Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Age and familiarity effects on musical memory

  • Sarah A. Sauvé ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ssauve@lincoln.ac.uk

    Affiliations Division of Community Health and Humanities, Faculty of Medicine, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John’s, Newfoundland and Labrador, Canada, School of Psychology, College of Health and Science, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, Lincolnshire, United Kingdom

  • Praveena Satkunarajah,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Community Health and Humanities, Faculty of Medicine, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John’s, Newfoundland and Labrador, Canada

  • Stephen Cooke,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Community Health and Humanities, Faculty of Medicine, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John’s, Newfoundland and Labrador, Canada

  • Özgen Demirkaplan,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Community Health and Humanities, Faculty of Medicine, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John’s, Newfoundland and Labrador, Canada

  • Alicia Follett,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Community Health and Humanities, Faculty of Medicine, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John’s, Newfoundland and Labrador, Canada

  • Benjamin Rich Zendel

    Roles Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Community Health and Humanities, Faculty of Medicine, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John’s, Newfoundland and Labrador, Canada

Abstract

Background

A common complaint in older adults is trouble with their memory, especially for new information. Current knowledge about normal aging and changes in memory identify a divide between memory tasks that are unaffected by aging and those that are. Among the unaffected are recognition tasks. These memory tasks rely on accessing well-known information, often include environmental support, and tend to be automatic. Negative age effects on memory are often observed at both encoding and during recall. Older adults often have difficulty with recall tasks, particularly those that require effortful self-initiated processing, episodic memory, and retention of information about contextual cues. Research in memory for music in healthy aging suggests a skill-invariance hypothesis: that age effects dominate when general-purpose cognitive mechanisms are needed to perform the musical task at hand, while experience effects dominate when music-specific knowledge is needed to perform the task [1].

Aims

The goals of this pair of studies were to investigate the effects of age and familiarity on musical memory in the context of real pieces of music, and to compare a live concert experimental setting with a lab-based experimental setting.

Method

Participants’ task was to click a button (or press the spacebar) when they heard the target theme in three pieces of music. One was Mozart’s Eine Kleine Nachtmusik and the others were original pieces commissioned for this study, one tonal and one atonal. Participants heard the relevant theme three times before listening to a piece of music. The music was performed by the Newfoundland Symphony Orchestra; participants either attended the concert, or watched a recording of the concert in the lab. Participants also completed two short cognitive tests and filled out a questionnaire collecting demographic information and a hearing abilities self-assessment.

Results

We find a significant effect of familiarity and setting but not of age or musical training on recognition performance as measured by d’. More specifically, performance is best for the familiar, tonal piece, moderate for the unfamiliar tonal piece and worst for the unfamiliar atonal piece. Performance was better in the live setting than the lab setting.

Conclusions

The absence of an age effect provides encouraging evidence that music’s diverse cues may encourage cognitive scaffolding, in turn improving encoding and subsequent recognition. Better performance in an ecological versus lab setting supports the expansion of ecological studies in the field.

Introduction

The association between aging and declining memory encoding and retrieving abilities is well documented [25]. Interestingly, there has been much less research on how aging impacts memory for music, however, interest in this topic has been growing [618]. One reason for the growing interest in music memory in aging is because of the growing interest in using music as an intervention for maintaining cognitive function with age [1932]. One little explored area of musical learning and memory is how it functions in a naturalized listening environment like a symphony concert. In this study, we explore the effects of age and familiarity of memory for new music at a live symphony concert compared to a similar in-lab to test to see if a live concert setting may affect memory performance, particularly in older adults.

Memory in older adults

A common complaint in older adults is trouble with their memory, especially for new information (i.e., encoding; [33, 34]). Current knowledge about normal aging and changes in memory identify a divide between memory tasks that are unaffected by aging and those that are. For this study, investigating familiarity for new music in an ecological listening situation, we focus on the processes of encoding and recognition, and semantic memory, as it underlies schematic knowledge of tonality. Memory tasks un- or less affected by aging include recognition tasks, tasks relying on well-known information, semantic knowledge and automatic processes [5, 35]. Negative age effects are seen for recall tasks and tasks that require effortful, self-initiated processing in real time [36, 37]. There is also evidence that aging negatively affects encoding and to a lesser degree retrieval processes [3841], but not memory storage [41]. This is in line with the aging theory of inhibition, which suggests that older adults are worse at inhibiting task-irrelevant information, making perception and encoding more difficult [42]. Interestingly, other work shows that meaning, predictability or familiarity aids in visual memory for older adults [43, 44].

Memory for music in older adults

Music is an ideal stimulus with which to study memory as it is multimodal and incredibly rich. It is also quite complex and musical memory can be quite poor [45, 46]. It appears that this difficulty cannot be accounted for by the level of processing [47] complexity of encoding as music is not encoded using elaborative encoding strategies [48]. Instead, familiarity and well-formedness are proposed as factors with more important effects on memory for music [48]. Familiarity is one of the two cognitive processes thought to underlie retrieval, along with recollection [49]. Specifically, familiarity refers to an overall feeling of “-known-ness” without being able to place a specific context, for instance not being able to place a tune that sounds ‘familiar’, while recollection refers to a conscious recollection of specific details, for instance hearing a tune at a particular place and time [48]. Recollection relies on episodic memory, but episodic memory cannot be engaged if a tune cannot be identified or named (i.e. is novel; [50]. For unfamiliar music, listeners need to rely on schematic knowledge of music, expressed through the tonal hierarchy [51]. The tonal hierarchy is the hierarchical organization of pitch in music reflecting the perceptual organization of the relationships between the twelve pitches of Western tonal music’s chromatic scale. Implicit knowledge of the tonal hierarchy generates expectations about what can come next and what is grammatically ‘correct’ [52] in the music, helping narrow possibilities and ease encoding. Importantly, this knowledge does not rely on formal musical training but both implicit knowledge [51, 53] and increased exposure (i.e. age; [54, 55]) to refine and strengthen tonal representations, supporting the encoding of novel material. The second factor proposed to have an important effect on musical memory is well-formedness. This is another way of expressing that intervals that follow the rules of tonality help ease encoding.

In terms of types of memory, existing literature on memory for music in normal aging suggests that overall explicit memory, such as in a recognition task suffers, while implicit memory, such as in a preference rating task is preserved [11, 35]. However, schematic knowledge of musical structure, accumulated over time in older adults, may help ease encoding and recognition [5, 15, 56].

Memory for music in older adults with musical training

The effect of musical training on recognition memory in older adults is unclear. One might expect musical training to minimize any age effects due to the multimodal nature of music (visual, auditory, motor) activating rich domain knowledge [16]. Indeed, some studies propose a skill-invariance hypothesis to explain apparently conflicting memory performance as a function of age and experience. Specifically, that age effects dominate when general-purpose cognitive mechanisms are needed to perform the musical task at hand, while experience effects dominate when music-specific knowledge is needed to perform the task [1, 12]. While Meinz [16] found no interaction between age and musical expertise on memory performance or perceptual speed, “there was some evidence that greater experience on the part of older adults attenuated the age-related effects on performance of relevant cognitive tasks, especially the memory tasks.” [16].

