Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Romantic relationship configurations and their correlates among LGBTQ+ persons: A latent class analysis

  • Fabio Cannas Aghedu,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Team ENACT, University of Nîmes, Nîmes, France

  • Martin Blais ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    blais.martin@uqam.ca

    Affiliations Research Chair in Sexual Diversity and Gender Plurality, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montréal, Québec, Canada, Département de Sexologie, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montréal, Québec, Canada

  • Léa J. Séguin,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Département de Sexologie, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montréal, Québec, Canada

  • Isabel Côté

    Roles Funding acquisition, Validation

    Affiliation Département de Travail Social, Université du Québec en Outaouais, Gatineau, Québec, Canada

Abstract

Research comparing monogamous and non-monogamous relationships on well-being indicators across diverse populations have yielded inconsistent findings. The present study investigates sociodemographic characteristics, as well as personal and relational outcomes, across different relationship configurations. Data were drawn from an online community-based sample of 1,528 LGBTQ+ persons aged 18 years and older in Quebec, Canada. A latent class analysis was performed based on legal relationship status, relationship agreement, cohabitation status, and the seeking of extradyadic sexual and romantic partners on the internet. Class differences on sociodemographic characteristics and well-being and relationship quality indicators were examined. A five-class solution best fit the data, highlighting five distinct relationship configurations: Formalized monogamy (59%), Free monogamy (20%), Formalized open relationship (11%), Monogamous considering alternatives (7%) and Free consensual non-monogamies (3%). Cisgender women were more likely to engage in monogamous relationships than cisgender men, who were overrepresented in open relationships. Lower levels of perceived partner support were observed in both free monogamous and consensually non-monogamous relationships, the latter of which also showed lower levels of well-being. Consensual non-monogamy researchers exploring relationship outcomes should examine relationship facets that go beyond relationship structure or agreement. Variations in monogamies and non-monogamies, both consensual and non-consensual, may be present within each broad relationship configuration, as reflected in different personal and relational needs, which can then translate to better or poorer outcomes.

Introduction

While monogamy remains the dominant, ideal type of romantic relationship in Western cultures [1, 2], recent research has found that interest in non-monogamy, notably in polyamory, is on the rise [3, 4]. In response to this growing interest, recent papers have delved deeper into this phenomenon, allowing for more extensive examination and discussion of non-monogamous relationship dynamics and experiences [57]. These relationship models are based on the explicit agreement that both partners are free to engage in sexual activity [e.g., open relationships; 4, 8] or in romantic relationships with others [e.g., polyamory; 9, 10]. Recent research in North America suggests that almost 5% of people are currently involved in a non-monogamous relationship [3, 11].

Overall, research suggests that LGBTQ+ persons are more likely to engage in consensually non-monogamous relationships than their heterosexual cisgender counterparts [1214]. Research has also shown that, among LGBTQ+ individuals, bisexual people are more likely than gay and lesbian individuals to practice non-monogamy [for a review, see 15]. Additionally, recent research has shown that transgender and nonbinary individuals are more likely to be involved in polyamorous relationships than their cisgender counterparts [9, 16, 17]. In a recent U.S. study, 44.6% of bisexual men and 35.0% of bisexual women, compared to 31.6% of gay men and 21.3% of lesbian women reported prior involvement in consensual non-monogamy [3]. According to the same study, 24.6% of heterosexual men and 16.3% of heterosexual women had been in a consensually non-monogamous relationship at some point in their lives. These group differences might be attributable to the fact that, because LGBTQ+ persons are less likely to endorse heteronormative scripts, they are more inclined to consider and explore alternative scripts and relationship configurations [18]. As for bisexual individuals more specifically, Me Lean posits that their greater likelihood of engaging in consensual non-monogamy might be due to the possibility that it allows for the pursuit of sexual and romantic experiences with partners of different genders [2]. Also, another study suggests that the exploration of non-monogamy in and of itself can be an incentive for the exploration of plurisexuality [19].

Because non-monogamous relationship configurations have long been regarded as harmful to both society and individuals [4, 20], researchers have investigated their potential effects on individuals’ psychological and relational well-being [21, 22]. Earlier studies have shown that being in a non-monogamous relationship is associated with poor well-being [23, 24]. By contrast, several recent studies have found that consensually non-monogamous relationships present high levels of sexual satisfaction [1], intimacy, commitment, and relationship satisfaction [25, 26]. Moreover, recent research has shown no differences between non-monogamous and monogamous individuals in terms of psychological and relational well-being [22, 27, 28], life satisfaction [21], and health [29]. It is noteworthy that the studies suggesting negative implications of non-monogamous relationships predominantly stem from research conducted in the 1970s and 1980s, while more contemporary investigations tend to emphasize the positive aspects of such relationships. One of the salient factors contributing to these disparities may be the shifting societal perspectives and attitudes toward non-monogamous relationships over time. Another possible explanation for these conflicting findings is the oversight or neglect of potentially confounding variables that may also impact relationship well-being and satisfaction, such as the legal relationship recognition, and infidelity.

Indeed, legal recognition has been documented as an important correlate of relational happiness and well-being [30, 31]. A large number of studies has documented that married couples report greater levels of overall health, happiness, and well-being than unmarried same-gender [32, 33] and different-gender couples [3437]. It is possible that the observed differences between monogamous and non-monogamous couples could be attributable to legal recognition. However, the association between marriage and positive relational outcomes may not only be due to civil recognition, but also to what marriage entails, such as the division of domestic labor and childrearing duties, the allocation of family resources, and daily social support, all of which are also relevant to unmarried cohabiting couples [30, 31].

Given that most studies have compared married and unmarried people without accounting for cohabitation, it is difficult to determine whether the documented positive relational outcomes are attributable to relationship status (i.e., being married) or to cohabitation more specifically [30]. Yet, studies having compared married and unmarried cohabiting couples have yielded conflicting results. On the one hand, some studies report that marriage, compared to nonmarital cohabitation, is associated with greater well-being [31, 38, 39] and relationship quality among different-sex couples [40, 41]. On the other hand, other studies did not find any differences between cohabitation and marriage [30, 42] or have found that differences in relational well-being favoring married individuals were attributable to other variables, such as gender roles and social and institutional support for marriage [43, 44].

