Figures
Abstract
Background
Community samples suggest that approximately 1 in 20 children and adults exhibit clinically significant anger, hostility, and aggression. Individuals with dysregulated emotional control have a greater lifetime burden of psychiatric morbidity, severe impairment in role functioning, and premature mortality due to cardiovascular disease.
Methods
With publically available data secured from dbGaP, we conducted a genome-wide association study of proneness to anger using the Spielberger State-Trait Anger Scale in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study (n = 8,747).
Results
Subjects were, on average, 54 (range 45–64) years old at baseline enrollment, 47% (n = 4,117) were male, and all were of European descent by self-report. The mean Angry Temperament and Angry Reaction scores were 5.8±1.8 and 7.6±2.2. We observed a nominally significant finding (p = 2.9E-08, λ = 1.027 - corrected pgc = 2.2E-07, λ = 1.0015) on chromosome 6q21 in the gene coding for the non-receptor protein-tyrosine kinase, Fyn.
Conclusions
Fyn interacts with NDMA receptors and inositol-1,4,5-trisphosphate (IP3)-gated channels to regulate calcium influx and intracellular release in the post-synaptic density. These results suggest that signaling pathways regulating intracellular calcium homeostasis, which are relevant to memory, learning, and neuronal survival, may in part underlie the expression of Angry Temperament.
Citation: Mick E, McGough J, Deutsch CK, Frazier JA, Kennedy D, Goldberg RJ (2014) Genome-Wide Association Study of Proneness to Anger. PLoS ONE 9(1): e87257. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087257
Editor: Andreas Reif, University of Wuerzburg, Germany
Received: January 14, 2013; Accepted: December 27, 2013; Published: January 28, 2014
Copyright: © 2014 Mick et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Funding: This work was supported by institutional funds provided by the departments of Psychiatry and Quantitative Health Sciences at the University of Massachusetts Medical School and by the UMass Center for Clinical and Translational Science (P30HD004147) supported by the NIH. Phenotypes and genotypes were downloaded from the National Center for Biotechnology Information (NCBI) database of genotypes and phenotypes (dbGaP) for the NHLBI funded Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study (Accessions: phs000280.v1.p1 and phs000090.v1.p1). ARIC is carried out as a collaborative study supported by National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute contracts N01HC55015, N01HC55016, N01HC- 55018, N01HC-55019, N01HC55020, N01HC-55021, N01-HC55022, R01-HL-087641, R01-HL-59367 and R01-HL-086694; National Human Genome Research Institute contract U01-HG-004402; and National Institutes of Health contract HHSN268200625226C. Infrastructure was partly supported by Grant Number UL1RR025005, a component of the National Institutes of Health and NIH Roadmap for Medical Research. The individual institutions represented had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis or interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication.
Competing interests: Dr. McGough receives grant or research support from the National Institutes of Health, NeuroSigma Inc., and Shire Pharmaceuticals. He has served as a consultant to Akili Interactive, Inc., Sunovion Pharmaceuticals, Targacept, and Theravance. Dr. Frazier receives or has received research funding from or participates in clinical trials with Bristol-Myers Squibb, Glaxo Smith Kline, Pfizer, Inc. and Seaside Therapeutics. All other authors have no disclosures. This disclosure does not alter the authors' adherence to all the PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.
Introduction
Anger has been characterized as a complex construct encompassing irritability, hostility, and aggressive behavior. Its components -- impulsive, unprovoked, and developmentally inappropriate outbursts of anger – may be associated with dysregulated emotional control or impaired neural circuits mediating emotion, cognition, and inhibition [1], [2]. Approximately 5% of children and adults in community samples exhibit extreme levels of anger, hostility, and aggression considered to be clinically significant [3]–[5].
Further, these traits are strongly associated with a range of psychiatric symptomatology. Behavioral dysregulation and aggression also predict psychiatric hospitalization and suicidality in childhood and a wide range of psychiatric morbidity in adulthood [6]–[11]. Moreover, dysregulated emotional control and intermittent explosive disorder in adults is associated with a greater lifetime burden of psychiatric morbidity and severe impairment in role functioning [2], [5]. Proneness to anger in the general community is also associated with premature all-cause mortality [12], primarily due to cardiovascular disease [13]–[18]. Consequently, effective primary or secondary interventions that reduce anger, hostility, and aggression might have a substantial public health impact on quality of life and, ultimately, longevity.
