, and found that sexual functioning (assessed by the FSFI) was positively correlated with SE and negatively correlated with SI [21]. A study by Paul and Carelheira using the SESII-W/M showed that women with higher SI scores (particularly in the Inhibitory Cognition subscale) were more likely to experience a lack of sexual interest, difficulties achieving orgasm, and painful intercourses than women with lower SI scores [22]. In a small study by Hodgeson et al. [65], 15 women were asked to complete the SESII-W, and then subjective (self-reported arousal) and objective (genital region temperature) arousal was measured while participants watched an erotic film. In that study, genital temperature was positively predicted by SE and negatively predicted by SI. However, subjective arousal was negatively predicted by SE [65]. Additionally, one of largest longitudinal studies in a sample of 2,214 German women (mean age = 30.6 years) measured sexual functioning using the FSFI at baseline and after a median time of 11 months [18]. Results of that longitudinal study demonstrated that having a steady sexual partner, Arousability, Sexual Power Dynamics, and Settings were positive predictors of present and future sexual functioning, whereas depressive symptoms, Arousal Contingency, and Concerns about Sexual Function were negative predictors of current and future sexual functioning [18]. Similar results were obtained by a study of 373 women (mean age = 34.1 years) by Quinta et al. in Spain [24,25]. Quinta et al. demonstrated that SE predicted desire, arousal, lubrication, and orgasm, as measured by the FSFI [24,25]. Only one Dutch study used DSM-IV criteria to assess FSD, and found that the presence of FSD was associated with lower total and subscale SE scores, and higher total and subscale SI scores [11]. Although we observed that the presence of FSD was only correlated with higher SI scores in the present study, this is the first study to our knowledge to apply strict DSM-5 criteria to define FSD. Given this strict criterion, we predict that level of inhibitory (but not excitatory) tone may predict FSD. However, this hypothesis must be confirmed in future studies.

Consistent with prior studies, our analysis of the construct validity of the SESII-W-PL demonstrated that higher SE/lower SI were associated with erotophilia tendencies (i.e., lower SOS-SF scores), a greater tendency for fun-seeking (i.e., higher BAS scores), and greater engagement in RSB (i.e., higher SSSS scores or self-reported RSB) [6,66,69]. Furthermore, in line with previous studies [7,10,15], women that were less restrictive in engaging in uncommitted relationship (i.e., lower SOI-R scores) had higher SE scores. However, in contrast to other studies [7,11] but in accordance with a study by Jozkowski et al. [26], we did not observe a correlation between being in relationship or relationship duration and SESII-W-PL. This null finding may be explained by the effects of age. That is, when controlling for age, the association between SESII-W-PL and relationship status reported in prior studies may be driven by age rather than by the current relationship. Interestingly, we found that higher WMRQ scores–reflecting a better relationship–were associated with lower SE. These findings are similar to results of prior studies demonstrating that women in stable relationships have lower SE scores [7,11,26,29]. These results may be interpreted as a better relationship–associated with more stability–may decrease the excitatory tone and result in lower SE. Further, given that higher similarity in a relationship, higher intimacy, and higher personal fulfilment were correlated with lower SE, we speculate that some differences between partners may be necessary for a satisfactory sexual life. Furthermore, these results challenge the hypothesis that SE and SI reflect traits tendencies, as suggested by Velten et al. [30], or that relationship quality modifies trait excitatory tone. More studies are needed to show if the observed correlation between relationship quality and SE is casual in nature.

Clinical implications

The present study has some important clinical implications. First, we demonstrated for the first time, that the presence of sexual dysfunction assessed by strict DSM-5 criteria is correlated with greater concerns about sexual functioning and higher SI. However, the presence of sexual problems (evaluated by the FSFI screening questionnaire), sexual distress, and distressing sexual concerns (based on FSFI and FSDS-R) that do not necessarily meet DSM-5 criteria were correlated with lower Arousability and higher Arousal Contingency and SI. Based on these results, we speculate that women with a higher propensity for SI might be at risk of sexual disorders [11,20,30]. Prior research suggests that psychotherapy combined with 5-hydroxytryptamine1A receptor agonist plus Phosphodiesterase-5 inhibitors (PDE-5i) could be a potential therapeutic option [35] for women with high Arousal Continence (i.e., worries that circumstances of sexual activity will not be “perfect”) and high Concerns about Sexual Function (i.e., performance anxiety). In contrast, both psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy (testosterone with PDE-5i to increase arousability) may be more appropriate for women [35] with low Arousal Contingency, low Concerns about Sexual function, and low arousability (reflecting “how easily one might become sexually aroused”). Similarly, in women with low arousability, hormonal oral contraceptives (OC’s) may not be the best option as these individuals may be more susceptible to endocrine changes induced by OC’s [16]. Thus, the SESII-W might be helpful in tailoring therapy for sexual problems, distress, and dysfunction.

