Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Fluoxetine during Development Reverses the Effects of Prenatal Stress on Depressive-Like Behavior and Hippocampal Neurogenesis in Adolescence

  • Ine Rayen,

    Affiliation Department of Neuroscience, School of Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands

  • Daniël L. van den Hove,

    Affiliation Department of Neuroscience, School of Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands

  • Jos Prickaerts,

    Affiliation Department of Neuroscience, School of Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands

  • Harry W. Steinbusch,

    Affiliation Department of Neuroscience, School of Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands

  • Jodi L. Pawluski

    j.pawluski@maastrichtuniversity.nl

    Affiliation Department of Neuroscience, School of Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands

Abstract

Depression during pregnancy and the postpartum period is a growing health problem, which affects up to 20% of women. Currently, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRIs) medications are commonly used for treatment of maternal depression. Unfortunately, there is very little research on the long-term effect of maternal depression and perinatal SSRI exposure on offspring development. Therefore, the aim of this study was to determine the role of exposure to fluoxetine during development on affective-like behaviors and hippocampal neurogenesis in adolescent offspring in a rodent model of maternal depression. To do this, gestationally stressed and non-stressed Sprague-Dawley rat dams were treated with either fluoxetine (5 mg/kg/day) or vehicle beginning on postnatal day 1 (P1). Adolescent male and female offspring were divided into 4 groups: 1) prenatal stress+fluoxetine exposure, 2) prenatal stress+vehicle, 3) fluoxetine exposure alone, and 4) vehicle alone. Adolescent offspring were assessed for anxiety-like behavior using the Open Field Test and depressive-like behavior using the Forced Swim Test. Brains were analyzed for endogenous markers of hippocampal neurogenesis via immunohistochemistry. Results demonstrate that maternal fluoxetine exposure reverses the reduction in immobility evident in prenatally stressed adolescent offspring. In addition, maternal fluoxetine exposure reverses the decrease in hippocampal cell proliferation and neurogenesis in maternally stressed adolescent offspring. This research provides important evidence on the long-term effect of fluoxetine exposure during development in a model of maternal adversity.

Introduction

Depression during pregnancy and the postpartum period is a growing health concern that affects up to 20% of women [1], [2], [3], [4]. Maternal stress, depression and anxiety, can have long-term effects on the physical and mental development of children [5], [6], [7]. For example, antenatal maternal depression can lead to neurobehavioral disturbances, such as impaired cognitive and social developmental outcomes [6], [7], [8], [9], [10], [11]. In rodent models, stress during gestation, which results in depressive-like behavior in the dam [12], [13], models the clinical findings [11], [14]. Several animal studies have indicated that adult offspring of prenatally stressed mothers show increases in affective-related behavior [14], [15], [16], [17] and decreased levels of hippocampal neurogenesis [18], [19], [20], [21]. Given the development effect of exposure to maternal depression, it is crucial to treat this disorder in order to improve maternal and child outcomes.

Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRIs) medications are commonly used for the treatment of maternal depression [22]. Current estimates suggest that there is an increasing incidence of SSRI use in mothers that ranges between 5–10% [23], [24], [25]. However, the effects of these medications on the developing child have yet to be fully determined [5], [26]. Recent clinical studies report that neonates exposed to SSRI medications during gestation, regardless of maternal mood state, have an increased risk for low birth weight, younger gestational age, neurobehavioral disturbances, and reduced heart rate variability [5], [27], [28]. Recent evidence also demonstrates that prenatal exposure to SSRI-medications may alter neurodevelopment as evidenced via alterations in S100B levels [29]. In addition, perinatal exposure to SSRI medications may have long term effects on mood in children [30], [31]. For example, children perinatally exposed to maternal depression and SSRIs exhibit increased internalizing behaviors at 3 years [31].

Preclinical data is beginning to show that exposure to SSRIs during development significantly impacts offspring affective-like behaviors and neural plasticity [5], [26], [32]. For example, SSRI treatment, via intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection to offspring, during the early postnatal period can result in increased depressive- and anxiety-like behavior during adulthood [33], [34], [35]. Developmental exposure to SSRIs may also influence neuroplasticity in the hippocampus, through effects on brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) mRNA levels [34].

Although these studies point to a role for SSRIs in development, it should be noted that in preclinical studies offspring are treated with or exposed to SSRIs alone, and not in combination with maternal depression. To date, very little research has looked at the effect of maternal stress and SSRIs on offspring outcomes [36], [37] and only one study has combined both a model of maternal stress and fluoxetine exposure [36]. This study demonstrated that postnatal oral administration of the SSRI, fluoxetine, to pups reverses the stress induced reduction in CA3 spine and synapse density in juveniles and young adults [36]. In this study, SSRI treatment alone, in the absence of maternal stress, had no affect on spine density measures in the CA3 region of the hippocampus [36]. Thus the actions of early exposure to SSRI medications may be very different in the presence of maternal adversity. Therefore to better translate these findings to the clinic, the effects of maternal use of SSRI medications need to be investigated in animal models of maternal adversity.

The aim of the present study was to investigate the developmental effect of fluoxetine, a popular SSRI antidepressant used during pregnancy, in a model of maternal adversity, on anxiety and depression-related behavior and hippocampal neurogenesis in adolescent male and female offspring. Although research has investigated the developmental impact of perinatal SSRI exposure on offspring outcomes, little research has been done on the neurodevelopmental effects of postnatal fluoxetine treatment in an animal model of maternal depression. In addition, much less research has looked at the long-term effects of developmental SSRI exposure during adolescence, a time of vulnerability to stress [38], [39], [40], [41], [42]. Our data shows that the exposure to fluoxetine during development can reverse the effect of prenatal stress on aspects of adolescent development. Knowledge of the effects of maternal depression and antidepressant treatment during the perinatal period is needed to ameliorate treatment and intervention options, and thus improve neurodevelopmental outcomes.