Symptoms of deficient emotional self-regulation in adults are familial [19] and, as recently shown in the Vietnam Era Twin Registry, are influenced by moderate genetic effects, particularly on the tendency to switch from euthymia to depression or anger [20]. A commonly used continuously-distributed measure of this trait in children, the Child Behavior Checklist Dysregulation Profile, has been found to be highly heritable with additive genetic effects consistently explaining up to 67% of its variance [3], [4], [21], [22]. A small GWAS of this trait in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder suggested possible associations with several genes implicated in neurodevelopment, synaptic plasticity, as well as hippocampal dependent memory and learning [23]. Taken together, these studies suggest the role of heritable factors in determining individual differences in the self-regulation of emotional and cognitive neural circuits, ones that may underlie proneness to anger, hostility, and aggression. The primary goal of this study was to identify genetic susceptibility loci for proneness to anger through a secondary analysis of publically-available data.
Materials and Methods
Phenotypes and genotypes were downloaded from the National Center for Biotechnology Information (NCBI) database of genotypes and phenotypes (dbGaP) [24] for the NHLBI funded Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study (Accessions: phs000280.v1.p1 and phs000090.v1.p1). In accord with the restrictions on the use of the data defined by participant informed consent agreements, this ARIC dataset may be used for general research use following the approval of the ARIC Data Access Committee and IRB approval at the approved investigators institution. The acquisition and use of these data was approved and overseen by the Human Subjects Institutional Review Board at the University of Massachusetts Medical School.
Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Cohort
Details regarding the design and objectives of the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study have been previously published and are available online (http://www.cscc.unc.edu/ARIC/)[25]. In brief, this is a large population-based sample of 15,792 individuals ascertained from a probability sample of four U.S. communities between 1987 and 1989. Subjects were followed every three years for several re-assessments (1990–1992, 1993–1995, and 1996–1998) to study the incidence and course of atherosclerosis in men and women aged 45–64 years at the time of initial clinic assessment.
Genotyping
Samples were genotyped on the Affymetrix Genome-Wide Human SNP Array 6.0 at the Broad Institute Center for Genotyping and Analysis for the Gene Environment Association Studies initiative (GENEVA, http://www.genevastudy.org). Genotypes of 12,771 ARIC participants were submitted to dbGaP and were available for analysis. For the present analyses, we first excluded related individuals (n = 927; 11,844 remaining); subsequently, subjects with missing data on the Spielberger Trait Anger Scale (n = 599; 11,245 remaining). Twenty percent of ARIC participants were African American (AA) by self-report and the remaining subjects classified only as “White” (i.e., individuals of European ancestry (EA)). Using the online genetic power calculator toolset [26], we determined the statistical power to detect association at a quantitative trait locus (QTL) to account for at least 1% of variance in the EA subsample to be 0.81; however, the power to detect genome-wide statistically significant associations in the AA subgroup was substantially lower, at 0.21. Therefore, we focused exclusively on the EA sample (with n = 2,498 excluded and n = 8,747 remaining).
Quality Control
Extensive quality control checks were conducted on the original data and we downloaded the cleaned dataset with genotypes flagged for chromosomal abnormalities (n = 840,606 SNPs). Within the selected sample of 8,747 subjects, we filtered SNPs by minor allele frequency (MAF) conditional on call rate (CR) including SNPs with: 0.01 ≤ MAF < 0.05 and CR >99%; 0.05 ≤ MAF <0.1 and CR >97%, MAF ≥0.1 and CR >95% (n = 148,142 SNPs excluded). Any SNPs found to be out of Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium (p<1.0E-4) were excluded from further consideration (n = 14,821 SNPs excluded). After applying the described quality control filters, the final sample consisted of 677,643 SNPs in 8,747 unrelated subjects.