Second, given that women with higher SE (in all domains except Smell) were more frequently engaged in RSB, those scoring high in SE should be consulted accordingly. Thus, the SESII-W might be helpful in screening for women at risk of engaging in RSB or hypersexual-related behaviors [70]. Further studies are required to confirm that hypothesis.

Third, women with lower SE reported a lower frequency and broader breadth of sexual experiences. Filling in the scale may therefore have an educational function–while reading the questions/statements women may learn about sexual responses, possible reasons for performance anxiety, and may acknowledge and accept changes in sexual responses capacity in different life stage. The SE scale, in the same mechanism, may also motivate women to seek sexually exciting situations and individual experiences to increase sexual responses in different models, not only the linear models [16], and irrespective of potential limitations (e.g. physical limitations) [27].

Finally, we found that personality type was correlated with SE and SI such that more extraverted women reported higher SE and more neurotic women reported higher SI. These findings suggest that personality type assessments may be useful for everyday clinical sexual medicine practice.

Study limitation and future studies

The present study has some limitations. Firstly, it is not free of volunteer bias. Given that the nature of the study was somewhat intimate, some women may have felt uncomfortable and did not wish to participate. Therefore, the study sample may not fully represent all women in Poland. Selecting participant according to population structure could be helpful in future studies, but this approach cannot guarantee full representativeness. Nonetheless, the minimal required sample was achieved, suggesting that this sampling limitation does not influence the usability of the scale. Secondly, although the sample of women in the present study varied in age, there was homogeneity in relationship status. Further studies on single women are needed to better examine the model in this population. Thirdly, the study sample consisted primarily of heterosexual individuals, so the results cannot be extrapolated to homosexual populations. Fourthly, the cross-sectional nature of this study precluded us from examining the potential causality of obtained results. Finally, a recent study by Kilimnik and Meston [71] demonstrated that body esteem was negatively correlated with SI and, in women with a history of child sexual abuse, with SE. They also reported that women with low body esteem have higher SI, and a history of sexual abuse is associated with low body esteem which, in turn, decreases SE [71]. Although we did not measure history of child sexual abuse in the present study, the impact of child sexual abuse history is beyond the scope of the present study and represents a possible area for further investigation. Further, a study by Velten et al. [10] showed a negative correlation between BMI and SI domain. We did not find that correlations in the present study. Despite these limitations, the present study was demonstrated to have adequate power to perform the analyses and examined the potential associations between SESII-W-PL and the presence of FSD, based on DSM-5 criteria. Additionally, a large battery of several related measures was used to test for convergent and divergent validity of the SESII-W-PL. Finally, a multigroup comparison enabled the evaluation of the final model for structural invariance. For these reasons, we believe that the current version of the SESII-W-PL can be used in the population of Polish women between the ages of 18–55 years and provides new data that may be useful in clinical practice.

Conclusions

The propensity for SE and SI among Polish women is comparable to rates observed in other European and North American populations. Although different from the original scale, the SESII-W-PL demonstrated good psychometric properties and can therefore be used in the population of Polish women between the ages of 18–55 years. Using the SESII-W-PL, we found that a higher propensity for SI correlated with the presence of sexual problems, distress, and sexual dysfunction, whereas a higher propensity for SE correlated with personality types (i.e., extraversion and openness to new experiences) and the tendency to engage in RSB. Although both inhibitory and excitatory tone was shown to decrease with age, excitatory tone may be altered by relationship quality.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Correlations between the SESII-W-PL and other variables (Pearson’s r correlations).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249560.s001

(DOCX)

S1 Appendix. The Polish version of the SESII-W (SESII-W-PL).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249560.s002

(DOCX)