Infidelity has also been found to be associated with lower relationship quality and psychological well-being [4547]. Recent studies have found infidelity to be strongly associated with insecure attachment [48], relationship dissatisfaction [49], and poor mental health outcomes [47, 50]. Given that approximately 20 to 25% of people in the U.S. [51] report having engaged in extradyadic sexual activity without their partner’s knowledge or consent, infidelity is an important factor to consider when examining well-being and relationship quality. However, most research typically defines infidelity as engaging in sexual or romantic activities outside of the primary relationship, without considering the importance of relationship agreement [52, 53]. Consequently, many participants in consensually non-monogamous relationships who reported extradyadic sexual or emotional intimacy could be inaccurately labeled as unfaithful in these studies, potentially leading to biased findings. In this context, it is crucial to emphasize that relationship agreement serves as an essential framework for comprehensively understanding the concept of infidelity and its implications within various relational dynamics.

The internet, including dating applications, has also been known to facilitate extradyadic connections and encounters [5457]. According to U.S. data, 42% of people using dating applications are in a committed relationship [55]. The seeking of sexual or romantic connections online while being partnered could be examined as an indicator of infidelity not only in monogamous relationships, but also in consensually non-monogamous relationships, depending on the specific relationship agreement (e.g., open relationship; polyamory; etc.).

Most studies having examined diverse relationship configurations concurrently with the aforementioned relationship elements have been conducted among gay men [9]. For this study we have selected five relationship indicators available in our dataset. To our knowledge, no study has investigated legal relationship status, cohabitation, relationship agreement, and the use of internet to seek sexual and romantic partner and their association with psychological and relational well-being in a sexually diverse sample. The first aim of the present study was to address this gap and to identify subgroups characterised by similar multidimensional romantic relationship patterns in a large, sexually diverse sample, using a person-centred approach. In addition, some authors have speculated that certain sociodemographic variables, such as sexual orientation, and socioeconomic status can predict one’s propensity to engage in consensually non-monogamous relationships [e.g., 6, 58, 59]. For instance, studies have shown that individuals with higher socioeconomic status are overrepresented in non-monogamous relationships [58, 59]. This overrepresentation may be attributed to financial barriers associated with non-monogamy (e.g., additional costs for travel, activities, and entertainment when dating multiple partners), which can disproportionately impact individuals from marginalized socioeconomic backgrounds.

However, very few studies have assessed the prevalence of these sociodemographic characteristics across relationship configurations. Therefore, the second objective of this study was to explore the relative distribution of these characteristics across various relationship structures. The present study’s third objective was to investigate well-being indicators and perceived partner support across different relationship profiles.

Materials and methods

Participants

A community sample (N = 4,746) completed an online survey as part of the Understanding the Inclusion and Exclusion of LGBTQ People (UNIE-LGBTQ) research project. Eligible participants needed to be able to understand French or English, be at least 18 years old, identify as LGBTQ+, and reside in Quebec (Canada). Participants were recruited via printed media, community partners’ listservs, websites, and social media, and through word of mouth. To address concerns about illegitimate responses and duplicate entries, we implemented several strategies to verify the authenticity of participant data. These strategies included examining the consistency of responses across survey sections, cross-referencing email and IP addresses to identify potential duplicates, and conducting manual reviews of unexpected responses to identify any patterns indicative of illegitimate responses. People who did not provide any information on their relationship status or duration and who were not currently in a relationship were excluded from analyses (n = 2,037). Further, as some of the selected measures used a 12-month timeframe, participants who had been in a relationship for less than one year were also excluded (n = 367). The analytical sample (n = 1,528) only included participants who provided complete data on the five latent profile indicators (excluding 814 more cases). See Table 1 for the sample’s demographic characteristics.

Measures

Demographics.

We collected data on respondents’ age, education (less than college degree; college education or more), household income, partners’ gender (same gender; different gender; partners from multiple genders), and relationship duration. Gender modality (cisgender vs transgender) and gender identity (man, woman, nonbinary) were documented using the Multidimensional Sex/Gender Measure [60]. For analytical purposes, participants were categorized as cisgender men, cisgender women, transgender men, transgender women, and nonbinary individuals. Polyamorous individuals were instructed to respond with their longest relationship in mind. This decision was made to simplify the data collection process and minimize response time. Yet, it may overlook the uniqueness of concurrent relationships, such as differences in investment, satisfaction, commitment, and communication between primary and secondary partners, which are critical to understanding the full spectrum of polyamorous experiences and relationships [6163].

Relationship indicators.

Legal relationship status. Participants’ legal relationship status was assessed using the following question: “What is your legal marital status right now?”. Participants were grouped into two categories: those who were in a legally recognized relationship (common law/civil union/married), and those who were not.

Relationship agreement. Relationship agreement was evaluated with the question: “What is your current relationship status?”, with three options to choose from: (1) monogamous (i.e., having one romantic partner and a monogamous sexual agreement), (2) sexually non-monogamous (having one romantic partner, but a non-monogamous sexual agreement) and (3) polyamorous (having more than one romantic partner).

Cohabitation status. Participants were asked: “Do you live with your partner?” (yes/no). Those who were in a polyamorous relationship were asked whether they lived with at least one of their partners.

Internet use to find sexual and/or romantic partners. Participants were asked whether they sought outside sexual partners (“In the past 12 months, how often have you used the Internet to find a sexual partner?”) and outside romantic partners (“In the past 12 months, how often have you used the Internet to find a romantic partner?”) on the Internet. Response anchors were: 1 –Never or almost never, 2 –Once or a few times in the last year, 3 –Once or a few times a month, 4 –Once or a few times a week, and 5 –Every day, or almost every day. For each item, participants were categorized into one of three groups: those who have never or almost never sought a partner on the internet (never or almost never), those who have done so once or a few times in the last year, and those who have done so once per month or more.

Outcome variables.