Spielberger State-Trait Anger Scale
The Spielberger State-Trait Anger Scale is a Likert-type four-level self-rating scale (1 = almost never, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often, 4 = almost always) [27]. Factor analysis of the Trait Anger items have yielded two weakly correlated (r2 = 0.29) factors labeled “Angry Temperament” and “Angry Reaction” [28]. The former refers to the propensity of individuals to express anger frequently, with little or no provocation, and includes four items: 1) I am quick tempered, 2) I have a fiery temper, 3) I am a hotheaded person, and 4) I fly off the handle. In contrast, the Angry Reaction subscale reflects frustration in response to criticism or mistreatment; it also includes four items: 1) I get angry when I am slowed down by others’ mistakes, 2) I feel annoyed when I am not given recognition for doing good work, 3) It makes me furious when I am criticized in front of others, and 4) I feel infuriated when I do a good job and get a poor evaluation. The items for each subscale are summed to generate specific scores each ranging from 4 to 16. Temperament scores above 8, and Reaction scores above 10, are considered to be elevated and have been assessed as risk factors for coronary heart disease outcomes in prior analyses of these data [14].
The Spielberger State-Trait Anger scale was first adminstered at visit 2 (1990–1992) in 8,747 subjects and again at visit 3 (1993–1995) in 7,246 subjects; moderate to strong test-retest reliability for the Angry Reaction (ICC = 0.50) and the Angry Temperament (ICC = 0.65) subscales was observed. Subjects with missing Spielberger State-Trait Anger scale data at visit 3 (n = 1,501) had slightly higher visit 2 scores on the Angry Temperament (6.0±1.9 vs. 5.7±1.7, p<0.0001) but not on the Angry Reaction (7.6±2.2 vs. 7.6±2.1, p = 0.7) subscales. To maximize the sample size, and to avoid misclassification of Angry Temperament at visit 3, we utilized information collected at visit 2 in this report.
Statistical Analysis
All genome-wide association analyses were conducted using PLINK [29] employing datasets downloaded and filtered as described earlier under genotyping. To control for multiplicity in the number of SNPs tested, we adopted the conservative recommendation of Dudbridge et al [30] and Pe’er et al [31], considering p-values less than 7.0E-08 to be statistically significant genome-wide. Our primary test of association was for the additive effects of minor allele dosage on the quantitative summary scores of Angry Temperament and Angry Reaction (each ranging from 4-16 points) in the selected sample. To further estimate magnitude and direction of effect, we then conducted case-control analyses (i.e., Angry Temperament >8 vs. ≤8 or Angry Reaction >10 vs. ≤10) for SNPs of interest from the quantitative association results. The potential for inflation of the test statistic due to population heterogeneity was estimated with the lambda statistic (defined as the observed median statistical test divided by the expected median statistical test) and with quantile-quantile plots of observed and expected p-values. To adjust for population stratification, we conducted genome-wide association tests on the model residuals generated for each anger phenotype regressed on the principal components representing genetic substructure provided by the ARIC investigators through dbGaP.
Gene Ontology (GO) Enrichment Analyses
Genetic enrichment or pathway analysis was conducted with the INRICH pathway analysis tool for GWAS, designed for detection of enriched association signals of linkage-disequilibrium (LD) independent genomic regions within biologically relevant gene sets (http://atgu.mgh.harvard.edu/inrich) [32]. Independent LD association intervals were identified by the “clumping” algorithm in PLINK [29]. This algorithm identifies genomic regions surrounding index SNPs (i.e. p<5E-05) defined by nearby SNPs (within 250 kb) that are in linkage disequilibrium (r2>0.4) with the index SNP and are also nominally statistically significant (p<5E-03). These association regions were referenced against genetic categories defined by the Gene Ontology project (http://www.geneontology.org/) according to known biological processes, cellular components, and molecular function of their gene products. For each GO term (e.g., “regulation of synaptic plasticity”) the number of association intervals containing genes associated with that GO term were counted to determine if the proportion of overlapping intervals is greater than expected by chance through multiple permutation (n = 5,000 permutations). The permutation procedure places each association interval at random genomic locations but conditions on SNP/gene density to control for potential bias associated with gene size and SNP density.
Results
Data from 8,747 Caucasian men and women participating in the ARIC study were included in our genome-wide association analyses. Subjects were, on average 54.3±5.7 (range 45–64 years) years old at baseline enrollment, 47% (n = 4,117) were men, and all were of European descent by self-report. The mean Angry Temperament and Angry Reaction scores were 5.8±1.8) and 7.6±2.2, respectively. Elevation of the Angry Reaction subscale (>10, n = 845, 9.7%) was seen more often than elevation of the Angry Temperament subscale (>8, n = 515, 5.9%).