References

  1. 1. Bancroft J. Central inhibition of sexual response in the male: a theoretical perspective. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 1999;23(6):763–84. Epub 1999/12/14. pmid:10541055.
  2. 2. Janssen E, Bancroft J. The dual control model: The role of sexual inhibition and excitation in sexual arousal and behavior. In: Janssen E, editor. The psychophysiology of sex. The Kinsey Institute series. Bloomington, IN, US: Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press; 2007. p. 197–222.
  3. 3. Kurpisz J, Mak M, Lew-Starowicz M, Nowosielski K, Samochowiec J. The dual control model of sexual response by J. Bancroft and E. Janssen. Theoretical basis, research and practical issues. Postępy Psychiatrii i Neurologii. 2015;24(3):156–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pin.2015.08.001.
  4. 4. Gray JA. Précis of The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1982;5(3):469–84. Epub 2010/02/04.
  5. 5. Janssen E, Vorst H, Finn P, Bancroft J. The Sexual Inhibition (SIS) and Sexual Excitation (SES) Scales: I. Measuring sexual inhibition and excitation proneness in men. Journal of Sex Research,. 2002;39(2):114–26. Epub 2002/12/12. pmid:12476243.
  6. 6. Carpenter D, Janssen E, Graham C, Vorst H, Wicherts J. Women’s scores on the sexual inhibition/sexual excitation scales (SIS/SES): gender similarities and differences. Journal of Sex Research,. 2008;45(1):36–48. Epub 2008/03/07. pmid:18321029.
  7. 7. Graham CA, Sanders SA, Milhausen RR. The sexual excitation/sexual inhibition inventory for women: psychometric properties. Archives of sexual behavior. 2006;35(4):397–409. Epub 2006/08/11. pmid:16900415.
  8. 8. Milhausen RR, Graham CA, Sanders SA, Yarber WL, Maitland SB. Validation of the sexual excitation/sexual inhibition inventory for women and men. Archives of sexual behavior. 2010;39(5):1091–104. Epub 2009/10/28. pmid:19859799.
  9. 9. Carpenter D, Janssen E, Graham C, Vorst H, Wicherts J. The sexual inhibition/sexual excitation scales–short form (SIS/SES-SF). 2010. p. 236–9.
  10. 10. Velten J, Scholten S, Graham CA, Margraf J. Psychometric Properties of the Sexual Excitation/Sexual Inhibition Inventory for Women in a German Sample. Archives of sexual behavior. 2016;45(2):303–14. Epub 2015/05/31. pmid:26025455.
  11. 11. Bloemendaal LB, Laan ET. The psychometric properties of the sexual excitation/sexual inhibition inventory for women (SESII-W) within a Dutch population. Journal of sex research. 2015;52(1):69–82. Epub 2013/11/13. pmid:24215152.
  12. 12. Granados MR, Salinas JM, Sierra JC. Spanish version of the Sexual Excitation/Sexual Inhibition Inventory for Women: Factorial structure, reliability and validity evidences. Int J Clin Health Psychol. 2017;17(1):65–76. Epub 2017/01/01. pmid:30487882; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6236321.
  13. 13. Granados MR, Salinas JM, Sierra JC. Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the Sexual Inhibition/Sexual Excitation Scales for men. Archives of Sexual Behavior,. 2018;47(3):783–96. Epub 2017/05/04. pmid:28466229.
  14. 14. Clifton J, Seehuus M, Rellini AH. Testing cognitive predictors of individual differences in the sexual psychophysiological responses of sexually functional women. 2015;52(7):957–68. pmid:25816911
  15. 15. Velten J, Scholten S, Graham CA, Adolph D, Margraf J. Investigating Female Sexual Concordance: Do Sexual Excitation and Sexual Inhibition Moderate the Agreement of Genital and Subjective Sexual Arousal in Women? Archives of sexual behavior. 2016;45(8):1957–71. Epub 2016/07/06. pmid:27379408.
  16. 16. Bancroft J, Graham C, Janssen E, Sanders S. The Dual Control Model: Current status and future directions. Journal of Sex Research,. 2009;46:121–42. pmid:19308839
  17. 17. Atlantis E, Sullivan T. Bidirectional association between depression and sexual dysfunction: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Sex Med. 2012;9(6):1497–507. Epub 2012/04/03. pmid:22462756.