Well-being was measured using the Mental Health Continuum–Short Form [MHC–SF; see 64], a 14-item questionnaire composed of three subscales: (1) emotional well-being (e.g. “How often did you feel interested in life?”), (2) social well-being (e.g. “How often did you feel that you had something important to contribute to society?”), and (3) psychological well-being (e.g. “How often did you feel that your life has a purpose or meaning?”). Participants were asked to answer each item with the last month in mind. Response options ranged from 0 (never) to 5 (every day). All three subscales’ internal consistency was adequate (Cronbach’s α ranging from .83 to .89).

Social provisions from a relationship partner was measured using the 10-item Canadian version of the Social Provisions Scale [SPS; 65]. The scale assesses five two-item constructs: (1) Attachment (e.g., “My romantic partner(s) provide(s) me with a sense of emotional security and well-being”), (2) Reliable alliance (e.g., “I can count on my romantic partner(s) in an emergency”), (3) Guidance (e.g., “I can talk to my romantic partner(s) about important decisions in my life”), (4) Reassurance of worth (e.g., “My competence and skill are recognized by my romantic partner(s)”), and (5) Social integration (e.g., “My romantic partner(s) enjoy(s) the same social activities I do”). Responses ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). Polychoric correlation coefficients showed satisfactory internal consistency for all scales: Attachment (r = .86), Reliability (r = .91), Guidance (r = .81), Reassurance of worth (r = .89) and Social integration (r = .67).

Procedure

A link to the survey hosted on LimeSurvey was shared online (via email, listservs, and the project’s website and social media), via printed media, and through word of mouth. Once on the LimeSurvey website and before beginning the survey, interested participants were presented a consent form. Upon indicating consent electronically, participants were required to answer eligibility questions. Eligible participants then accessed the survey, which took 50 to 75 minutes to complete. Participants did not receive any incentive for completing the survey. This study was approved by the Institutional Research Ethics Board of the Université du Québec à Montréal (Québec, Canada) (Protocol #2775).

Data analyses

To identify relationship structure classes, we performed a latent class analysis (LCA) using Latent Gold 6.0 [66]. LCA uses observed indicators to identify homogeneous patterns, or “classes”, of specific latent constructs [67]. Five indicators were included to describe relationship structure: (1) social and legal relationship status, (2) cohabitation status, (3) relationship agreement, (4) the seeking of outside sexual partners, and (5) the seeking of outside romantic partners. Latent Gold accounts for missing data using the full information maximum likelihood estimation [68]. First, we estimated LCA models including 1 to 7 classes. The models were compared across fit indices and class sizes. Low Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), low maximum bivariate residuals, and L2 p-values greater than .05 suggest better model fit and entropy values closer to 1 indicate better class separation [69]. Class interpretability was also considered [70]. Second, based on posterior probabilities, participants were assigned to latent classes [71]. Third, to assess class membership differences on sociodemographic covariates, we performed between-class ANOVA-type comparisons implemented in Latent Gold using the maximum likelihood estimator, controlling for age, where the category-specific effects are interpreted in terms of deviation from the average [72]. Class membership differences in outcomes (well-being, and social provision) were explored through multinomial regressions and adjusting for sociodemographic variables such as for age, gender modality and identity, education, gender of the partner, and relationship duration. Class membership differences were estimated using the Bolck-Croon-Hagenaars (BCH) modified bias-correction method, which accounts for uncertainty in class membership [73, 74]. The modified BCH approach has been recommended for both continuous and binary outcomes variables [73]. At each step, we computed robust variance estimations to prevent standard errors underestimation. When omnibus tests utilizing Wald’s criterion revealed significant between-class differences, we examined the Bonferroni-corrected post hoc comparisons to maintain the familywise Type I error rate at 0.05 (by dividing the Type 1 error rate by the number of pairwise comparisons). Missing data were handled via Latent Gold default parameters [72].

Results

Sample characteristics

On average, participants were 37.03 years old (SD = 13.57). The analytical sample was relatively evenly distributed across age groups (see Table 1). Approximately 87% of respondents were cisgender, and 13% were transgender or nonbinary. Over half of participants reported holding a college or university degree (64%), and 85% indicated having a household income greater than $30,000 CAD. Approximately half of respondents reported a relationship duration of 1 to 5 years (50%).

Latent classes

A five-class solution was identified as the optimal model (see Table 2). The indicators’ conditional probabilities for each class are presented in Table 3. Class 1 (59%; formalized monogamy) includes participants in legally recognized unions (married, civil union, or common-law) with a monogamous agreement, who are cohabiting, and who had never or rarely used the internet to seek another sexual or romantic partner in the last year. Class 2 (20%; free monogamy) was composed of participants in relationships that were not legally recognized, who had a monogamous agreement, who were not cohabiting, and who had never or almost never used the internet to seek another sexual or romantic partner in the last year. Class 3 (7%; monogamous considering alternatives) included participants who were monogamous and cohabiting and had used the internet once or a few times over the previous year to seek sexual and romantic partners. Class 4 (11%; formalized open relationship) described participants who were in open, cohabiting, and legally recognized unions, and who used the internet once a month or more often to find sexual, but not romantic partners, in the last year. Class 5 (3%; free consensual non-monogamies) was the smallest and included participants who were in open relationships that had not been formalized, and who had used the internet once a month or more often in the past year to look for other sexual and romantic partners.

thumbnail
Table 3. Class description across relationship indicators.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309954.t003

Class differences on sociodemographic characteristics

Table 4 shows the five latent classes’ sociodemographic composition. Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons revealed that participants in the free monogamy class were younger than those in formalized monogamy and formalized open relationship classes (all p < .001). As age is a potential confounder for most sociodemographic variables [75, 76], all subsequent Wald tests and corresponding p-values were computed on age-adjusted proportions. The free monogamy, monogamous considering alternatives and free consensual non-monogamies classes reported lower relationship durations than those in other classes (all p < .001). The formalized monogamy and formalized open relationship classes reported higher household incomes (> $99,000 CAD) than the other classes (all p < .001).