Association with each anger phenotype was assessed across the 677,643 SNPs filtered by call rate conditional on minor allele frequency and deviation from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium. We observed a potentially statistically significant finding (smallest p = 2.9E-08) for Angry Temperament but not for Angry Reaction (smallest p = 2.5E-07). The QQ plot (Figure 1) shows the distribution of expected p-values against the observed distribution for both Angry Temperament and Angry Reaction. Inspection of the Angry Temperament plot clearly reveals a greater number of significant findings than expected by chance with the distribution of p-values being only slightly inflated (lambda = 1.027). Adjustment for genetic background reduced lambda to 1.0015 and attenuated the uppermost findings (smallest pGC = 2.2E-07) but the departure from the diagonal in the tail of both the corrected and uncorrected QQ plots indicates enrichment of significant associations (Figure 1).
The red symbols depict results from analysis uncorrected for ancestry, the blue symbols depict results corrected for ancestry (i.e. PGC), and the dashed red line depicts the 95% confidence interval for the distribution of results. There was no evidence of inflation of the test statistic for the Angry Reaction scores in the uncorrected (λ = 0.9998) or the corrected (λ = 1.0029) analyses. For the Angry Temperament score test statistics, there was slight inflation of the uncorrected analysis (λ = 1.0272) that was further reduced with correction for genetic ancestry (λ = 1.0015).
Table 1 lists the top 38 association regions defined by an index SNP with nominal p<5E-05 and at least one additional proximal SNP (<250 kb) in LD (r2>0.4) and significant at p<5E-03 for the Angry Temperament subscale. We assessed enrichment of Gene Ontology (GO) terms overlapping with association regions using the INRICH algorithm to control for potential biases caused by SNP/gene density and gene size. There was evidence of enrichment for no GO targets among the top regional associations for Angry Reaction scores (Table 2), but we found some nominal evidence of enrichment for 6 GO targets (Table 3) in the Angry Temperament association regions in Table 1 (none survived correction for the number of targets tested). Among the targets nominally significant at p<0.05, four genes overlap and are present in more than one target: ABAT (4-aminobutyrate aminotransferase), FYN (Fyn oncogene related to Src, Fgr, Yes), PDE3A (phosphodiesterase 3A, cGMP-inhibited), and VEGFC (vascular endothelial growth factor C).
Discussion
In this study we conducted a genome-wide association study of trait anger in a large sample of middle-aged and elderly men and women recruited from 4 large U.S. communities. We found evidence of genetic susceptibility for the anger trait associated with a proclivity for unprovoked (i.e., Angry Temperament scores) but not provoked (i.e., Angry Reaction scores) anger. In contrast to the Angry Reaction results, for Angry Temperament we identified a greater than expected number of extreme p-values and nominal evidence of GO target enrichment.
We have previously studied mood dysregulation using data from the ADHD sub study of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (PGC) [23]. In this study of 341 referred ADHD assessed with the Child-Behavior Checklist [23], [33], we found suggestive but weak associations with BDNF (brain-derived neurotrophic factor), it’s preferred receptor (NTKR2, neurotrophic tyrosine kinase receptor, type 2), and a scaffolding protein (LRRC7, leucine rich repeat containing 7) anchoring a downstream protein kinase (CAMK2A, calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II alpha) required for initiation and maintenance of early-long-term potentiation [34], [35]. We also found preliminary evidence of association with prion protein (PRNP) and it’s ligand (STIP1; stress-induced-phosphoprotein) that together mediate astrocyte differentiation/survival [36], [37] and homeostatic function of hippocampal circuits [38]. Disruption of STIP1-PRPN or BDNF-NTKR2 binding in the hippocampus [39]–[41] impair long-term potentiation, spatial learning, memory consolidation, and hippocampal development.
The most statistically significant association in the current study was with rs2148710 and the Angry Temperament score in FYN. Interestingly, the mechanisms underlying the pathways suggested by our small studies of mood dysregulation in children are dependent on Fyn activity. For example, post-synaptic BDNF-NTRK2 binding activates Fyn to phosphorylate NDMA-receptors (N-methyl-d-aspartate) and increases subsequent calcium influx associated with long-term potentiation initiation [35], [42], [43]. Fyn also phosphorylates endoplasmic reticulum inositol-1,4,5-trisphosphate (IP3)-gated channels to stimulate the release of intracellular calcium [44], [45] in response to both NTKR2 [35] and PRNP [46] activity.
