Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Humor in parenting: Does it have a role?

  • Lucy Emery,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Penn State College of Medicine, Hershey, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Anne Libera,

    Roles Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Theatre Department, Columbia College, Chicago, IL, United States of America

  • Erik Lehman,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Public Health Sciences, Penn State College of Medicine, Hershey, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Benjamin H. Levi

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    bhlevi@psu.edu

    Affiliation Departments of Humanities & Pediatrics, Penn State College of Medicine, Hershey, Pennsylvania, United States of America

Abstract

Background

Despite the widespread use of humor in social interactions and the considerable literature on humor in multiple fields of study, the use of humor in parenting has received very little formal study. The purpose of this pilot study was to gather preliminary data on the use of humor in the raising of children.

Materials and methods

We developed and administered a 10-item survey to measure people’s experiences being raised with humor and their views regarding humor as a parenting tool. Responses were aggregated into Disagree, Indeterminate, and Agree, and analyzed using standard statistical methods.

Results

Respondents (n = 312) predominantly identified as male (63.6%) and white (76.6%) and were (by selection) between the ages of 18–45 years old. The majority of participants reported that they: were raised by people who used humor in their parenting (55.2%); believe humor can be an effective parenting tool (71.8%) and in that capacity has more potential benefit than harm (63.3%); either use (or plan to use) humor in parenting their own children (61.8%); and would value a course on how to utilize humor in parenting (69.7%). Significant correlations were found between the use of humor and both i) the quality of respondents’ relationships with their parents and ii) assessments of how good a job their parents had done.

Conclusions

In this pilot study, respondents of childbearing/rearing age reported positive views about humor as a parenting tool.

Introduction

Parenting can be challenging, especially considering the wide array of responsibilities and other stressors most parents have in their lives. [Parent will be used here as a general term to describe any adult who has a primary role in helping raise a child–including biological parents, stepparents, foster parents, adoptive parents, grandparents, as well as others household members who take on a significant role in child-rearing.] Yet parents are expected (by themselves and others) to know how to respond appropriately when their child needs attention. What makes this particularly difficult is that not only is there no script for raising children, but that when we’re frustrated with our children our reactions often may be driven by experiences with our own parents–which may have been less-than-ideal [1]. There is, of course, no shortage of expert advice for how to raise children [25]. A central tenet of such guidance is the importance of providing structure and consistency–which helps children better understand their environment and its interconnections, anticipate events and their consequences, and appreciate how these inter-relationships create both expectations and responsibilities [6, 7]. That said, it’s also known that both children and parents benefit from developing flexibility and skills for adapting when situations (or our perceptions of them) change [8, 9]. Flexibility of mind (i.e., the ability to switch between different mindsets, tasks, or strategies) [10] promotes resilience by helping us bounce back from a misfortune, recalibrate one’s goals and strategies, or carry on in the face of barriers and setbacks [1113]. For better or worse, raising children offers a great many opportunities to both develop and model such flexibility of mind, to see ‘resistance’ not as a thing to be beaten down, but as an opportunity for reflection [14]. Toward this end, humor could be a valuable parenting tool in part because there are links between humor and cognitive flexibility [1519]. The study described in this paper is a first step in exploring people’s views about humor as a parenting tool–both as children and raising their own children–which could help lay the groundwork for strategies about how to use humor appropriately and productively in times of frustration.

That being said, there is no consensus regarding either what constitutes humor or what grounds it–in part because the term humor is used in various contexts to mean different things [20]. Over the past half-century, a growing body of research has explored the types and structure of humor; [2124] styles of humor (self-enhancing, affiliative, aggressive, and self-defeating); [25] and correlations between particular types of humor and a variety of outcomes–including subjective wellbeing, emotional regulation, healthy relationships, leadership, teaching, and others [2633]. Though humor often manifests verbally (jokes, stories, puns, riddles, satire, sarcasm, parody, everyday conversation, etc.) and the written word (e.g., prose, verse, comics), humor can also take a more physical form–paradigmatically, slapstick, but also visual and performing arts [3437].