A review of the literature on memory for melodies [48] suggests that experience effects are found in short-term recognition tasks [57, 58] or where fine-grained pitch processing is required such as detecting non-contour violating variations on a theme [12]. Others find no effect of training [12, 45, 50, 59, 60]. Another, more recent, review [61] suggests that musical training is beneficial, where musicians hold more items in memory and for longer than non-musicians [62]. The link between musical training and working memory in general seems to be stronger than the link between musical training and memory for music specifically [6365]. This advantage may start at the perceptual level, as auditory streams persist in echoic memory longer in musicians [66].

In summary, the potential benefit of musical training on memory for music in older adults seems dependent on both the music itself (i.e. complexity, tonality) and the type of task (i.e. short- or long-term, recognition or recall). Meanwhile, older adults generally perform worse than younger adults on remembering complex, unfamiliar music in short-term and recognition tasks but not simple, familiar music in long-term memory tasks [1]. Additionally, according to intervention-based training studies, the benefit may be mediated by improving working memory in general [22, 2629, 32, 67, 68].

Speech-in-noise in older adults

Another common complaint in older adults is difficulty understanding speech in noisy environments [69], which can lead to social isolation [70], and potentially cognitive decline [71]. Along with auditory stream segregation and attention, memory–especially working memory–is an important component to understanding speech in noise [69]. While speech perception suffers in old age, music perception does not seem to be affected, as evidenced by the large percentage of older adults who still enjoy music on a daily basis [72, 73]. Therefore, the theoretical framework for this study is cognitive scaffolding [74], specifically the potential for cognitive skills maintained in music listening being used as a cognitive scaffold upon which to build speech in noise listening skills [75]. Having previously found that sequential auditory streaming by loudness and timbre is maintained in older adults [76], in addition to auditory streaming by pitch [77], this study focuses on memory, specifically the encoding of new information and its subsequent recognition [18].

Current study

Music can be considered an engaging, active task, particularly when listeners are given a task to complete in relation to the music itself, such as identifying a theme each time it occurs in a piece of music. In this study, attendees of the Newfoundland Symphony Orchestra’s 2022–2023 Season Masterworks 3 concert–and a separate sample of participants from the general population for the lab-based study identified when they heard the theme for three pieces of music manipulated for familiarity. Different from a feeling of “known-ness”, in this study familiarity was operationalized along two dimensions: tonality (schematic memory) and novelty (veridical memory). The first and most familiar piece was Eine Kleine Nachtmusik by W.A. Mozart, both tonal and previously heard to most. It also served as a control to ensure participants understood the task. The second and third pieces were commissioned from students of Memorial University of Newfoundland’s School of Music. One was tonal and the other atonal so that the second piece, Pirate Waltz, was tonal and novel and the third piece, Unexpectedly Absent, atonal and novel. In this way, the study includes three levels of familiarity considering both schematic (tonal vs atonal) and veridical (previously heard vs novel) forms. Participants completed a demographic questionnaire, including information on musical background, a hearing self-assessment [78] and two short cognitive tests.

As an effortful task requiring active processing (of the incoming sound), the suppression of irrelevant information (ambient noise; non-melodic content to a certain extent) and the encoding of new information (the theme), it would be expected that older adults would perform worse than younger adults at such a task. However, enough time to ‘digest’ the information presented (i.e. later repetitions of the theme [6, 10]) and degree of familiarity may mitigate these challenges. Combined with work demonstrating that older adults rely on tonal structure more than note-to-note interval information to process music [55], we expect older adults to perform a theme-identifying musical memory task better for familiar, tonal music than for unfamiliar, atonal music [7, 8, 15, 79]. It is possible that access to tonal information offsets the typical encoding difficulty observed in older adults, where older adults would perform at a similar level, or better, than younger adults for familiar, tonal music and similarly but not better for unfamiliar tonal music [8, 15]. In other words, we predict an interaction between age and familiarity for melody recognition. As this study includes age as a key variable, we also measure confounds related to cognitive fitness including social and physical activity and education [80]. We expect these, and musical background to correlate positively with task performance.

Materials and methods–live concert

Participants

Participants were attendees of the Newfoundland Symphony Orchestra’s Masterworks 3 concert from their 2021–2022 season which took place on May 20th, 2022. Those interested in taking part in the study provided written informed consent in accordance with the Interdisciplinary Committee on Ethics in Human Research at Memorial University of Newfoundland and Labrador (20210452-ME). Participants could enter into a draw for one of ten $25 cash gift cards.

Concert attendees might have included locals, settler or Indigenous, immigrants and tourists, though we did not collect this specific information as part of our demographic questions. We did however collect information about musical background and age. For additional context, Newfoundland and Labrador has a rich musical culture comprising many genres, including widespread and popular Irish/Newfoundland folk; it is more difficult to find someone without any musical performance experience, formal or informal, than with. This province also has the largest aging population in Canada, with those born between 1945–59 (i.e., ‘baby boomers’) making up 31.1% of the total population [81].

Due to the live setting of this study, the dataset is incomplete. We received experimental data from 108 participants. Of these, 92 completed the questionnaires, which included the key demographic detail of age. The mean age of this group was 46.8 (SD = 17.4, range = 18–86). Of these, 31 also completed the cognitive tests. The mean age of this group was 45.7 (SD = 14.1, range = 19–64). Table 1 shows the demographic distribution of our participants for each data subset by decade of age, including mean years of education, hours of activity per week, hours of socializing per week, hours listening to music per week, years of formal training, adapted hearing screening inventory and mean letter-number sequencing, Trail Making A and Trail Making B scores.

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic distribution of participants for data subsets in the live concert setting, before removal any participants.

Questionnaire = clicker and questionnaire. Cognitive Tests = clicker, questionnaire and cognitive tests. Response ranges for Hours of activity, socializing and listening to music were translated into a single integer before averaging, in this case the high end of the range.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0305969.t001

Materials

Pieces of music.

Three pieces of music were used in this study. Eine Kleine Nachtmusik by W.A. Mozart was a control piece likely familiar to participants to ensure the task was understood. The theme was identified as the first 8 measures of the piece. The piece was performed with no repeats, resulting in a total of two iterations of the theme. Two 5-minute experimental pieces were commissioned from students of Memorial University of Newfoundland and Labrador’s School of Music. Their instructions were to include 10–12 exact iterations of a chosen theme and that some should be easy to perceive, some difficult, and some of medium perceptibility. This gradient was primarily achieved through texture and was introduced to avoid floor or ceiling effects. For example, an “easy” theme could be performed solo, or by a prominent instrument with little accompaniment. A “hard” theme is often played by an instrument in its weak range among the entire orchestra playing loudly. Composers were free to include secondary themes and foils, or part of the theme. One piece was composed in the tonal system and another in the atonal system. The final version of each piece included 11 themes. Figs 1 and 2 illustrate each piece’s theme, as well as an example of the theme in an easy, medium and difficult context. Audio examples of each context can be found on the project’s OSF page (OSF | Age and familiarity effects on musical memory).

thumbnail
Fig 1.

Theme (A) and sample iterations of the theme in an easy (B), medium (C) and hard (D) context for Pirate Waltz, the tonal, unfamiliar piece.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0305969.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2.

