Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Effect of matcha green tea on cognitive functions and sleep quality in older adults with cognitive decline: A randomized controlled study over 12 months

  • Kazuhiko Uchida ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kazuhiko Uchida, Kohji Meno

    Roles Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    kazuhiko.uchida@cbiri.org

    Affiliations Institute of Biomedical Research, MCBI Inc., Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan, Research Division, MCBI Inc., Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan

  • Kohji Meno ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kazuhiko Uchida, Kohji Meno

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Research Division, MCBI Inc., Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan

  • Tatsumi Korenaga,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Resources

    Affiliation Research Division, MCBI Inc., Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan

  • Shan Liu,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation

    Affiliation Research Division, MCBI Inc., Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan

  • Hideaki Suzuki,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Resources

    Affiliation Research Division, MCBI Inc., Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan

  • Yoshitake Baba,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation Central Research Institute, ITO EN, LTD., Makinohara, Shizuoka, Japan

  • Chika Tagata,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation Central Research Institute, ITO EN, LTD., Makinohara, Shizuoka, Japan

  • Yoshiharu Araki,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation Central Research Institute, ITO EN, LTD., Makinohara, Shizuoka, Japan

  • Shuto Tsunemi,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation Central Research Institute, ITO EN, LTD., Makinohara, Shizuoka, Japan

  • Kenta Aso,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation Central Research Institute, ITO EN, LTD., Makinohara, Shizuoka, Japan

  • Shun Inagaki,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation Central Research Institute, ITO EN, LTD., Makinohara, Shizuoka, Japan

  • Sae Nakagawa,

    Roles Investigation, Resources

    Affiliation Central Research Institute, ITO EN, LTD., Makinohara, Shizuoka, Japan

  • Makoto Kobayashi,

    Roles Investigation, Resources

    Affiliation Central Research Institute, ITO EN, LTD., Makinohara, Shizuoka, Japan

  • Tatsuyuki Kakuma,

    Roles Data curation, Supervision, Validation

    Affiliation Biostatistics Center, Kurume University Graduate School of Medicine, Kurume, Japan

  • Takashi Asada,

    Roles Resources, Supervision

    Affiliations Memory Clinic Toride, Toride, Ibaraki, Japan, Tokyo Medical and Dental University, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo, Japan

  • Miho Ota,

    Roles Investigation, Resources

    Affiliation Institute of Medicine, University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan

  • Takanobu Takihara,

    Roles Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Central Research Institute, ITO EN, LTD., Makinohara, Shizuoka, Japan

  •  [ ... ],
  • Tetsuaki Arai

    Roles Resources, Supervision, Validation

    Affiliation Institute of Medicine, University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan

  • [ view all ]
  • [ view less ]

Abstract

Objective

Lifestyle habits after middle age significantly impact the maintenance of cognitive function in older adults. Nutritional intake is closely related to lifestyle habits; therefore, nutrition is a pivotal factor in the prevention of dementia in the preclinical stages. Matcha green tea powder (matcha), which contains epigallocatechin gallate, theanine, and caffeine, has beneficial effects on cognitive function and mood. We conducted a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical study over 12 months to examine the effect of matcha on cognitive function and sleep quality.

Methods

Ninety-nine participants, including 64 with subjective cognitive decline and 35 with mild cognitive impairment were randomized, with 49 receiving 2 g of matcha and 50 receiving a placebo daily. Participants were stratified based on two factors: age at baseline and APOE genotype. Changes in cognitive function and sleep quality were analyzed using a mixed-effects model.

Results

Matcha consumption led to significant improvements in social acuity score (difference; -1.39, 95% confidence interval; -2.78, 0.002) (P = 0.028) as evaluated by the perception of facial emotions in cognitive function. The primary outcomes, that is, Montreal Cognitive Assessment and Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study Activity of Daily Living scores, showed no significant changes with matcha intervention. Meanwhile, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index scores indicated a trend toward improvement with a difference of 0.86 (95% confidence interval; -0.002, 1.71) (P = 0.088) between the groups in changes from baseline to 12 months.

Conclusions

The present study suggests regular consumption of matcha could improve emotional perception and sleep quality in older adults with mild cognitive decline. Given the widespread availability and cultural acceptance of matcha green tea, incorporating it into the daily routine may offer a simple yet effective strategy for cognitive enhancement and dementia prevention.

Introduction

Dementia, for which aging is the greatest risk factor, is a social burden in an aging society. According to the World Alzheimer Report 2022, the number of dementia patients will increase from 57.4 million in 2019 to 152.8 million in 2050 [1]. Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and related disorders have multifactorial and complex etiologies [2]. Despite advancements in the development of therapeutic agents, no curative therapies are currently available.

Accumulating evidence suggests that lifestyle habits after middle age (approximately 45–54 years-old) significantly impact the maintenance of cognitive function in older adults [35]. Tea intake is closely related to lifestyle habits. Matcha green tea powder (matcha), which contains epigallocatechin gallate (EGCG), theanine, and caffeine, has the beneficial effects of each constituent on cognitive functions and mood [68]. Catechins, key components of green tea, are polyphenols with antioxidant and anti-inflammatory properties. The oral administration of EGCG has been reported to reduce Aβ deposition in the olfactory cortex and hippocampus of AD mice [9]. Theanine has been shown to work synergistically with catechins to alleviate stress and improve sleep in both clinical and animal studies [10]. A cross-sectional observational study revealed that the risk of cognitive decline, indicated by a score of < 26 on the Mini Mental State Examination-Japanese version (MMSE-J), was significantly lower in the group that consumed ≥ 200 ml of green tea per day, indicating a positive correlation between regular green tea consumption and maintained cognitive function [11]. With a cutoff point of 26 in MMSE-J score, the odds ratios for the different frequencies of green tea consumption were 0.42 for 2–3 cups per day, compared with 1.00 (reference) for 3 cups per week. Two to three cups of green tea contain 135–200 mg of catechins, with green tea providing 67.5 mg of catechins per 100 ml [11, 12]. In comparison, in a traditional tea ceremony, Tea Otemae, 2 g of matcha contain 170 mg of catechins.

Randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trials have reported beneficial effects of matcha on cognitive function [6, 7, 13]. However, the intervention periods in these studies were relatively short, lasting only 12 weeks. Longer intervention periods, extending several months or more, are required to comprehensively evaluate the effects of matcha consumption as a lifestyle habit.

