Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Person-centred study on higher-order interactions between students’ motivational beliefs and metacognitive self-regulation: Links with school language achievement

Abstract

Variable-centred studies assume that the links between motivation and metacognition with academic achievement are uniform across all students. However, this assumption may not hold and multiple interactions between motivational beliefs and metacognitive self-regulation may occur. To this end, the present study sought to explore these higher-order interactions and their links with school language achievement in a low-performance context. A large sample (N = 1046, 53.14% girls) of Greek secondary school students (M = 13.97, SD = .80) was drawn. Latent profile analyses were deployed to mimic higher-order interactions. Unexpectedly, the results indicated only three distinct well-defined profiles of students’ motivated metacognitive self-regulation, namely exceptional motivation and metacognitive self-regulation (23.3%), adequate motivation and metacognitive self-regulation (48.2%), and minimal motivation and metacognitive self-regulation (28.5%). Incompatible profiles of motivation and metacognitive self-regulation did not emerge, contrary to previous findings suggesting negative higher-order interactions. The BCH method revealed large mean differences in school language achievement between the profiles, adjusting for covariates. Latent multinomial logistic regression indicated that gender and age predicted greater odds of membership to the minimal motivation and metacognitive self-regulation profile. Socio-economic status and spoken language at home predicted less chances of membership to the minimal profile only. Educational interventions are needed to target both motivational beliefs and metacognition to prevent underachievement.

Introduction

Motivation and metacognition have been posited as important components of students’ self-regulated learning (SRL) [13] and have been linked with adaptive student outcomes, such as achievement [47]. However, extant studies make an assumption that may not be tenable; that is, they take a variable-centred approach and assume that the links between students’ motivational beliefs and application of metacognitive strategies are uniform across all sample members [8,9]. In other words, these studies estimate “averaged” separate effects of motivation and metacognition on achievement. In reality though, students in a sample may constitute a heterogenous population and demonstrate different levels of motivation and metacognition that are connected differentially with achievement. However, profiling studies of motivation and metacognitive self-regulation are inconclusive. To address this important gap in extant research, we can deploy a person-centred latent profile analysis that assigns students in groups with similar characteristics and is able to mimic higher-order interactions [10,11] between multiple motivational variables and metacognitive self-regulation.

Some person-centred studies have explored heterogeneity in students’ motivational and metacognitive characteristics and potential connections with achievement [1214]. However, most of these studies use university/college samples. Consequently, their findings do not reflect the motivational and metacognitive processes that take place in secondary school classrooms during adolescence when metacognitive abilities are malleable and subject to developmental spurt [15] and academic motivation usually declines [1618]. Additionally, extant studies were conducted in high-performance contexts (e.g., USA, Germany, China, etc.), as defined by PISA rankings [19], and do not illustrate what occurs in low-performance contexts, such as the Greek one [20] that is the focus of the present study. Finally, there exist studies that explore secondary school students’ motivational or metacognitive strategies separately [21,22], whereas we need to consider both aspects simultaneously to identify potential areas for theoretical and practical educational innovations.

Hence, the present study sought to address these important gaps in the literature by exploring unobserved heterogeneity and higher-order interactions between motivational beliefs and metacognitive strategies of adolescent secondary school students.

Interactions between motivation and metacognition in SRL

Current models of SRL [13,23] integrate both metacognitive processes and academic motivational beliefs and postulate that learners need to activate both in order to be academically successful. Metacognition is broadly defined as “cognition about cognition” and involves cognitive processes that take place on a “meta-level” requiring monitoring and control of cognition [24]. In the present study, the focus is specifically on metacognitive self-regulation which is defined as the regulation of cognitive processing (i.e., planning, monitoring, and regulating strategies) instead of metacognitive knowledge [25] since metacognitive self-regulation is considered a higher-order process that reflects students’ ongoing processing and strategies deployed to improve performance, rather than static general knowledge about the person, task, and strategies [26]. Motivation is broadly defined as the forces that make people act and academic motivation comprises self-beliefs of competence (e.g., self-efficacy), goals (e.g., goal orientations), and intrinsic (e.g., interest) and extrinsic (e.g., incentives) motivation, amongst others [27].

Empirical research has indicated how critical motivation is for metacognition [6,28]. Additionally, other researchers have specified both motivation and metacognition as joint predictors of students’ performance [2932]. Yet, these studies have not captured the complexity of SRL since they have not tested the multiplicative effects of motivational beliefs with metacognition. As Efklides et al. (38) accurately mention, SRL is not only informed by the recursive prediction from motivation and metacognition but also by the interactions between these components. This study adopts Efklides’ [1] MASRL- Metacognitive and Affective Model of SRL- which takes an interactionist perspective. The MASRL is structured at two levels, namely the person-level and the task X person level [33]. The person-level is more generic and encompasses the interactions between students’ relatively trait-like cognitive, metacognitive, motivational, and emotional characteristics, whereas the task X person level involves cognitive, emotional, and motivational regulation while engaging with a task [34]. At each level of the model, it is hypothesised that the postulated components would interact [1]. The present study focuses only on the motivational and metacognitive self-regulatory aspect at the person-level. However, the directional nature of these interactions is under-researched.

Students’ motivational beliefs positively correlate [3537] indicating that students hold multiple motivational beliefs at the same time. Thus, the order of possible interactive effects of motivation with metacognitive self-regulation becomes large and difficult to estimate [38]. Therefore, the present study contributes to theoretical models of SRL by deploying a person-centred latent profile model to explore the higher-order interaction of motivation and metacognitive self-regulation (i.e., self-efficacy X mastery goal X performance goal X intrinsic motivation X extrinsic motivation X metacognitive self-regulation) and to link this with school language achievement.

Related profiling approaches to the study of motivation and metacognitive strategy use

Several studies have explored the interactions of students’ metacognitive self-regulation in terms of metacognitive strategy use and motivational factors. These studies use mostly person-centred approaches to identify profiles of students’ motivational and metacognitive self-regulatory characteristics. Person-centred approaches, such as latent profile analysis, do not accommodate interaction effects in the same ways as conventional regression modelling. Nevertheless, the distinct subgroups/profiles of students’ characteristics mimic higher-order interactions that would have been impossible to disentangle otherwise using regression modelling [10,11,39]. Therefore, person-centred evidence can be interpreted in terms of both heterogenous profiles of and complex interactions between motivational beliefs and metacognitive self-regulation.

Studies having deployed person-centred (i.e., latent profile/ cluster analyses) approaches indicate the possibility of encountering more than three subgroups of students’ motivational and metacognitive self-regulation. Yet, conclusive results cannot be reached since each study offers new insights. Research studies using school-aged student samples have also revealed various emerging profiles. For instance, a study revealed four profiles of motivational and metacognitive self-regulation characteristics ranging from unmotivated learners to maximal learners [13]. Another study explored profiles of motivation and metacognitive strategy use of second language learners and found four profiles of students that were characterised by varying scores on motivational and strategy variables [12].