Bonferroni-corrected between-class comparisons also revealed that cisgender men were overrepresented in the formalized open relationship (72%) and free consensual non-monogamies (68%) classes and underrepresented in the formalized monogamy (33%) and free monogamy (38%) classes. Conversely, cisgender women were significantly more likely to belong to the formalized monogamy (55%) and free monogamy (52%) classes than to any other class (all p < .001). While nonbinary individuals predominantly belonged to the monogamous considering alternatives and formalized open relationship classes, the prevalence of nonbinary persons in the remaining classes were not statistically different from one another, likely due to limited statistical power afforded by the small subsample of nonbinary individuals. Regarding partner gender, unadjusted results indicate that individuals in the monogamous considering alternatives, formalized open relationship, and free consensual non-monogamies classes were more likely to be paired with multiple genders (all p < .05). However, the Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc comparisons yielded nonsignificant results.

Class differences on well-being and social provisions from partner

Table 5 shows between-class differences on well-being and social provisions from a relationship partner. After adjusting the models for potential confounders (age, gender modality and identity, education, partner gender, and relationship duration), participants in the formalized monogamy class reported higher levels of social and emotional well-being than those in the formalized open relationship class. No other class differences on well-being indicators were found. Regarding social provision indicators, individuals in the free monogamy class indicated lower levels of reliable alliance, guidance, and social integration compared to those in the formalized monogamy class. Similarly, the free consensual non-monogamies class reported lower levels of guidance and attachment from their relationship partners than individuals in the formalized monogamy class.

thumbnail
Table 5. Class differences on well-being indicators and social provisions from relationship partner.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309954.t005

Discussion

This research used LCA to investigate relationship structure patterns and their correlates among LGBTQ+ individuals in Canada. Five latent classes were identified. Two classes were composed of participants in a monogamous relationship: those who were legally married and cohabiting (formalized monogamy; 59%), and those who were legally single and non-cohabiting (free monogamy; 20%). The three other classes were participants with non-monogamous practices. One class was composed of cohabiting participants with a monogamous relationship agreement who sought outside sexual and/or romantic partners on the internet once or a few times a year (monogamous considering alternatives; 7%). The other two classes were composed of consensual monogamous participants: those in a legally recognized relationship with a non-monogamous relationship agreement who cohabit and who often seek outside sexual partners on the internet (formalized open relationship; 11%), and those who are legally single with a non-monogamous relationship agreement, who do not live with their partner, and who often seek outside sexual and/or romantic partners (free consensual non-monogamies; 3%).

The proportion of participants involved in consensual non-monogamy in the overall sample (14% open relationship; 3% polyamorous) was greater than that reported in previous studies [3, 10], likely reflecting the absence of cisheterosexual persons in the present sample. As suggested by Rutherford et al., LGBTQ+ persons’ non-normative sexuality and gender expression may provide a context that is conducive to the development of non-normative relationship configurations in which sexual and affective exclusivity are negotiated rather than assumed [17].

Sociodemographic differences were found between classes. First, congruently with previous research [8, 11, 25, 77, 78], the present study showed that cisgender sexual minority women were more likely than cisgender sexual minority men to engage in monogamous relationships, and the latter were more likely than the former to be in open relationships. Some studies suggest that lesbian women may have a greater adhesion to heteronormative gender roles prescribing monogamy, marriage, and family [79, 80] than do gay men. This discrepancy may be attributed to apprehensions linked to sexual double-standards and the societal censure that women might encounter when articulating their sexual inclinations and preferences within consensually non-monogamous relationships [52]. In contrast, gay men have been found to be more resistant to heteronormative sexuality, allowing them to more freely embrace diverging relationship ideals and configurations or to resist those that are socially enforced [81]. Some evolutionary psychology hypotheses suggest the existence of sex differences in mating strategies, with cisgender women being more likely to endorse monogamy, and cisgender men, non-monogamy [82, 83].

Contrary to previous findings [58, 59], we did not find white and higher household income participants to be overrepresented in non-monogamous classes. In the present sample, the free monogamy class was comprised of individuals reporting lower household income, which may reflect the overrepresentation of younger participants in this class. The fact that these individuals generally have lower personal incomes and are less likely to cohabitate with their partners can negatively affect their household income.

Further, in the present study, educational attainment was not significantly associated with class membership. Research having examined this question presents conflicting findings, with some studies suggesting lower levels of education among individuals engaged in non-monogamous relationships [84], and others reporting greater proportions of highly educated individuals among non-monogamous participants [59]. These inconsistencies suggest the involvement of other unmeasured variables and underscore the need for further investigation.

The present study also found that participants in the free consensual non-monogamies class reported lower levels of emotional and social well-being than those in the formalized monogamy class. Similarly, individuals who were legally single (i.e., free monogamy and free consensual non-monogamies) reported lower overall levels of perceived partner support than those in the formalized monogamy class. Such group differences could be attributable to several factors. First, choosing or intending to have one’s relationship legally recognized might reflect preexisting relationship characteristics, such as greater levels of satisfaction [85] or mutual support. Second, between-group differences in perceived partner support may be indicative of relational marginalization, that is, the stigmatization of same-sex unions at the family, community, and societal levels and its detrimental impact on such relationships [86, 87]. While legal recognition can provide some validation to same-sex unions, its absence might lead to further marginalization or stigmatization, which are known to negatively affect relationship functioning [88].

A third possible explanation for the low levels of perceived partner support within the free consensual non-monogamies class is the possibility that some individuals in the former group were in a transitional stage toward an open relationship. Many non-monogamous relationships were initially monogamous, and the process of negotiating rules and setting boundaries, as well as the trials and errors that many partners engage in when first practicing non-monogamy can create a context that fosters relationship instability, which could feed into perceptions of lower partner support.

Finally, it is also possible that some participants in the free consensual non-monogamies class reluctantly consented to a non-monogamy agreement or were otherwise dissatisfied with their relationship agreement. One recent study suggest that non-monogamous relationships characterized by lower levels of mutual consent and comfort regarding their relationship agreement demonstrated the lowest levels of individual and relationship functioning [89]. Future non-monogamy research examining well-being and relationship quality should account for relationship stage (i.e., transitional/negotiation stage) as well as consent and comfort vis-à-vis relationship agreement.