Aging causes sleep quality to deteriorate. Approximately 20% of adults in Japan experience sleep disturbance [14, 15], and 50–70 million people have sleep disorders in the United States [16]. Sleep disturbance is a risk factor for AD, and sleep deprivation leads to increased inflammation and amyloid beta (Aβ) burden in the brain [17, 18]. A sleep duration of ≤ 6 h at ages 50 and 60 years, compared with a 7-h sleep duration, was associated with a 30% increased dementia risk in the Whitehall II study [19]. Thus, sleep quality during midlife may contribute to the pathophysiology of cognitive impairment in old age, and improving sleep quality may prevent dementia.

Preventing disease progression and disease onset is crucial for mitigating the prevalence of dementia and other lifestyle-related diseases. Controlling blood pressure and blood sugar levels in midlife is vital for the prevention of cardiovascular disease and diabetes mellitus, respectively. Several cohort studies have identified blood-based biomarkers for dementia risk assessment, including phosphorylated tau-217 [20], α2-macroglobulin [21], apolipoproteins [22], and complement proteins [23]. Indicators of protein nutrition are likely associated with the disease processes in AD [2426]. Llewellyn et al. reported an association between low serum albumin levels and risk of cognitive impairment in older individuals [26]. In our previous studies, a composite marker of apoA1, transthyretin, and complement protein C3 in the serum, which are involved in lipid metabolism, protein nutrition, and inflammation, could differentiate between older adults with cognitive impairment and those without dementia at 74–84% accuracy [27, 28]. Thus, lifestyle habits and related biomarkers are pivotal factors in the prevention of dementia in the preclinical stages.

Here, we conducted a 12-month randomized, double-blind, and placebo-controlled clinical trial to assess the potential of matcha in improving sleep quality and delaying cognitive decline in older adults.

Materials and methods

Study design

A 12-month intervention was executed employing a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group design. Before the initiation of the study, the clinical trial was registered with the UMIN Clinical Trials Registry (UMIN-CTR), which is recognized by the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (ICMJE) as an approved registry. This study was conducted and reported as a randomized controlled trial, adhering strictly to the CONSORT guidelines.

A meta-analysis was conducted to ascertain whether the effect size and statistical power were commensurate with the determined sample size, referencing three intervention studies that employed the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) as the evaluative criterion [2931]. Although the efficacy indices in this study vary slightly from those in previous research, we concluded that observing the anticipated effects outlined in the meta-analysis would render the data from all 66 patients a significant evidence base for preparing the subsequent validation study.

The required sample size was calculated using G*Power 3 software (Heinrich Heine University, Düsseldorf, Germany). We chose to assess the difference between the medians of two independent groups using the Wilcoxon or Mann–Whitney test. With the parameters set as two-tailed, logistic distribution, an effect size of 0.7, a significance level of 0.05, and a power of 0.8, it was determined that each group required 31 cases. Anticipating a dropout rate of 7 of 40 recruited cases per group, we established a target sample size of at least 33 cases per group to validate the robustness of our findings (details are described in S1 File).

Randomization

Sequence generation.

Eligible participants with SCD or MCI were randomized using a computer-generated sequence. Participants were stratified based on two stratification factors, age at baseline (≥ 74 years / < 74 years) and APOE genotype (ε4 positive / negative). Participants were stratified based on an age cut-off of 74-years, because the 10-year AD risk is higher in people ≥ 74 years old than in those < 74 years old [32]. Allocation to either the control or intervention group was intended to be randomized in a 1:1 ratio using computerized minimization, which aims to minimize differences between groups with respect to the stratification factors [33].

Allocation concealment mechanism.

Participants underwent randomization using a dynamic program implemented via a Microsoft Excel macro, employing simple randomization rather than permuted block randomization. Upon running the program, the participants were assigned to either Group A or Group B. However, the program did not reveal which was the intervention or control group. The randomization table created by the program was retained until all baseline stratification factors were fully aligned and participant inclusion was complete, ensuring concealment of the allocation.

All eligible participants who provided informed consent and met the predefined inclusion criteria underwent randomization, a process administered at the discretion of the study site personnel overseeing recruitment and medical interviews. Subsequently, the study site transmitted a stratification factor response form to an independent allocation manager who was responsible for the allocation process, ensuring separation from the outcome evaluation analysis. The allocation manager utilized a dynamic randomization program incorporating provided stratification factors to assign participants to their respective groups based on the program’s output. Physicians and nurses responsible for participant inclusion and symptom assessment remained blinded to group assignments.

The allocation sequence utilized a stratified approach, considering study site and medication compliance, employing a dynamic allocation method with the minimization technique. Concealment of randomization details from data management personnel and statistical analysts was upheld throughout the study while ensuring the database remained publicly accessible. The randomization list was securely maintained by designated allocation personnel throughout the study, mitigating any potential influence from principal investigators involved in the research.

Blinding.

Throughout the study duration, participants were unaware of whether the capsules they consumed contained matcha or placebo until the study’s conclusion was obtained. Physicians, nurses, and principal investigators at the study site were informed that the participants had been assigned to provisional groups (Groups A and B) yet remained uninformed about the specific allocation to either the matcha or placebo group until the randomization list was released after the study period. No member of the research team knew the allocated sequence until the end of the statistical analysis, after the database was locked.

Participants

Community-dwelling older adults aged 60–85 years were recruited and assessment for eligibility was performed at the University of Tsukuba Hospital (Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan) and Memory Clinic Toride (Toride, Ibaraki, Japan) (Fig 1). Participants were enrolled based on predefined inclusion and exclusion criteria as follows: (1) Japanese-speaking men and women aged 60–85 years living with a study partner responsible for checking capsule intake and escorting visitors to the hospital, (2) presence of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or subjective cognitive decline (SCD) assessed as described in the study design and participants, (3) no history of treatment for any serious disease (cancer, myocardial infarction, stroke) within 5 years; (4) no dementia treatment (donepezil, galantamine, rivastigmine, or memantine); (5) no brain atrophy seen in dementia on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI); (6) no participation in daycare interventions such as exercise intervention or cognitive training; and (7) no history of interventions such as exercise intervention or cognitive training. The exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) diagnosed with dementia or major depressive disorder at baseline; (2) MMSE-J score < 24; (3) history of therapeutic inpatient surgery due to a stroke, subarachnoid hemorrhage, or other head injury; (4) using medications that may affect this study (e.g., antipsychotics, anxiolytics, antidepressants); and (5) regular consumption of functional foods or more than seven cups of matcha per week that may affect the maintenance of cognitive function.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Study design and flowchart of participants throughout the study.