In the same vein, studies with college/university samples have illustrated significant variation of emerging profiles. For instance, studies have indicated five [40,41] or four [42] profiles. The emerging profiles included a mixture of different characteristics of motivation, metacognitive and cognitive self-regulation with some studies suggesting the existence of both positive and negative interactions. However, a research gap is that all the above studies did not examine the links between the profiles with students’ native language attainment, but focused on specific grades in certain university/college samples. Finally, most studies with university samples had small-to-medium sample sizes, which may be underpowered for profile analyses [43].

There is also preceding evidence that profiled students’ motivational [22,44] and (meta-)cognitive self-regulatory strategies [21,45] separately. However, such profiling evidence is limiting since it does not permit study of how different aspects of academic motivation and metacognitive self-regulation interact in the prediction of language achievement. To this end, the present study sought to address this limitation by including both a range of motivational indicators and metacognitive self-regulation in the profiling to predict school language achievement.

What is interesting, though, is that both secondary school and university studies identified some profiles where students scored higher on motivation but lower on certain metacognitive or cognitive strategies. This suggests that the potential interactions between students’ motivational beliefs and metacognitive self-regulation may be more complicated than initially thought, namely they may be negative. Since the MASRL model does not specify the sign of the interactions [1,46], more research is needed on the complex interactions between motivational beliefs and metacognition to better understand the SRL processes at the general person-level.

School language achievement: A critical outcome

Language is such a necessary ingredient for human communication since our mental representations, learning, and cognition rely heavily on language [47,48]. Young students live in a swiftly changing world where the demands to understand and produce a quantity and variety of written materials are increasing and becoming more complicated [49]. Given these new challenges that students face, they require to acquire new cognitive and metacognitive competencies in order to process linguistic inputs [49]. However, most of the studies profiling students’ motivational and metacognitive self-regulation characteristics have not connected these profiles/ interactions with school first language (L1) attainment, which captures a range of linguistic skills (e.g., abstract writing, text comprehension, essay writing, reading literacy, etc.) that are taught in the secondary school in Greece (Law 21072b/C2). This is a significant gap in the literature that the present study seeks to address.

The nature of the Greek language subject and the educational system

The Greek language subject in the Greek secondary schools is usually taught by specialised language teachers that have completed four-years university education in linguistics/ philology. The educational system is centralised and all students at the same year-groups are taught using the same language textbooks (that are centrally provided) based on the common national curriculum for Greek modern language [20]. The Greek modern language subject is taught between two and three hours per week in the junior high school (Gymnasium) [50] and two to four hours in senior high school (lyceum) [51]. According to official guidelines for teaching the Greek modern language, the main aims are to enhance the linguistic and communicative capabilities of students through reading and comprehending written and oral speech, the development of critical thinking, the understanding of grammatical phenomena, the recognition of text genres, and production of written texts in authentic communication frameworks [50]. Given the above, students’ school language achievement reflects to what extent students had acquired a range of competencies. However, according to PISA 2018 findings, more than 25% of the Greek adolescent students (aged 15 years old) had achieved below the “minimum level of proficiency” that all students should achieve by the end of secondary education, as outlined in the United Nations Sustainable Development Goals [19]. Therefore, it is more crucial to conduct research in this low-performance setting to understand the reasons that drive students’ achievement and underachievement.

The present study

It has become clear that most of reviewed studies were conducted in high-performance countries, with small-to-medium sample sizes, and using university/college student samples. A smaller proportion of extant studies utilised second-language learners. All these facts pose some theoretical and practical limitations in terms of developmental sensitivity and educational value. Hence, it is important to address these limitations by including a range of motivational beliefs (e.g., self-efficacy, mastery and performance goal orientations, intrinsic and extrinsic motivation) in the profiling of metacognitive self-regulation of secondary school students and to link these profiles with school language achievement. To address the potential issue of confounding, we also included a few socio-demographic variable as covariates based on preceding evidence. These covariates were the following. Firstly, gender was included as a covariate since gender differences have been noted in both motivation and metacognition [6]. Secondly, students’ socio-economic status (SES) was also included since it has been associated with achievement gaps [52], but is seldomly included as a predictor of students’ motivational and metacognitive profiles. Thirdly, students’ age was also accounted for given that greater age has been associated with better metacognition [15] but lower academic motivation [16]. Finally, whether students spoke Greek at home (as a proxy of non-native language learners) with their families was also controlled since it has been found to influence students’ achievement [53]. Overall, to guide this study, the following research questions and hypotheses were formulated:

RQ1: How do motivational and metacognitive characteristics co-occur in the same students? How are the interactions between motivational and metacognitive characteristics formed?

RQ2: How do different profiles of common motivational and metacognitive characteristics influence school language achievement?

RQ3: How are the covariates (socio-economic status, sex, age, and (main) language spoken at home) associated with the profiles of motivation and metacognitive self-regulation?

It is hypothesised that more than three profiles of students’ motivational and metacognitive self-regulatory characteristics would emerge (H1). It is expected that one profile would include interactions between high motivation and high metacognitive self-regulation (H2), whilst another profile would include interactions between low motivation and low metacognitive self-regulation (H3). It is possible that another profile including a negative interaction would occur such as low motivation and high metacognitive self-regulation, or the reverse (H4).It is expected that the emergent interactions through the person-centred approach would be linked differentially with school language achievement (H5). Additionally, identifying as a male would be associated with membership to the less motivated and metacognitive self-regulation profiles (H6). Further, we hypothesised that students from higher SES backgrounds would be more motivated and display greater metacognitive self-regulation (H7). Given the ambiguity regarding the association of age (i.e., a proxy of developmental level) with motivation and metacognitive self-regulation profiles, we did not formulate a specific directional hypothesis but only assumed that an association would emerge (H8). Finally, it was expected that speaking Greek at home would be associated with greater likelihood of membership to the more motivated and metacognitively self-regulated profiles since native speakers would probably face less difficulties in the school language lesson (H9).

Materials and method

Participants

The sample of the present study comprise 1,046 Greek secondary school students (Mage = 13.97, SD = .80) from 19 schools. The data collection took place between late 2022 and late April 2023. The sample consisted of 46.86% boys and 53.14% girls. Students come from a range of socio-economic backgrounds but overall were of average SES (M = 6.38, SD = 1.6). The majority of the students spoke Greek at home (94.42%), and were sampled from urban (N = 12), semi-urban (N = 4), and a few rural areas (N = 3). The above demographics are aligned with population estimates according to national averages [54]. The present sample is situated within the developmental stage of adolescence, when significant biological (e.g., puberty, brain development), educational (e.g., transitions from primary to secondary school), and relational (e.g., romantic relationships) changes occur [55]. Thus, the present sample’s developmental characteristics differ from those of previous studies with college students.