Methods

Animals

Twenty-two adult female Sprague-Dawley rats (250–300 g; Charles River Laboratories, France) were used in the present study. Rats were kept under standard laboratory conditions in a 12h∶12h light/dark schedule (lights on at 07:00 h) with ad libitum access to rat chow (Sniff) and tap water. All experiments were approved by the Animal Ethics Board of Maastricht University in accordance with Dutch governmental regulations (approval IDs: DEC 2008-157 and DEC 2008-158). All efforts were made to minimize the pain and stress levels experienced by the animals.

On gestation day (GD) 15, dams were randomly assigned to stress (n = 12) or control groups (n = 10). Dams in the stress group were individually restrained three times a day for 45 min in transparent plastic cylinders under bright light (between 8–10am, 12–2pm, 4–6pm) on GD15–20 and twice on GD21 as previously described [43], [44]. This time period during pregnancy is when stress can result in postpartum depressive-like behavior in the dam [12], [13] and a period of stress that affects offspring outcomes [17], [45].

One day after birth (birth day = P0), litters were culled to 5 males and 5 females and dams (with offspring) were randomly assigned to one of two treatment groups: fluoxetine (5 mg/kg/day) or vehicle, for a total of four groups of dams: 1) Prenatal Stress + Vehicle (PSV; n = 5), 2) Prenatal Stress + Fluoxetine (PSF; n = 7), 3) Control + Fluoxetine (CF; n = 5), and 4) Control + Vehicle (CV; n = 5). A maximum of 2 male and 2 female offspring per litter were used in the present experiment (n = 9–11/sex/group). Offspring litter was weighed on P21 and individual weights were taken once between P29–31 and once between P39–42. For assessment of hippocampal cell proliferation and neurogenesis, 5 animals per group were used (1 male and 1 female from each litter). For a time line of the experiment see Figure 1.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Timeline of experiment.

Stress was administered between GD15-21. Fluoxetine treatment to the mother began 1 day after birth and continued until weaning (P21). Between P32 and P39, offspring were subjected to behavioral tasks. At P42, offspring were sacrificed.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0024003.g001

Fluoxetine treatment

Fluoxetine treatment was administered via osmotic minipumps (Alzet Osmotic pumps, 2ML4) to the dam on P1. Fluoxetine and its active metabolite, norfluoxetine, can pass to offspring through lactation [46], therefore we used this mode of delivery to prevent the stress of administration via injection, or oral gavage to the offspring. In addition, rodent brain development during the early postpartum period is analogous to human brain development during the third trimester [47].

Implants were filled with either fluoxetine (Fagron, Belgium), dissolved in vehicle (50% propylenediol in saline; 5 mg/kg/day), or with vehicle as previously described [48]. Minipumps were implanted subcutaneously in the dorsal region while the dams were under mild isofluorene anesthesia on post-partum day 1 (P1).

Maternal care

Maternal care was assessed twice a day for 5 minutes from P2 to P7 based on previous literature [49]. Scoring took place in the morning (between 8:30 a.m. and 10:30 a.m.) and the afternoon (between 13:30 p.m. and 15:30 p.m.) with at least 3 h between the sessions. During each testing period the duration of the following maternal behaviors was assessed: licking (licking/grooming; licking/grooming/nursing), nursing (arched-back nursing, “blanket” nursing, and passive nursing) and nest building. Data were aggregated across days and were calculated as total percent time spent in each behavior.

The Open Field Test (OFT)

The OFT was used to study anxiety-like behavior and locomotor activity in adolescent offspring [50]. The open field test consisted of a 100 cm×100 cm area divided into central and peripheral areas with 40 cm high walls. For the test, a rat was placed in the centre of the field and behavior was recorded for five minutes. All animals were tested once between 9:30 a.m. and 2 p.m. (age P32–34). A video-tracking system (Anymaze, Stoelting) was used to score the distance travelled, number of entries into the central and peripheral areas, and total time spent in the central and peripheral areas. The apparatus was cleaned with 70% ethanol and dried between rats.

The Forced Swim Test (FST)

The forced swim test (FST) was used to assess depressive-like behavior in the adolescent offspring as previously described [51], [52], [53]. The apparatus consisted of a vertical cylindrical glass tank (height 50 cm × diameter 20 cm) filled to a depth of 20 cm with tap water at 27 ± 1°C. For the test, an animal was placed in a cylindrical glass tank for 10 min. Offspring were tested on the FST, between 9 a.m. and 1 p.m. (age P37–39). Using the Best Collection System (Educational Consulting Inc.), behaviors scored in the FST were (1) immobility – floating with the absence of any movement and (2) struggling – quick movements of the forelimbs such that the front paws break the surface of the water.