MCI, mild cognitive impairment; SCD, subjective cognitive decline.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.g001

In this study, we included patients with SCD and those with MCI to evaluate the effects of matcha consumption in the pre-stage of cognitive impairment. Although SCD is not classified as a disease, an increasing number of studies have suggested an association between SCD and an elevated risk of developing cognitive impairment [34, 35].

Participants with SCD and MCI were randomly divided into matcha and placebo groups (see Materials and Methods in S1 File). No member of the research team knew the allocated sequence until the end of the statistical analysis, after the database was locked. Clinical information, including neuropsychological test data, was collected, and managed using the Alliance Clinical Research Supporting System (ACReSS) V01L51 provided by the University Hospital Clinical Trial Alliance (UHCT) in Japan.

Neurocognitive tests were performed before and after the intervention (baseline and 12 months) and 6 months after the intervention. Amyloid PET was performed at the baseline and 12 months after the intervention in 12 participants each in the matcha group and placebo group. Written informed consent was obtained from all the eligible participants. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and all protocols used in this study were approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee of the University of Tsukuba, Memory Clinic Toride, and MCBI. Prior to inclusion in the study, written informed consent was obtained from each participant, and all samples were rendered anonymous before being sent to the laboratory for analysis. We actively embraced the principles of diversity, equity, and inclusion (DEI) throughout the entirety of our research process. Our commitment to DEI was reflected not only in our team composition but also in our study design, execution, and interpretation of results. Each participant was treated with respect, dignity, and fairness, and every effort was made to ensure that the research process was culturally competent and responsive to the needs of the diverse participants.

Procedures for diagnosis

The MCI and SCD criteria were applied to individuals without dementia after the conference. The team reviewed functional, medical, neurological, psychiatric, and neuropsychological data to reach a diagnostic consensus on dementia and MCI according to the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) criteria. SCD was defined according to recent criteria [36].

Consistent with the standard criteria, for all subtypes of MCI described below, the participants considered to have MCI were required to have (1) objective impairment in one cognitive domain based on the average of scores on neuropsychological measures within that domain, and 1 standard deviation (SD) and 1.5 SD cutoffs derived from normative corrections for age, years of education, and sex; (2) essentially preserved activities of daily living; (3) presence of memory complaints; and (4) no diagnosis of dementia by group consensus.

The participants labeled as having SCD were required to have (1) no diagnosis of MCI or dementia; (2) subjective complaints of forgetfulness; (3) decline in cognitive functioning unrelated to an event explaining the cognitive deficits; (4) Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) global score 0 and (5) MMSE-J score between 27 and 30, mild hippocampal atrophy on MRI, or mild regional cerebellar blood flow (rCBF) reduction on single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT).

Intervention and outcome

The participants were required to consume nine capsules of matcha (equivalent to 2 g of matcha) or placebo daily at any time. As the amount of matcha in the traditional tea ceremony, Tea Otemae, is 2 g, the daily intake quantity of matcha was set at 2 g in this study. The matcha used, “Hojin no shiro” (provided by ITO EN, LTD., Tokyo, Japan) contained 170.8 mg of catechin, 48.1 mg of theanine, and 66.2 mg of caffeine per daily serving. The catechins consisted of 105.3 mg of EGCG, 1.5 mg of gallocatechin gallate, 20.3 mg of epicatechin gallate, 0.1 mg of catechin gallate, 33.1 mg of epigallocatechin, 1.6 mg of gallocatechin, 8.1 mg of epicatechin, and 0.8 mg of catechin. The placebo capsules, which were identical in appearance, color, and odor to the matcha capsules, were composed of cornstarch. Both capsules consisted of white porcine gelatin. A daily supply (nine capsules) was sealed in a sachet, and a large bag containing 1 month’s supply of capsules was provided to the participants every 3 months. During the 3-, 6-, 9-, and 12-month visits throughout the intervention periods, intake compliance was assessed by counting the number of capsules that remained in the bag.

Primary outcomes measures were Montreal Cognitive Assessment-Japanese version (MoCA-J) and Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study Activity of Daily Living (ADCS-MCI-ADL) scores, and secondary outcome measures were MMSE-J, Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status-Japanese version (RBANS), Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-Cognitive Subscale-Japanese version (ADAS-Jcog), CNS vital signs computerized neurocognitive battery, Cognitrax [37, 38], and the Japanese version of the Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) scores.

Cognitive function and sleep quality measures

Cognitive function was assessed using MMSE-J, MoCA-J, ADAS-Jcog, RBANS, and CNS vital signs. CNS vital signs contains 6 neurocognitive domain scores assessed by 10 tests. In this study, we used the stroop test and tests for shift attention, continuous performance, and perception of emotion to evaluate cognitive functioning, based on neurocognitive domain scores of social acuity, reaction time, complex attention, cognitive flexibility, executive function, and simple attention (S1 Table). ADCS-ADL-MCI, with 24 items, was used to describe the participants’ ADL [39, 40]. Sleep quality was measured by PSQI [41].

Neuroimaging

Amyloid PET was performed at the AIC Imaging and Inspection Center in Tsukuba, Japan. Scans were performed using a TruePoint Biograph-PET-CT system (Siemens AG, Munich, Germany). 18F-Florbetapir (Fujifilm, Tokyo, Japan) were used as imaging agents. Transmission imaging (CT imaging for attenuation correction) was performed for approximately 30 s, and emission imaging (PET imaging) was performed in ListMode for 20 min. The standard uptake ratio (SUVR) was calculated from acquired PET images.

The voxel-based specific regional analysis system for atrophy detection (VSRAD) approach in MRI allows the evaluation of atrophy in specific regions of interest (ROIs) within medial temporal structures, particularly the entorhinal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala. Each processed segmented image was compared with the mean and standard deviation of gray matter or white matter images derived from 80 healthy (40 male and 40 female, aged 54 and 86 years) controls. The severity of atrophic changes was calculated using the Z-score, defined as follows: Z-score = ([control mean]–[individual value]) / (control standard deviation). Each voxel had dimensions of 2 × 2 × 2 mm. The severity of ROI atrophy was determined by using the following equation: sum of positive Z-scores in the target ROI/total number of voxels in the target ROI. ROI atrophy was categorized into four states: 0–1 representing normal, 1–2 mild, 2–3 moderate, and > 3 severe, based on the reference literature [42].