Philosophical attempts to characterize humor (sometimes in terms of its progeny, laughter) include works by Plato, Hobbes, Kant, Bergson, and others [19, 38]. At present three of the most commonly referenced theories of humor have emerged from both philosophy and psychology and were intended to encompass all situations of humor and laughter (including inadvertent laughter such as that caused by tickling). Superiority Theory holds that humor and laughter are the result of feelings of superiority over someone else (or the way that we used to be); Incongruity Theory suggests that humor occurs when there is a mismatch between expectations and experience; and Tension and Release Theory, which evolved from Freud’s Theory of the Unconscious, suggests that humor results when a buildup of energy created through repression of negative thoughts or discomfort is released through laughter [38]. A more recent theory proposed by behavioral scientist Peter McGraw combines elements of incongruity theory and superiority theory in the Benign Violation Theory, which posits that a joke or a moment can be perceived as humorous if it is perceived simultaneously as (i) a violation of norms and (ii) benign [39].

On their face, such theories might lead us to view humor as an odd strategy for parenting, and that certain types of humor (especially those involving superiority theory, which includes sarcasm and mockery) are likely to be ineffective if not unhealthy for child-rearing [40]. For example, incongruity-based humor relies on the very opposite of consistency–leveraging surprise, breaking expectations, going the other way [4143]. So, too, because humor’s success is determined in part by how others react, the use of humor by parents could be seen as a ‘relinquishing of authority’ to their child. Moreover, when done poorly or thoughtlessly, humor can send the wrong message to a child or become a weapon for inflicting harm. That said, humor also has several quite promising features. Notably, humor can induce frameshifts (i.e., changes in perspective) that alter how we interpret an event or response, and thereby open new possibilities for children and parents alike. This includes serving as a distraction that helps children reframe their experience; helping parents create psychological distance (e.g., a moment for perspective-taking) between the immediate experience and their reaction to it; and helping create shared experience that can forge deeper connections amongst family members.

In popular culture, one of the strongest associations between humor and parenting is the “dad joke”–often corny, pun-based, and groan-inducing [44]. But humor’s expansive repertoire may have strategic applications for positive parenting techniques akin to how humor has been applied successfully to address various real-world challenges. Tailored improvisation games that use humor and play-based exercises have shown promise for improving the mental health of both individuals with early-stage dementia [45]. and their family caregivers [46]. In the workplace, humor-based play (often cast in terms of improv) has been shown to improve attitudes and coping strategies, leading to greater resilience, cohesiveness, and effective leadership [31, 4750]. So, too, adaptive forms of humor (i.e., affiliative and self-enhancing) are associated with secure attachment [5156], though with children their developmental stage clearly impacts their ability to appreciate different forms of humor [5760].

When used appropriately, humor can change the dynamic of situations that are headed for conflict [6163]. By introducing surprise, incongruity, humor can disrupt established patterns of behavior, which in turn can promote improvisation to yield a different outcome. Imagine, for example, a toddler throwing a full-blown tantrum that continues to escalate despite efforts to calm them. Here, humor as a parenting strategy might have the child’s parent declare “OK, it’s my turn now,” and then dramatically throw their own tantrum. Because this catches the upset toddler by surprise, many children will stop crying and watch the parent’s tantrum-performance. As the parent’s ‘tantrum’ winds down, another adult can take their own turn throwing a tantrum. When it becomes the child’s turn again, they will typically resume tantruming as if they had never stopped. After a couple of rounds of this, the parent can clap their hands and say “OK, let’s play a different game.” Playful disruption of this sort not only helps resolve tension, but also models creativity and flexibility of mind, which can serve the interests of both children and parents. Moreover, for parents, the irony of turning a tantrum into a game may provide psychological distance [16] that helps parents relieve their own stress and respond more effectively.

There is also considerable evidence that positive parent-child relationships and greater parental responsiveness to a child’s temperament are correlated with greater child well-being [6466]. There is an expansive literature on the use of play in parenting [6772].–a related but distinct kind of engagement [73]. So, too, several authors have written about how to promote humor development in children [7476] and a few small qualitative studies have shown how families of children with disabilities sometimes use humor as an effective coping strategy [77, 78]. But there is little, if any, empirical research on humor as a parenting strategy. In fact, in its more than 30-year existence, the premier journal for humor research (Humor) has not published any articles (conceptual, methods, or outcomes) related to the use of humor in parenting.

There is evidence that people can be taught to use humor more effectively [79, 80]. But it is an open question whether humor (specifically, its uplifting aspects) can be leveraged to help parents respond constructively to challenging situations that might otherwise lead them to respond harshly to their children. As a preliminary step toward answering this question, this pilot study sought to explore whether 18–45-year-olds (putatively, the age range when parents are most likely to be raising young children) consider humor a valuable and viable tool for parenting. To do so, we asked respondents to reflect on their parents’ use of humor and how they (i.e., respondents) saw humor as something that could serve a useful role in parenting children.