Theme (A) and sample iterations of the theme in an easy (B), medium (C) and hard (D) context for Unexpectedly Absent, the atonal, unfamiliar piece.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0305969.g002

Clickers.

150 LCD clickers by Turning Technologies Canada (Turning Technologies - Wikipedia; no longer exist as a company) were rented from Data on the Spot (Home – Data On The Spot) along with a receiver. Clickers were attached to lanyards and were numbered 1–150 with stickers on the back.

Questionnaire.

The questionnaire contained two parts. The first part collected demographic information including age, gender, education, hours of physical activity, hours of socializing, hours actively listening to music, familiarity with Eine Kleine Nachtmusik by W.A. Mozart, and details about musical training including years of non-formal musical training, years of formal musical training, whether or not participants were currently practicing, and their primary instrument. The second part is adapted with permission from The Hearing Screening Inventory [78] and consisted of 12 items describing common situations related to hearing. For example, “Are you ever bothered by feelings that your hearing is poor?” or “Can you hear the water boiling in a pot when you are in the kitchen?”, for which answer options were “never”, “seldom”, “occasionally”, “frequently” or “always”. Other questions were statements, such as “Overall, I would judge my hearing in my right ear to be…” or “Overall, I would judge my ability to make out speech or conversation to be…”, for which answer options were “good”, “average”, “slightly below average”, “poor” or “very poor”.

Cognitive tests.

Two cognitive tests were selected for their relevance to working memory and general cognitive functioning, and brevity due to the live concert setting. These are the letter-number sequencing subtest from the Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition [82] and the Trail Making Test, A and B, a common neuropsychological instrument [83]. In the letter-number sequencing test, participants are read a series of letters and numbers, a “trial”. Participants are asked to recall the numbers first in ascending order and then the letters in alphabetical order. For example, “3 –W– 5” would be “3–5 –W”. Trials begin with two letters or numbers and gradually increase to eight. Each level has three trials; the test stops when a participant scores 0 on all three trials in a level. The total possible score is 21. In the Trail Making Tests, participants connect circles containing numbers 1–25 (A) in ascending order or circles containing letters and numbers in alternating ascending and alphabetical order (B; e.g. 1-A-2-B-3-C). Participants are timed in seconds; performance will be interpreted in the context of normative data by age and education [84]. All materials can be found on the project’s OSF page (OSF | Age and familiarity effects on musical memory).

Procedure

Given that this study took place in a live concert setting, the procedure is non-standard. The research team was set up in the lobby of the concert hall at two tables with one team member each, and the rest floating around. Research team members were members of the Cognitive Aging and Auditory Neuroscience laboratory, as well as volunteer recruited from Memorial University of Newfoundland and Labrador’s Human Neuroscience Research Group and School of Music. All volunteers were trained for at least one hour to familiarize themselves with the protocol of the study and conducting the cognitive tests. At the concert, anyone interested in taking part in the study could approach a member of the research team to collect a study packet (two copies of the informed consent form, the questionnaire, written instructions and a prize draw ticket) and a clicker, provided the informed consent form was read and signed. The questionnaires were pre-numbered so that participants received matching questionnaires and clickers. Questionnaire and cognitive tests could be completed at any time before the concert, during intermission, or after the concert. Questionnaires could be completed by participants themselves. To complete the cognitive tests, participants needed to find a member of the research team, who would take them to a relatively isolated spot around the building to complete the tests. This was not ideal, as the surrounding area was noisy; however, given the live concert environment, this was the context we had to work with.

The study pieces were programmed at the beginning of the concert. Before the music started, the first author introduced the research lab, the study, and gave participants oral instructions from the stage as follows:

“Before each study piece, you’ll hear a theme 3 times. The first time, the whole orchestra will play it. The second and third times, it’ll be a soloist. Your job is to click a button–any button–on the clicker when you hear the theme. Remember to listen for the notes, not the instrument who’s playing. Also–this one’s important–make sure to wait until you hear the whole theme before clicking; sometimes the composer uses only the beginning but then goes somewhere else with it. Last thing: try as much as you can to keep your clicker hidden, maybe under a scarf, jacket, or in your hands. Ok, I hope that’s clear. Click a button when you hear the theme. Thank you for taking part in this live concert study! Enjoy the music!”

The orchestra performed Eine Kleine Nachtmusik, Pirate Waltz, and finally Unexpectedly Absent, playing the theme three times before each piece [9], once by the entire orchestra in unison and two more times by a soloist. For Eine Kleine Nachtmusik, the soloists’ instruments were a violin and a double bass. For Pirate Waltz, the soloists’ instruments were a clarinet and a horn. For Unexpectedly Absent, the soloists’ instruments were a cello and a trumpet.

Data collection

Clickers were operated using Turning Technology software. This software is designed for voting and is not specifically adapted for the application required in this study. The software was implemented to accept clicks in intervals of 5 seconds (i.e. a new poll opened every 5 seconds). The software reported the computer’s time (hh:mm:ss) at which a slide was viewed, the computer’s time at which polling opened, the computer’s time at which polling closed, and for each clicker device, the time in seconds since polling opened when a button was pressed. Indeed, no more than one click per 5 second window was recorded for any device.

Data processing

Clickers.

Clicker data was converted from PDF documents exported by the software to Excel spreadsheets by the research team. The Turning Technology software produced a session log, containing the polling opened and polling closed time of each slide, and a participant response time log, which included the clicker (participant) number and the response time of each click for each slide in seconds (to the hundredth millisecond). Session log data was stored in one spreadsheet and response times in another to the nearest second (hh:mm:ss), where the common variable was the time range of a slide (e.g. 1:30–1:35). All data was encoded by one team member and verified by another. Given the start time of the piece and the start time of each poll (slide), it was possible to determine the time (mm:ss) of each click from the beginning of the piece for each device (participant).

Questionnaire.

Questionnaires were digitized to Excel spreadsheets. One team member encoded the data and another team member checked the data. The hearing abilities questionnaire responses were converted to numbers, where questionnaires responses were encoded as either 1 through 5 from left to right so that “never” and “good” were equivalent to 1 and “always” and “very poor” were equivalent to 5 for questions 1, 5, 6, and 9–12 and the opposite for questions 2, 3, 4, 7 and 8. Where one or two questions were unanswered, data was interpolated as the mean of all answered questions; 0.5% of the data was interpolated in this way. Responses to each question were totaled for an overall hearing abilities self-assessment score, where the higher the score, the worse the hearing ability.

Cognitive tests.

Cognitive test results were digitized to Excel spreadsheets. One team member encoded the data and another checked the data. A score out of 21 was recorded for the letter-number sequencing test, where the higher the score the better the performance. Time in seconds and milliseconds was recorded for the Trail Making A and B tests, where a shorter time represents better performance. Tester initials were also recorded for each participant in order to test for any experimenter effects. Trail-making times were compared to normative data stratified by age and education [84] to screen out participants with abnormally long times, which may indicate some type of cognitive impairment; one participant from the 51–60 age range was removed.

Data analysis

Ground truth.