The rCBF was evaluated using SPECT. The Z-score also allowed comparisons of the SPECT scans of participants and age-matched normal controls. Statistical analysis of the perfusion reduction was conducted. Severity perfusion reduction in (posterior cingulate gyrus, precuneus, and parietal cortex) was calculated using the following equation: severity = sum of positive Z-scores in the target ROI / total voxels in the target ROI [43].

Serum and plasma sampling

Serum and EDTA plasma stored at −80°C were transferred to the laboratory for analysis and thawed on ice; 40-μl aliquots were generated and stored at −80°C for immunoassay. Almost all serum samples were frozen and thawed twice, whereas some were frozen and thawed three times. Freezing and thawing up to three times did not affect the results of the immunoassay.

Immunoassay

Plasma Aβ1–40 and Aβ1–42 levels were determined by immunoassay (Euroimmun, Lübeck, Germany). Plasma levels of amyloid precursor protein (APP) and beta-site amyloid precursor protein cleaving enzyme 1 (BACE1), which are involved in Aβ production, were measured using enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA). The human sAPPα assay kit (IBL 27734) and human sAPPβ-w assay kit (IBL 27732) were purchased from Immuno-Biological Laboratories (Gumma, Japan), and BACE-1 ELISA (Euroimmun) was used. The immunoassay kit for brain-derived neurotrophic factor was purchased from R&D Systems (Minneapolis, MN). The blood levels of malondialdehyde (MDA)-modified low-density lipoprotein (MDA-LDL), IL-6, and IL-10 were measured at the Clinical Chemistry Laboratory (BIKEN, Osaka, Japan). Plasma levels of apoA1, transthyretin, and complement protein C3 were determined as previously described [28].

Statistical analyses

The analysis population consisted of all participants, excluding those who met the discontinuation criteria assessed by a physician, discontinued participation in the study, and conformed to the study protocol. We evaluated the effects of the matcha intervention on cognitive function and sleep quality measures using a mixed-effects model that flexibly modeled the variance structure of the effect measures, including intra-individual correlations generated by repeated measurements as follows. Specifically, we employed an unstructured variance that did not presuppose a specific correlation structure and included group effects, timing effects, and group-by-time interaction terms.

Yijt represents the measured value of the evaluation item at time point t (= 1,2,3) for participant I who was randomly assigned to group j (= 1,2). We assume the following mixed-effects model:

α is the intercept, βj is the group effect, γt is the timing effect, θjt is the group and timing interaction, εijt is the random error mean following a normal distribution with zero variance structure

The primary hypothesis to be examined is a two-group comparison of the mean change between baseline and time point 3 (and time point 2). We denote this hypothesis as . Here, δt(1–3) is the group difference in the mean change between baseline and time 3, defined as . The two-group difference in mean change between baseline and time point 2 is defined as δt(1−2).

We also estimated the change (mean difference) by time period, Δ(BL, 6M) = 6M –baseline or Δ(BL, 12M) = 12M-baseline. The difference between groups was defined by the following equation to estimate the difference in means (Estimate) between groups. Estimate ((difference of means A6M –A0M)–(difference of means B6M –B0M)), or Estimate ((difference in means A12M –A0M)–(difference in means B12M –B0M)). A t-test was conducted with the null (H) and alternative (K) hypotheses. H: δ = 0, K: δ≠0. The null hypothesis is “no variation, no intervention effect” and the null hypothesis is rejected, which is “there is an intervention effect”. In a mixed-effects model, if an intervention effect is found, there is at least a timing effect and a group effect, and an interaction between timing and group is also found. If δ is negative, there is an interaction between time and group, which indicates is an intervention effect of matcha intake, meaning higher values in the matcha group.

For biomarker analysis, differences between the intervention and non-intervention groups were tested using the Mann–Whitney U test. The associations between the cognitive tests were analyzed using Spearman’s correlation coefficients. P-value of ≤ 0.05 was deemed significant.

Results

Participants

As shown in Fig 1, we recruited 939 community-dwelling older adults aged 60–85 years and 124 participants were enrolled (from May 30, 2019 to August 31, 2020) according to the inclusion and exclusion criteria at the University of Tsukuba Hospital (Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan) and Memory Clinic Toride (Toride, Ibaraki, Japan). A total of 99 participants were randomized and allocated to the matcha group (n = 49) and matched placebo group (n = 50). Age, sex, and APOE genotype were adjusted for. The intervention was initiated from July 2019 to September 2020 and ended from August 2020 to October 2021.

Eight participants discontinued the study due to withdrawal of consent and disease onset, which could have affected continuation of the intervention. Discontinuations due to illnesses included one patient who developed lung cancer, one patient who developed pancreatic cancer, one patient with increased intraocular pressure, one patient with increased transaminases, and two patients with poor physical condition. Two participants withdrew consent.

There were no cases of a causal relationship between harmful events and matcha consumption. The total number of completed questionnaires was 46 in the matcha group and 45 in the placebo group. During the intervention, each participant with MCI in the matcha and placebo groups was diagnosed with dementia at the 12-month evaluation. After excluding these two participants, the per-protocol analysis of the intervention effects included 45 and 44 participants in the matcha and placebo groups, respectively. According to the intention-to-treat principle, we analyzed the changes in outcome variables from baseline to 12 months were statistically analyzed using a mixed-effects model, with 49 and 50 participants in the matcha and placebo groups, respectively.

Baseline characteristics of participants

All the baseline characteristics of biochemical blood tests and neuroimaging data of the participants, including APOE genotype, plasma Aβ, MRI, SPECT, amyloid PET, and Aβ sequester protein [27, 28] levels, are shown in Tables 13 and S2 Table.

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic characteristics of participants in the matcha and placebo groups at baseline.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Brain atrophy and rCBF levels of participants in matcha and placebo groups at baseline.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Amyloid PET of participants in matcha and placebo groups at baseline.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.t003

In amyloid PET, participants > 75 years of age and participants with APOE ε4 were included regardless of age. MMSE-J, GDS, WMS-R, CDR, MRI, SPECT, and biochemical blood test were performed at baseline. APOE genotyping was performed only at baseline, as it was necessary for allocation.