Measures

Several scales from the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ) [56,57] were deemed appropriate for adolescents aged ~14 years old and were administered. Additionally, the MSLQ has been utilised in a plethora of studies, translated to different languages (including Greek- [30,58,59], and administered to various populations in different educational and cultural contexts [60]. All MSLQ items are scored using a 7-point rating scale ranging from 1 “not at all true of me” to 7 “very true of me” [57]. The question prompts were adapted to refer to the modern Greek language lessons that students are taught as part of the national curriculum. Item wordings and coding decisions are presented in supplementary materials.

Self-efficacy.

The scale comprises 9 items (see Table in S1 Table) measuring perceptions of competence and performance in the language class [57]. Reliability coefficient indicated exceptional reliability, Cronbach’s α = .91.

Mastery and performance goal orientations.

Two scales were tapping into mastery and performance goals. The mastery goal orientation scale comprises 4 items (see Table in S2 Table) measuring students’ perceptions of the reasons why they engage with the lesson, such as developing competence, curiosity, and challenge [56]. The reliability coefficient indicated adequate reliability, Cronbach’s α = .68. The performance goal orientation scale consists of 4 items (see Table in S3 Table) that measure students’ perceptions of engagement in the modern Greek language lesson for reasons of displaying competence, rewards, and outperforming others [56]. The reliability coefficient indicated good reliability, Cronbach’s α = .78.

Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motives.

The 6-item long task value scale was split into two separate scores because a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) rejected the unidimensional structure. However, a CFA (see results section) confirmed the two-factor structure. Two items that described interest and liking the topics and content of the class (i.e., emotional aspect) were assumed to reflect intrinsic motivation (see Table in S4 Table). Cronbach’s α = .82 indicated good reliability for the intrinsic motivation scale. The other four items that described utility, valuing, and importance of the language class content were assumed to capture the extrinsic motivation aspect (see Table in S5 Table), in line with self-determination theory [61]. Cronbach’s was α = .86, demonstrating a good level of reliability.

Metacognitive self-regulation.

The metacognitive component covers the control and self-regulatory part of metacognition [25]. Metacognitive self-regulatory strategies in this measure include planning, monitoring, and regulating [60]. Planning is operationalised as goal-setting, whilst monitoring is measured using indicators of tracking one’s task-/goal-relevant attention [56]. Finally, correcting one’s cognitive activities is used as an index of metacognitive control [56]. From the original nine items comprising the metacognitive self-regulation, three loaded on the factor very weakly even after reverse-scoring them due to negative content and were, thus, dropped from the scale (see Table in S6 Table). Cronbach’s coefficient of reliability indicated that the scale was reliable, α = .77.

Modern Greek language achievement.

Grades in modern Greek language classes were reported by students. Studies have shown that self-reported grades are very reliable [6264]. The Greek grading system in secondary education is numerical and ranges from one to twenty. Thus, grades in modern Greek language class potentially ranging from one to twenty were utilised.

Covariates.

A few demographics were collected as covariates based on preceding research suggesting appreciable associations between these covariates, motivation, and metacognition.

Gender. A binary variable was used to code students’ gender as male versus female.

Socio-economic status (SES). In this study, the families’ SES was measured using the youth version of the MacArthur scale [65], which is a ladder where students mark where they think their families are standing in the society. The possible values can range from 1 “lowest SES” to 10 “highest SES”.

Age. Students’ age was coded using an ordinal variable ranging from 13 to 16. Age was controlled.

Language spoken at home. A binary variable (1 = yes, 0 = no) was used to code whether students spoke Greek with their family at home.

Current school grade. Students’ current school grade was measured using an ordinal variable reflecting which grade students were studying in and was ranging from 0 “A Gymnasium” to 3 “A Lyceum”. We did not include this variable in the main analyses as a control variable because of overlap with students’ age and due to very possible confounding.

Statistical analyses

Confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) were estimated per scale to explore the internal structure validity of the scales [66]. A weighted composite score was derived per scale by multiplying each item by its factor loading [67]. Afterwards, the six weighted composite variables were z-scored (M = 0, SD = 1) so that they were placed on the same metric and were more comparable [68]. Latent profile analyses (LPA) were estimated based on the z-scored variables. LPA is a probability model that assigns participants into k latent profiles that share similar characteristics on the measures [69]. The LPA model, unlike cluster analysis, allows for a mathematical evaluation of how well each LPA solution fits the data and accounts for both measurement and classification error [43].

To examine the links of profile memberships (including the interactions) with school language achievement, the manual BCH method was deployed since it does not shift the number of latent profiles by including more indicators and can, additionally, accommodate relevant covariates [70,71]. Given a fraction of missing data on covariates, a multiple imputation was conducted in Mplus [72] using all the variables above since full-information maximum likelihood is not available with the BCH method [70]. The full conceptual model is presented in Fig 1.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Categorical latent variable predicting language achievement conditionally on covariates.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.g001

The small sample size at the school-level (N = 19), did not justify multilevel modelling or cluster-robust standard errors since the estimated parameters exceeded the number of clusters [73]. However, intra-class correlation coefficients (ICC) were derived from variance components (no-predictor random-intercept) models to explore the extent to which nesting of students within schools had to be taken into account using alternative approaches. An ICC below .05 would indicate that a multilevel modelling will have few benefits and be unreliable to estimate [74]. In case of an ICC greater than .05, a fixed-effects estimator was chosen to adjust the standard errors and account for unobserved school-level covariates [75,76]. This was achieved by regressing the achievement and the latent categorical variables on 18 dummy variables (19 schools minus 1).

To evaluate how well a proposed CFA model represents the data, the conventional fit indices were utilised. Specifically, CFI and TLI values close to/above .95 along with RMSEA and SRMR values below .06 and .08, respectively, were considered indicators of exceptional fit of the model to the data [77]. Factor score determinacy (FDET), defined as the correlation between the true and estimated factor scores, was also calculated. FDET is an indicator of the uncertainty of the latent factor measurement [78] with values close to 1 indicating less uncertainty and better measurement [79].

For profile enumeration in LPA, multiple fit indices were considered along with the practical interpretability of the profile solutions [80,81]. The Bayesian (BIC) and the sample-adjusted Bayesian information criteria (a-BIC) were evaluated along with the Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin (VLMR), the Lo-Mendell-Rubin (LMR), and bootstrapped (BLRT) likelihood ratio tests that compare the model with the n-1 profiles with the model with n profiles. It is known that the information criteria can keep decreasing with additional profile solutions [43]. Similarly, the likelihood ratio tests can reach statistical significance in the presence of a large sample size [80]. Therefore, the ‘elbow’ plot of the information criteria values can be used to identify the point of diminishing returns similar to a variance explained measure as additional profiles are extracted [69,82]. Additionally, the classification quality was also considered, whereby entropy (E) values above .8 are preferable [69].