Materials and methods

After a review of the literature, we developed a 10-item survey to measure individuals’ experiences being raised with humor, as well as their assessment of humor as a potential tool for use in parenting (see Fig 1). The survey was designed using an iterative process that included focus group discussion, construction of question items, field-testing for clarity and face validity, wording revisions, and cognitive interviews to ensure that interpretation of question items corresponded to their intended objectives. Following IRB approval, a REDCap survey was made available from 12/1/21–12/31/21 for up to 400 U.S. participants between the ages of 18–45 using Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk), an online tool that matches eligible participants with public-facing surveys. Consent to participate was implied by individuals proceeding to complete the survey after reading the summary explanation of research. Responses to the 8 quantitative items were measured on a 7-point Likert-type scale and subsequently aggregated into Disagree (Strongly disagree and Disagree), Indeterminate (Somewhat disagree, Neither agree nor disagree, and Somewhat agree), and Agree (Agree and Strongly agree). The survey also included 2 open-ended question-items (not included here), and 5 demographic items. All variables were summarized prior to analysis to assess their distributions and determine the best course of analysis. Chi-square tests were used to determine which demographic items were significantly associated with each of the 8 quantitative outcome variables, and to investigate potential associations between 2 of the outcome variables (parent relationship, good job parenting), and the other 6 outcome variables. A significance level of 0.05 was used as the cut-off for statistical significance, and all analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).

Results

Of 320 respondents, 312 provided answers to the quantitative items. Most identified as male (63.6%) and White (76.6%), of whom 11.3% were 18–25 years of age, 49.4% were 26–35 years-old, and 39.4% were 36–45 years-old (see Table 1 for full demographics). The majority Agreed that they had a positive relationship with the people who raised them (80.1%); that the people who raised them did a good job (64.7%); that they would raise (or already are raising) children in the same way they were raised (54.8%); and that the people who raised them used humor in their parenting (55.2%). So, too, the majority of participants Agreed that humor could be an effective parenting tool (71.8%) (see Fig 2); that humor as a parenting tool has more potential benefit than harm (63.3%); that they would value a course on how to utilize humor in parenting (69.7%) (see Fig 3); and that they either plan to (or already do) use humor in parenting their own children (61.8%) (see Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Views regarding potential efficacy for humor as a parenting tool.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0306311.g002

Interestingly, there were no statistically significant correlations related to participants’ age. The same was true for gender, with the exception that more females reported they use (or plan to use) the same parenting style as their own parents (62.3%) compared to males (50.5%, p = 0.036).

By contrast, we found multiple significant differences related to participants’ reports regarding the relationships they have with their own parents (see Table 2), and how good a job their own parents had done raising them (see Table 3). Specifically, the ~80% of participants who reported having a good relationship with their parents were significantly more likely to report that their parents had used humor in raising them (63.0%, p < .001) compared to those who provided an indeterminate response (23.5%) or denied having a good relationship with their parents (20.0%). So, too, participants who reported a good relationship with their parents were more likely to believe that humor can be an effective parenting tool (77.1%, p < .001) compared to those who provided an indeterminate response (49.0%) or denied having a good relationship (54.6%). Those reporting good relationships with their parents were also more likely to say they plan to (or already do) use humor in parenting their own children (65.2%, p < .001) compared to participants who provided an indeterminate response (50.0%) or denied having a good relationship (36.4%).

thumbnail
Table 2. Association between parents’ use of humor and relationship with parents.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0306311.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Association between parents’ use of humor and parenting.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0306311.t003

The same general pattern was seen with the 64.6% of participants who reported that their own parents did a good job raising them–with such individuals reporting higher rates of their parents having used humor (68.4%, p < .001) compared to participants who provided an indeterminate response (33.0%) or denied having a good relationship with their parents (28.6%). Individuals reporting that their parents did a good job raising them were also more likely to say that humor can be an effective parenting tool (82.5%, p < .001) compared to those who gave indeterminate responses (52.5%) or denied having a good relationship with their parents (57.1%); were more likely to use (or plan to use) humor in their own parenting (73.9% vs. 39.4% vs. 42.9%, respectively, p < .001); and were more likely to agree that humor as a parenting tool has more potential benefit than harm (72.8% vs. 44.4% vs. 42.9%, respectively, p < .001).