There was no difference in the average age (P = 0.907) and percentage of APOE4 carriers (P = 0.876) between the groups, indicating random assignment. At baseline, all outcomes, including PSQI, cognitive functioning test scores, and ADCS-MCI-ADL scores, showed no difference between the matcha and placebo groups. The average PSQI scores in the matcha and placebo groups were 4.0 and 5.0, respectively, with no significant differences between the groups. Vitamin B12 and folic acid levels at baseline were similar between the groups (S2 Table). There were no significant differences in brain atrophy or rCBF at baseline. Amyloid PET was performed on 24 (9 male and 15 female) participants, harboring APOE4 genotype at baseline and 12 months, with no significant differences between the groups at baseline (Table 3).

Effect of 12-month matcha consumption on sleep quality and cognitive function

Intake compliance was assessed by counting the number of capsules that remained in the bag at the 3-, 6-, 9-, and 12-month visits. The intake rates were 98–99% throughout the intervention periods. We also verified matcha intake during the intervention period by measuring theanine levels in red blood cells. The matcha group demonstrated a significant increase in theanine levels (P = 1.9E-04), which was not observed in the placebo group. This means the participants consumed matcha and placebo capsules correctly and consistently throughout the intervention period.

For the intention-to-treat analysis of each outcome, we tested the intervention effects using mixed-effects models, with 46 and 45 participants in the matcha group and placebo groups, respectively. The model equation consisted of a time effect (baseline, 6 months, and 12 months), group effect (matcha and placebo), and interaction between time and group (see Methods and S1 File). As shown in Table 4, sleep quality was measured using the PSQI, and cognitive function was assessed using the MMSE-J, MoCA-J, ADAS-Jcog, RBANS, and CNS vital signs.

thumbnail
Table 4. Effect of 12-month matcha intervention on sleep quality and cognitive functioning.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.t004

The effect of match consumption on the outcomes was evaluated at 12 months. Social acuity in the neurocognitive domain scores of CNS vital signs showed significant improvement (difference; -1.39, 95%CI; -2.78, 0.002) (P = 0.028) and PSQI scores differed by 0.86 between the groups, indicating a trend of an improvement in sleep quality (95% CI; -0.002, 1.71) (P = 0.088) by 12-month matcha consumption (Table 4). Other cognitive functions including the primary outcomes (MoCA-J and ADCS-MCI-ADL) showed no significant changes in the matcha group compared with the placebo control group. We obtained results from both intent-to-treat and per-protocol analyses. The per-protocol analysis showed that differences in social acuity and PSQI scores between the groups were -1.44 (95% CI; -2.88, 0.001; P = 0.077) and 0.89 (95% CI; -0.001, 1.74; P = 1.08), respectively (S3 and S4 Tables).

Since the score for social acuity was assessed by the perception of emotion, we analyzed the outcomes of each cognitive test performed in CNS vital signs (Fig 2). Commission errors were reduced in the matcha group compared with the placebo group (difference; 1.16, 95% CI; 2.5E-03, 2.31) (P = 0.035) in the perception of emotions. In the shifting attention test, the simple reaction time of the matcha group seemed to be shortened at 12 months compared with that at the baseline (difference; -48.93, 95% CI; -97.91, 0.06) (P = 0.068) in the placebo group. In the Stroop test, the matcha group demonstrated a reduction in reaction time at 12 months compared with that in the baseline, whereas the placebo group exhibited virtually no change.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Effect of 12-month matcha consumption on neurocognitive domain score in CNS vital signs.

The intervention effects on neurocognitive domain score in CNS vital signs are shown. Differences in the mean change from baseline to 12 months between the two groups were calculated using the mixed-effects model. Black circles represent the matcha group, and open circles represent the placebo group. The tables provided below represent the estimated differences between the groups; a positive value implies a higher value in the matcha group than in the placebo group, whereas a negative value suggests a lower value in the matcha group than in the placebo group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.g002

Although there was no significant improvement in cognitive function as evaluated by conventional cognitive functioning tests used in clinical diagnosis (Fig 3), 12-month matcha consumption improved social acuity, as assessed by the neurocognitive domain scores of CNS vital signs (Fig 2). Accordingly, we analyzed the association between social acuity and conventional cognitive functioning test scores in all participants and the stratified groups. At both baseline and 12 months, significant positive correlations were observed between the social acuity score and both MoCA-J and MMSE-J scores, whereas negative correlations were observed between the social acuity score and ADAS-Jcog score. No correlation was observed between social acuity and ADCS-MCI-ADL scores. Stronger correlations between the social acuity score and MoCA-J were observed in participants with SCD than in those with MCI and in the APOE4 noncarriers than in the APOE4 carriers. The social acuity scores of all participants were significantly correlated with the MoCA-J, MMSE-J, ADCS-MCI-ADL, ADAS-Jcog, and RBANS scores. However, in the MCI group, there was no significant correlation between social acuity and cognitive test scores. Conversely, in the SCD group, social acuity scores were significantly correlated with the MoCA-J, ADCS-MCI-ADL, and ADAS-Jcog scores.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Effect of 12-month matcha consumption on sleep quality and cognitive function.

The intervention effects on PSQI, ADCS-MCI-ADL, cognitive function assessed by MMSE-J, MoCA-J, ADAS-Jcog, and RBANS are shown. Differences in the mean change from baseline to 12 months between the two groups were calculated using the mixed-effects model. Black circles represent the matcha group, and open circles represent the placebo group. The tables provided below represent the estimated differences between the groups; a positive value implies a higher value in the matcha group than in the placebo group, whereas a negative value suggests a lower value in the matcha group than in the placebo group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.g003

Neuroimaging biomarkers

The amyloid PET SUVR displayed no difference from the baseline and at 12 months in either group (Table 5). No significant changes were observed in hippocampal atrophy or rCBF levels after 12 months of matcha consumption.

thumbnail
Table 5. Effect of 12-month matcha intervention on neuroimaging.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.t005

Discussion

In this study, unbiased randomization was conducted, and the trial was implemented as a triple-blind study with blinding for the intervention and placebo groups as well as the statistical analyst, thus enhancing the reliability of the study. In the current study, amyloid PET measurements using 18F-Florbetapir were performed in 24 (9 male and 15 female) participants who were APOE4 carriers. There were no biases in age, sex, APOE genotype, and amyloid burden between the intervention and non-intervention groups.

Present findings suggest that the consumption of matcha enhances certain cognitive functions, such as facial expression recognition and attention, and improves sleep quality, which are beneficial for maintaining cognitive function in older adult. Matcha consumption may be considered as a lifestyle improvement strategy for dementia prevention.