Loss of Fyn function in mice has also been associated with blunted long-term potentiation at hippocampal synapses and impaired learning and memory on the hidden platform water maze task [47]. These findings are consistent with learning-disordered/transactional model of explosive anger in which lagging higher-order cognitive skills play a central role [48], [49]. Specific executive functioning deficits could contribute to explosive reactivity through inefficient encoding of previous consequences of noncompliance, thereby interfering with the ability to anticipate consequences of potential actions [49].
The Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (PGC) [50] has recently published cross- disorder analyses documenting pleiotropic effects of associated genes for psychiatric disorders that suggest a common genetic susceptibilities that underlie psychiatric morbidity. These pleiotropic genetic variants in the PGC analyses were enriched for brain expression quantitative trait loci (eQTL) and more specifically for calcium channel activity genes facilitating transmembrane ion diffusion (GO:0005262, calcium channel activity). Interestingly, we also observed a nominal association with a SNP in CACNA1C and Angry Temperament in our association analyses (Table 1). CACNA1C encodes the α subunit of the L-type voltage-gated calcium channel and is a strong candidate for both bipolar disorder and general psychiatric morbidity [32], [50]. Thus, our results may suggest that regulation of calcium-dependent intracellular signaling could play a role not only for psychiatric morbidity but also for variable expression of normative symptoms such as anger.
GO enrichment analyses suggest that there may be shared genetic susceptibility for affect regulation and cardiovascular disease, as well. Using these data, other groups have documented an increased risk for coronary heart disease with an elevated proneness to anger [13], [14], [17], [51], [52], and we have found nominally significant (p<0.05) enrichment of genes associated with the negative regulation of blood pressure (GO:0045776; ABAT and VEGFC) and platelet activation (GO:0030168; FYN, VEGFC, and PDE3A) in our study of Angry Temperament (Table 3). In addition to impacting hippocampal memory and learning, Fyn is involved with the regulation of platelet shape/response [53] and cardiac myocyte excitability by modulating voltage-gated cardiac sodium channels [54]. Similarly, ABAT, a metabolizer of the inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA, is associated with negative regulation of blood pressure and has previously been associated with disorders (e.g., schizophrenia [55] and autism [56]) that are associated with angry, aggressive behavior. Though the overlapping patterns of association might represent pleiotropic genes, they also might be the result of a confounding by phenotypic correlation (e.g., between affect regulation and cardiovascular disease) [57].
We have evaluated these results in the context of methodological limitations, including considerations that (a) replication studies for this phenotype are not yet available, (b) the measures demonstrated moderate test-retest reliability, and (c) our available sample size may be inadequate to identify genome-wide statistically significant associations that survive correction for population stratification. This may weaken the strength of association for the regions identified and, consequently, no targets survived correction for the number of GO categories evaluated for enrichment. (4) Another limitation exists with respect to the range of ethnicity: there was only a modest number of African American subjects, thus we limited our analyses and generalizability to individuals of European descent.
These limitations notwithstanding, the differences in the distribution of results for the Angry Temperament and Angry Reaction phenotypes are noteworthy. The null results observed for Angry Reaction scores (i.e., an absence of any inflation of test statistic, extreme p-values, or evidence of GO enrichment among top findings) suggest that the associations observed for Angry Temperament may not be due to chance alone, particularly since both scores were generated from a single questionnaire. Compared to those of the Angry Reaction phenotype, the behavioral characteristics measured by the Angry Temperament items (unprovoked, frequent, and extreme anger) more closely resemble the behavioral characteristics of the childhood phenotype --- ones that resulted in putative associations with genes involved in hippocampal synaptic plasticity, memory and learning [23]. The substantial clinical impact of this form of dysregulated emotion on interpersonal functioning [5], [11] and cardiovascular health [14], [15] strongly suggests that additional etiologic research is indicated in order to identify targets for primary and secondary interventions across the life-cycle.