All main statistical analyses were performed using the Mplus 8.7 software [79], whilst preliminary data management was performed using Stata 16 [83]. Since the BCH method does not provide effect sizes, Cohen’s d was calculated using Lakens’ (2013) calculator, whereby values equal to .2 are small, values equal to .5 are medium, and values equal to/above .8 are large [84]. Partial eta squared (η2) effect sizes were also calculated for between-profile differences, whereby values equal to .01 are small, .06 are medium, and .14 are large [84].

Procedure

This study’s data collection protocols have received ethical approval by the Psychology and Education Research Ethics committee at the Faculty of Education, University of Cambridge, UK (29/7/2022). Parents/legal guardians of the students provided written informed consent and the students assented to participate. The questionnaires were administered by the lead researcher in schools. Permission to conduct the research in schools was granted by the Greek Ministry of Education (REF: 145640/Δ2/23-11-2022). The authors have no access to information that could identify individual participants during or after data collection.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Descriptive statistics and correlations are presented in Table 1. As seen in Table 1, students simultaneously hold multiple motivational beliefs since the correlations were all positive between the motivational beliefs. Outlier and missing data analyses are presented in the supplementary materials (Text in S1 Text). The intra-class correlation coefficients (ICC) revealed that a multilevel modelling was not justified given the small between-school heterogeneity in motivational beliefs and metacognitive self-regulation. The only exception to this was school language achievement, whereby 15% of school language achievement was attributable to school-level differences.

thumbnail
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations between covariates and outcomes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.t001

Construct validity testing

As seen in Table 2, CFA indicated good construct validity for the scales following some minor modifications by introducing residual correlations according to the modification indices. The parameter estimates of the CFA models are presented in the supplementary materials in S7S11 Tables.

thumbnail
Table 2. Fit indices for internal structure validity analyses (CFA).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.t002

Exploring complex interactions between motivational beliefs and metacognitive self-regulation: A latent profile analysis

To begin with the analyses, a likelihood ratio test was calculated based on the two-profile solution to estimate whether the latent profile indicator variances should be freely estimated across latent profiles or constrained to equal. The LRT(6) = 176.99, p = .001, indicated that the latent profile indicators’ variances should be freely estimated across profiles. Hence, the LPA models were estimated with unequal indicator variances and means. The fit indices for the LPA modelling are presented in Table 3.

From the (a-)BIC values in Table 3, it became clear that more than five profiles should be extracted. However, the VLMR and LMR LRT were in favour of the three-profile solution since the four-profile solution did not differ significantly from the three-profile solution. Given the inconsistent fit indices, the (a-)BIC values were plotted as shown in Fig 2. The ‘elbow’ plot indicated that the point of diminishing returns is reached at three profiles. Further profiles were also extracted but they did not represent theoretically meaningful solutions since they simply splintered the already well-defined three-profile solution. Therefore, the three-profile solution was retained.

thumbnail
Fig 2. ‘Elbow’ plot of BIC and a-BIC values for LPA models with 2 to 5 profiles.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.g002

LPA latent indicator means were plotted (Fig 3) to understand how the variables interacted within and across profiles. It appeared that motivational beliefs went hand-in-hand with metacognitive self-regulation. The analysis showed that the students within each profile had similar levels of motivation and metacognitive self-regulation, whilst profiles where students scored high on motivation but low on metacognitive self-regulation, or the reverse, did not emerge. Thus, it was assumed that the sixth-order interaction between self-efficacy beliefs, mastery and performance goal orientations, intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, and metacognitive self-regulation was positive.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Plot of LPA indicator means; Profile 1: Minimal MMSR (28.5%); Profile 2: Adequate MMSR (48.2%); Profile 3: Exceptional MMSR (23.3%).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.g003

A significant proportion (28.5%) of the students belonged to profile 1 that was characterised by extremely low scores on motivational beliefs and metacognitive self-regulation, and was, thus, called “minimally motivated and metacognitively self-regulated students” (minimal MMSR). Consequently, these students seemed to have extremely low capability to self-regulate their learning. Only 23.3% of the students were scoring nearly 1 SD above the mean of the sample and were named “exceptionally motivated and metacognitively self-regulated students” (exceptional MMSR). These students were expected to be extremely proficient in self-regulating their learning. Finally, the majority (48.2%) of the students appeared to score on the mean of the motivational beliefs and the metacognitive self-regulation scales and were labelled “adequately motivated and metacognitively self-regulated students” (adequate MMSR). In Table 4, the indicator means and standard deviations per profile are presented and compared to gain deeper understanding of how the interactions function. As seen in Table 4, the differences in motivational variables and metacognitive self-regulation were very large between profiles.

thumbnail
Table 4. Motivational and metacognitive self-regulation indicator means and standard deviations across profiles.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.t004

Variation in school language achievement across profiles

Next, the manual BCH method was deployed to explore whether profile membership was predicting achievement in language lessons adjusting for covariates. Given that ICC revealed 15% of the variance in school language achievement was explained by school-level factors, a fixed-effects estimation was implemented.

In the minimal MMSR profile, achievement was the lowest, M = 13.02, SD = 2.89, p < .001. The language grade in the adequate MMSR profile 2 was nearly two units higher with M = 16.05, SD = 2.18, p < .001, whereas mean achievement was the highest M = 18.01, SD = 2.13, p < .001, in the exceptional MMSR profile 3. Adjusting for covariates (Table 5), the mean language achievement between profiles 2 and 3 differed significantly with a large effect size, Z = -3.08, p = .002, Cohen’s d = .90. The mean achievement differed even more between profiles 1 and 2, Z = -2.69, p = .007, Cohen’s d = 1.23. Finally, the effect size of the mean achievement difference between profiles 1 and 3 was even larger, Z = -4.79, p = .000, Cohen’s d = 1.94.

thumbnail
Table 5. School language achievement differences across profiles and latent multinomial regressions predicting profile membership (fixed-effects estimation).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.t005

Using the manual BCH method, multinomial logistic regressions were also estimated to explore the predictive effects of covariates on profile memberships. The BCH method revealed that being male predicted greater odds of membership in the minimal MMSR profile 1 (OR = 3.12, 95% CI 2.05, 4.76) and in the adequate MMSR profile 2 (OR = 1.65, 95% CI 1.12, 2.41) compared to the exceptional MMSR profile. Being older was related to greater odds of belonging to minimal MMSR profile 1 (OR = 1.45, 95% CI 1.06, 1.99) compared to the exceptional MMSR profile, confirming the decline in academic motivation in adolescence. Coming from a household with higher SES was related to lower likelihood of being in the minimal MMSR profile 1 (OR = .80, 95% CI = .70, .92). Speaking Greek language at home was associated only with lower odds of belonging to the minimal MMSR profile 1 (OR = .22, 95% CI .08, .60).

Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the higher-order interactions between students’ language class motivation and metacognitive self-regulation guided by the MASRL model. Additionally, the study aimed to explore how these interactions were linked with students’ language class achievement in the Greek low-performance context. To achieve these objectives, LPA was used to mimic those higher-order interactions and link profile membership with school language class achievement. Hence, the present study contributes to the existing literature in multiple ways that will be described below.

Profiles of motivational beliefs and metacognitive self-regulation: Exploring the form of the interaction hypothesis

The current LPA analyses revealed that Greek students’ motivated metacognitive self-regulation exhibited not only quantitative but also qualitative individual differences. Surprisingly, number of subgroups of students with similar motivational and metacognitive characteristics was smaller than initially hypothesised. Thus, H1 was rejected. In other words, although past studies indicated four or more emerging profiles [4143,85], the current evidence suggests the existence of only three well-defined profiles of motivated metacognitive self-regulation.

In greater detail, the present findings indicated three profiles contained students who exhibited minimal, adequate, and exceptional scores on all indicators, respectively. The results revealed that 28.5% of the students had minimal scores in motivation and metacognitive self-regulation, whilst only 23.3% had exceptional scores on motivation and metacognitive self-regulation in school language lesson. 48.2% of the students had adequate scores across all variables. These findings are indicative of sixth-order interactions that occur in the same students [10,11]. Across the spectrum of different possible values of motivation and metacognitive self-regulation in the school language lesson, the interactions between self-efficacy, mastery and performance goal orientations, intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, and metacognitive self-regulation were synergistic, meaning that greater motivational beliefs were associated with greater planning, monitoring, and regulating cognitive processes in the language lesson.

Regarding the shape of the profiles, the initial hypotheses were that we would identify a profile including the interaction between high motivation and high metacognitive self-regulation, which was confirmed (H2). Another hypothesis was that a profile would emerge including low motivation and low metacognitive self-regulation, which was also confirmed (H3). Surprisingly, a profile including negative interaction between academic motivation and metacognitive self-regulation did not emerge. Thus, our H4 was rejected. This finding contradicts preceding evidence since the reviewed studies demonstrated that emerging profiles could comprise different levels of motivational beliefs and metacognitive strategies. For example, a study [13] found a profile of students whose motivational beliefs were low but their metacognitive knowledge was high. Similarly, another study [41] reported a profile of students with moderate scores on metacognitive strategies but low scores on motivational beliefs. Another study presented a profile with low self-regulation but high motivation [42]. An argument presented in some preceding studies [13] is that low levels of metacognitive self-regulation may be compensated by high motivation. However, the present study did not find any support for that argument.

It may be possible that profiles containing students with incompatible levels of motivation and metacognitive self-regulation or knowledge may indicate methodological differences such as the use of scales with different anchoring points which could induce variation in the sample variance of the measures, resulting, thus, in level-specific differences. Additionally, it is noted that profiles of students’ motivation and metacognitive self-regulation is dynamic and may exhibit significant differences across academic subjects [41] and developmental stages (e.g., adolescence vs. emerging adulthood) since different measurements and cognitive capabilities [86] could explain the inconclusive results across studies.

Additionally, this finding answers an important question since the MASRL model, which illustrates the potential for interactions between motivation and metacognition, does not specifically set what would be the directional nature of these interactions [1,46]. Indeed, MASRL outlines scenarios where the interaction between motivation self-beliefs, affect, and metacognition can be either positive or negative. An example of such a negative scenario would be a student that feels good about a school subject, has high self-efficacy that she can accomplish the task in the subject, and has a mastery- and performance-approach goal but is metacognitively inaccurate in judging the demands of a task in this subject (e.g., a test) and, thus, overestimates her capability. Nevertheless, the current findings urge us to reconsider how these particular interactions between motivational beliefs and metacognitive self-regulation should be theoretically modelled at the generic person-level since no such negative interactions emerged in this large sample of adolescent students. This may be specifically tied to the nature of the school language subject and the developmental period under study here but it, nevertheless, needs further research.

Linking higher-order interactions between motivation and metacognition with school language achievement

Some person-centred studies have explored the effects of the interactive effects on students’ academic achievement. However, these studies focused on university/college course grades [41,43,87], performance in second language [12], or mathematics [88]. Nevertheless, it is contended that it is also important to address the issue of how these interactions are predictive of school language achievement, especially since language-related competencies, such as reading literacy, writing, comprehension, are increasingly important for society but many adolescents lag behind in their acquisition [89]. Additionally, especially in the Greek context, language achievement in adolescence has been found to be declining sharply across the years [19,90].

Hence, to address this important question of whether the interactions between motivational beliefs and metacognitive self-regulation were predictive of language achievement, students’ school language academic achievement was regressed on the latent categorical variable, adjusting for relevant covariates. The results indicated that those students with the lowest motivation and metacognitive self-regulation were the worst performing, whilst students with high motivation and metacognitive self-regulation were the most successful in school language courses. Although these findings do not indicate causality, they confirm, in line with theoretical evidence [3], that students need a high level of academic motivation to become highly metacognitively self-regulators. However, it remains to be seen whether motivation can compensate for failure in metacognitive self-regulation and how this may be linked with academic achievement.

Covariate effects on motivational and metacognitive self-regulation profiles

In addition to the above, we accounted for the potential associations between a few control variables and motivational and metacognitive self-regulation profiles. The results of multinomial regressions revealed statistically significant gender differences in adolescent students’ motivational and metacognitive profiles, with male students having a greater probability of membership in the minimal and sufficient profiles. Thus, H6 was confirmed. Preceding person-centred studies did not account for students’ socio-economic (SES) background [13,42,88], we included this variable since it has been connected with achievement gaps [52]. The findings indicated that coming from a higher SES household was associated with lower likelihood of being less motivated and metacognitively self-regulated, confirming, thus, our seventh hypothesis (H7).

Although greater age has been associated with a decline in academic motivation [17,18], but an increase in the capability for metacognitive capabilities [15], we did not formulate clear theoretical assumptions given that ambiguity of the association between age and motivational and metacognitive profiles. Despite that, the regression results illustrated that being older was predictive only of greater likelihood of membership in the minimal motivation and metacognitive self-regulation profile. This confirmed our hypothesis suggesting that an association would emerge (H8). The observed negative association appears to be bilateral since the bivariate correlations suggested that both motivational variables and metacognitive self-regulation decreased with greater age. This may suggest that older students in this sample might be more careless and may not deploy maintain a sufficient level of metacognitive self-regulation in school language class, which may explain why there is a large percentage of low-performing students in PISA studies. Finally, students speaking Greek at home with their families had lower chances of being less motivated and metacognitively self-regulated. This suggests that speaking the school’s official language at home has a protective effect against becoming less motivated and metacognitively self-regulated, confirming, thus, our ninth hypothesis.