  18. 18. Velten J, Scholten S, Graham CA, Margraf J. Sexual Excitation and Sexual Inhibition as Predictors of Sexual Function in Women: A Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Study. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy. 2017;43(2):95–109. pmid:26735804
  19. 19. Bancroft J, Graham CA, Janssen E, Sanders SA. The dual control model: current status and future directions. Journal of sex research. 2009;46(2–3):121–42. Epub 2009/03/25. pmid:19308839.
  20. 20. Sanders SA, Graham CA, Milhausen RR. Predicting sexual problems in women: the relevance of sexual excitation and sexual inhibition. Archives of sexual behavior. 2008;37(2):241–51. Epub 2007/09/29. pmid:17902042.
  21. 21. Velten J, Brailovskaia J, Margraf J. Exploring the Impact of Personal and Partner Traits on Sexuality: Sexual Excitation, Sexual Inhibition, and Big Five Predict Sexual Function in Couples. Journal of sex research. 2019;56(3):287–99. Epub 2018/07/22. pmid:30028211.
  22. 22. Paul V, Carvalheira A. The Relevance of the SESII-W/M in Establishing a Relationship between Sexual Difficulties and Sexual Excitation/Inhibition. 2019. 2019;8(1):18%J Psychology, Community & Health. Epub 2019-06-11.
  23. 23. Hodgson B, Kukkonen TM, Binik YM, Carrier S. Using the Dual Control Model to Investigate the Relationship Between Mood, Genital, and Self-Reported Sexual Arousal in Men and Women. Journal of sex research. 2016;53(8):979–93. Epub 2016/01/05. pmid:26726841.
  24. 24. Quinta Gomes AL, Janssen E, Santos-Iglesias P, Pinto-Gouveia J, Fonseca LM, Nobre PJ. Validation of the Sexual Inhibition and Sexual Excitation Scales (SIS/SES) in Portugal: Assessing gender differences and predictors of sexual functioning. Archives of Sexual Behavior,. 2018;47(6):1721–32. Epub 2018/03/15. pmid:29536260.
  25. 25. Velten J. The dual control model of sexual response: Relevance of sexual excitation and sexual inhibition for sexual function. Current Sexual Health Reports. 2017;9(2):90–7.
  26. 26. Jozkowski KN, Sanders SA, Rhoads K, Milhausen RR, Graham CA. Examining the Psychometric Properties of the Sexual Excitation/Sexual Inhibition Inventory for Women (SESII-W) in a Sample of Lesbian and Bisexual Women. Journal of sex research. 2016;53(7):836–48. Epub 2015/10/20. pmid:26479215.
  27. 27. Bell S, Reissing ED. Sexual Well-Being in Older Women: The Relevance of Sexual Excitation and Sexual Inhibition. Journal of sex research. 2017;54(9):1153–65. Epub 2016/12/03. pmid:27911092.
  28. 28. Tavares IM, Laan ETM, Nobre PJ. Sexual Inhibition is a Vulnerability Factor for Orgasm Problems in Women. The Journal of Sexual Medicine. 2018;15(3):361–72. pmid:29396021
  29. 29. Dang SS, Gorzalka BB, Brotto LA. Dual Control Model in a Cross-Cultural Context: Role of Sexual Excitation in Sexual Response and Behavior Differences Between Chinese and Euro-Caucasian Women in Canada. Archives of sexual behavior. 2019;48(8):2519–35. Epub 2019/09/15. pmid:31520178.
  30. 30. Velten J, Zahler L, Scholten S, Margraf J. Temporal Stability of Sexual Excitation and Sexual Inhibition in Women. Archives of sexual behavior. 2019;48(3):881–9. Epub 2019/02/17. pmid:30771055.
  31. 31. Ramet S P. The Catholic Church in Polish History From 966 to the Present. Jelen T, G.; Rozell M., J., editor. New York, NY, USA: Palgrave Macmillan; 2017.
  32. 32. Gradskova Y, Kondakov A, Shevtsova M. Post-socialist Revolutions of Intimacy: An Introduction. Sexuality & Culture. 2020;24(2):359–70.
  33. 33. Koscianska A. Beyond Divisions: Re-thinking Sexuality, Reproduction and Catholicism in Poland. 16th EASA Biennial Conference, EASA2020: New anthropological horizons in and beyond Europe; 23 July 2020; Lisboa, Spain2020.
  34. 34. Kellogg Spadt S, Rosenbaum TY, Dweck A, Millheiser L, Pillai-Friedman S, Krychman M. Sexual health and religion: a primer for the sexual health clinician (CME). J Sex Med. 2014;11(7):1607–18; quiz 19. Epub 2014/07/06. pmid:24989443.