Immunohistochemistry (IHC)

A minimum of 2 days after the last behavioral test, offspring were deeply anesthetised with an overdose of pentobarbital, and decapitated. Half of the brain was used for IHC, the hippocampus of the other half was used for further analysis not included in the present study. Brains were post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 48 hours, cryoprotected in 30% sucrose/phosphate-buffered saline solution for up to one week, frozen on dry ice and kept at −80°C. Brain tissue was sliced in 40 µm sections on a cryostat (Leica). Tissue was stored in antifreeze solution and maintained at −15°C. The number of proliferating cells and immature neurons were assessed in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus using endogenous markers, i.e. Ki67 for cell proliferation and doublecortin (DCX) for immature neurons. Every 6th section throughout half the hippocampus was stained as previously described [54], [55]. Sections were blocked with H2O2 and incubated overnight in either rabbit anti-Ki67 (1∶500; Vector Laboratories) or goat anti-DCX (1∶200; Santa Cruz). Sections were then incubated overnight in biotinylated donkey anti-rabbit (1∶500; Jackson ImmunoResearch, Suffolk, UK) or for 2 hours in biotinylated rabbit anti-goat (1∶500; Jackson ImmunoResearch) secondary antibody. Brain sections were further processed by using the avidine-biotine complex (ABC Elite kit; 1∶1000; Vector laboratories). DAB (3,3-diaminobenzidine; Sigma) was used as a substrate to obtain a color reaction. Sections were mounted on gelatin-coated slides, dried overnight, counterstained with Cresyl Violet acetate, dehydrated and coverslipped with Permount (Fisher Scientific).

The number of Ki67 immunoreactive (-ir) cells and DCX-ir cells were counted under 40× objective with oil as previously described [54], [55]. Cells were considered Ki67-ir if they were intensely stained and exhibited medium round or oval cell bodies (Figure 2A). Cells were considered DCX-ir if they exhibited medium round or oval cell bodies and dendrites (Figure 2B). The areas of the granule cell layer/subgranular zone (GCL/SGZ) and hilus were measured using StereoInvestigator software (MicroBrightField, Williston, VT, USA) and estimates of GCL/SGZ and hilus volumes were made using Cavalieri's principle [56].

thumbnail
Figure 2. Photomicrographs of representative A) Ki67-ir cells and B) DCX-ir cells in the GCL/SGZ and mean (± SEM) number of C) Ki67-ir cells and D) DCX-ir cells in the GCL/SGZ.

C) PSV adolescent offspring had significantly fewer Ki-67-ir cells in the GCL/SVZ compared to all other groups (.0001≤p≤.01). CF adolescent offspring had significantly more Ki67-ir cells in the GCL/SVZ compared to PSV offspring (p≤.0001), but significantly fewer Ki67-ir cells in the GCL/SVZ compared to CV and PSF offspring (.002≤p≤.04), regardless of sex. D) PSV adolescent offspring had significantly fewer number of DCX-ir cells in the GCL/SGZ of the hippocampus compared to all other groups (.006≤p≤.03), regardless of sex. ‘*’denotes significantly different from all other groups. (n = 5/sex/group).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0024003.g002

Statistical analyses

Analysis of variance tests (ANOVA) were done for maternal behaviors with condition (prenatal stress/no stress) and treatment (fluoxetine/vehicle) as independent factors. ANOVAs were done on offspring weight gain, FST measures, OFT measures, Ki67-ir and DCX-ir cell numbers with condition (prenatal stress/no stress), treatment (fluoxetine/vehicle), and sex (male/female) as independent factors. Pearson product moment correlations were conducted between behaviors on the OFT (central entries, central time, and central distance) and FST (struggling and floating), and the total number of Ki67-ir and DCX-ir cells for all groups and separately by treatment and condition. Any differences in age, weight, time of testing or test order of the litter, were accounted for, where appropriate, via an analysis of covariance. In cases where clear sex differences were evident stratified analysis were done separately for each sex. Posthoc comparisons utilized the Fisher LSD test.

Results

Maternal care

Stressed dams spent a significantly greater percentage of time nest building compared to non-stressed dams, regardless of fluoxetine treatment (main effect of treatment; F(1, 18) = 10.66, p≤.004; Table 1). Independent of treatment condition, dams spent a significantly greater percentage of time nursing offspring than licking offspring (main effect of time; F(1, 18) = 358.37, p≤.00001; Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Mean (± SEM) percentage of time in maternal behaviors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0024003.t001

Body weight change

CF and PSF offspring gained significantly less weight than CV and PSV offspring, regardless of stress (main effect of treatment; F(1,68) = 15.33, p≤.0002; Figure 3). Overall, male offspring gained significantly more weight than female offspring (main effect of sex; F(1,68) = 16.71, p≤.0001; Figure 3), even when controlling for any age differences at the time of weighing.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Mean (± SEM) percentage in weight change from P29 to P42.

CF and PSF offspring gained significantly less weight than CV and PSV offspring, regardless of stress (p≤.0002). Overall, male adolescent offspring gained significantly more weight than female offspring (p≤.0001). ‘*’denotes CF and PSF significantly different from CV and PSV groups. ‘**’ denotes males significantly different from females. (n = 9–11/sex/group).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0024003.g003

The OFT

PSV male offspring made significantly fewer central entries compared to CF and PSV female offspring (.007≤p≤.02: condition×treatment×sex effect; F(1,69) = 4.43, p≤.04; Figure 4A). There was also a significant main effect of sex with male offspring making significantly fewer central entries compared to female offspring (F(1,69) = 4.14, p≤.05). Further analysis by sex revealed that PSV males made fewer central entries than CV, CF and PSF adolescent males, however this did not reach significance (p≥0.09) and there were no significant effects of treatment or condition in female adolescent offspring (p≥0.14). There were no other significant differences between groups in measures on the OFT (.07≤p≤.90; Table 2).

thumbnail
Figure 4. Mean (± SEM) A) number of central entries (OFT) and B) time spent immobile (FST).