It was necessary to generate a ground truth in order to identify whether a click was a hit or a false alarm. To do so, the start and end time (mm:ss) of each iteration of a theme was recorded based on a video recording of the concert. Once video time markers were recorded, the start time of the piece was subtracted from each theme time to arrive at a start and end time from the start of the piece. To determine a “hit” window, 2 seconds were added to each theme’s end time, corresponding to approximately 2–4 measures, depending on the tempo of the piece. Given participants were instructed to wait until the end of the theme to click, it was necessary to extend the window in which a click would be counted as a hit. However, it was also important to consider that participants may click early (i.e. during the theme; many did) and to also consider these correct identifications of the theme. Clicks from each device, converted to mm:ss format in data processing, can be compared to the hit windows identified in the ground truth.

Signal detection.

With hits and false alarms generated from the ground truth and clicker data, these were converted to d’ using the R package psycho [85]. In 0.03% cases, there were two hits recorded for a theme; these were recoded as a single hit. In order to calculate d’, the number of possible hits and possible false alarms is necessary; d’ was calculated for each participant at each difficulty level, for each piece. There were 2, 11 and 11 possible hits for Eine Kleine Nachtmusik, Pirate Waltz and Unexpectedly Absent, respectively. These can be broken down to: 2 easy themes and no medium or hard themes for Eine Kleine Nachtmusik; 4 easy, 4 medium and 3 hard themes for Pirate Waltz; and 3 easy, 3 medium and 5 hard themes for Unexpectedly Absent. Possible false alarms were the number of theme-length segments that were not themes, calculated as the number of non-theme measures divided by the length of the theme, in measures. There were 42, 27 and 30 possible false alarms for Eine Kleine Nachtmusik, Pirate Waltz and Unexpectedly Absent, respectively. The same number of possible false alarms was used for each difficulty level, as we considered it misleading to label a differently difficult theme a non-theme.

Participants who did not correctly identify any themes in Eine Kleine Nachtmusik were considered to not have understood the task and were excluded from further analysis. Twenty-two participants were excluded in this way, 6 in the 40–51 age range and 1–3 in each of the remaining age ranges.

Materials and methods–lab setting

Participants

Participants were members of the university and general community, who may have included locals, settler or Indigenous, and immigrants, though this demographic information was not collected. These participants were specifically not attendees of the live concert event and took part in the study between August 25th and September 22nd, 2022. Due to the controlled lab setting, complete datasets were collected from 31 participants. These participants were recruited to roughly match the number of participants in each age group from the live study, where an age group is a decade (starting from 20s), and to extend into the 70s and 80s age groups (which are not represented in the live study full dataset). The mean age of participants in the lab study was 50.7 (SD = 20.3, range = 22–84). Table 2 shows the demographic distribution of our participants for each data subset by decade of age, including mean years of education, hours of activity per week, hours of socializing per week, hours listening to music per week, years of formal training, adapted hearing screening inventory and mean letter-number sequencing, Trail Making A and Trail Making B scores. Participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Interdisciplinary Committee on Ethics in Human Research at Memorial University of Newfoundland and Labrador and were compensated $10 for their time.

thumbnail
Table 2. Demographic distribution of participants in the lab setting, before removal of any participants.

Response ranges for Hours of activity, socializing and listening to music were translated into a single integer before averaging, in this case the high end of the range.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0305969.t002

Materials

The materials were the same as in the live concert event.

Procedure

Given that this study took place in a lab setting, the procedure was standardized and slightly modified. Participants were invited into the lab, read the informed consent form and were given the opportunity to ask question before providing consent. Instructions were then paraphrased to them, emphasizing that they would be pressing the spacebar on the keyboard instead of a button on a device. They were then comfortably seated in a double-walled sound proof booth to watch a video recording of the concert, beginning at the point where instructions were delivered and ending at the end of the third piece. The camera was placed in the audience so that the orchestra was centered in the frame and took up the width of the frame. The first few rows of the audience are visible. The recording used the Newfoundland Symphony Orchestra’s standard in-house equipment. Participants listened to the video over free field speakers at a comfortable level.

Data collection

A MaxMSP (What is Max? | Cycling ’74 (cycling74.com)) patch was written to collect the computer’s internal clock (hh:mm:ss) at the time of the click. The experimenter clicked a mouse on a computer outside the booth at the start of each piece. Once the concert recording ended, participants were invited back into the lab space to complete the questionnaire and cognitive tests.

Data processing

Key presses.

Key press and piece start times were saved in a text file. Using the lubridate R package [86], key press times were tared to the start of Eine Kleine Nachtmusik.

Questionnaire.

Questionnaires were digitized to Excel spreadsheets in the same way as the live concert setting. There was no missing data.

Cognitive tests.

Cognitive test results were digitized to Excel spreadsheets in the same way as the live concert setting. Trail-making times were compared to normative data stratified by age and education [84] to screen out participants with abnormally long times, which may indicate some type of cognitive impairment; two participants were removed, one each from the 31–40 and 51–60 age ranges. One more participant performed abnormally fast on the TM-A test as there was some confusion and it was re-done; furthermore, this participant did not consider transpositions of the theme as identifiable, which would result in an underestimate of their recognition memory. Therefore, this participant from the 31–40 age range was also removed.

Data analysis

Ground truth.

The ground truth is the same as in the live concert setting except that theme start and end times were relative to the beginning of Eine Kleine Nachtmusik, instead of the beginning of each piece. This was to minimize variation due to experimenter error, where the start of each piece was initially recorded by a mouse click.

Signal detection.

With hits and false alarms generated from the ground truth and key press data, these were converted to d’ using the R package psycho [85]. There were no double hits in this setting. The number of possible hits and false alarms is the same as in the live concert setting. One participant from the 21–30 age range was excluded due to not correctly identify any themes in Eine Kleine Nachtmusik.

Pre-registered analysis.

The main analysis for this project was pre-registered on OSF (OSF Registries | Age and familiarity effects on musical memory). As pre-registered, a mixed effects multiple linear regression model predicting d’ was implemented. The model only included full data sets for better comparison between live and lab settings. All live setting participants with a missing value for age, hearing self-assessment or letter-number sequencing were excluded from analysis, as missing this information equated to not having completed the questionnaire and/or the cognitive tests. Eighteen live setting and 28 lab setting participants were thus included in the pre-registered analysis.

We have made one change to the original planned model: familiarity with Eine Kleine Nachtmusik was removed as a predictor variable through the review process. Therefore, the model reported below had the following fixed effects: age, familiarity (piece), setting (live or lab), theme identification difficulty (easy, medium, hard), musicianship (all relevant questionnaire items), and all covariates except familiarity with Eine Kleine Nachtmusik. Categorical variables familiarity, setting, theme identification difficulty, gender, highest degree obtained and the binary ‘currently practicing’ were treated as factors. Categorical variables hours of activity, socializing and listening to music were recoded to be continuous. The remaining variables were continuous. All continuous variables were mean-centered and an interaction between age and familiarity was also included in the model. The random effects structure included random intercepts for each participant and random slopes as a function of familiarity (piece) for each participant.

This analysis was carried out in the frequentist framework with an alpha of .05. Significance of each predictor was determined by the t and p values produced by the lm function using the combined lme4 [87] and lmerTest [88] R packages. Model quality was assessed using multiple R2 and the overall model’s F-statistic, as produced by the same lm function.

Exploratory analysis.

Exploratory analyses were also carried out in the frequentist framework with an alpha of .05.