Strengths and limitations

At this point, it is critical to reflect on some important strengths and limitations of this study. The large sample size, the low-performance context, the probability-based LPA, and the developmental stage of the participants can be considered amongst the strengths of this study. However, it ought to be noted that the sampling design is a convenience one and may not represent the whole Greek student population. Additionally, further measures indicative of SRL, such as cognitive strategies or task anxiety, were not included in this study, which limits the extent of the generalisations with respect to students’ SRL. Finally, the self-reported nature of the measures might have been influenced by social desirability.

Policy, practice, and theoretical implications

In line with the current findings, teachers should facilitate the development of students’ motivational beliefs, values about school language achievement, and metacognitive self-regulatory strategies. Given the significant proportion of students with minimal motivation and metacognitive self-regulation, emphasis should be placed on the identification and targeted educational intervention for those students. Due to the low academic performance educational system [91], systematic metacognitive strategy teaching and enhancement of students’ language motivation are recommended to be integrated in the national curriculum in order to raise students’ school language achievement. Particular emphasis should be placed on maintaining male students’ motivation and metacognitive self-regulation because we found that male students had greater chances of belonging to the minimal MMSR profile. From an educational perspective, we also suggest providing individualised assistance to students from low-SES backgrounds and those who do not speak Greek at home to prevent them from becoming unmotivated, metacognitively unregulated, and, subsequently, low-performers.

From a theoretical viewpoint, the present study contributes to existing social-cognitive models of SRL [1,2] that typically do not explicate what would be the consequences of a failure either in metacognitive self-regulation or self-motivation for academic achievement. That is, the current study examined through a person-centred perspective whether low functioning in either metacognitive self-regulation or motivation could be compensated by higher functioning in the other domain. The results suggest that current SRL models should clearly explicate that students’ motivation and metacognitive self-regulation are synergistically interacting across all levels of achievement. Additionally, the present findings challenge existing knowledge by suggesting that there is a limited number of subgroups of students with individual differences in motivation and metacognitive self-regulation.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Academic self-efficacy in school language class.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.s001

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Performance Goals- Extrinsic Goals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.s003

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Confirmatory factor analysis of the mastery scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.s007

(DOCX)

S8 Table. Confirmatory factor analysis of the performance goal scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.s008

(DOCX)

S9 Table. Confirmatory factor analysis of the self-efficacy.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.s009

(DOCX)

S10 Table. Confirmatory factor analysis of the intrinsic and extrinsic motivation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.s010

(DOCX)

S11 Table. Confirmatory factor analysis of the metacognitive self-regulation scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.s011

(DOCX)

S1 Text. Outliers and missing data analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289367.s012

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the students and schools that participated in the study.