A) PSV male offspring made significantly fewer central entries compared to CF and PSV female offspring (.007≤p≤.02). There was also a significant main effect of sex with male offspring making significantly fewer central entries compared to female offspring (p≤.05). Further analysis by sex revealed that PSV males made fewer central entries than CV, CF and PSF adolescent males, however this did not reach significance (p≥0.09) and there were no significant effect of treatment or condition in female adolescent offspring (p≥0.14). B) PSV adolescent offspring spent significantly less time immobile compared to CV and PSF offspring (.02≤p≤.04). (n = 9–11/sex/group).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0024003.g004

thumbnail
Table 2. Mean (± SEM) total distance and time in the centre of the OFT and percent time spent struggling during the FST.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0024003.t002

The FST

PSV adolescent offspring spent significantly less time immobile compared to CV and PSF offspring (.02≤p≤.04: condition×treatment effect; F(1, 68) = 7.17, p≤.09, controlling for weight differences; Figure 4B). There were no significant differences between conditions, treatment or sex in amount of time spent struggling in the FST and no other significant main effects or interactions on measures of the FST (0.12≤p≤.90).

Ki67-ir cells

There were no significant differences between groups in the volume of the GCL/SGZ and the hilus of the hippocampus (p>.07), therefore total number of Ki67-ir cell counts were used for statistical analysis. Results demonstrate that PSV adolescent offspring had significantly fewer Ki-67-ir cells in the GCL/SVZ compared to all other groups (.0001≤p≤.01). CF adolescent offspring had significantly more Ki67-ir cells in the GCL/SVZ compared to PSV offspring (p≤.0001), but significantly fewer Ki67-ir cells in the GCL/SVZ compared to CV and PSF offspring (.002≤p≤.04; condition × fluoxetine × region (GCL, hilus) effect; F(1, 32) = 14.58, p≤.0006; Figure 2C), regardless of sex. There was also a significant interaction effect between stress and fluoxetine (F(1,32) = 18.1, p≤.0002), a main effect of stress (F(1,32) = 4.18, p≤.05) and a significant effect of region with more Ki67-ir cells in the GCL/SVZ compared to the hilus (F(1,32) = 466.03, p≤.0001). There were no significant correlations between number of Ki67-ir in the GCL/SVZ and measures on the OFT or FST and no other significant main effects of interactions (0.07≤p≤.92).

DCX-ir cells

PSV adolescent offspring had significantly fewer number of DCX-ir cells in the GCL/SGZ of the hippocampus compared to all other groups (.009≤p≤.03, condition × treatment effect; F(1,32) = 5.97, p≤.020; Figure 2D), regardless of sex. There was a significant negative correlation between number of DCX-ir cells in the GCL/SGZ and time spent in the centre of the OFT in PSV offspring (r = −.83, p≤.003) and a significant positive correlation between number of DCX-ir cells in the GCL/SVZ and amount of time spent struggling in the FST in CF offspring (r = .67, p≤.03). There were no other significant correlations between number of DCX-ir cells in the GCL/SVZ and measures on the OFT or FST and no other significant main effects of interactions (.07≤p≤.84).

Discussion

The results of the present study demonstrate that early exposure to fluoxetine in combination with maternal stress has long-term effects on body weight, depressive-like behavior, and hippocampal neurogenesis in offspring. Our primary findings show that early postnatal exposure to maternal fluoxetine reversed the decrease in immobility in the FST, hippocampal cell proliferation and hippocampal neurogenesis in maternally stressed adolescent offspring. In addition, we found that fluoxetine exposure alone significantly reduced hippocampal cell proliferation in comparison to controls and maternally stressed offspring exposed to fluoxetine. We did not find any differences in pup-directed maternal care with fluoxetine treatment or maternal stress suggesting that our data point to a developmental impact of fluoxetine, in the presence of maternal adversity, on offspring outcomes.

Developmental exposure to fluoxetine reduces weight gain in adolescent offspring

We found that postnatal fluoxetine exposure, regardless of exposure to prenatal maternal stress, significantly decreased post-weaning weight gain in adolescent male and female offspring. Previous research showed that a high dose of in utero fluoxetine, via drinking water to dams, resulted in a decrease of birth weight and also a reduction in weight gain during the pre-weaning period in rats [57]. Moreover, several studies have shown that postnatal treatment of fluoxetine, via injection to offspring, leads to a loss of body weight in adult mice and guinea pigs [34], [58], [59]. A reduction in weight gain during the pre-weaning period may be a result of the involvement of 5-HT in glucoregulation in the hypothalamus [60] such that high levels of 5-HT, as a result of the blockade of 5-HT reuptake by fluoxetine, may inhibit the ingestion of carbohydrates and, as a consequence, lead to weight loss [61].

Prenatal stress and anxiety-like behavior in adolescent offspring

In the present study we found that adolescent male offspring had increased anxiety-like behaviors, as evident by fewer central entries in the open field test, compared to adolescent female offspring. We also report that prenatal stress increased anxiety-like behavior in the adolescent male, but only significantly different in comparison to prenatally stressed or fluoxetine-treated adolescent females. Although further work is needed on the effect of prenatal stress and maternal fluoxetine use on anxiety-like behavior of offspring during adolescence, these data are in partial agreement with work done in prenatally stressed adult offspring. For example, previous research has shown an increase in anxiety-like behavior in prenatally stressed adult male, but not female, offspring [62]. More recently work has also demonstrated an increase in anxiety-like behavior in prenatally stressed male offspring, using a similar maternal stress paradigm as in the present study [63].

We did not find an effect of developmental fluoxetine exposure on anxiety-like behavior in adolescent offspring. Previous work has also shown minimal effects of early fluoxetine exposure alone, via i.p. injections to pups from postnatal day 4–21, on anxiety-like behavior in adult male mice in the light-dark box or open field test [34]. However, more recent work has shown that administration of fluoxetine during gestation, to healthy non-stressed dams, results in increased anxiety-like behavior in adult male rats, as measured on the elevated plus maze [64]. Therefore, it seems likely that the effect of early exposure to SSRIs, on anxiety-like behavior later in life may depend on many factors which include the timing of the SSRI exposure, the timing of the test, and the test used to assess anxiety-like behavior.