The absence of significant group differences on well-being indicators is inconsistent with studies that have documented poorer well-being among individuals in non-monogamous relationships [23, 24]. However, this finding aligns with research showing no differences between monogamous and non-monogamous individuals regarding well-being [22, 27], life satisfaction [21], health, and happiness [29]. Some studies also suggest that compersion—the positive attitudes, thoughts, and actions that arise in response to one’s intimate partner’s extradyadic intimate relationships [90]—significantly correlates with relationship satisfaction in consensually non-monogamous relationships [9092]. As Rubel and Bogaert hypothesized, it might not be the adhesion to non-traditional relationships per se that predicts negative outcomes, but intra-relational characteristics that go beyond sexual and romantic exclusivity [22].

Strengths and limitations

To our knowledge, this study is the first to examine different patterns of relationship configurations and to investigate group differences on sociodemographic characteristics, well-being indicators, and perceived partner support in a large LGBTQ+ sample. However, some limitations should be noted. First, its cross-sectional and retrospective design is subject to recall bias, precluding any causal inferences. Further, as all data were self-reported, they may be subject to social desirability bias. Because the sampling for this study was convenient (non-probabilistic), no claim about the representativeness of our results can be made. In addition, considering that multiple and diverse recruitment strategies were utilized, the present findings may not be generalizable to the broader population of LGBTQ+ individuals. Further, relationship agreement was measured differently for monogamous and sexually non-monogamous (i.e., open) relationships than it was for polyamorous relationships. Number of partners (i.e., one romantic partner) and relationship agreement (i.e., having a monogamous/non-monogamous sexual agreement) was assessed in the former, while only the number of partners (i.e., having more than one romantic partner) was assessed in the latter. This means that participants with an open relationship agreement did not need to have any outside sexual partners at the time of the study, while those with a polyamorous agreement needed to be actively practicing polyamory to be categorized as polyamorous. The results might have been different if relationship agreement had been measured the same way across relationship types. Additionally, it is important to note that we only collected participants’ relationship agreements, which might differ from what their partners would have answered. Lastly, the measure assessing internet use to seek outside sexual/romantic partners may not have fully captured the extent of extradyadic romantic and sexual involvement and having “Never or almost never” as a response anchor rather than separate “Never” and “Almost never” anchors could have limited the measure’s precision, muddling possible distinctions between individuals who are consistently faithful and those who are not.

Implications

While the current findings highlight the importance of considering different types of relationship configurations in future studies–notably in LGBTQ+ samples–they also suggest that relationship agreement alone is insufficient when examining well-being and relationship quality. Indeed, cohabitation, legal relationship status, and the seeking of outside romantic or sexual partners online, can also influence relational outcomes. To better understand the associations between romantic relationships and individual and relational well-being among LGBTQ+ individuals, future research should consider additional variables such as the intention or desire to formalize one’s relationship, relationship secrecy or perceived social support for one’s relationship, satisfaction with one’s relationship agreement, and internalized sexual stigma. Future infidelity research would also benefit from accounting for relationship agreement or configuration to avoid misclassifying individuals practicing consensual non-monogamy as unfaithful. Future relationship diversity research collecting data from both (or more) partners could also yield greater insight.

Future programs for LGBTQ+ individuals, as well as therapists working with these populations, should not assume monogamy and should account for the diversity of sexual and romantic configurations. Therapists working with individuals in consensually non-monogamous relationships also should not assume that they involve infidelity or that their relationship agreement is the cause of poor relationship functioning. These approaches can foster a more inclusive environment that promotes healthy relationships and overall well-being. Considering the prevalence of seeking outside sexual partners online, sexual health education should also incorporate discussions about safer practices, consent, and risk reduction in online encounters. This education can empower individuals to make informed decisions and mitigate potential health risks associated with extradyadic sexual activities.

Conclusion

The present study explored various relationship structures among LGBTQ+ individuals in Quebec (Canada), identifying five distinct patterns ranging from formalized monogamy to non-monogamous agreements. The prevalence of non-monogamous relationships in this sample exceeded previous estimates, reflecting the unique sociocultural context of LGBTQ+ individuals. Gender played a significant role, with cisgender women more inclined towards monogamy, while cisgender men leaned towards non-monogamy, challenging conventional assumptions. The study also revealed disparities in perceived partner support across different relationship classes, potentially linked to factors such as the cultural significance of marriage, internalized sexual stigma, and relationship stage (e.g., pre-, post-, or during transition from monogamy to consensual non-monogamy). However, no significant differences were found in well-being between monogamous and non-monogamous individuals, suggesting that relationship dynamics may be a more important factor than relationship agreement per se. This study offers valuable insight into the diverse landscape of LGBTQ+ relationships in Canada, emphasizing the need for nuanced exploration of the factors shaping these relationships, with implications for research and support services within the LGBQ+ community. Understanding this complexity is also essential for creating healthier, more inclusive, and supportive environments for relationship diversity among LGBQ+ communities.

Acknowledgments

The Understanding the Inclusion and Exclusion of LGBTQ People research (UNIE-LGBTQ) is a research partnership of universities, public agencies, semi-public and community-based organizations, and private enterprises dedicating their efforts to better understand situations in which LGBTQ + people are demeaned, rejected, and belittled, and deprived of the full extent of their rights in important life domains. The authors extend their gratitude to their research partners as well as to the participants who generously shared their experiences with them.