Results

Pre-registered analysis

The pre-registered model is summarized in Table 3. Familiarity, level of difficulty, non-formal musical training and the hearing self-assessment score were significant predictors of task performance as measured by d’. Crucially, age was not a significant predictor, nor did it significantly interact with familiarity.

thumbnail
Table 3. Summary of pre-registered linear model including coefficient estimate, coefficient standard error, t-value and p-values.

EKN = Eine Kleine Nachtmusik.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0305969.t003

More specifically, both unfamiliar pieces predicted worse performance (M = -0.07 and M = -0.41 for the tonal and atonal pieces, respectively) than the familiar Eine Kleine Nachtmusik (M = 2.40), but the unfamiliar pieces did not differ from each other, suggesting that tonality conferred no particular advantage, or atonality a particular disadvantage in learning and recognizing a new musical theme (see Fig 3). Participants performed worse for the hard themes (M = -0.26 and M = -0.45 for Pirate Waltz and Unexpectedly Absent, respectively) than for the medium (M = 0.01 and M = -0.37 for Pirate Waltz and Unexpectedly Absent, respectively) and easy themes (M = 2.40, M = 0.05 and M = -0.41 for Eine Kline Nachtmusik, Pirate Waltz and Unexpectedly Absent, respectively). Non-formal musical training positively influenced performance (coefficient 0.56). Finally, worse hearing as approximated by the hearing self-assessment predicted declining performance (coefficient -0.31).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Task performance as measured by d’ as a function of the piece of music, where Eine Kleine Nachtmusik is familiar and tonal, Pirate Waltz is unfamiliar and tonal and Unexpectedly Absent is both unfamiliar and atonal, and setting.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0305969.g003

Though there were no age effects on task performance, age predicted performance on cognitive tasks and the number of years of musical training (see Table 4). Participants in the live setting generally performed worse on the letter-number sequencing and both Trail-Making tests than participants in the lab setting, which is unsurprising given the noisy environment in the concert hall and atrium. Participants in the live setting also had significantly less years of non-formal musical training (see Table 5) than participants in the lab setting.

thumbnail
Table 4. Summary of linear models for age predicting measures of cognitive function and musical training, including coefficient estimate, coefficient standard error, t-value, p-values and model multiple R2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0305969.t004

thumbnail
Table 5. Summary of linear models for setting (lab or live) predicting measures of cognitive function and musical training, including coefficient estimate, coefficient standard error, t-value, p-values and model multiple R2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0305969.t005

Exploratory analysis

Refined model, larger dataset.

Given many live setting participants were excluded from the pre-registered analysis due to missing data and that neither age nor cognitive test scores were significant predictors of performance, performance based on familiarity alone can be analyzed with a larger sample size. Accordingly, a model predicting task performance (d’) with familiarity (piece)and theme identification difficulty as fixed effects was fitted. All confound predictors included in the pre-registered model were removed due to missing data. The model had χ2 (4, 636) = 238.88, p < .05 and marginal R2 = 0.68. Effects of familiarity and difficulty were significant in the same pattern as the pre-registered model (see Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Task performance as measured by d’ as a function of theme iteration difficulty and familiarity for all live and lab setting participants (except those excluded in pre-processing).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0305969.g004

Removing Eine Kleine Nachtmusik.

In case any effect of tonality alone was masked by the effect of veridical knowledge of Eine Kleine Nachtmusik, and given it was also used as a control piece, a model predicting task performance (d’) with familiarity (piece) and theme identification difficulty that excluded Eine Kleine Nachtmusik was fitted. In this way, the tonality aspect of familiarity can be tested separately from the veridical knowledge aspect of familiarity. Thirty-two participants were included in this model. It produced a significant effect of familiarity (β = -0.35, SE = 0.09, t(29) = -4.00, p < .05), where performance for Pirate Waltz (M = -0.04) was better than for Unexpectedly Absent (M = -0.38) and for theme iteration difficulty, where hard themes (M = -0.34, β = -0.15, SE = 0.07, t (128) = -2.13, p < .05) were more difficult to detect than easy themes (M = -0.19). This model had χ2 (3) = 21.51, p < .05 and marginal R2 = 0.10.

Given there is an effect of familiarity without the control piece, the effect of age on task performance for the unfamiliar tonal and unfamiliar atonal pieces can be re-estimated. For this model, task performance was predicted by age, familiarity and theme iteration difficulty, with an interaction between age and familiarity. Theme iteration difficulty was again a significant predictor, in the same pattern and direction as previously but critically, age was once again not a significant predictor of task performance, nor did it interact with familiarity, which was also no longer a significant main effect (see Table 6). This model had χ2 (5) = 24.82, p < .05, marginal R2 = .13.

thumbnail
Table 6. Summary of exploratory linear model investigating effect of age, familiarity and difficulty on performance, including coefficient estimate, coefficient standard error, t-value and p-values.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0305969.t006

Hit rate as a function of time in piece.

Based on evidence that listeners struggle to detect matching contours but are better at discriminating intervals given a musically filled interval [48, 89] and the recency effect [90, 91], it is possible that performance also depends on the position in time of each iteration of the theme in each novel piece. Eine Kleine Nachtmusik was not analyzed here because it only contained two iterations of the theme. To test this hypothesis, the sum of hits for each theme iteration was calculated, divided by number of participants in each setting (104 participants total), and predicted by iteration number, difficulty and familiarity in a linear model. No predictor was significant. The model had F (11, 28) = 1.59, p > .05, multiple R2 = .38.

Hits and false alarms.

An analysis of hits and false alarms separately was motivated for three reasons. First, we might expect more false alarms in the context of a full musical piece compared to typical signal detection paradigms due to increased similarities across the stimulus. Incorporating melody, harmony, rhythm and timbre, there is much higher scope for mistaking any part of the music heard as sounding similar to the theme. Second, poor encoding could explain the high rate of false alarms, where participants simply do not correctly remember the theme. Third, a higher rate of false alarms has previously been reported for recognition memory in older adults while hit rates are comparable to younger adults [8, 48, 57]. This interaction is hypothesized to be due to an increased reliance on familiarity or resemblance rather than recollection or retrieval more specifically [8].Here we explore this possibility by modelling the effects of age, setting, musical training (binary ‘currently practicing’, hours listening to music, years of formal and non-formal training) and familiarity (piece), along with their interactions, on hit and false alarm rates calculated for each participant (92 participants total).

Hit rate.

Familiarity (β = -0.30, SE = 0.05, t(32) = -6.56, p < .05 for Pirate Waltz and β = -0.33, SE = 0.04, t(33) = -7.52, p < .05 for Unexpectedly Absent) and age (β = -0.00, SE = 0.00, t(31) = -2.04, p < .05) but not musical training (all predictors > .05) were significant predictors of hit rate, where the model had F (9, 208) = 43.13, p < .05, multiple R2 = .65. Specifically, mean hit rates were M = 0.33, M = 0.06 and M = 0.05 for Eine Kleine Nachtmusik, Pirate Waltz and Unexpectedly Absent, respectively. Age was negatively correlated with hit rate, with a very small model coefficient of -0.001. This is equivalent to a decreased hit rate of 0.06 for an 80-year-old participant compared to a 20-year-old participant.

False alarm rate.