We also did not find a marked relationship between anxiety-like behaviors and measures of hippocampal neurogenesis in the adolescent rats. Previous work has demonstrated that hippocampal neurogenesis is associated with anxiety-like behavior in adulthood; Revest et al (2009) demonstrated that transgenic mice with decreased levels of hippocampal neurogenesis had increased anxiety-like behavior [65]. However, more research is needed to determine the role of hippocampal neurogenesis in anxiety-like behavior during development. It should also be noted that the relationship between hippocampal neurogenesis and anxiety-like behavior in adolescent rats may significantly vary compared to that of transgenic adult mice as there are well known strain and species differences in hippocampal neurogenesis [66].

Developmental exposure to fluoxetine reverses the effects of prenatal stress on immobility in adolescent offspring

In the present study we found that developmental fluoxetine exposure to prenatally stressed offspring reversed the decrease in immobility in the FST seen in adolescent offspring exposed to prenatal stress alone, while developmental fluoxetine exposure alone had no significant effect on immobility in adolescent offspring. Previous work on the effects of postnatal SSRI treatment on depressive-like behavior in offspring has shown that i.p. injection of SSRIs to offspring during development, in the absence of maternal stress, leads to increased immobility in the FST during adulthood [33], [67]. Others have shown that oral fluoxetine administration to the dam during pregnancy and lactation increases immobility in the forced swim test during adolescence (P30) and adulthood (P70) in female mouse offspring [68]. Discrepancies between our findings and others may be due to the timing and dose of fluoxetine administration, the species tested, and when, after weaning, animals were tested. For example, Lisboa et al (2007) found that fluoxetine exposure (7.5 mg/kg), via oral gavage, to mouse dams during pregnancy and lactation resulted in increased depressive-like behavior in female mouse offspring during adolescence, where as we administered fluoxetine (5 mg/kg) to rat dams during lactation only and tested rat offspring during adolescence. Therefore, the developmental impact of SSRIs may also depend on when during development the exposure occurred. It is also possible that other tests of depressive-like behavior, such as the sucrose preference test, may provide more insight in to the effects of stress and/or SSRIs on offspring behavior.

Although, the exact mechanisms by which fluoxetine counteracts the decrease in immobility in prenatally stressed adolescent offspring is not known, considerable evidence suggests that prenatal maternal stress programs the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis as well as behavior, and that plasticity of the developing monoamine system in the brain underlies, in part, these changes [11], [69]. Furthermore, prenatal exposure to fluoxetine can alter HPA function [5], [36], [70], and thus may act to ‘regulate’ physiological systems impacted by early exposure to maternal adversity.

Developmental exposure to fluoxetine increases hippocampal neurogenesis in prenatally stressed adolescent offspring

In the present study postnatal fluoxetine exposure to maternally stressed offspring reversed the decrease in hippocampal neurogenesis evident after prenatal stress. In addition, postnatal fluoxetine exposure alone decreased hippocampal cell proliferation but had no effect on hippocampal neurogenesis. During adulthood, chronic fluoxetine treatment can significantly upregulate hippocampal neurogenesis [71], [72]. However, our data suggests that developmental exposure to fluoxetine reverses the decrease in hippocampal cell proliferation and hippocampal neurogenesis in prenatally stressed offspring and returns the levels of hippocampal neurogenesis back to those of control animals. Interestingly, these data point to a long-term impact of developmental exposure to fluoxetine on hippocampal neurogenesis which are dependent on exposure to maternal adversity. Whether these changes in hippocampal cell proliferation and production of immature neurons impact hippocampal circuitry and behavioral correlates remains to be determined. Further work is also needed to investigate the persistence of the effects of maternal adversity and developmental exposure to fluoxetine on hippocampal neurogenesis in adult offspring.

The mechanism behind the effects of SSRI exposure on the developing hippocampus has yet to be determined, but developmental exposure to SSRIs have been reported to affect the developing serotonergic system [73], [74], and BDNF levels in the hippocampus [34]. For example, postnatal citalopram treatment, via subcutaneous injections to the pups (P8–21) can lead to a decrease in the serotonin transporter levels in the hippocampus of rat offspring [73]. In addition, postnatal treatment with SSRIs, via i.p. injections to the offspring, can lead to upregulation of BDNF mRNA in the hippocampus [34]. Thus exposure to SSRIs during development may act to alter hippocampal neurogenesis through its actions on many systems of the developing brain.

Our data also demonstrates that the action of fluoxetine on the hippocampal neurogenesis varies in the presence of maternal stress. Although most research to date has investigated the developmental impact of SSRIs in offspring of healthy mothers, one study has shown that early treatment with fluoxetine may act to ‘correct’ the effect of maternal stress on neuron morphology [36]. In this work Ishiwata et al (2005) demonstrated that postnatal SSRI administration, via oral administration of fluoxetine to pups, reverses the prenatal stress induced reduction in CA3 spine density at 3 and 9 weeks of age but SSRI treatment alone, in the absence of maternal stress, had no long-term effect on spine density measures in the CA3 region of the hippocampus of offspring [36]. As mentioned previously, it is likely that developmental exposure to fluoxetine in offspring exposed to maternal adversity, may act to regulate the HPA axis and thus ‘normalize’ the effect of glucocorticoids on hippocampal plasticity in prenatally stressed offspring. Further work is needed to investigate the mechanism of fluoxetine action on the developing brain in response to maternal adversity.