Acknowledgments
Phenotypes and genotypes were downloaded from the National Center for Biotechnology Information (NCBI) database of genotypes and phenotypes (dbGaP) for the NHLBI funded Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study (Accessions: phs000280.v1.p1 and phs000090.v1.p1). The authors thank the staff and participants of the ARIC study for their important contributions.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: EM JM CD RG. Analyzed the data: EM. Wrote the paper: EM JM CD JF DK RG.
References
- 1. Leibenluft E (2011) Severe mood dysregulation, irritability, and the diagnostic boundaries of bipolar disorder in youths. The American journal of psychiatry 168: 129–142.
- 2. Barkley RA, Fischer M (2010) The unique contribution of emotional impulsiveness to impairment in major life activities in hyperactive children as adults. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 49: 503–513.
- 3. Hudziak J, Althoff RR, Rettew DC, Derks EM, Faraone SV (2005) The prevalence and genetic architecture of CBCL-juvenile bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry 58: 562–568.
- 4. Volk HE, Todd RD (2007) Does the Child Behavior Checklist juvenile bipolar disorder phenotype identify bipolar disorder? Biol Psychiatry 62: 115–120.
- 5. Kessler RC, Coccaro EF, Fava M, Jaeger S, Jin R, et al. (2006) The prevalence and correlates of DSM-IV intermittent explosive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of general psychiatry 63: 669–678.
- 6. Youngstrom E, Youngstrom JK, Starr M (2005) Bipolar diagnoses in community mental health: Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist profiles and patterns of comorbidity. Biol Psychiatry 58: 569–575.
- 7. Diler RS, Birmaher B, Axelson D, Goldstein B, Gill M, et al. (2009) The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and the CBCL-bipolar phenotype are not useful in diagnosing pediatric bipolar disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 19: 23–30.
- 8. Meyer SE, Carlson GA, Youngstrom E, Ronsaville DS, Martinez PE, et al. (2009) Long-term outcomes of youth who manifested the CBCL-Pediatric Bipolar Disorder phenotype during childhood and/or adolescence. J Affect Disord 113: 227–235.
- 9. Biederman J, Petty CR, Monuteaux MC, Evans M, Parcell T, et al. (2009) The child behavior checklist-pediatric bipolar disorder profile predicts a subsequent diagnosis of bipolar disorder and associated impairments in ADHD youth growing up: a longitudinal analysis. J Clin Psychiatry 70: 732–740.
- 10. Ayer L, Althoff R, Ivanova M, Rettew D, Waxler E, et al. (2009) Child Behavior Checklist Juvenile Bipolar Disorder (CBCL-JBD) and CBCL Posttraumatic Stress Problems (CBCL-PTSP) scales are measures of a single dysregulatory syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 50: 1291–1300.
- 11. Althoff RR, Verhulst FC, Rettew DC, Hudziak JJ, van der Ende J (2010) Adult outcomes of childhood dysregulation: a 14-year follow-up study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49: 1105–1116.
- 12. Iribarren C, Jacobs DR, Kiefe CI, Lewis CE, Matthews KA, et al. (2005) Causes and demographic, medical, lifestyle and psychosocial predictors of premature mortality: the CARDIA study. Social science & medicine 60: 471–482.
- 13. Williams JE, Paton CC, Siegler IC, Eigenbrodt ML, Nieto FJ, et al. (2000) Anger proneness predicts coronary heart disease risk: prospective analysis from the atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) study. Circulation 101: 2034–2039.
- 14. Williams JE, Nieto FJ, Sanford CP, Tyroler HA (2001) Effects of an angry temperament on coronary heart disease risk : The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study. American journal of epidemiology 154: 230–235.
- 15. Kubzansky LD, Park N, Peterson C, Vokonas P, Sparrow D (2011) Healthy Psychological Functioning and Incident Coronary Heart Disease: The Importance of Self-regulation. Archives of general psychiatry 68: 400–408.
- 16. Iribarren C, Sidney S, Bild DE, Liu K, Markovitz JH, et al. (2000) Association of hostility with coronary artery calcification in young adults: the CARDIA study. Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults. JAMA : the journal of the American Medical Association 283: 2546–2551.
- 17. Williams JE, Couper DJ, Din-Dzietham R, Nieto FJ, Folsom AR (2007) Race-gender differences in the association of trait anger with subclinical carotid artery atherosclerosis: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study. American journal of epidemiology 165: 1296–1304.