Years of formal training was a significant predictor of false alarm rate (β = -0.00, SE = 0.00, t(22) = -2.11, p < .05), but not age, familiarity or the interaction between these two, where the model had F = (9, 208) = 7.45, p < .05, multiple R2 = .24. Years of formal training were negatively correlated with false alarm rate, with a model coefficient of -0.0007, indicating a small reduction in false alarms with increased formal musical training.

Combined, these models suggest the opposite pattern than that expected: older adults obtain slightly less hits than younger adults, but a similar number of false alarms.

Discussion

This study explored the effects of age, familiarity on a recognition memory task for new music in a live concert and lab setting. Overall, we find no main effect of age when tasked with recognizing a theme in a piece of music, nor any significant interaction of age with familiarity, setting or musical training. Age was only a significant predictor for hit rate analysed separately. Familiarity was consistently a significant predictor, where performance was best overall for Eine Kleine Nachtmusik, a tonal and familiar piece. When removed, we also report a significant difference between the unfamiliar tonal (Pirate Waltz) and unfamiliar atonal (Unexpectedly Absent) pieces, where tonality conferred an advantage. Musical training had small positive effects, where formal musical training predicted a small reduction in false alarm rate and non-formal musical training predicted better task performance. We will now discuss these findings in the context of the literature, consider their implications for music as a cognitive scaffold for auditory rehabilitation training in older adults and the limits of generalizability of this study.

Findings in context

Lack of age effect.

The absence of an age effect for this recognition memory task is in line with literature on aging and memory in general [5, 35] and some music-specific literature [15]. On the other hand, this is different from studies that do find an age effect [7, 8, 11]. One of the challenges of the literature on musical memory and aging is that studies employ a range of paradigms and stimuli, where as a highly complex stimulus, small differences can have a large impact.

In this study, participants heard a theme three times then were tasked with identifying it in a piece of music in real time. A discrepancy in the age effect was reported between Bartlett et al. [7], where a melody was presented once and older adults performed worse than younger adults, and Blanchet et al. [8], where a melody was presented twice and older adults performed similarly to younger adults, both in a recognition task. Our study aligns with Blanchet et al.’s [8] results, where increased exposure–time to consolidate–seems to relieve otherwise negative effects of aging on encoding and/or recognizing new information.

The first iteration of the theme was always immediate (slight exception where there are a few bars of timpani roll and some bells in Pirate Waltz before the first theme) and additional iterations occurred separated by music-filled intervals, which Dowling [89] found aided learning. However, this was in a same-different task; in this study, all iterations were exact repetitions or transpositions of the theme, without any modifications to the interval content. We did not find any effect of iteration, either an advantage of repetition easing recognition by the end of the piece, or any primacy or recency effects.

Degrees of familiarity.

We consistently found effects of familiarity on recognition memory. Overall, performance was best for Eine Kleine Nachtmusik, where the piece is well-known and tonal. Furthermore, the theme is clearly played by the whole ensemble. When Eine Kleine Nachtmusik was removed from analysis, we still found an effect of familiarity on performance. This effect suggests that tonality, a schematically familiar musical system, does indeed help to new musical material, at least when compared to atonality, a schematically unfamiliar system. This is expected and in line with other literature on the role of tonality or enculturation on memory [7, 8, 12, 15, 60, 79].

Lack of setting effect.

The question of setting is particularly interesting as there is increasing interest in conducting experiments in live concert settings for increased ecological validity [9296]. However, there are valid concerns about the loss of control that comes with a naturalistic setting that can be mitigated in-lab. Our results suggest that there is no difference, loss or benefit, of a live setting compared to a lab setting. This is encouraging; however, further studies should continue to directly compare paradigms in live and lab settings to better evaluate and identify the benefits and challenges of live setting studies.

The musical training non-advantage.

The weak positive results around musical training mirror a mixed literature. Overall, all significant model coefficients relating to musical training were small or very small, suggesting that even if there is a benefit or hindrance of musical training on memory performance, it likely does not have much practical impact on day to day listening. The decrease in false alarms with increased musical training suggests that musicians are more discerning of what is not a theme, though are not necessarily better at identifying themes. It is interesting that musical training did not confer an advantage for the atonal piece, where interval discrimination is more difficult without schematic knowledge of the musical system, and where previous literature suggests musicians actually have an advantage due to domain-specific knowledge [1, 12]. The richness of the stimuli, here fully orchestrated pieces of music instead of monophonic melodies as in most studies, may have mitigated learning and recognition difficulties by providing enough context cues for everyone to pick up on.

It is also possible that the lack of a musicianship effect is related to the relationship between perceptual advantages in musicians and the motor system, where it is the playing of the music that confers an advantage rather than listening alone [75, 97, 98]. In this context, it is possible that many musician participants have played Eine Kleine Nachtmusik but can never have played Pirate Waltz or Unexpectedly Absent and therefore do not have the advantage of motor processing in addition to perceptual processing.

Music as a cognitive scaffold

As discussed in the introduction, this study was designed in the context of the theory of cognitive scaffolding [74], where cognitive skills maintained in music listening can be built upon to improve speech-in-noise perception [65, 75]. There is a general idea that active engagement in an activity like music is beneficial in old age [99, 100]. More specifically, several longitudinal training studies have investigated the effect of musical training on working memory in older adults, with positive outcomes [19, 22, 26, 27, 32]. Some work even specifically suggests changes in neural oscillations as the mechanism by which training improves working memory [61]. This study further supports the use of music in particular as a medium for cognitive maintenance and training in older adults by offering evidence that recognition memory is not affected by age in a realistic listening situation. Accordingly, music recognition could be considered a strength, onto which other aspects of memory could be scaffolded in a rehabilitation setting. For example, it may be easier to remember something if you pair a melody with it. In fact, this idea is not new. Throughout history songs have been used to transmit information orally between generations.

Limits of generalizability

Though the lack of a detrimental effect of age on recognition memory for real music is encouraging, we must be careful of how we generalize these findings. Theoretically, we sampled from the general population, which in St. John’s, Newfoundland has been increasing in diversity over the past years due to immigration and the presence of Memorial University of Newfoundland [101]. However, attending a symphony concert and having free time to participate in a lab experiment are representative of a higher socio-economic status. While the Newfoundland Symphony Orchestra makes efforts to engage with the public and be more accessible to a wider audience, it is still best to assume more wealth than less in this context. Higher socio-economic status has implications for other demographics such as race (white), sexuality (heterosexual) and disability (able-bodied), where global systems of power benefit those most similar to the cis-hetero white man [102]. Furthermore, immigration at the current rate is a recent phenomenon in Newfoundland and Labrador, resulting in a justified assumption that most older adults in attendance at our concert and lab studies were white. The Indigenous population of Newfoundland and Labrador is likely to be under-represented, if included at all, due to historical and ongoing systems of oppression that impoverish Indigenous populations [103, 104].