Conclusions

A growing number of children are exposed to SSRI medications during perinatal development [5], yet our knowledge of the long-term impact of this drug exposure is limited. Findings from our work show that developmental exposure to maternal fluoxetine, in combination with exposure to prenatal maternal stress, reverses the effects of prenatal stress on depressive-like behavior and hippocampal neurogenesis in adolescent offspring. Thus, there may be a potential beneficial role of developmental exposure to fluoxetine in the presence of maternal adversity. However, before conclusions can be made much more work is needed not only in models of maternal adversity, but using other popular SSRIs, serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs) and psychotropic medications being used to treat mood disorders during pregnancy and postpartum [32].

In conclusion, further preclinical work is needed to understand the long-term implications of developmental exposure to SSRIs and other antidepressant medications in the presence of maternal adversity before conclusions can be made about the use of antidepressant medications to treat maternal depression during the perinatal period.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the technical help from Dr. Thierry Charlier, Julia Vennemeier, Therese Alich, Hellen Steinbusch, and Denise Hermes.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: JLP DLvdH. Performed the experiments: JLP DLvdH IR. Analyzed the data: JLP IR. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: HWS JP. Wrote the paper: JLP IR.

References

  1. 1. Almond P (2009) Postnatal depression: a global public health perspective. Perspect Public Health 129: 221–227.
  2. 2. Leung BM, Kaplan BJ (2009) Perinatal depression: prevalence, risks, and the nutrition link–a review of the literature. J Am Diet Assoc 109: 1566–1575.
  3. 3. Limlomwongse N, Liabsuetrakul T (2006) Cohort study of depressive moods in Thai women during late pregnancy and 6–8 weeks of postpartum using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS). Arch Womens Ment Health 9: 131–138.
  4. 4. Marcus SM (2009) Depression during pregnancy: rates, risks and consequences–Motherisk Update 2008. Can J Clin Pharmacol 16: e15–22.
  5. 5. Oberlander TF, Gingrich JA, Ansorge MS (2009) Sustained neurobehavioral effects of exposure to SSRI antidepressants during development: molecular to clinical evidence. Clin Pharmacol Ther 86: 672–677.
  6. 6. Talge NM, Neal C, Glover V (2007) Antenatal maternal stress and long-term effects on child neurodevelopment: how and why? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 48: 245–261.
  7. 7. Huizink AC, Robles de Medina PG, Mulder EJ, Visser GH, Buitelaar JK (2003) Stress during pregnancy is associated with developmental outcome in infancy. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 44: 810–818.
  8. 8. Niederhofer H, Reiter A (2004) Prenatal maternal stress, prenatal fetal movements and perinatal temperament factors influence behavior and school marks at the age of 6 years. Fetal Diagn Ther 19: 160–162.
  9. 9. Van den Bergh BR, Mennes M, Oosterlaan J, Stevens V, Stiers P, et al. (2005) High antenatal maternal anxiety is related to impulsivity during performance on cognitive tasks in 14- and 15-year-olds. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29: 259–269.
  10. 10. Laplante DP, Barr RG, Brunet A, Galbaud du Fort G, Meaney ML, et al. (2004) Stress during pregnancy affects general intellectual and language functioning in human toddlers. Pediatr Res 56: 400–410.
  11. 11. Glover V, O'Connor TG, O'Donnell K (2010) Prenatal stress and the programming of the HPA axis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35: 17–22.
  12. 12. Smith JW, Seckl JR, Evans AT, Costall B, Smythe JW (2004) Gestational stress induces post-partum depression-like behaviour and alters maternal care in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29: 227–244.
  13. 13. O'Mahony SM, Myint AM, van den Hove D, Desbonnet L, Steinbusch H, et al. (2006) Gestational stress leads to depressive-like behavioural and immunological changes in the rat. Neuroimmunomodulation 13: 82–88.
  14. 14. Zagron G, Weinstock M (2006) Maternal adrenal hormone secretion mediates behavioural alterations induced by prenatal stress in male and female rats. Behav Brain Res 175: 323–328.
  15. 15. Maccari S, Darnaudery M, Morley-Fletcher S, Zuena AR, Cinque C, et al. (2003) Prenatal stress and long-term consequences: implications of glucocorticoid hormones. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 27: 119–127.
  16. 16. Maccari S, Morley-Fletcher S (2007) Effects of prenatal restraint stress on the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and related behavioural and neurobiological alterations. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32: Suppl 1S10–15.
  17. 17. Weinstock M (2008) The long-term behavioural consequences of prenatal stress. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32: 1073–1086.
  18. 18. Coe CL, Kramer M, Czeh B, Gould E, Reeves AJ, et al. (2003) Prenatal stress diminishes neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of juvenile rhesus monkeys. Biol Psychiatry 54: 1025–1034.
  19. 19. Kawamura T, Chen J, Takahashi T, Ichitani Y, Nakahara D (2006) Prenatal stress suppresses cell proliferation in the early developing brain. Neuroreport 17: 1515–1518.