- 18. Player MS, King DE, Mainous AG 3rd, Geesey ME (2007) Psychosocial factors and progression from prehypertension to hypertension or coronary heart disease. Annals of family medicine 5: 403–411.
- 19. Surman CB, Biederman J, Spencer T, Yorks D, Miller CA, et al. (2011) Deficient emotional self-regulation and adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a family risk analysis. The American journal of psychiatry 168: 617–623.
- 20.
Coccaro EF, Ong AD, Seroczynski D, Bergeman CS (2011) Affective intensity and lability: Heritability in adult male twins. Journal of affective disorders.
- 21. Althoff RR, Rettew DC, Faraone SV, Boomsma DI, Hudziak JJ (2006) Latent class analysis shows strong heritability of the child behavior checklist-juvenile bipolar phenotype. Biol Psychiatry 60: 903–911.
- 22. Hudziak JJ, Derks EM, Althoff RR, Rettew DC, Boomsma DI (2005) The genetic and environmental contributions to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder as measured by the conners' rating scales--revised. Am J Psychiatry 162: 1614–1620.
- 23.
Mick E, McGough J, Loo S, Doyle AE, Wozniak J, et al.. (2011) Genome-wide association study of the child behavior checklist dysregulation profile. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 50: 807–817 e808.
- 24. Mailman MD, Feolo M, Jin Y, Kimura M, Tryka K, et al. (2007) The NCBI dbGaP database of genotypes and phenotypes. Nature Genetics 39: 1181–1186.
- 25. The ARIC Investigators (1989) The atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) study: design and objectives. American journal of epidemiology 129: 687–702.
- 26. Purcell S, Cherny SS, Sham PC (2003) Genetic Power Calculator: design of linkage and association genetic mapping studies of complex traits. Bioinformatics 19: 149–150.
- 27.
Spielberger CD, Jacobs G, Russell S, Crane RS (1983) Assessment of anger: The state-trait anger scale. In: Butcher JN, Spielberger CD, editors. Advances in Personality Assessment. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 161–189.
- 28.
Gold AE, Johhnston DW (1996) Anger, hypertension and heart disease. In: Bennett P, Weinman J, Spurgeon P, editors. Current Developments in Health Psychology. 2nd ed. Amsterdam: Harwood Acedemic Publishers. pp. 105–128.
- 29. Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MA, et al. (2007) PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet 81: 559–575.
- 30. Dudbridge F, Gusnanto A (2008) Estimation of significance thresholds for genomewide association scans. Genet Epidemiol 32: 227–234.
- 31. Pe'er I, Yelensky R, Altshuler D, Daly MJ (2008) Estimation of the multiple testing burden for genomewide association studies of nearly all common variants. Genet Epidemiol 32: 381–385.
- 32. Sklar P, Ripke S, Scott LJ, Andreassen OA, Cichon S, et al. (2011) Large-scale genome-wide association analysis of bipolar disorder identifies a new susceptibility locus near ODZ4. Nature genetics 43: 977–983.
- 33.
Mick E, Monuteaux M, Wilens TE, Wozniak J, Byrne D, et al. A Genetic association study of emotional dysregulation indexed by the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) in Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD); 2010 10/2010; New York. AACAP.
- 34. Thalhammer A, Trinidad JC, Burlingame AL, Schoepfer R (2009) Densin-180: revised membrane topology, domain structure and phosphorylation status. J Neurochem 109: 297–302.
- 35. Minichiello L (2009) TrkB signalling pathways in LTP and learning. Nature reviews Neuroscience 10: 850–860.
- 36. Lopes MH, Hajj GN, Muras AG, Mancini GL, Castro RM, et al. (2005) Interaction of cellular prion and stress-inducible protein 1 promotes neuritogenesis and neuroprotection by distinct signaling pathways. J Neurosci 25: 11330–11339.
- 37. Arantes C, Nomizo R, Lopes MH, Hajj GN, Lima FR, et al. (2009) Prion protein and its ligand stress inducible protein 1 regulate astrocyte development. Glia 57: 1439–1449.
- 38. Rangel A, Madronal N, Gruart A, Gavin R, Llorens F, et al. (2009) Regulation of GABA(A) and glutamate receptor expression, synaptic facilitation and long-term potentiation in the hippocampus of prion mutant mice. PloS one 4: e7592.