The PSQI total score indicated a trend toward improved sleep with matcha consumption. Despite the presence of caffeine, which disrupts sleep, matcha demonstrated a sleep-enhancing effect. This beneficial effect is attributed to theanine, a constituent of matcha. A crossover study by Hidese et al. on healthy participants with an average age of 48 years revealed that theanine intake (200 mg/day) for 4 weeks significantly reduced the total PSQI score [44]. As the change in the total score in the matcha group compared with that in the placebo group showed a decreasing trend (P = 0.073), theanine was suggested to improve sleep quality. Furthermore, Ota et al. reported that in patients with schizophrenia with an average age of 42 years, theanine (250 mg/day) intake for 8 weeks alongside standard treatment led to a significant decrease in total PSQI scores [45]. Thus, while theanine intake has been demonstrated to enhance sleep, this study provides the first report of the sleep-improving effects of matcha.

Substantial evidence suggests a close relationship between sleep and cognitive function [4648]. Brachem et al. reported that in a longitudinal study of local residents aged 50–80 years, tracked for an average of 5.2 years, the risk of developing MCI was significantly increased (relative risk (RR) = 1.43, 95% CI; 1.12, 1.82) when the total PSQI score exceeded 5 points, as compared with those scoring 5 or less. Notably, in patients with MCI suffering from sleep disturbances (PSQI ≥ 5), significant improvement has been reported in the total PSQI and MoCA-J scores after 6 months of continuous positive airway pressure therapy in conjunction with donepezil administration [49]. These results suggest that enhancement of sleep quality could improve cognitive function.

In this study, a significant enhancement in social cognition was observed alongside an improvement in the total PSQI scores. It has been postulated that matcha may improve sleep quality and prevent or ameliorate cognitive decline. Thus, improvements in sleep quality associated with matcha intake may lead to cognitive function enhancement, even without the implementation of full-fledged sleep therapy. In the placebo group, a certain level of theanine was detected in the blood cells. This intervention study did not impose any restrictions on green tea intake before or during the study period. This may account for the presence of theanine, which may have been ingested during habitual green tea consumption.

Facial emotion recognition is impaired in patients with SCD and MCI [50, 51]. A cross-sectional analysis of 4,039 participants revealed that recognition of the emotion of facial expressions is affected at an early stage of cognitive impairment [52]. In the present study, a significant enhancement in social cognition was observed following matcha consumption. Social cognition is related to the perception of emotions, one of the tests of CNS vital signs, and the incidence of false responses was reduced by matcha consumption. False responses in the perception of emotions test pertained to inaccurate responses to facial expressions and descriptions (word meanings) administered during the test. A decrease in false response scores suggests that participants were more proficient in recognizing facial expressions and descriptions following 12-month matcha consumption. Social acuity, as evaluated by emotion recognition, was significantly associated with the MoCA-J score in patients with SCD and in APOE4 noncarriers, suggesting that this measure of social acuity may be a suitable test for the early stages of cognitive decline. However, the matcha intervention had no effect on the primary outcomes, MoCA-J and ADCS-MCI-ADL, possibly due to their sensitivity to slight declines in cognitive function. These tests are typically used during clinical diagnoses of cognitive impairment rather than for evaluating early-stage cognitive decline in the disease.

The matcha used in this study was powdered tea leaves. Its cultivation method differs from that of conventional green tea, as the tea leaves designated for matcha production are protected from sunlight prior to harvesting. This alternative cultivation approach increases the content of catechins and theanine, making it potentially superior to green tea in terms of the health benefits attributable to its ingredients. Participants drinking seven cups of matcha were excluded since it is equivalent to consuming 2 g of matcha contain 170 mg of catechins and 48 mg of theanine every day, which is the same amount of matcha as in this intervention study. In terms of catechin content, a single cup of matcha in a traditional tea ceremony, Tea Otemae, is equivalent to 250 mL (2–3 cups) of green tea; however, it should be noted that various types of green tea exist, and not all are directly comparable to matcha. Given the established health benefits of green tea and its constituents, we hypothesized that the increased catechin and theanine contents in matcha could help suppress cognitive decline and improve sleep quality.

The beneficial effects of matcha consumption revealed in this study may contribute to dementia prevention strategies, as these are relatively easy lifestyle changes to implement in daily life. Matcha consumption improved facial emotion recognition, as assessed by a computerized neurocognitive battery, since conventional psychological tests may not be suitable for evaluating cognitive decline in very mild cases. Thus, more sensitive neuropsychological testing, along with intervention strategies that are easy for elderly people to maintain, is necessary to assess the efficacy of these interventions in dementia prevention.

A limitation of the current study is the small number of participants. The observed PSQI score improvement within the intervention group suggests a potential influence of matcha consumption on sleep quality. However, future studies should employ more objective measures beyond the PSQI to comprehensively assess sleep architecture, including quantification of REM and non-REM sleep stages. A comprehensive clinical trial is required for a more robust evaluation of its effects on facial expression recognition. Furthermore, it is crucial to analyze the association between intervention outcomes and biomarkers to enhance our understanding of the mechanisms underlying the effects of matcha. Such investigations would make a significant contribution to the establishment of an effective ecosystem for dementia prevention.

Conclusions

The findings of this long-term intervention study suggest that regular matcha consumption can enhance emotional perception and sleep quality in older adults experiencing cognitive decline.

Supporting information

S1 File. Supporting materials and methods.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.s001

(PDF)

S1 Table. Subsets of neurocognitive test corresponding to neurocognitive domain score used in the matcha intervention study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.s002

(PDF)

S2 Table. Baseline clinical chemistry parameters of participants in the matcha and placebo groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.s003

(PDF)

S3 Table. Effect of 12-month matcha intervention on sleep quality and cognitive functioning in per-protocol analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.s004

(PDF)

S4 Table. Effect of 12-month matcha intervention on neuroimaging in per-protocol analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.s005

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We thank all the participants in this study for their commitment and help in advancing this research. We thank Hitomi Ito and Makoto Inoue for helping to recruit the participants and Akiko Kaito for sample preparation. We also thank Tosie Shinagawa for critical reading of the manuscript. Registration number of present clinical research are UMIN000035658 (UMIN-CTR). The full trial protocol can be accessed in this clinical research registry. The data in this study can be accessed in Dryad Digital Repository (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2280gb61r).