  35. 35. Bloemers J, van Rooij K, Poels S, Goldstein I, Everaerd W, Koppeschaar H, et al. Toward personalized sexual medicine (part 1): integrating the "dual control model" into differential drug treatments for hypoactive sexual desire disorder and female sexual arousal disorder. J Sex Med. 2013;10(3):791–809. Epub 2012/11/08. pmid:23130782.
  36. 36. Velten J, Scholten S, Margraf J. Psychometric properties of the Sexual Excitation/Sexual Inhibition Inventory for Women and Men (SESII-W/M) and the Sexual Excitation Scales/Sexual Inhibition Scales short form (SIS/SES-SF) in a population-based sample in Germany. PLoS One. 2018;13(3):e0193080. Epub 2018/03/13. pmid:29529045; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5846736.
  37. 37. Swami V, Barron D. Translation and validation of body image instruments: Challenges, good practice guidelines, and reporting recommendations for test adaptation. Body Image. 2019;31:204–20. Epub 2018/09/18. pmid:30220631.
  38. 38. Beaton DE, Bombardier C, Guillemin F, Ferraz MB. Guidelines for the process of cross-cultural adaptation of self-report measures. Spine. 2000;25(24):3186–91. Epub 2000/12/22. pmid:11124735.
  39. 39. Karakuła H, Grzywa A, Spila B, Baszak J, Gieroba A, Kosikowski W, et al. [Use of Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale in psychosomatic disorders]. Psychiatr Pol. 1996;30(4):653–67. pmid:8975264.
  40. 40. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®): American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.
  41. 41. Nowosielski K, Wrobel B, Sioma-Markowska U, Poreba R. Development and validation of the Polish version of the Female Sexual Function Index in the Polish population of females. J Sex Med. 2013;10(2):386–95. Epub 2012/12/06. pmid:23211010.
  42. 42. Nowosielski K, Wrobel B, Sioma-Markowska U, Poreba R. Sexual dysfunction and distress—development of a Polish version of the female sexual distress scale-revised. J Sex Med. 2013;10(5):1304–12. Epub 2013/03/01. pmid:23444946.
  43. 43. Sell RL. Defining and measuring sexual orientation for research. In: Meyer IH, Northridge ME, editors. The health of sexual minorities: Public health perspectives on lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender populations. New York, NY, US: New York, NY: Springer Science + Business Media; 2007. p. 355–74.
  44. 44. Nowosielski K, Sipinski A, Kuczerawy I, Kozlowska-Rup D, Skrzypulec-Plinta V. Tattoos, piercing, and sexual behaviors in young adults. Journal of Sexual Medicine. 2012;9(9):2307–14. Epub 2012/05/24. pmid:22616886.
  45. 45. Müller JM, Wytykowska AM. Psychometric properties and validation of a Polish adaptation of Carver and White’s BIS/BAS scales. Personality and Individual Differences. 2005;39(4):795–805.
  46. 46. Janda LH. Love and sex tests: twenty-four psychological tests to help you evaluate your readiness for relationships, love, and sex: Warszawa: Adams Media Corporation; 1998.
  47. 47. Jankowski K. Charakterystyka psychometryczna polskiej wersji Zrewidowanego Inwentarza Orientacji Socjoseksualnej (SOI-R). In: Oniszczenko W, Rynkiewicz A, Jankowski KS, Scholar ., editors. Wybrane metody i paradygmaty badawcze w psychologii Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe Scholar; 2016. p. 77–92.
  48. 48. Drwal RŁ, Wilczyńska JT. Opracowanie kwestionariusza aprobaty społecznej. [Elaboration of the social desirability questionnaire.]. Przegląd Psychologiczny. 1980;23(3):569–83.
  49. 49. Łaguna M, Bąk W, Purc E, Mielniczuk E, Oleś P. Short measure of personality TIPI-P in a Polish sample. Annals of Psychology. 2014;17(2):403–37.
  50. 50. Plopa M. Kwestionariusz Dobranego Małżeństwa (KDM-2). In: Plopa M, editor. Więzi w małżeństwie i rodzinie—metody badań. 3 ed. Kraków: Kraków: Oficyna Wydawnicza "Impuls"; 2007.
  51. 51. Wood ND, Akloubou Gnonhosou DC, Bowling JW. Combining parallel and exploratory factor analysis in identifying relationship scales in secondary data. Marriage & Family Review. 2015;51(5):385–95. pmid:26494935
  52. 52. Tabachnick B, Fidell LS. Using Multivarite Statistics (5th ed.): Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon/Pearson Education; 2007. 980 p.