Research on the intersection of memory and race seems to be focused on memory for faces, demonstrating a same-race preference [105]. We found no work on race effects for auditory memory; however, some cross-cultural research in other musical domains can formulate some possibilities. Overall, it appears that enculturation has an effect on musical dimensions such as grouping, hierarchy, tonality and entrainment [106]. Generally, while non-Western listeners ‘understand’ Western tonal and their own cultural conventions, Western listeners do not. Therefore, in the context of our study, we would not expect to find an effect of race on memory performance. However, we can make some other possible connections. For example, living in a white supremacist settler state like Canada exposes a racialized person to ongoing trauma [107] and Indigenous populations additionally cope with intergenerational trauma [104]. Trauma in turn negatively affects memory processes [108]. On the other hand, racialized populations, especially Indigenous peoples, have strong oral memory traditions, which might suggest improved memory for auditory content and for music more specifically. Either way, the implications of demographic traits such as race, sexuality and ability are not known because they were not studied. Our study is likely representative of a predominantly white, middle to upper class settler population and should not be generalized to other populations.

We must also address a number of limitations on the design of this study. First, there was an unequal number of themes in the three pieces. This discrepancy was difficult to avoid as the authors could not think of any existing pieces of orchestral music that contained 10–12 iterations of an exactly transposed themes in controlled levels of textural density (levels of difficulty). However, if we had composed pieces of music with only two easy to identify themes, we likely would have produced ceiling effects, which would not be informative. We believe we have partially addressed this discrepancy in our exploratory analysis by comparing only the novel pieces to evaluate the effect of familiarity as operationalized by tonality alone.

It is also possible that participants may have taken more than 2s after the end of the theme to identify it. We were limited in our time window to the shortest amount of time before a second iteration of the theme. This was unavoidable for Eine Kleine Nachtmusik but could have been addressed in the commissioned pieces. However, it is telling that both composers chose to write two themes in short succession and is a common thing to do. It is also possible that participants identified a theme while it was still unfolding, rather than only after, as instructed. We chose to include the entire theme as part of the hit window because we assumed that many participants would be tempted to respond early regardless of the instructions. This may have inflated the false alarm rate as some of these false alarms may have been for the one or two themes that were ‘lures’. However, since there is only one instance of this in Pirate Waltz, we did not explore the frequency of this in detail. Future work could either avoid lures entirely, or systematically include lures and analyze their role. Relatedly, the structure of music increases the likelihood that any particular feature or excerpt “sounds like” something else in the piece, potentially inflating the rate of false alarms. The signal detection paradigm is designed for binary stimuli, where there is a much clearer correct or incorrect response. In music, a rhythm or harmony or interval may have sounded similar to the theme, even though the target was an exact match. This may explain the low d’ scores overall for the novel pieces of music, where the false alarm rates greatly outnumbered the hit rates. The authors are not currently aware of any techniques to adapt signal detection to this particular context. Our exploratory analysis separating hit and false alarm rates attempts to mitigate this performance issue somewhat, revealing an age effect on hit rate alone that may have been masked by the high rates of false alarms.

Within the confines of the design and population limitations of our study, our findings suggest that recognition memory for new music is not particularly adversely affected by age when presented live as a full piece of music. As far as the authors are aware, memory for music in older adults focuses on short excerpts and in laboratory settings. Our findings also suggest that learning and subsequently recognizing a new musical excerpt is a difficult task for all ages, and that veridical knowledge is a better predictor of performance than tonality. Finally, performance on the task was not affected by whether the study took place in the laboratory or in a live setting, which is encouraging both for more ecologically valid work and laboratory work generalizability alike.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the land on which we live and work as the stolen and unceded ancestral homelands of the Beothuk, the island of Newfoundland as part of the traditional territories of the Mi’kmaq and Labrador as the traditional territories of the Inuk of Nunatsiavut and NunatuKavut and the Innu of Nitassinan. We are thankful to live and work here as settlers (SAS, SC, AF and BRZ), and immigrants (PS, ӦD).