References

  1. 1. Gauthier S, Webster C, Servaes S, Morais J, Rosa-Neto P. World Alzheimer Report 2022: Life after diagnosis: Navigating treatment, care and support. Alzheimer’s Disease International; 2022.
  2. 2. Kivipelto M, Mangialasche F, Snyder HM, Allegri R, Andrieu S, Arai H, et al. World-Wide FINGERS Network: A global approach to risk reduction and prevention of dementia. Alzheimer Dement. 2020;16: 1078–1094. pmid:32627328
  3. 3. Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, Levälahti E, Ahtiluoto S, Antikainen R, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2015;385: 2255–2263. pmid:25771249
  4. 4. Chen H, Dhana K, Huang Y, Huang L, Tao Y, Liu X, et al. Association of the Mediterranean Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension Intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay (MIND) diet with the risk of dementia. JAMA Psychiatry. 2023. pmid:37133875
  5. 5. Spartano NL, Demissie S, Himali JJ, Dukes KA, Murabito JM, Vasan RS, et al. Accelerometer-determined physical activity and cognitive function in middle-aged and older adults from two generations of the Framingham Heart Study. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;5: 618–626. pmid:31660424
  6. 6. Baba Y, Inagaki S, Nakagawa S, Kobayashi M, Kaneko T, Takihara T. Effects of daily matcha and caffeine intake on mild acute psychological stress-related cognitive function in middle-aged and older adults: A Randomized Placebo-Controlled Study. Nutrients. 2021;13. pmid:34067795
  7. 7. Baba Y, Kaneko T, Takihara T. Matcha consumption maintains attentional function following a mild acute psychological stress without affecting a feeling of fatigue: A randomized placebo-controlled study in young adults. Nutr Res. 2021;88: 44–52. pmid:33744591
  8. 8. Lyon MR, Kapoor MP, Juneja LR. The effects of L-theanine (Suntheanine®) on objective sleep quality in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial. Altern Med Rev. 2011;16: 348.
  9. 9. Rezai-Zadeh K, Arendash GW, Hou H, Fernandez F, Jensen M, Runfeldt M, et al. Green tea epigallocatechin-3-gallate (EGCG) reduces beta-amyloid mediated cognitive impairment and modulates tau pathology in Alzheimer transgenic mice. Brain Res. 2008;1214: 177–187. pmid:18457818
  10. 10. Unno K, Hara A, Nakagawa A, Iguchi K, Ohshio M, Morita A, et al. Anti-stress effects of drinking green tea with lowered caffeine and enriched theanine, epigallocatechin and arginine on psychosocial stress induced adrenal hypertrophy in mice. Phytomedicine. 2016;23: 1365–1374. pmid:27765356
  11. 11. Kuriyama S, Hozawa A, Ohmori K, Shimazu T, Matsui T, Ebihara S, et al. Green tea consumption and cognitive function: a cross-sectional study from the Tsurugaya Project. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;83: 355–361.
  12. 12. Khokhar S, Magnusdottir SGM. Total phenol, catechin, and caffeine contents of teas commonly consumed in the United kingdom. J Agric Food Chem. 2002;50: 565–570. pmid:11804530
  13. 13. Sakurai K, Shen C, Ezaki Y, Inamura N, Fukushima Y, Masuoka N, et al. Effects of matcha green tea powder on cognitive functions of community-dwelling elderly individuals. Nutrients. 2020;12. pmid:33256220
  14. 14. Doi Y, Minowa M, Okawa M, Uchiyama M. Prevalence of sleep disturbance and hypnotic medication use in relation to sociodemographic factors in the general Japanese adult population. J Epidemiol. 2000;10: 79–86. pmid:10778031
  15. 15. Kim K, Uchiyama M, Okawa M, Liu X, Ogihara R. An epidemiological study of insomnia among the Japanese general population. Sleep. 2000;23: 41–47. pmid:10678464
  16. 16. Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Sleep Medicine and Research. Sleep disorders and sleep deprivation: An unmet public health problem. Colten HR, Altevogt BM editors. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 2006.
  17. 17. Shokri-Kojori E, Wang G-J, Wiers CE, Demiral SB, Guo M, Kim SW, et al. β-Amyloid accumulation in the human brain after one night of sleep deprivation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115: 4483–4488.
  18. 18. Irwin MR, Vitiello MV. Implications of sleep disturbance and inflammation for Alzheimer’s disease dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18: 296–306. pmid:30661858
  19. 19. Sabia S, Fayosse A, Dumurgier J, van Hees VT, Paquet C, Sommerlad A, et al. Association of sleep duration in middle and old age with incidence of dementia. Nat Commun. 2021;12: 2289. pmid:33879784
  20. 20. Mattsson-Carlgren N, Salvadó G, Ashton NJ, Tideman P, Stomrud E, Zetterberg H, et al. Prediction of longitudinal cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer Disease using plasma biomarkers. JAMA Neurol. 2023. pmid:36745413
  21. 21. Varma VR, Varma S, An Y, Hohman TJ, Seddighi S, Casanova R, et al. Alpha-2 macroglobulin in Alzheimer’s disease: a marker of neuronal injury through the RCAN1 pathway. Mol Psychiatry. 2017;22: 13–23. pmid:27872486
  22. 22. Zuin M, Cervellati C, Trentini A, Passaro A, Rosta V, Zimetti F, et al. Association between serum concentrations of apolipoprotein A-I (apoA-I) and Alzheimer’s disease: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Diagnostics. 2021;11: 984. pmid:34071695
  23. 23. Rasmussen KL, Nordestgaard BG, Frikke-Schmidt R, Nielsen SF. An updated Alzheimer hypothesis: Complement C3 and risk of Alzheimer’s disease—A cohort study of 95,442 individuals. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14: 1589–1601. pmid:30243924
  24. 24. Velayudhan L, Killick R, Hye A, Kinsey A, Güntert A, Lynham S, et al. Plasma transthyretin as a candidate marker for Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;28: 369–375. pmid:22002789
  25. 25. Tien Y-T, Lee W-J, Liao Y-C, Wang W-F, Jhang K-M, Wang S-J, et al. Plasma transthyretin as a predictor of amnestic mild cognitive impairment conversion to dementia. Sci Rep. 2019;9: 18691. pmid:31822765
  26. 26. Llewellyn DJ, Langa KM, Friedland RP, Lang IA. Serum albumin concentration and cognitive impairment. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2010;7: 91–96. pmid:20205675
  27. 27. Uchida K, Shan L, Suzuki H, Tabuse Y, Nishimura Y, Hirokawa Y, et al. Amyloid-β sequester proteins as blood-based biomarkers of cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2015;1: 270–280.