  53. 53. Gaskin J, Lin J. Multigroup Analysis. AMOS plugins05.06.2020]. Available from: http://statwiki.kolobkreations.com/index.php?title=Main_Page.
  54. 54. Koo TK, Li MY. A Guideline of Selecting and Reporting Intraclass Correlation Coefficients for Reliability Research. Journal of chiropractic medicine. 2016;15(2):155–63. Epub 2016/06/23. pmid:27330520; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4913118.
  55. 55. Deng L, Chan W. Testing the Difference Between Reliability Coefficients Alpha and Omega. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2016;77(2):185–203. pmid:29795909
  56. 56. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112(1):155–9. pmid:19565683
  57. 57. Kornbrot D. Point Biserial Correlation. In: Everitt BS, Howell DC, editors. Encyclopedia of Statistics in Behavioral Science: Hatfield, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.; 2005. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.1.10 pmid:15702960
  58. 58. Lakens D.Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. 2013;4(863). pmid:24324449
  59. 59. Nowosielski K, Jankowski KS, Kowalczyk R, Kurpisz J, Normantowicz-Zakrzewska M, Krasowska A. Sexual Self-Schema Scale for Women—Validation and Psychometric Properties of the Polish Version. Sexual Medicine. 2018;6(2):131–42. pmid:29463474
  60. 60. Pietraszkiewicz A, Kaufmann MC, Formanowicz MM. Masculinity Ideology and Subjective Well-Being in a Sample of Polish Men and Women. Polish Psychological Bulletin. 2017;(No 1).
  61. 61. Lew-Starowicz M. Tortures, tramples, tires and harasses.. Characters [Internet]. 2017 01.10.2020; (9(248)):[44–7 pp.]. Available from: https://charaktery.eu/artykul/katuj-tratuj-mecz-i-drecz.
  62. 62. Basson R. The female sexual response: a different model. J Sex Marital Ther. 2000;26(1):51–65. Epub 2000/02/29. pmid:10693116.
  63. 63. Nowosielski K, Kurpisz J, Kowalczyk R. Body image during sexual activity in the population of Polish adult women. Prz Menopauzalny. 2019;18(4):198–209. Epub 2020/03/07. pmid:32132883; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7045354.
  64. 64. Nowosielski K, Wrobel B, Kowalczyk R. Women’s endorsement of models of sexual response: Correlates and predictors. Archives of Sexual Behavior,. 2016;45(2):291–302. Epub 2015/11/26. pmid:26601676.
  65. 65. Hodgson B, Kukkonen TM, Binik YM, Carrier S. Using the dual control model to investigate the relationship between mood, genital, and self-reported sexual arousal in men and women. Journal of Sex Research,. 2016;53(8):979–93. pmid:26726841
  66. 66. Velten J, Scholten S, Graham CA, Margraf J. Unprotected Intercourse and One-Night Stands: Impact of Sexual Excitation, Sexual Inhibition, and Atypical Sexual Arousal Patterns on Risky Sexual Behaviors in Women. The Journal of Sexual Medicine. 2016;13(3):361–73. pmid:26803457.
  67. 67. Pinxten W, Lievens J. An exploratory study of factors associated with sexual inhibition and excitation: findings from a representative survey in flanders. Journal of Sex Research,. 2015;52(6):679–89. pmid:24670220
  68. 68. Pinxten W, Lievens J. Gender Differences in the Development of Sexual Excitation and Inhibition Through the Life Course: Preliminary Findings from a Representative Study in Flanders. The Journal of Sex Research. 2016;53(7):825–35. pmid:26488561
  69. 69. van Lankveld JJDM, Platteau T, van Montfort K, Nieuwenhuijs F, Syroit J. The predictive validity of SIS/SES and BIS/BAS scores for sexual and non-sexual risk behavior. Personality and Individual Differences. 2015;79:7–12.
  70. 70. Walton MT, Cantor JM, Bhullar N, Lykins AD. Hypersexuality: A critical review and introduction to the “sexhavior sycle”. Archives of Sexual Behavior,. 2017;46(8):2231–51. pmid:28687897
  71. 71. Kilimnik CD, Meston CM. Role of Body Esteem in the Sexual Excitation and Inhibition Responses of Women With and Without a History of Childhood Sexual Abuse. J Sex Med. 2016;13(11):1718–28. Epub 2016/10/25. pmid:27692843; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5096591.