Less surprisingly, but still statistically significant was the finding that participants reporting a good relationship with their own parents were more likely to use (or plan to use) the same parenting style (63.4%, p < .001) compared to those who gave indeterminate responses (22.0%) or denied having a good relationship with their parents (9.1%). A similar association was seen between participants who reported that their parents did a good job raising them, and participants who use (or plan to use) the same parenting style (67.4%, p < .001), compared to participants who gave indeterminate responses (34.3%) or denied having a good relationship with their parents (14.3%).

Worth noting is that fewer than 1% of all respondents reported they did not believe that a brief course on how to use humor as a parenting tool would be a valuable resource, whereas 69.7% endorsed such a course, and 29.6% provided an indeterminate response. When comparing respondents currently raising children <18 years old to those who were not, no significant differences (p = .464) were found as to whether they would value such a course on humor. However, individuals currently raising children (vs. those who were not) were more likely to report that humor could be an effective parenting tool (77.0% vs. 65.9%, p = .044), and that humor has more potential benefit than harm (71.1% vs. 53.6%, p = .002). Such differences did not emerge with regard to respondents’ race and ethnicity, with the exception of how many people reported that their parents used humor in raising them (61.3% White, 48.3% Black, 30.8% Latino, and 40.0% other, p < .001), and how many planned to use the same parenting style as their own parents (59.7% White, 59.3% Black, 30.8% Latino, and 37.5% other, p = .048).

Discussion

If the findings of this pilot study are generalizable, they provide preliminary evidence that a significant number of Americans of childbearing/rearing age have had positive experiences being raised with humor and would be interested in learning more about how to use humor as a tool for parenting their own children. Though the use of “play” with young children has an extensive literature, to our knowledge, this is the first study to explore the use of humor in raising children. This is particularly surprising given humor’s widespread use and prominence in American culture and well-recognized ability to both bridge differences and help people see things from a new perspective.

Emerging research is demonstrating how humor appreciation is detectable in infants as young as six months of age [8186]. Over the last 40 years, various authors have discussed the developmental stages of humor as it evolves during childhood [15, 8789], with some concluding that humor is the inevitable outcome of the progressive evolution of a biological disposition to play [90]. More recently, humor has been recommended as a strategy for early childhood educators [16, 9193], and there is evidence that humor can be an effective coping strategy for families who have children with disabilities [78, 9395].

Of course, one must be careful in extrapolating the advantages of humor, as humor is not without its risks–particularly when significant power imbalances exist. So, too, the current findings do not differentiate the various types of humor or their respective utility, nor do they identify when or how best to leverage humor with children. Nonetheless, the current pilot data suggest that adults of child-raising age are open to the use of humor as one strategy for raising their children and that there is an association between parents’ use of humor and subsequent perceptions of one’s parents. As such, humor may be an over-looked strategy for helping parents develop, model, and promote cognitive and emotional flexibility. Establishing a foundation that can ground effective strategies for using humor as a parenting tool will require answering basic questions such as:

What characteristics make a situation more or less amenable to using humor as a parenting tool? Which forms of parenting humor are more effective at particular stages of child development and for which goals? What humor-related strategies are better for helping parents and children develop the flexibility of mind for solving problems that might otherwise escalate? How can such skills be taught in a way that minimizes their likelihood of misuse or unintentional harm? Would parents prefer to use humor in place of harsh discipline if it was equally (or more) effective?

Limitations

The generalizability of these pilot data is limited by several factors, chief among them selection bias. The current findings arise from a relatively small convenience sample that is predominantly white and male. It is also unknown how the views of MTurk respondents compare to the general population. Separately, there are 3 question-items that use parenthetical phrasing to acknowledge that respondents may not yet have children and that respondents’ parents may no longer be living. It is possible that respondents interpreted these items as double-barreled questions, though interviews during field-testing did not identify any such confusion. Additionally, it is not clear whether the meaning of ‘humor’ or ‘humor in parenting’ was similarly understood by all respondents, nor what factors might affect people’s various interpretations of these terms. As noted in the introduction, there is no single agreed-upon definition or interpretation of what constitutes humor. That said, the present study did seek to explore this matter with several qualitative items, but responses to these questions were too meager to offer useful data. Nonetheless, the findings of this pilot study 1) provide a good starting point for conducting a larger, more robust examination of people’s experiences and views regarding humor and parenting, and 2) suggest that using humor as a parenting tool may be associated with various beneficial outcomes.