  20. 20. Odagiri K, Abe H, Kawagoe C, Takeda R, Ikeda T, et al. (2008) Psychological prenatal stress reduced the number of BrdU immunopositive cells in the dorsal hippocampus without affecting the open field behavior of male and female rats at one month of age. Neurosci Lett 446: 25–29.
  21. 21. Lucassen PJ, Bosch OJ, Jousma E, Kromer SA, Andrew R, et al. (2009) Prenatal stress reduces postnatal neurogenesis in rats selectively bred for high, but not low, anxiety: possible key role of placental 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2. Eur J Neurosci 29: 97–103.
  22. 22. Fleschler R, Peskin MF (2008) Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) in pregnancy: a review. MCN Am J Matern Child Nurs 33: 355–361; quiz 362–353.
  23. 23. Oberlander TF, Warburton W, Misri S, Aghajanian J, Hertzman C (2006) Neonatal outcomes after prenatal exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor antidepressants and maternal depression using population-based linked health data. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63: 898–906.
  24. 24. Cooper WO, Willy ME, Pont SJ, Ray WA (2007) Increasing use of antidepressants in pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol 196: 544 e541–545.
  25. 25. Ververs T, Kaasenbrood H, Visser G, Schobben F, de Jong-van den Berg L, et al. (2006) Prevalence and patterns of antidepressant drug use during pregnancy. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 62: 863–870.
  26. 26. Homberg JR, Schubert D, Gaspar P (2010) New perspectives on the neurodevelopmental effects of SSRIs. Trends Pharmacol Sci 31: 60–65.
  27. 27. Moses-Kolko EL, Bogen D, Perel J, Bregar A, Uhl K, et al. (2005) Neonatal signs after late in utero exposure to serotonin reuptake inhibitors: literature review and implications for clinical applications. JAMA 293: 2372–2383.
  28. 28. Nulman I, Rovet J, Stewart DE, Wolpin J, Pace-Asciak P, et al. (2002) Child development following exposure to tricyclic antidepressants or fluoxetine throughout fetal life: a prospective, controlled study. Am J Psychiatry 159: 1889–1895.
  29. 29. Pawluski JL, Galea LA, Brain U, Papsdorf M, Oberlander TF (2009) Neonatal S100B protein levels after prenatal exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Pediatrics 124: e662–670.
  30. 30. Oberlander TF, Weinberg J, Papsdorf M, Grunau R, Misri S, et al. (2008) Prenatal exposure to maternal depression, neonatal methylation of human glucocorticoid receptor gene (NR3C1) and infant cortisol stress responses. Epigenetics 3: 97–106.
  31. 31. Oberlander TF, Papsdorf M, Brain UM, Misri S, Ross C, et al. (2010) Prenatal effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor antidepressants, serotonin transporter promoter genotype (SLC6A4), and maternal mood on child behavior at 3 years of age. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 164: 444–451.
  32. 32. Pawluski JL (in press) Perinatal SSRI antidepressant exposure: impact on brain development and neural plasticity Neuroendocrinology
  33. 33. Hansen HH, Sanchez C, Meier E (1997) Neonatal administration of the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor Lu 10-134-C increases forced swimming-induced immobility in adult rats: a putative animal model of depression? J Pharmacol Exp Ther 283: 1333–1341.
  34. 34. Karpova NN, Lindholm J, Pruunsild P, Timmusk T, Castren E (2009) Long-lasting behavioural and molecular alterations induced by early postnatal fluoxetine exposure are restored by chronic fluoxetine treatment in adult mice. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 19: 97–108.
  35. 35. Ansorge MS, Morelli E, Gingrich JA (2008) Inhibition of serotonin but not norepinephrine transport during development produces delayed, persistent perturbations of emotional behaviors in mice. J Neurosci 28: 199–207.
  36. 36. Ishiwata H, Shiga T, Okado N (2005) Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment of early postnatal mice reverses their prenatal stress-induced brain dysfunction. Neuroscience 133: 893–901.
  37. 37. Van den Hove DL, Blanco CE, Scheepens A, Desbonnet L, Myint AM, et al. (2008) Prenatal maternal paroxetine treatment and neonatal mortality in the rat: a preliminary study. Neonatology 93: 52–55.
  38. 38. Romeo RD, Karatsoreos IN, Ali FS, McEwen BS (2007) The effects of acute stress and pubertal development on metabolic hormones in the rat. Stress 10: 101–106.
  39. 39. McCormick CM, Mathews IZ (2007) HPA function in adolescence: role of sex hormones in its regulation and the enduring consequences of exposure to stressors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 86: 220–233.
  40. 40. Lopez-Duran NL, Kovacs M, George CJ (2009) Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysregulation in depressed children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34: 1272–1283.
  41. 41. Romeo RD (2010) Adolescence: a central event in shaping stress reactivity. Dev Psychobiol 52: 244–253.
  42. 42. Romeo RD, McEwen BS (2006) Stress and the adolescent brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1094: 202–214.
  43. 43. Ward IL, Weisz J (1984) Differential effects of maternal stress on circulating levels of corticosterone, progesterone, and testosterone in male and female rat fetuses and their mothers. Endocrinology 114: 1635–1644.
  44. 44. Van den Hove DL, Blanco CE, Aendekerk B, Desbonnet L, Bruschettini M, et al. (2005) Prenatal restraint stress and long-term affective consequences. Dev Neurosci 27: 313–320.
  45. 45. Darnaudery M, Maccari S (2008) Epigenetic programming of the stress response in male and female rats by prenatal restraint stress. Brain Res Rev 57: 571–585.