  28. 28. Liu S, Suzuki H, Ito H, Korenaga T, Akatsu H, Meno K, et al. Serum levels of proteins involved in amyloid-β clearance are related to cognitive decline and neuroimaging changes in mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2019;11: 85–97.
  29. 29. Tsolaki M, Kounti F, Agogiatou C, Poptsi E, Bakoglidou E, Zafeiropoulou M, et al. Effectiveness of nonpharmacological approaches in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neurodegener Dis. 2011;8: 138–145. pmid:21135531
  30. 30. Marzolini S, Oh P, McIlroy W, Brooks D. The effects of an aerobic and resistance exercise training program on cognition following stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2013;27: 392–402. pmid:23161865
  31. 31. Sukontapol C, Kemsen S, Chansirikarn S, Nakawiro D, Kuha O, Taemeeyapradit U. The effectiveness of a cognitive training program in people with mild cognitive impairment: A study in urban community. Asian J Psychiatr. 2018;35: 18–23. pmid:29723721
  32. 32. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;396: 413–446. pmid:32738937
  33. 33. Pocock SJ, Simon R. Sequential treatment assignment with balancing for prognostic factors in the controlled clinical trial. Biometrics. 1975;31: 103–115. pmid:1100130
  34. 34. Mitchell AJ, Beaumont H, Ferguson D, Yadegarfar M, Stubbs B. Risk of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older people with subjective memory complaints: meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2014;130: 439–451. pmid:25219393
  35. 35. Pike KE, Cavuoto MG, Li L, Wright BJ, Kinsella GJ. Subjective cognitive decline: Level of risk for future dementia and mild cognitive impairment, a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Neuropsychol Rev. 2022;32: 703–735. pmid:34748154
  36. 36. Molinuevo JL, Rabin LA, Amariglio R, Buckley R, Dubois B, Ellis KA, et al. Implementation of subjective cognitive decline criteria in research studies. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13: 296–311. pmid:27825022
  37. 37. Iverson GL, Brooks BL, Weiss MD, Gualtieri CT, Johnson LG. Clinical usefulness of CNS vital signs for assessing neurocognition in ADHD. Clin Neuropsychol. 2007;21: 686.
  38. 38. Gualtieri CT, Johnson LG. Reliability and validity of a computerized neurocognitive test battery, CNS Vital Signs. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2006;21: 623–643. pmid:17014981
  39. 39. Galasko D, Bennett D, Sano M, Ernesto C, Thomas R, Grundman M, et al. An inventory to assess activities of daily living for clinical trials in Alzheimer’s disease. The Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1997;11 Suppl 2: S33-9. pmid:9236950
  40. 40. Galasko D, Bennett DA, Sano M, Marson D, Kaye J, Edland SD, et al. ADCS prevention instrument project: Assessment of instrumental activities of daily living for community-dwelling elderly individuals in dementia prevention clinical trials. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2006;20: S152. pmid:17135809
  41. 41. Doi Y, Minowa M, Uchiyama M, Okawa M, Kim K, Shibui K, et al. Psychometric assessment of subjective sleep quality using the Japanese version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI-J) in psychiatric disordered and control subjects. Psychiatry Res. 2000;97: 165–172. pmid:11166088
  42. 42. Matsuda H, Mizumura S, Nemoto K, Yamashita F, Imabayashi E, Sato N, et al. Automatic voxel-based morphometry of structural MRI by SPM8 plus diffeomorphic anatomic registration through exponentiated lie algebra improves the diagnosis of probable Alzheimer Disease. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2012;33: 1109–1114. pmid:22300935
  43. 43. Matsuda H, Mizumura S, Nagao T, Ota T, Iizuka T, Nemoto K, et al. Automated discrimination between very early Alzheimer disease and controls using an easy Z-score imaging system for multicenter brain perfusion single-photon emission tomography. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2007;28: 731–736. pmid:17416830
  44. 44. Hidese S, Ogawa S, Ota M, Ishida I, Yasukawa Z, Ozeki M, et al. Effects of L-theanine administration on stress-related symptoms and cognitive functions in healthy adults: A randomized controlled trial. Nutrients. 2019;11. pmid:31623400
  45. 45. Ota M, Wakabayashi C, Sato N, Hori H, Hattori K, Teraishi T, et al. Effect of L-theanine on glutamatergic function in patients with schizophrenia. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 2015;27: 291–296. pmid:25896423
  46. 46. Yaffe K, Falvey CM, Hoang T. Connections between sleep and cognition in older adults. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13: 1017–1028. pmid:25231524
  47. 47. Sun J, Kang J, Wang P, Zeng H. Self-relaxation training can improve sleep quality and cognitive functions in the older: a one-year randomised controlled trial. J Clin Nurs. 2013;22: 1270–1280. pmid:23574290
  48. 48. Xu L, Jiang CQ, Lam TH, Liu B, Jin YL, Zhu T, et al. Short or long sleep duration is associated with memory impairment in older Chinese: the Guangzhou Biobank Cohort Study. Sleep. 2011;34: 575–580. pmid:21532950
  49. 49. Huang H, Li M, Zhang M, Qiu J, Cheng H, Mou X, et al. Sleep quality improvement enhances neuropsychological recovery and reduces blood Aβ42/40 ratio in patients with mild–moderate cognitive impairment. Medicina. 2021;57: 1366.
  50. 50. Pietschnig J, Aigner-Wöber R, Reischenböck N, Kryspin-Exner I, Moser D, Klug S, et al. Facial emotion recognition in patients with subjective cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr. 2016;28: 477–485. pmid:26377027
  51. 51. Weiss EM, Kohler CG, Vonbank J, Stadelmann E, Kemmler G, Hinterhuber H, et al. Impairment in emotion recognition abilities in patients with mild cognitive impairment, early and moderate Alzheimer disease compared with healthy comparison subjects. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;16: 974–980. pmid:19038896
  52. 52. Virtanen M, Singh-Manoux A, Batty GD, Ebmeier KP, Jokela M, Harmer CJ, et al. The level of cognitive function and recognition of emotions in older adults. PLoS One. 2017;12: e0185513. pmid:28977015