Conclusion

This pilot study provides preliminary evidence that Americans of childbearing/rearing age have positive views about humor as a parenting tool, and that such use of humor may be associated with various beneficial outcomes. If these findings are generalizable, they potentially open the door to a much deeper and broader exploration of how ‘parenting humor’ functions and how it can be appropriately leveraged to enhance the experiences of both children and their parents. To that end, future research should examine the ways parents currently use different kinds of humor; children’s lived experiences with such humor; and how these uses of humor map onto existing knowledge and theory about how humor functions.

References

  1. 1. Conger RD, Belsky J, Capaldi DM. The intergenerational transmission of parenting: Closing comments for the special section. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45(5):1276–1283. pmid:19702391
  2. 2. Information for patients and their families. American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. Published June 9, 2022. (Accessed July 23, 2023) https://www.aacap.org/AACAP/Resources_for_Primary_Care/Information_for_Patients_and_Their_Families/Home.aspx.
  3. 3. American Academy of Pediatrics. HealthyChildren.org—From the American Academy of Pediatrics. HealthyChildren.org. Published 2019. (Accessed July 23, 2023) https://www.healthychildren.org/English/Pages/default.aspx.
  4. 4. Altmann TR, Hill DL, Shelov SP, Hannemann RE. Caring for Your Baby and Young Child. Bantam Books; 2019.
  5. 5. Sege RD, Siegel BS. Effective discipline to raise healthy children. Pediatrics. 2018;142(6):e20183112. pmid:30397164
  6. 6. Lavrič M, Naterer A. The power of authoritative parenting: A cross-national study of effects of exposure to different parenting styles on life satisfaction. Children and Youth Services Review. 2020;116(6):105274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105274
  7. 7. Wong TKY, Konishi C, Kong X. Parenting and prosocial behaviors: A meta‐analysis. Social Development. 2020;30(2):343–373. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12481
  8. 8. Brassell AA, Rosenberg E, Parent J, Rough JN, Fondacaro K, Seehuus M. Parent’s psychological flexibility: Associations with parenting and child psychosocial well-being. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science. 2016;5(2):111–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2016.03.001
  9. 9. Kashdan TB, Rottenberg J. Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010;30(7):865–878. pmid:21151705
  10. 10. Diamond A. Executive functions: Insights into ways to help more children thrive. Zero to Three, 2014;35(2): 9–17.
  11. 11. Soltani E, Shareh H, Bahrainian SA, Farmani A. The mediating role of cognitive flexibility in correlation of coping styles and resilience with depression. Pajoohandeh Journal. 2013;18(2):88–96. https://pajoohande.sbmu.ac.ir/article-1-1518-en.html
  12. 12. Arici-Ozcan N, Cekici F, Arslan R. The relationship between resilience and distress tolerance in college students: The mediator role of cognitive flexibility and difficulties in emotion regulation. International Journal of Educational Methodology. 2019;5(4),525–533.
  13. 13. Mestre JM, Núñez-Lozano JM, Gómez-Molinero R, Zayas A, Guil R. Emotion regulation ability and resilience in a sample of adolescents from a suburban area. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017;8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01980
  14. 14. Dewey J. Human Nature and Conduct: An Introduction to Social Psychology. Henry Holt & Co; New York; 1922.
  15. 15. Loizou & Kyriakou . Young children’s appreciation and production of verbal and visual humor, Humor, 2016;29(1):99–124.
  16. 16. Paul McGhee, McGhee Paul. "Humor in the ECE classroom: A neglected form of play whose time has come." Research on young children’s humor: Theoretical and practical implications for early childhood education (2019): 83–106.
  17. 17. Yılmaz Betül. "Examining humor in early childhood period from teacher and child aspects." Master’s thesis, Middle East Technical University, 2019.
  18. 18. Luria S. R., Baer J., & Kaufman J. C. (2018). Creativity and humor. Academic Press.
  19. 19. Morreall J. The Philosophy of Laughter and Humor. State University of New York Press, Albany; 1987.