References

  1. 1. Efklides A. Interactions of Metacognition With Motivation and Affect in Self-Regulated Learning: The MASRL Model. Educational Psychologist. 2011;46: 6–25.
  2. 2. Zimmerman BJ. Investigating Self-Regulation and Motivation: Historical Background, Methodological Developments, and Future Prospects. American Educational Research Journal. 2008;45: 166–183.
  3. 3. Zimmerman BJ, Schunk DH, DiBenedetto MK. The Role of Self‑Efficacy and Related Beliefs in Self-Regulation of Learning and Performance. second edition. In: Elliot AJ, Dweck CS, Yeager DS, editors. Handbook of Competence and Motivation. second edition. New York: Guilford Press; 2017. pp. 313–333.
  4. 4. Huang C. Achievement goals and self-efficacy: A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review. 2016;19: 119–137.
  5. 5. Karlen Y, Suter F, Hirt C, Maag Merki K. The role of implicit theories in students’ grit, achievement goals, intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, and achievement in the context of a long-term challenging task. Learning and Individual Differences. 2019;74: 101757.
  6. 6. Katsantonis I. Self-regulated learning and reading comprehension: The effects of gender, motivation and metacognition. Hellenic Journal of Psychology. 2020;17: 286–307.
  7. 7. Říčan J, Chytrý V, Medová J. Aspects of self-regulated learning and their influence on the mathematics achievement of fifth graders in the context of four different proclaimed curricula. Frontiers in Psychology. 2022;13. Available: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.963151. pmid:36304860
  8. 8. Cleary TJ, Kitsantas A. Motivation and Self-Regulated Learning Influences on Middle School Mathematics Achievement. School Psychology Review. 2017;46: 20.
  9. 9. Zheng B, Chang C, Lin C-H, Zhang Y. Self-Efficacy, Academic Motivation, and Self-Regulation: How Do They Predict Academic Achievement for Medical Students? MedSciEduc. 2021;31: 125–130. pmid:34457872
  10. 10. Bauer D, Shanahan M. Modeling Complex Interactions: Person-Centered and Variable-Centered Approaches. In: Little TD, Bovaird JA, Card NA, editors. Modeling Contextual Effects in Longitudinal Studies. Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2007. pp. 255–284.
  11. 11. Lanza ST, Rhoades BL, Nix RL, Greenberg MT, Group CPPR. Modeling the interplay of multilevel risk factors for future academic and behavior problems: A person-centered approach. Development and psychopathology. 2010;22: 313–335.
  12. 12. Chon YV, Shin T. Profile of second language learners’ metacognitive awareness and academic motivation for successful listening: A latent class analysis. Learning and Individual Differences. 2019;70: 62–75.
  13. 13. Karlen Y. Differences in students’ metacognitive strategy knowledge, motivation, and strategy use: A typology of self-regulated learners. The Journal of Educational Research. 2016;109: 253–265.
  14. 14. Lin L, Gong Y, Xu N. Online Self-Regulated Learning Profiles: A Study of Chinese as a Foreign Language Learners. Frontiers in Psychology. 2021;12. Available: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.797786. pmid:34966340
  15. 15. Weil LG, Fleming SM, Dumontheil I, Kilford EJ, Weil RS, Rees G, et al. The development of metacognitive ability in adolescence. Consciousness and Cognition. 2013;22: 264–271. pmid:23376348
  16. 16. Fredricks JA, Eccles JS. Children’s competence and value beliefs from childhood through adolescence: growth trajectories in two male-sex-typed domains. Developmental psychology. 2002;38: 519. pmid:12090482
  17. 17. Gottfried AE, Marcoulides GA, Gottfried AW, Oliver PH, Guerin DW. Multivariate latent change modeling of developmental decline in academic intrinsic math motivation and achievement: Childhood through adolescence. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2007;31: 317–327.
  18. 18. Wigfield A, Eccles JS, Fredricks JA, Simpkins S, Roeser RW, Schiefele U. Development of Achievement Motivation and Engagement. In: Lerner R, editor. Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science. American Cancer Society; 2015. pp. 1–44. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy316
  19. 19. OECD. PISA 2018 Results (Volume I): What Students Know and Can Do. OECD; 2019. https://doi.org/10.1787/5f07c754-en
  20. 20. Katsantonis I, McLellan R, Torres PE. Unraveling the complexity of the associations between students’ science achievement, motivation, and teachers’ feedback. Frontiers in Psychology. 2023;14. Available: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1124189. pmid:37063560
  21. 21. Kwarikunda D, Schiefele U, Muwonge CM, Ssenyonga J. Profiles of learners based on their cognitive and metacognitive learning strategy use: occurrence and relations with gender, intrinsic motivation, and perceived autonomy support. Humanit Soc Sci Commun. 2022;9: 337.
  22. 22. Tóth-Király I, Morin AJS, Litalien D, Valuch M, Bőthe B, Orosz G, et al. Self-determined profiles of academic motivation. Motiv Emot. 2022;46: 152–170.
  23. 23. Pintrich P. The role of motivation in promoting and sustaining self-regulated learning. International Journal of Educational Research. 1999;31: 459–470.
  24. 24. Flavell JH. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. American Psychologist. 1979;34: 906–911.
  25. 25. Tock JL, Moxley JH. A comprehensive reanalysis of the metacognitive self-regulation scale from the MSLQ. Metacognition Learning. 2017;12: 79–111.
  26. 26. Nietfeld JL, Cao L, Osborne JW. Metacognitive Monitoring Accuracy and Student Performance in the Postsecondary Classroom. The Journal of Experimental Education. 2005;74: 7–28.
  27. 27. Wigfield A, Muenks K, Eccles JS. Achievement Motivation: What We Know and Where We Are Going. Annu Rev Dev Psychol. 2021;3: 87–111.
  28. 28. Sungur S. Modeling the Relationships among Students’ Motivational Beliefs, Metacognitive Strategy Use, and Effort Regulation. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research. 2007;51: 315–326.
  29. 29. Coutinho SA, Neuman G. A model of metacognition, achievement goal orientation, learning style and self-efficacy. Learning Environ Res. 2008;11: 131–151.
  30. 30. Metallidou P, Vlachou A. Motivational beliefs, cognitive engagement, and achievement in language and mathematics in elementary school children. International Journal of Psychology. 2007;42: 2–15. pmid:24274775
  31. 31. Özcan ZÇ. The relationship between mathematical problem-solving skills and self-regulated learning through homework behaviours, motivation, and metacognition. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology. 2016;47: 408–420.
  32. 32. Pérez-González J-C, Filella G, Soldevila A, Faiad Y, Sanchez-Ruiz M-J. Integrating self-regulated learning and individual differences in the prediction of university academic achievement across a three-year-long degree. Metacognition Learning. 2022;17: 1141–1165.
  33. 33. Affect Efklides A., Emotions Epistemic, Metacognition, and Self-Regulated Learning. Teachers College Record. 2017;119: 1–22.
  34. 34. Efklides A. Gifted students and self-regulated learning: The MASRL model and its implications for SRL. High Ability Studies. 2019;30: 79–102.
  35. 35. Putarek V, Pavlin-Bernardić N. The role of self-efficacy for self-regulated learning, achievement goals, and engagement in academic cheating. Eur J Psychol Educ. 2020;35: 647–671.
  36. 36. Ross M, Perkins H, Bodey K. Academic motivation and information literacy self-efficacy: The importance of a simple desire to know. Library & Information Science Research. 2016;38: 2–9.
  37. 37. Skaalvik EM, Federici RA, Klassen RM. Mathematics achievement and self-efficacy: Relations with motivation for mathematics. International Journal of Educational Research. 2015;72: 129–136.
  38. 38. Harrell FE Jr. Regression Modeling Strategies. 2nd ed. New York, NY, USA: Springer; 2015.
  39. 39. Merz EL, Roesch SC. A latent profile analysis of the Five Factor Model of personality: Modeling trait interactions. Personality and Individual Differences. 2011;51: 915–919. pmid:21984857
  40. 40. Broadbent J, Fuller-Tyszkiewicz M. Profiles in self-regulated learning and their correlates for online and blended learning students. Education Tech Research Dev. 2018;66: 1435–1455.
  41. 41. Nelson KG, Shell DF, Husman J, Fishman EJ, Soh L-K. Motivational and Self-Regulated Learning Profiles of Students Taking a Foundational Engineering Course. Journal of Engineering Education. 2015;104: 74–100.
  42. 42. Dörrenbächer L, Perels F. Self-regulated learning profiles in college students: Their relationship to achievement, personality, and the effectiveness of an intervention to foster self-regulated learning. Learning and Individual Differences. 2016;51: 229–241.
  43. 43. Nylund-Gibson K, Choi AY. Ten frequently asked questions about latent class analysis. Translational Issues in Psychological Science. 2018;4: 440–461.
  44. 44. Korpershoek H, Kuyper H, van der Werf G. Differences in students’ school motivation: A latent class modelling approach. Soc Psychol Educ. 2015;18: 137–163.
  45. 45. Ning HK, Downing K. A latent profile analysis of university students’ self-regulated learning strategies. Studies in Higher Education. 2015;40: 1328–1346.