  46. 46. Gentile S, Rossi A, Bellantuono C (2007) SSRIs during breastfeeding: spotlight on milk-to-plasma ratio. Arch Womens Ment Health 10: 39–51.
  47. 47. Romijn HJ, Hofman MA, Gramsbergen A (1991) At what age is the developing cerebral cortex of the rat comparable to that of the full-term newborn human baby? Early Hum Dev 26: 61–67.
  48. 48. Alahmed S, Herbert J (2008) Strain differences in proliferation of progenitor cells in the dentate gyrus of the adult rat and the response to fluoxetine are dependent on corticosterone. Neuroscience 157: 677–682.
  49. 49. Pawluski JL, Charlier TD, Lieblich SE, Hammond GL, Galea LA (2009) Reproductive experience alters corticosterone and CBG levels in the rat dam. Physiol Behav 96: 108–114.
  50. 50. Prut L, Belzung C (2003) The open field as a paradigm to measure the effects of drugs on anxiety-like behaviors: a review. Eur J Pharmacol 463: 3–33.
  51. 51. Pawluski JL, Lieblich SE, Galea LA (2009) Offspring-exposure reduces depressive-like behaviour in the parturient female rat. Behav Brain Res 197: 55–61.
  52. 52. Reed AL, Happe HK, Petty F, Bylund DB (2008) Juvenile rats in the forced-swim test model the human response to antidepressant treatment for pediatric depression. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 197: 433–441.
  53. 53. Pawluski JL, van den Hove DL, Rayen I, Prickaerts J, Steinbusch HW (2011) Stress and the pregnant female: Impact on hippocampal cell proliferation, but not affective-like behaviors. Horm Behav.
  54. 54. Epp JR, Barker JM, Galea LA (2009) Running wild: neurogenesis in the hippocampus across the lifespan in wild and laboratory-bred Norway rats. Hippocampus 19: 1040–1049.
  55. 55. Balthazart J, Boseret G, Konkle AT, Hurley LL, Ball GF (2008) Doublecortin as a marker of adult neuroplasticity in the canary song control nucleus HVC. Eur J Neurosci 27: 801–817.
  56. 56. Gundersen HJ, Bagger P, Bendtsen TF, Evans SM, Korbo L, et al. (1988) The new stereological tools: disector, fractionator, nucleator and point sampled intercepts and their use in pathological research and diagnosis. APMIS 96: 857–881.
  57. 57. Bairy KL, Madhyastha S, Ashok KP, Bairy I, Malini S (2007) Developmental and behavioral consequences of prenatal fluoxetine. Pharmacology 79: 1–11.
  58. 58. Ansorge MS, Zhou M, Lira A, Hen R, Gingrich JA (2004) Early-life blockade of the 5-HT transporter alters emotional behavior in adult mice. Science 306: 879–881.
  59. 59. McAdam TD, Brien JF, Reynolds JN, Dringenberg HC (2008) Altered water-maze search behavior in adult guinea pigs following chronic prenatal ethanol exposure: lack of mitigation by postnatal fluoxetine treatment. Behav Brain Res 191: 202–209.
  60. 60. Tuomisto J, Mannisto P (1985) Neurotransmitter regulation of anterior pituitary hormones. Pharmacol Rev 37: 249–332.
  61. 61. Leibowitz SF, Alexander JT (1998) Hypothalamic serotonin in control of eating behavior, meal size, and body weight. Biol Psychiatry 44: 851–864.
  62. 62. Zuena AR, Mairesse J, Casolini P, Cinque C, Alema GS, et al. (2008) Prenatal restraint stress generates two distinct behavioral and neurochemical profiles in male and female rats. PLoS One 3: e2170.
  63. 63. Morley-Fletcher S, Mairesse J, Soumier A, Banasr M, Fagioli F, et al. (2011) Chronic agomelatine treatment corrects behavioral, cellular, and biochemical abnormalities induced by prenatal stress in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl).
  64. 64. Olivier JD, Valles A, van Heesch F, Afrasiab-Middelman A, Roelofs JJ, et al. (2011) Fluoxetine administration to pregnant rats increases anxiety-related behavior in the offspring. Psychopharmacology (Berl).
  65. 65. Revest JM, Dupret D, Koehl M, Funk-Reiter C, Grosjean N, et al. (2009) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis is involved in anxiety-related behaviors. Mol Psychiatry 14: 959–967.
  66. 66. Galea LA, Spritzer MD, Barker JM, Pawluski JL (2006) Gonadal hormone modulation of hippocampal neurogenesis in the adult. Hippocampus 16: 225–232.
  67. 67. Noorlander CW, Ververs FF, Nikkels PG, van Echteld CJ, Visser GH, et al. (2008) Modulation of serotonin transporter function during fetal development causes dilated heart cardiomyopathy and lifelong behavioral abnormalities. PLoS One 3: e2782.
  68. 68. Lisboa SF, Oliveira PE, Costa LC, Venancio EJ, Moreira EG (2007) Behavioral evaluation of male and female mice pups exposed to fluoxetine during pregnancy and lactation. Pharmacology 80: 49–56.
  69. 69. Charil A, Laplante DP, Vaillancourt C, King S (2010) Prenatal stress and brain development. Brain Res Rev 65: 56–79.
  70. 70. Morrison JL, Riggs KW, Rurak DW (2005) Fluoxetine during pregnancy: impact on fetal development. Reprod Fertil Dev 17: 641–650.
  71. 71. Malberg JE, Eisch AJ, Nestler EJ, Duman RS (2000) Chronic antidepressant treatment increases neurogenesis in adult rat hippocampus. J Neurosci 20: 9104–9110.
  72. 72. Perera TD, Coplan JD, Lisanby SH, Lipira CM, Arif M, et al. (2007) Antidepressant-induced neurogenesis in the hippocampus of adult nonhuman primates. J Neurosci 27: 4894–4901.
  73. 73. Weaver KJ, Paul IA, Lin RC, Simpson KL (2010) Neonatal exposure to citalopram selectively alters the expression of the serotonin transporter in the hippocampus: dose-dependent effects. Anat Rec (Hoboken) 293: 1920–1932.
  74. 74. Laine K, Heikkinen T, Ekblad U, Kero P (2003) Effects of exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors during pregnancy on serotonergic symptoms in newborns and cord blood monoamine and prolactin concentrations. Arch Gen Psychiatry 60: 720–726.