References

  1. 1. Conley TD, Matsick JL, Moors AC, Ziegler A. Investigation of consensually nonmonogamous relationships: Theories, methods, and new directions. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2017 Mar 1;12(2):205–32.
  2. 2. McLean K. Negotiating (non)monogamy. J Bisexuality. 2004 Jul 1;4(1–2):83–97.
  3. 3. Haupert ML, Gesselman AN, Moors AC, Fisher HE, Garcia JR. Prevalence of experiences with consensual nonmonogamous relationships: Findings from two national samples of single Americans. J Sex Marital Ther. 2017 Jul 4;43(5):424–40. pmid:27096488
  4. 4. Moors AC. Has the American public’s interest in information related to relationships beyond “the couple” increased over time? J Sex Res. 2017 Jul 24;54(6):677–84. pmid:27215273
  5. 5. Hamilton LD, De Santis C, Thompson AE. Introduction to the Special Section on Consensual Non-Monogamy. Arch Sex Behav. 2021 May 1;50(4):1217–23. pmid:34089124
  6. 6. Haritaworn J, Lin C ju, Klesse C. Poly/logue: A critical introduction to polyamory. Sexualities. 2006 Dec 1;9(5):515–29.
  7. 7. Klesse C, Cardoso D, Pallotta-Chiarolli M, Raab M, Schadler C, Schippers M. Introduction: Parenting, polyamory and consensual non-monogamy. Critical and queer perspectives. Sexualities. 2022 Jul 14;13634607221114466.
  8. 8. Levine EC, Herbenick D, Martinez O, Fu TC, Dodge B. Open relationships, onconsensual nonmonogamy, and monogamy among U.S. adults: Findings from the 2012 national survey of sexual health and behavior. Arch Sex Behav. 2018 Jul 1;47(5):1439–50.
  9. 9. Balzarini RN, Dharma C, Kohut T, Holmes BM, Campbell L, Lehmiller JJ, et al. Demographic comparison of american individuals in polyamorous and monogamous relationships. J Sex Res. 2019 Jul 24;56(6):681–94. pmid:29913084
  10. 10. Matsick JL, Conley TD, Ziegler A, Moors AC, Rubin JD. Love and sex: polyamorous relationships are perceived more favourably than swinging and open relationships. Psychol Sex. 2014 Ottobre;5(4):339–48.
  11. 11. Fairbrother N, Hart TA, Fairbrother M. Open relationship prevalence, characteristics, and correlates in a nationally representative sample of Canadian adults. J Sex Res. 2019 Jul 24;56(6):695–704. pmid:30932711
  12. 12. Blumstein P, Schwartz P. American couples: Money, work, sex. New York: William Morrow; 1983.
  13. 13. Hoff CC, Beougher SC. Sexual agreements among gay male couples. Arch Sex Behav. 2010 Jun 1;39(3):774–87. pmid:18686027
  14. 14. Solomon SE, Rothblum ED, Balsam KF. Money, housework, sex, and conflict: Same-sex couples in civil unions, those not in civil unions, and heterosexual married siblings. Sex Roles. 2005 May 1;52(9):561–75.
  15. 15. Barker M, Langdridge D. Whatever happened to non-monogamies? Critical reflections on recent research and theory. Sexualities. 2010 Dicembre;13(6):748–72.
  16. 16. Perez J, Pepping CA. Relationship experiences of transgender and non-binary adults: exploring dating goals, relationship structures, minority stress, sexual fetishization, and relationship victimization. Int J Transgender Health. 2024;0(0):1–20.
  17. 17. Rutherford L, Stark A, Ablona A, Klassen BJ, Higgins R, Jacobsen H, et al. Health and well-being of trans and non-binary participants in a community-based survey of gay, bisexual, and queer men, and non-binary and Two-Spirit people across Canada. PLOS ONE. 2021 Feb 11;16(2):e0246525. pmid:33571252
  18. 18. Kontula O, Haavio-Mannila E. Sexual Pleasures: Enhancement of Sex Life in Finland 1971–1992. Aldershot: Dartmouth; 1995.
  19. 19. Braida N. Plurisexualities and consensual non-monogamies: Challenging normativities in Italy. In: Bisexuality in Europe. Routledge; 2020.
  20. 20. Conley TD, Ziegler A, Moors AC, Matsick JL, Valentine B. A critical examination of popular assumptions about the benefits and outcomes of monogamous relationships. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2013 Maggio;17(2):124–41. pmid:23175520
  21. 21. LaSala MC. Monogamy of the heart. J Gay Lesbian Soc Serv. 2004 Ottobre;17(3):1–24.
  22. 22. Rubel AN, Bogaert AF. Consensual nonmonogamy: Psychological well-being and relationship quality correlates. J Sex Res. 2015 Nov 22;52(9):961–82. pmid:25189189
  23. 23. Gilmartin BG. Sexual deviance and social networks: A study of social, family, and marital interaction patterns among co-marital sex participants. Monogamy. 1974;291–323.
  24. 24. Murstein BI, Case D, Gunn SP. Personality correlates of ex-swingers. Lifestyles. 1985 Sep 1;8(1):21–34.
  25. 25. Træen B, Thuen F. Non-consensual and consensual non-monogamy in Norway. Int J Sex Health. 2021 Luglio;0(0):1–16. pmid:38595687
  26. 26. Mitchell JW. Between and Within Couple-Level Factors Associated with Gay Male Couples’ Investment in a Sexual Agreement. AIDS Behav. 2014 Aug 1;18(8):1454–65. pmid:24327185
  27. 27. Hosking W. Agreements about extra-dyadic sex in gay men’s relationships: Exploring differences in relationship quality by agreement type and rule-breaking behavior. J Homosex. 2013 May 1;60(5):711–33. pmid:23593955
  28. 28. Séguin LJ, Blais M, Goyer MF, Adam BD, Lavoie F, Rodrigue C, et al. Examining relationship quality across three types of relationship agreements. Sexualities. 2017 Feb 1;20(1–2):86–104.
  29. 29. Fleckenstein JR, Cox DW. The association of an open relationship orientation with health and happiness in a sample of older US adults. Sex Relatsh Ther. 2015 Jan 2;30(1):94–116.
  30. 30. Musick K, Bumpass L. Cohabitation, marriage, and trajectories in well-being and relationships. 2006 Feb 1 [cited 2022 Feb 12]; https://escholarship.org/uc/item/34f1h2nt
  31. 31. Waite LJ, Bachrach C. The ties that bind: Perspectives on marriage and cohabitation. Transaction Publishers; 2000. 418 p.