  20. 20. Martin Rod A., and Ford Thomas. The psychology of humor: An integrative approach. Academic press, 2018. 3.
  21. 21. Long D. L., & Graesser A. C. (1988). Wit and humor in discourse processing. Discourse processes, 11(1), 35–60.
  22. 22. Norrick N. R. (1993). Conversational joking: Humor in everyday talk. Indiana University Press.
  23. 23. Norrick N. R. (2003). Issues in conversational joking. Journal of pragmatics, 35(9), 1333–1359.
  24. 24. Berger A. A. (2017). An anatomy of humor. Routledge
  25. 25. Martin R. A., Puhlik-Doris P., Larsen G., Gray J., & Weir K. (2003). Individual differences in uses of humor and their relation to psychological well-being: Development of the Humor Styles Questionnaire. Journal of research in personality, 37(1), 48–75.
  26. 26. Leist A. K., & Müller D. (2013). Humor types show different patterns of self-regulation, self-esteem, and well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14, 551–569
  27. 27. Jiang F., Lu S., Jiang T., & Jia H. (2020). Does the relation between humor styles and subjective well-being vary across culture and age? A meta-analysis. Frontiers in psychology, 11, 2213. pmid:33071846
  28. 28. Martin R. A., & Lefcourt H. M. (1983). Sense of humor as a moderator of the relation between stressors and moods. Journal of personality and social psychology, 45(6), 1313.
  29. 29. Ziv A. (2010). The social function of humor in interpersonal relationships. Society, 47, 11–18.
  30. 30. Hall J. A. (2017). Humor in romantic relationships: A meta‐analysis. Personal Relationships, 24(2), 306–322.
  31. 31. Holmes Janet, and Marra Meredith. "Humor and leadership style." (2006): 119–138.
  32. 32. Rosenberg C., Walker A., Leiter M., & Graffam J. (2021). Humor in workplace leadership: A systematic search scoping review. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 610795 pmid:34385944
  33. 33. Şahin A. (2021). Humor use in school settings: The perceptions of teachers. SAGE Open, 11(2), 21582440211022691.
  34. 34. Reaves W. W. (2001). The art in humor, the humor in art. American Art, 15(2), 2–9.
  35. 35. Jones J. (2014). Make’Em Laugh: Examining the Role of Humor in Dance. (Masters Thesis)
  36. 36. Gérin A. (2013). A second look at laughter: Humor in the visual arts. Humor, 26(1), 155–176.
  37. 37. Mera M. (2002). Is funny music funny? Contexts and case studies of film music humor. Journal of Popular Music Studies, 14(2), 91–113.
  38. 38. Morreall, John, "Philosophy of Humor", The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2023 Edition), Edward N. Zalta & Uri Nodelman (eds.)
  39. 39. McGraw AP, Williams L, Warren C. The rise and fall of humor: Psychological distance modulates humorous responses to tragedy. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2014,5: 566, Mays Business School Research Paper No. 2015–13. https://ssrn.com/abstract=2558066
  40. 40. Glenwright M, Tapley B, Rano JKS, Pexman PM. Developing appreciation for sarcasm and sarcastic gossip: It depends on perspective. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2017;60(11):3295–3309. pmid:29098274
  41. 41. Hurley MM, Dennett DC, Adams RB. Inside Jokes: Using Humor to Reverse-Engineer the Mind. MIT Press; 2011.
  42. 42. Martin R. Approaches to the sense of humor: A historical review humor and sense of humor.; 1998;15. Accessed July 26, 2023. http://www.humoursummerschool.org/01/articlesNhandouts/ApproachesSoH.pdf
  43. 43. Rodden F. The neurology and psychiatry of humor, smiling, and laughter: A tribute to Paul McGhee part II. Neurological studies and brain imaging. Humor. 2018;31(2):373–399.
  44. 44. Hye‐Knudsen M, Kristensen-McLachlan RD, Clasen M. How Stephen King writes and why: Language, immersion, emotion. Orbis Litterarum. 2023. https://doi.org/10.1111/oli.12401
  45. 45. Dunford CM, Yoshizaki-Gibbons HM, Morhardt D. The Memory Ensemble: Improvising connections among performance, disability, and ageing. Research in Drama Education: The Journal of Applied Theatre and Performance. 2017;22(3):420–426. pmid:30555268
  46. 46. Brunet HE, Banks SJ, Libera A, Willingham-Jaggers M, Almén RA. Training in improvisation techniques helps reduce caregiver burden and depression: Innovative Practice. Dementia. 2021;20(1):364–372. pmid:31408359
  47. 47. Aaker JL, Bagdonas N. Humor Seriously: Why humor is a secret weapon in business and life. Currency; 2021.