  46. 46. Efklides A, Schwartz BL, Brown V. Motivation and affect in self-regulated learning: Does metacognition play a role? In: Schunk DH, Greene JA, editors. Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance, 2nd ed. New York, NY, US: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group; 2018. pp. 64–82. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315697048-5
  47. 47. Gottlieb M. Assessing English Language Learners: Bridges to Educational Equity: Connecting Academic Language Proficiency to Student Achievement. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Corwin; 2016.
  48. 48. Halliday MAK. Towards a language-based theory of learning. Linguistics and Education. 1993;5: 93–116.
  49. 49. OECD. PISA 2018 Assessment and Analytical Framework. Paris, France: OECD Publishing; 2019. https://doi.org/10.1787/b25efab8-en
  50. 50. Ministry of Education. Guideliness for teaching the subjects of Modern Greek language and literature- grades A, B, C Gymnasium for the school year 2022–2023. Athens, Greece: Greek Ministry of Education; 2022. Available: https://www.esos.gr/sites/default/files/articles-legacy/n_glossa_-gram_gymn_2022_2023.pdf.
  51. 51. Ministry of Education. Timetable of the school subjects in A, B, and C of general lyceum. Athens, Greece: Ministry of Education; 2020. Available: https://www.esos.gr/sites/default/files/articles-legacy/lykeio_orologia_progarmmata.pdf.
  52. 52. Sirin SR. Socioeconomic Status and Academic Achievement: A Meta-Analytic Review of Research. Review of Educational Research. 2005;75: 417–453.
  53. 53. Van Laere E, Aesaert K, van Braak J. The Role of Students’ Home Language in Science Achievement: A multilevel approach. International Journal of Science Education. 2014;36: 2772–2794.
  54. 54. Hellenic Statistical Authority. Greece in Numbers: October- December 2022. Greek Office of Statistics; 2022. Available: https://www.statistics.gr/greece-in-figures.
  55. 55. Katsantonis I, McLellan R, Marquez J. Development of subjective well‐being and its relationship with self‐esteem in early adolescence. British J of Dev Psycho. 2022; bjdp.12436. pmid:36414023
  56. 56. Pintrich P, Smith DA, Garcia T, McKeachie W. Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire. Washington, D.C: Office of Educational Research and Improvement; 1991.
  57. 57. Pintrich P, Groot EVD. Motivational and Self-Regulated Learning Components of Classroom Academic Performance. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1990;82: 33–40. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.33.
  58. 58. Andreou E, Metallidou P. The relationship of academic and social cognition to behaviour in bullying situations among Greek primary school children. Educational Psychology. 2004;24: 27–41.
  59. 59. Metallidou P, Vlachou A. Children’s self-regulated learning profile in language and mathematics: The role of task value beliefs. Psychology in the Schools. 2010;47: 776–788.
  60. 60. Duncan TG, McKeachie WJ. The Making of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire. Educational Psychologist. 2005;40: 117–128.
  61. 61. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation from a self-determination theory perspective: Definitions, theory, practices, and future directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2020;61: 101860.
  62. 62. Caprara GV, Fida R, Vecchione M, Del Bove G, Vecchio GM, Barbaranelli C, et al. Longitudinal analysis of the role of perceived self-efficacy for self-regulated learning in academic continuance and achievement. Journal of educational psychology. 2008;100: 525.
  63. 63. Kuncel NR, Credé M, Thomas LL. The Validity of Self-Reported Grade Point Averages, Class Ranks, and Test Scores: A Meta-Analysis and Review of the Literature. Review of Educational Research. 2005;75: 63–82.
  64. 64. Sticca F, Goetz T, Bieg M, Hall NC, Eberle F, Haag L. Examining the accuracy of students’ self-reported academic grades from a correlational and a discrepancy perspective: Evidence from a longitudinal study. PLOS ONE. 2017;12: e0187367. pmid:29112979
  65. 65. Goodman E, Adler NE, Kawachi I, Frazier AL, Huang B, Colditz GA. Adolescents’ perceptions of social status: development and evaluation of a new indicator. Pediatrics. 2001;108: E31. pmid:11483841
  66. 66. Brown TA. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research. Second edition. New York; London: The Guilford Press; 2015.
  67. 67. Finch WH, Immekus CJ, French BF. Applied Psychometrics Using SPSS and AMOS. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing; 2016.
  68. 68. Cooper C. Psychological testing: theory and practice. 1st. London: Routledge; 2019.
  69. 69. Ferguson SL G. Moore EW, Hull DM. Finding latent groups in observed data: A primer on latent profile analysis in Mplus for applied researchers. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2020;44: 458–468.
  70. 70. Asparouhov T, Muthen B. Auxiliary Variables in Mixture Modeling: Using the BCH Method in Mplus to Estimate a Distal Outcome Model and an Arbitrary Secondary Model. Mplus: Muthen & Muthen; 2021. Report No.: 21.
  71. 71. Nylund-Gibson K, Grimm RP, Masyn KE. Prediction from Latent Classes: A Demonstration of Different Approaches to Include Distal Outcomes in Mixture Models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2019;26: 967–985.
  72. 72. Asparouhov T, Muthen B. Multiple Imputation with Mplus. 2022. Available: http://www.statmodel.com/missingdata.shtml.
  73. 73. McNeish D, Stapleton LM. The Effect of Small Sample Size on Two-Level Model Estimates: A Review and Illustration. Educ Psychol Rev. 2016;28: 295–314.
  74. 74. Dyer NG, Hanges PJ, Hall RJ. Applying multilevel confirmatory factor analysis techniques to the study of leadership. The Leadership Quarterly. 2005;16: 149–167.
  75. 75. McNeish D, Kelley K. Fixed effects models versus mixed effects models for clustered data: Reviewing the approaches, disentangling the differences, and making recommendations. Psychological Methods. 2019;24: 20–35. pmid:29863377
  76. 76. McNeish D, Stapleton LM. Modeling Clustered Data with Very Few Clusters. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2016;51: 495–518. pmid:27269278
  77. 77. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6: 1–55.
  78. 78. Rigdon EE, Becker J-M, Sarstedt M. Factor Indeterminacy as Metrological Uncertainty: Implications for Advancing Psychological Measurement. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2019;54: 429–443. pmid:30829544
  79. 79. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. 8th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2017.
  80. 80. Bauer J. A Primer to Latent Profile and Latent Class Analysis. In: Goller M, Kyndt E, Paloniemi S, Damşa C, editors. Methods for Researching Professional Learning and Development: Challenges, Applications and Empirical Illustrations. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2022. pp. 243–268. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-08518-5_11
  81. 81. Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the Number of Classes in Latent Class Analysis and Growth Mixture Modeling: A Monte Carlo Simulation Study. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2007;14: 535–569.
  82. 82. Masyn KE. Latent class analysis and finite mixture modeling. In: Little TD, editor. The Oxford Handbook of Quantitative Methods. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press; 2013. pp. 551–611.
  83. 83. StataCorp. User’s Guide, Release 16. STATA Corporation; 2019. Available: https://www.stata.com/bookstore/users-guide/.
  84. 84. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
  85. 85. Pintrich P, García T. Intraindividual differences in students’ motivation and selfregulated learning. German journal of educational psychology. 1993;7: 99–107.
  86. 86. Hirosawa E, Oga-Baldwin WLQ. Surveys and questionnaires with young language learners. In: Butler YG, Huang HB, editors. Research Methods for Understanding Child Second Language Development. New York, USA: Routledge; pp. 33–48.
  87. 87. Hong W, Bernacki ML, Perera HN. A latent profile analysis of undergraduates’ achievement motivations and metacognitive behaviors, and their relations to achievement in science. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2020;112: 1409–1430.
  88. 88. Cleary TJ, Slemp J, Pawlo ER. Linking student self-regulated learning profiles to achievement and engagement in mathematics. Psychology in the Schools. 2021;58: 443–457.
  89. 89. Council of European Union. Council Recommendation of 22 May 2018 on key competences for lifelong learning (2018/C 189/01). Official Journal of the European Union. 2018 [cited 19 Mar 2023]. Available: https://eur-lex.europa.eu/legal-content/EN/TXT/?uri=uriserv:OJ.C_.2018.189.01.0001.01.ENG&toc=OJ:C:2018:189:TOC.
  90. 90. OECD. PISA 2015 Results (Volume I): Excellence and Equity in Education. OECD; 2016. https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264266490-en
  91. 91. Dominguez-Gil C, Segovia-Gonzalez MM, Contreras I. A multiplicative composite indicator to evaluate educational systems in OECD countries. Compare: A Journal of Comparative and International Education. 2022;52: 1296–1313.