- 39. Coitinho AS, Lopes MH, Hajj GN, Rossato JI, Freitas AR, et al. (2007) Short-term memory formation and long-term memory consolidation are enhanced by cellular prion association to stress-inducible protein 1. Neurobiol Dis 26: 282–290.
- 40. Minichiello L, Korte M, Wolfer D, Kuhn R, Unsicker K, et al. (1999) Essential role for TrkB receptors in hippocampus-mediated learning. Neuron 24: 401–414.
- 41. Korte M, Carroll P, Wolf E, Brem G, Thoenen H, et al. (1995) Hippocampal long-term potentiation is impaired in mice lacking brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 92: 8856–8860.
- 42. Levine ES, Crozier RA, Black IB, Plummer MR (1998) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor modulates hippocampal synaptic transmission by increasing N-methyl-D-aspartic acid receptor activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95: 10235–10239.
- 43. Mizuno M, Yamada K, He J, Nakajima A, Nabeshima T (2003) Involvement of BDNF receptor TrkB in spatial memory formation. Learning & memory 10: 108–115.
- 44. Jayaraman T, Ondrias K, Ondriasova E, Marks AR (1996) Regulation of the inositol 1,4,5-trisphosphate receptor by tyrosine phosphorylation. Science 272: 1492–1494.
- 45. Vanderheyden V, Devogelaere B, Missiaen L, De Smedt H, Bultynck G, et al. (2009) Regulation of inositol 1,4,5-trisphosphate-induced Ca2+ release by reversible phosphorylation and dephosphorylation. Biochimica et biophysica acta 1793: 959–970.
- 46. Krebs B, Wiebelitz A, Balitzki-Korte B, Vassallo N, Paluch S, et al. (2007) Cellular prion protein modulates the intracellular calcium response to hydrogen peroxide. Journal of neurochemistry 100: 358–367.
- 47. Grant SG, O'Dell TJ, Karl KA, Stein PL, Soriano P, et al. (1992) Impaired long-term potentiation, spatial learning, and hippocampal development in fyn mutant mice. Science 258: 1903–1910.
- 48. Greene RW, Doyle AE (1999) Toward a transactional conceptualization of oppositional defiand disorder: Implications for assessment and treatment. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 2: 129–148.
- 49. Greene RW, Ablon JS, Goring JC (2003) A transactional model of oppositional behavior: underpinnings of the Collaborative Problem Solving approach. J Psychosom Res 55: 67–75.
- 50. Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics C, Lee SH, Ripke S, Neale BM, Faraone SV, et al (2013) Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Nat Genet 45: 984–994.
- 51. Williams JE, Nieto FJ, Sanford CP, Couper DJ, Tyroler HA (2002) The association between trait anger and incident stroke risk: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study. Stroke; a journal of cerebral circulation 33: 13–19.
- 52. Williams JE, Din-Dzietham R, Szklo M (2006) Trait anger and arterial stiffness: results from the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Preventive cardiology 9: 14–20.
- 53. Kim S, Kunapuli SP (2011) Negative regulation of Gq-mediated pathways in platelets by G(12/13) pathways through Fyn kinase. The Journal of biological chemistry 286: 24170–24179.
- 54. Ahern CA, Zhang JF, Wookalis MJ, Horn R (2005) Modulation of the cardiac sodium channel NaV1.5 by Fyn, a Src family tyrosine kinase. Circulation research 96: 991–998.
- 55. Jia P, Wang L, Meltzer HY, Zhao Z (2010) Common variants conferring risk of schizophrenia: a pathway analysis of GWAS data. Schizophrenia research 122: 38–42.
- 56. Chakrabarti B, Dudbridge F, Kent L, Wheelwright S, Hill-Cawthorne G, et al. (2009) Genes related to sex steroids, neural growth, and social-emotional behavior are associated with autistic traits, empathy, and Asperger syndrome. Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research 2: 157–177.
- 57. Vansteelandt S, Goetgeluk S, Lutz S, Waldman I, Lyon H, et al. (2009) On the adjustment for covariates in genetic association analysis: a novel, simple principle to infer direct causal effects. Genetic Epidemiology 33: 394–405.