  48. 48. Hoffmann-Longtin K, Rossing JP, Weinstein E. Twelve tips for using applied improvisation in medical education. Medical Teacher. 2017;40(4):351–356. pmid:29025298
  49. 49. Hughes L, Avey J. Transforming with levity: Humor, leadership, and follower attitudes. Leadership & Organization Development Journal. 2009;30(6):540–562.
  50. 50. The Second City Works-program. Published June 9, 2022. Accessed July 24, 2023. https://www.secondcity.com/works-program/
  51. 51. Saroglou V., & Scariot C. (2002). Humor Styles Questionnaire: Personality and educational correlates in Belgian high school and college students. European Journal of Personality, 16(1), 43–54.
  52. 52. Mireault G., Sparrow J., Poutre M., Perdue B., & Macke L. (2012). Infant humor perception from 3-to 6-months and attachment at one year. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(4), 797–802. pmid:22982281
  53. 53. Besser A., Luyten P., & Mayes L. C. (2012). Adult attachment and distress: The mediating role of humor styles. Individual Differences Research, 10(3), 153–164.
  54. 54. Dionigi A., Duradoni M., & Vagnoli L. (2023). Humor and Attachment: Exploring the Relationships between Insecure Attachment and the Comic Styles. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 13(1), 161–169. pmid:36661762
  55. 55. Kazarian S. S., & Martin R. A. (2004). Humour styles, personality, and well‐being among Lebanese university students. European journal of Personality, 18(3), 209–219.
  56. 56. Cann A., Norman M. A., Welbourne J. L., & Calhoun L. G. (2008). Attachment styles, conflict styles and humour styles: Interrelationships and associations with relationship satisfaction. European Journal of Personality: Published for the European Association of Personality Psychology, 22(2), 131–146.
  57. 57. McGhee P. Understanding and Promoting the Development of Children’s Humor. Kendall/Hunt Publishing Company; 2002.
  58. 58. Bergen D. Humor as a developmental phenomenon: the contributions of Paul McGhee. HUMOR. 2018;31(2): 213–231
  59. 59. Bergen D. (2021). Humour as a resource for children. The Palgrave Handbook of Humour Research, 311–323.
  60. 60. Telli B. S., & Hoicka E. (2022). Humor and social cognition: Correlational and predictive relations in 3-to 47-month-olds. Cognitive Development, 64, 101245.
  61. 61. Norrick NR, Spitz A. Humor as a resource for mitigating conflict in interaction. Journal of Pragmatics. 2008;40(10):1661–1686. (52)
  62. 62. Sclavi M. The Role of Play and Humor in Creative Conflict Management. Negotiation Journal. 2008;24(2):157–180.
  63. 63. Heinemann T. Managing unavoidable conflicts in caretaking of the elderly: Humor as a mitigating resource. International Journal of the Sociology of Language. 2009;200:103–127.
  64. 64. Chen Y, Haines J, Charlton BM, VanderWeele TJ. Positive parenting improves multiple aspects of health and well-being in young adulthood. Nature Human Behaviour. 2019;3(7):684–691. pmid:31061491
  65. 65. Davids EL, Roman NV, Leach L. The link between parenting approaches and health behavior: A systematic review. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment. 2017;27(6):589–608. https://doi.org/10.1080/10911359.2017.1311816
  66. 66. Yap MBH, Pilkington PD, Ryan SM, Jorm AF. Parental factors associated with depression and anxiety in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2014;156:8–23. pmid:24308895
  67. 67. Hulbert A. Raising America: Experts, Parents, and a Century of Advice about Children. Vintage Books; 2004.
  68. 68. Roopnarine JL. Cultural Variations in Beliefs about Play, Parent-Child Play, and Children’s Play: Meaning for Childhood Development. In Pellegrini A. D., ed. The Oxford handbook of the development of play. Oxford University Press; 2011:19–37.
  69. 69. Milteer RM, Ginsburg KR, Council on Communications and Media Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health, Mulligan DA Ameenuddin N, Brown A Christakis DAet al. The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent-child bond: Focus on children in poverty. Pediatrics. 2012;129(1):e204–e213. pmid:22201149
  70. 70. Roopnarine J, Davidson K. Parent-Child Play across Cultures Advancing Play Research. American Journal of Play. 2015;7(2):228–252.
  71. 71. Teufl L, Ahnert L. Parent–child play and parent–child relationship: Are fathers special? Journal of Family Psychology. 2022;36(3), 416–426. pmid:34843326