  32. 32. Lannutti PJ. “This is not a lesbian wedding”: Examining same-sex marriage and bisexual-lesbian couples. J Bisexuality. 2008 Jul 1;7(3–4):237–60.
  33. 33. Ramos C, Goldberg NG, Badgett MVL. The effects of marriage equality in massachusetts: A survey of the experiences and impact of marriage on same-sex couples. 2009 May 17 [cited 2022 Mar 15]; https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9dx6v3kj
  34. 34. Glenn ND, Weaver CN. The changing relationship of marital status to reported happiness. J Marriage Fam. 1988;50(2):317–24.
  35. 35. Hu Y, Goldman N. Mortality differentials by marital status: An international comparison. Demography. 1990 May 1;27(2):233–50. pmid:2332088
  36. 36. Lee GR, Seccombe K, Shehan CL. Marital status and personal happiness: An analysis of trend data. J Marriage Fam. 1991;53(4):839–44.
  37. 37. Stack S, Eshleman JR. Marital status and happiness: A 17-nation study. J Marriage Fam. 1998;60(2):527–36.
  38. 38. Brown SL. The effect of union type on psychological well-being: Depression among cohabitors versus marrieds. J Health Soc Behav. 2000;41(3):241–55. pmid:11011503
  39. 39. Horwitz AV, White HR. The relationship of cohabitation and mental health: a study of a young adult cohort. J Marriage Fam. 1998;60(2):505–14.
  40. 40. Brown SL, Booth A. Cohabitation versus marriage: A comparison of relationship quality. J Marriage Fam. 1996;58(3):668–78.
  41. 41. Stanley SM, Whitton SW, Markman HJ. Maybe I do: Interpersonal commitment and premarital or nonmarital cohabitation. J Fam Issues. 2004 May 1;25(4):496–519.
  42. 42. Ross CE. Reconceptualizing marital status as a continuum of social attachment. J Marriage Fam. 1995;57(1):129–40.
  43. 43. Fowers BJ, Laurenceau JP, Penfield RD, Cohen LM, Lang SF, Owenz MB, et al. Enhancing relationship quality measurement: The development of the Relationship Flourishing Scale. J Fam Psychol. 2016;30(8):997–1007. pmid:27918187
  44. 44. Lee KS, Ono H. Marriage, cohabitation, and happiness: A cross-national analysis of 27 countries. J Marriage Fam. 2012;74(5):953–72.
  45. 45. Gibson KAV, Thompson AE, O’Sullivan LF. Love thy neighbour: Personality traits, relationship quality, and attraction to others as predictors of infidelity among young adults. Can J Hum Sex. 2016 Dec;25(3):186–98.
  46. 46. Mattingly BA, Wilson K, Clark EM, Bequette AW, Weidler DJ. Foggy faithfulness: Relationship quality, religiosity, and the perceptions of dating infidelity scale in an adult sample. J Fam Issues. 2010 Nov 1;31(11):1465–80.
  47. 47. Warach B, Josephs L. The aftershocks of infidelity: A review of infidelity-based attachment trauma. Sex Relatsh Ther. 2021 Jan 2;36(1):68–90.
  48. 48. Warach B, Josephs L, Gorman BS. Pathways to infidelity: The roles of self-serving bias and betrayal trauma. J Sex Marital Ther. 2018 Jul 4;44(5):497–512. pmid:29240538
  49. 49. Fincham FD, May RW. Infidelity in romantic relationships. Curr Opin Psychol. 2017 Feb 1;13:70–4. pmid:28813298
  50. 50. Cano A, O’Leary KD. Infidelity and separations precipitate major depressive episodes and symptoms of nonspecific depression and anxiety. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68:774–81. pmid:11068963
  51. 51. Luo S, Cartun MA, Snider AG. Assessing extradyadic behavior: A review, a new measure, and two new models. Personal Individ Differ. 2010 Aug 1;49(3):155–63.
  52. 52. Scoats R, Campbell C. What do we know about consensual non-monogamy? Curr Opin Psychol. 2022 Dec 1;48:101468. pmid:36215906
  53. 53. Thompson AE, Wilder D, Kulibert D. Examining variations in participation and outcomes of consensual and nonconsensual extradyadic behavior among Ashley Madison users. J Sex Res. 2021 Nov 22;58(9):1194–204. pmid:33871291
  54. 54. Hobbs M, Owen S, Gerber L. Liquid love? Dating apps, sex, relationships and the digital transformation of intimacy. J Sociol. 2017 Giugno;53(2):271–84.
  55. 55. Timmermans E, De Caluwé E, Alexopoulos C. Why are you cheating on tinder? Exploring users’ motives and (dark) personality traits. Comput Hum Behav. 2018 Dicembre;89:129–39.
  56. 56. Weiser DA, Niehuis S, Flora J, Punyanunt-Carter NM, Arias VS, Hannah Baird R. Swiping right: Sociosexuality, intentions to engage in infidelity, and infidelity experiences on Tinder. Personal Individ Differ. 2018 Ottobre;133:29–33.
  57. 57. Vossler A. Internet Infidelity 10 Years On: A Critical Review of the Literature. Fam J. 2016 Oct 1;24(4):359–66.
  58. 58. Klesse C. Theorizing multi-partner relationships and sexualities–Recent work on non-monogamy and polyamory. Sexualities. 2018 Oct 1;21(7):1109–24.
  59. 59. Sheff E, Hammers C. The privilege of perversities: Race, class and education among polyamorists and kinksters. Psychol Sex. 2011 Settembre;2(3):198–223.
  60. 60. Bauer GR, Braimoh J, Scheim AI, Dharma C. Transgender-inclusive measures of sex/gender for population surveys: Mixed-methods evaluation and recommendations. PLOS ONE. 2017 mag;12(5):e0178043. pmid:28542498
  61. 61. Balzarini RN, Campbell L, Kohut T, Holmes BM, Lehmiller JJ, Harman JJ, et al. Perceptions of primary and secondary relationships in polyamory. PLOS ONE. 2017 mag;12(5):e0177841. pmid:28542619
  62. 62. Mogilski JK, Memering SL, Welling LLM, Shackelford TK. Monogamy versus Consensual Non-Monogamy: Alternative Approaches to Pursuing a Strategically Pluralistic Mating Strategy. Arch Sex Behav. 2017 Feb 1;46(2):407–17. pmid:26679305
  63. 63. Mogilski JK, Reeve SD, Nicolas SCA, Donaldson SH, Mitchell VE, Welling LLM. Jealousy, Consent, and Compersion Within Monogamous and Consensually Non-Monogamous Romantic Relationships. Arch Sex Behav. 2019 Aug 1;48(6):1811–28. pmid:30607710