  72. 72. Tamis-LeMonda C, Bornstein MH, Kuchirko Y, Escobar K. Being and Becoming a Parent. In Bornstein MH ed. Handbook of Parenting. New York, Routledge; 2019, vol.3:189–213.
  73. 73. Bergen D, Rousta M. Developing creativity and humor: The role of the playful mind. In Creativity and Humor; 2018:61–81.
  74. 74. McGhee P. Understanding and Promoting the Development of Children’s Humor. Kendall/Hunt Publishing Company; 2002.
  75. 75. Bergen D. (2021). Play as a context for humor development. In Fromberg D. P.&Bergen (Eds), Play from birth to twelve and beyond (pp. 324–337). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003249702-48
  76. 76. Stenius TH, Karlsson L, Sivenius A. Young Children’s Humour in Play and Moments of Everyday Life in ECEC Centres. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research. 2022;66(3):396–410.
  77. 77. BROOKS N GUTHRIE D, GAYLORD C. Therapeutic humor in the family: An exploratory study. HUMOR. 1999;12(2): 151–160. https://doi.org/10.1515/humr.1999.12.2.151
  78. 78. Fritz HL. Coping with caregiving: Humor styles and health outcomes among parents of children with disabilities. Res Dev Disabil. 2020 Sep;104:103700. Epub 2020 Jun 1. pmid:32497974.
  79. 79. Tagalidou N., Distlberger E., Loderer V. et al. Efficacy and feasibility of a humor training for people suffering from depression, anxiety, and adjustment disorder: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry 19, 93 (2019). pmid:30894148
  80. 80. Libera A. Funnier: A Theory of Comedy with Practical Application, Northwestern University Press, Evanston (forthcoming, 2024).
  81. 81. Hoicka E. Parents and toddlers distinguish joke, pretend and literal intentional contexts through communicative and referential cues. Journal of Pragmatics. 2016 Apr;95:137–55.
  82. 82. Hoicka E, Gattis M. Do the wrong thing: How toddlers tell a joke from a mistake. Cognitive Development. 2008 Jan;23(1):180–90.
  83. 83. Hoicka E, Martin C. Two-Year-Olds Distinguish Pretending and Joking. Child Dev. 2016 May;87(3):916–28. Epub 2016 May 12. pmid:27170036.
  84. 84. Mireault GC, Crockenberg SC, Sparrow JC, Cousineau K, Pettinato CA, Woodard KM. Laughing matters: Infant humor in the context of parental affect. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2015 Aug 1;136:30–41. pmid:25897958
  85. 85. Mireault GC, Crockenberg SC, Sparrow JE, Pettinato CA, Woodard KC, Malzac K. Social looking, social referencing and humor perception in 6- and-12-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development. 2014 Nov;37(4):536–45. pmid:25061893
  86. 86. Mireault G. C., Crockenberg S. C., Heilman K., Sparrow J. E., Cousineau K., & Rainville B. (2018). Social, cognitive, and physiological aspects of humour perception from 4 to 8 months: Two longitudinal studies. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 98–109. pmid:28944500
  87. 87. McGhee PE. Humor and Children’s Development A Guide to Practical Applications, ed. Paul McGhee, 1989. Humor and Children’s Development. Routledge; 1989.
  88. 88. Bariaud F. (1989). Chapter 1: Age differences in children’s humor. Journal of Children in Contemporary Society, 20(1–2), 15–45.
  89. 89. Bergen D. Humor as a developmental phenomenon: the contributions of Paul McGhee. HUMOR. 2018;31(2): 213–231. https://doi.org/10.1515/humor-2016-0091
  90. 90. Goldstein J., & Ruch W. (2018). Paul McGhee and humor research. Humor, 31(2), 169–181.
  91. 91. Yılmaz-Çam B. An Investigation of the Relationship between Early Childhood Teachers’ Classroom Management Profiles and Humor Styles. Journal of Teacher Education and Educators; 2023.
  92. 92. Bergen D, Modir Rousta M. Developing Creativity and Humor. Creativity and Humor. 2019;61–81.
  93. 93. Yunus FW, Dalli C. Creating a Sense of Togetherness through Humor and Laughter in Early Childhood Education. International Journal of Early Childhood Learning. 2019 Dec;26(2):21–34.
  94. 94. Fritz HL. Caregiving in quarantine: Humor styles, reframing, and psychological well-being among parents of children with disabilities. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2021 Nov 12;39(3):026540752110435.
  95. 95. Rieger A, McGrail JP. Coping humor and family functioning in parents of children with disabilities. Rehabil Psychol. 2013 Feb;58(1):89–97. pmid:23438005.