  64. 64. Keyes CLM. «Overview of the Mental Health Continuum Short Form (MHC-SF)» [Internet]. 2014. http://www.aacu.org/bringing_theory/documents/MHC-SFBriefintroduciont9.18.203.pdf.
  65. 65. Orpana HM, Lang JJ, Yurkowski K. Validation of a brief version of the Social Provisions Scale using Canadian national survey data. Health Promot Chronic Dis Prev Can. 2019 Dec;39(12):323–32.
  66. 66. Vermunt JK, Magidson J. How to perform three-step latent class analysis in the presence of measurement non-invariance or differential item functioning. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 2021 May 4;28(3):356–64.
  67. 67. Tein JY, Coxe S, Cham H. Statistical power to detect the correct number of classes in latent profile analysis. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 2013 Oct 1;20(4):640–57. pmid:24489457
  68. 68. Edwards SL, Berzofsky ME, Biemer PP. Addressing nonresponse for categorical data items using full information maximum likelihood with latent GOLD 5.0. RTI Press. 2018;9135.
  69. 69. Berlin KS, Williams NA, Parra GR. An introduction to latent variable mixture modeling (Part 1): Overview and cross-sectional latent class and latent profile analyses. J Pediatr Psychol. 2014 Mar 1;39(2):174–87. pmid:24277769
  70. 70. Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A monte carlo simulation study. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 2007 Oct 23;14(4):535–69.
  71. 71. Magidson J, Vermunt JK. Latent class models. The Sage handbook of quantitative methodology for the social sciences; 2004. 175–198 p.
  72. 72. Vermunt JK, Magidson J. Technical Guide for Latent GOLD 5.1: Basic, Advanced, and Syntax. 2016.
  73. 73. Bakk Z, Tekle FB, Vermunt JK. Estimating the association between latent class membership and external variables using bias-adjusted three-step approaches. Sociol Methodol. 2013 Aug 1;43(1):272–311.
  74. 74. Bolck A, Croon M, Hagenaars J. Estimating latent structure models with categorical variables: One-step versus three-step estimators. Polit Anal. 2004;12(1):3–27.
  75. 75. Schuler MS, Prince DM, Collins RL. Disparities in Social and Economic Determinants of Health by Sexual Identity, Gender, and Age: Results from the 2015–2018 National Survey on Drug Use and Health. LGBT Health. 2021 Jul;8(5):330–9. pmid:34101498
  76. 76. Turban JL, Ehrensaft D. Research Review: Gender identity in youth: treatment paradigms and controversies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2018;59(12):1228–43. pmid:29071722
  77. 77. Atkins DC, Baucom DH, Jacobson NS. Understanding infidelity: Correlates in a national random sample. J Fam Psychol. 2001;15(4):735–49. pmid:11770478
  78. 78. Blow AJ, Hartnett K. Infidelity in committed relationships Ii: a substantive review. J Marital Fam Ther. 2005;31(2):217–33. pmid:15974059
  79. 79. Potârcă G, Mills M, Neberich W. Relationship preferences among gay and lesbian online daters: Individual and contextual influences. J Marriage Fam. 2015;77(2):523–41.
  80. 80. Sorensen A. Lesbian Stereotypes in the United States. In: The Wiley Blackwell Encyclopedia of Gender and Sexuality Studies [Internet]. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2016 [cited 2022 Feb 17]. p. 1–5. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1002/9781118663219.wbegss413
  81. 81. van Eeden-Moorefield B, Malloy K, Benson K. Gay men’s (non)monogamy ideals and lived experience. Sex Roles. 2016 Jul 1;75(1):43–55.
  82. 82. Brandon M. Monogamy and Nonmonogamy: Evolutionary Considerations and Treatment Challenges. Sex Med Rev. 2016 Oct 1;4(4):343–52. pmid:27872028
  83. 83. Lippa RA. Sex Differences in Sex Drive, Sociosexuality, and Height across 53 Nations: Testing Evolutionary and Social Structural Theories. Arch Sex Behav. 2009 Oct 1;38(5):631–51. pmid:17975724
  84. 84. Aral SO, Leichliter JS. Non-monogamy: Risk factor for STI transmission and acquisition and determinant of STI spread in populations. Sex Transm Infect. 2010 Dec 1;86:iii29–36. pmid:20924047
  85. 85. Tai T o, Baxter J, Hewitt B. Do co-residence and intentions make a difference? Relationship satisfaction in married, cohabiting, and living apart together couples in four countries. Demogr Res. 2014;31:71–104.
  86. 86. Lehmiller JJ, Agnew CR. Perceived Marginalization and the Prediction of Romantic Relationship Stability. J Marriage Fam. 2007;69(4):1036–49.
  87. 87. Rosenthal L, Starks TJ. Relationship stigma and relationship outcomes in interracial and same-sex relationships: Examination of sources and buffers. J Fam Psychol. 2015;29(6):818–30. pmid:26121534
  88. 88. Doyle DM, Molix L. Social Stigma and Sexual Minorities’ Romantic Relationship Functioning: A Meta-Analytic Review. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2015 Oct 1;41(10):1363–81. pmid:26199218
  89. 89. Hangen F, Crasta D, Rogge RD. Delineating the boundaries between nonmonogamy and infidelity: Bringing consent back into definitions of consensual nonmonogamy with latent profile analysis. J Sex Res. 2020 May 3;57(4):438–57. pmid:31584295
  90. 90. Thouin-Savard MI, Flicker SM. Compersion. In: Shackelford TK, editor. Encyclopedia of Sexual Psychology and Behavior [Internet]. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2023 [cited 2024 Jun 1]. p. 1–7.
  91. 91. Buczel KA, Szyszka PD, Mara I. Exploring Compersion: A Study on Polish Consensually Non-Monogamous Individuals and Adaptation of the COMPERSe Questionnaire [Internet]. 2023 [cited 2024 Jun 1]. https://www.researchsquare.com/article/rs-2838247/v1
  92. 92. Flicker SM, Thouin-Savard MI, Vaughan MD. Factors that Facilitate and Hinder the Experience of Compersion Among Individuals in Consensually Non-Monogamous Relationships. Arch Sex Behav. 2022 Aug 1;51(6):3035–48. pmid:35876980