References

  1. 1. Halpern AR, Bartlett JC. Aging and memory for music: A review. Psychomusicology J Res Music Cogn. 2002;18: 10.
  2. 2. Bugaiska A, Clarys D, Jarry C, Taconnat L, Tapia G, Vanneste S, et al. The effect of aging in recollective experience: The processing speed and executive functioning hypothesis. Conscious Cogn. 2007;16: 797–808. pmid:17251040
  3. 3. Bunce D. Cognitive support at encoding attenuates age differences in recollective experience among adults of lower frontal lobe function. Neuropsychology. 2003;17: 353–361. pmid:12959501
  4. 4. Craik FI, Salthouse TA. The handbook of aging and cognition. Psychology press; 2011.
  5. 5. Hedden T, Gabrieli JDE. Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5: 87–96. pmid:14735112
  6. 6. Andrews MW, Dowling WJ, Bartlett JC, Halpern AR. Identification of speeded and slowed familiar melodies by younger, middle-aged, and older musicians and nonmusicians. Psychol Aging. 1998;13: 462. pmid:9793121
  7. 7. Bartlett JC, Halpern AR, Dowling WJ. Recognition of familiar and unfamiliar melodies in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Mem Cognit. 1995;23: 531–546. pmid:7476239
  8. 8. Blanchet S, Belleville S, Peretz I. Episodic Encoding in Normal Aging: Attentional Resources Hypothesis Extended to Musical Material. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2006;13: 490–502. pmid:16887785
  9. 9. Deffler SA, Halpern AR. Contextual information and memory for unfamiliar tunes in older and younger adults. Psychol Aging. 2011;26: 900. pmid:21534691
  10. 10. Dowling WJ, Bartlett JC, Halpern AR, Andrews MW. Melody recognition at fast and slow tempos: Effects of age, experience, and familiarity. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2008;70: 496–502. pmid:18459260
  11. 11. Gaudreau D, Peretz I. Implicit and explicit memory for music in old and young adults. Brain Cogn. 1999;40: 126–129.
  12. 12. Halpern AR, Bartlett JC, Dowling WJ. Aging and experience in the recognition of musical transpositions. Psychol Aging. 1995;10: 325. pmid:8527054
  13. 13. Halpern AR, Kwak S, Bartlett JC, Dowling WJ. Effects of aging and musical experience on the representation of tonal hierarchies. Psychol Aging. 1996;11: 235. pmid:8795052
  14. 14. Halpern AR, Bartlett JC, Dowling WJ. Perception of mode, rhythm, and contour in unfamiliar melodies: Effects of age and experience. Music Percept Interdiscip J. 1998;15: 335–355.
  15. 15. Lynch MP, Steffens ML. Effects of aging on processing of novel musical structure. J Gerontol. 1994;49: P165–P172. pmid:8014397
  16. 16. Meinz E. Experience-based attenuation of age-related differences in music cognition tasks. Psychol Aging. 2000;15: 297–312. pmid:10879584
  17. 17. Parks SL, Clancy Dollinger S. The positivity effect and auditory recognition memory for musical excerpts in Young, Middle-Aged and Older adults. Psychomusicology Music Mind Brain. 2014;24: 298–308.
  18. 18. Samson S, Baird A, Moussard A, Clément S. Does pathological aging affect musical learning and memory? Music Percept Interdiscip J. 2012;29: 493–500.
  19. 19. Bugos JA. The Benefits of Music Instruction on Processing Speed, Verbal Fluency, and Cognitive Control in Aging. 2010;4.
  20. 20. Bugos J. Protocol for a randomized controlled trial of piano training on cognitive and psychosocial outcomes. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2018;1423: 360–367.
  21. 21. Bugos JA. The Effects of Bimanual Coordination in Music Interventions on Executive Functions in Aging Adults. Front Integr Neurosci. 2019;13. Available: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnint.2019.00068.
  22. 22. Diaz Abrahan V, Shifres F, Justel N. Impact of music-based intervention on verbal memory: an experimental behavioral study with older adults. Cogn Process. 2021;22: 117–130. pmid:32955643
  23. 23. Galinha IC, Farinha M, Lima ML, Palmeira AL. Sing4Health: protocol of a randomized controlled trial of the effects of a singing group intervention on the well-being, cognitive function and health of older adults. BMC Geriatr. 2020;20: 354. pmid:32948120
  24. 24. James CE, Altenmüller E, Kliegel M, Krüger THC, Van De Ville D, Worschech F, et al. Train the brain with music (TBM): brain plasticity and cognitive benefits induced by musical training in elderly people in Germany and Switzerland, a study protocol for an RCT comparing musical instrumental practice to sensitization to music. BMC Geriatr. 2020;20: 418. pmid:33087078
  25. 25. Kakihara M, Wang X, Iwasaki S, Soshi T, Yamashita M, Sekiyama K. The association between performance skills and cognitive improvement in a 10-week musical instrument training for older adults. Proceedings of the 17th International Conference for Music Perception and Cognition. Tokyo and online; 2023.
  26. 26. Marie D, Müller CAH, Altenmüller E, Van De Ville D, Jünemann K, Scholz DS, et al. Music interventions in 132 healthy older adults enhance cerebellar grey matter and auditory working memory, despite general brain atrophy. Neuroimage Rep. 2023;3: 100166.
  27. 27. Mussoi BS. The Impact of Music Training and Working Memory on Speech Recognition in Older Age. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2021;64: 4524–4534. pmid:34586881
  28. 28. Roden I, Grube D, Bongard S, Kreutz G. Does music training enhance working memory performance? Findings from a quasi-experimental longitudinal study. Psychol Music. 2014;42: 284–298.
  29. 29. Román-Caballero R, Arnedo M, Triviño M, Lupiáñez J. Musical practice as an enhancer of cognitive function in healthy aging—A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLOS ONE. 2018;13: e0207957. pmid:30481227
  30. 30. Worschech F, Marie D, Jünemann K, Sinke C, Krüger THC, Großbach M, et al. Improved Speech in Noise Perception in the Elderly After 6 Months of Musical Instruction. Front Neurosci. 2021;15. Available: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2021.696240 pmid:34305522
  31. 31. Worschech F, Altenmüller E, Jünemann K, Sinke C, Krüger THC, Scholz DS, et al. Evidence of cortical thickness increases in bilateral auditory brain structures following piano learning in older adults. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2022;1513: 21–30. pmid:35292982
  32. 32. Zhang L, Fu X, Luo D, Xing L, Du Y. Musical Experience Offsets Age-Related Decline in Understanding Speech-in-Noise: Type of Training Does Not Matter, Working Memory Is the Key. Ear Hear. 2020;42: 258–270. pmid:32826504
  33. 33. Kryscio RJ, Abner EL, Cooper GE, Fardo DW, Jicha GA, Nelson PT, et al. Self-reported memory complaints: Implications from a longitudinal cohort with autopsies. Neurology. 2014;83: 1359–1365. pmid:25253756
  34. 34. Zacks RT, Hasher L, Li KZH. Human memory. The handbook of aging and cognition, 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2000. pp. 293–357.
  35. 35. Anderson ND, Craik FI. 50 years of cognitive aging theory. J Gerontol Ser B. 2017;72: 1–6. pmid:27974471
  36. 36. Craik FI. Memory changes in normal aging. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 1994;3: 155–158.
  37. 37. Hasher L, Zacks RT. Automatic and effortful processes in memory. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1979;108: 356.
  38. 38. Craik FI, Rose NS. Memory encoding and aging: a neurocognitive perspective. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36: 1729–1739. pmid:22155274
  39. 39. Finnigan SO’Connell RG, Cummins TD, Broughton M, Robertson IH. ERP measures indicate both attention and working memory encoding decrements in aging. Psychophysiology. 2011;48: 601–611. pmid:21039584
  40. 40. Friedman D, Nessler D, Johnson R Jr. Memory encoding and retrieval in the aging brain. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2007;38: 2–7. pmid:17319586
  41. 41. Haaland KY, Price L, Larue A. What does the WMS–III tell us about memory changes with normal aging? 2001.
  42. 42. Salthouse TA, Meinz EJ. Aging, inhibition, working memory, and speed. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1995;50: P297–P306. pmid:7583809
  43. 43. Hale S, Rose NS, Myerson J, Strube MJ, Sommers M, Tye-Murray N, et al. The structure of working memory abilities across the adult life span. Psychol Aging. 2011;26: 92. pmid:21299306
  44. 44. Rose NS, Myerson J, Sommers MS, Hale S. Are there age differences in the executive component of working memory? Evidence from domain-general interference effects. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2009;16: 633–653. pmid:19401863
  45. 45. Halpern AR, Müllensiefen D. Effects of timbre and tempo change on memory for music. Q J Exp Psychol. 2008;61: 1371–1384. pmid:19086302
  46. 46. Li J, Baker DJ, Burgoyne JA, Honing H. Is Pitch Information Indispensable for Music Recognition? A Pilot Study Based on a Musical Matching-Pairs Game. Proceedings of the 17th International Conference for Music Perception and Cognition. Tokyo and online; 2023.
  47. 47. Craik FI, Lockhart RS. Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav. 1972;11: 671–684.
  48. 48. Halpern AR, Bartlett JC. Memory for Melodies. In: Riess Jones M, Fay RR, Popper AN, editors. Music Perception. New York, NY: Springer New York; 2010. pp. 233–258. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-6114-3_8
  49. 49. Yonelinas AP. The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity: A Review of 30 Years of Research. J Mem Lang. 2002;46: 441–517.
  50. 50. McAuley JD, Stevens C, Humphreys MS. Play it again: did this melody occur more frequently or was it heard more recently? The role of stimulus familiarity in episodic recognition of music. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2004;116: 93–108. pmid:15111232
  51. 51. Krumhansl CL, Shepard RN. Quantification of the hierarchy of tonal functions within a diatonic context. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1979;5: 579. pmid:528960
  52. 52. Margulis EH. A Model of Melodic Expectation. Music Percept Interdiscip J. 2005;22: 663–714.
  53. 53. Besson M, Faïta F. An event-related potential (ERP) study of musical expectancy: Comparison of musicians with nonmusicians. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1995;21: 1278.
  54. 54. Cuddy LL, Badertscher B. Recovery of the tonal hierarchy: Some comparisons across age and levels of musical experience. Percept Psychophys. 1987;41: 609–620. pmid:3615155
  55. 55. Halpern AR, Zioga I, Shankleman M, Lindsen J, Pearce MT, Bhattarcharya J. That note sounds wrong! Age-related effects in processing of musical expectation. Brain Cogn. 2017;113: 1–9. pmid:28064077
  56. 56. Krumhansl CL. Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. Oxford University Press; 1990. Available: https://books.google.co.uk/books?hl=en&lr=&id=J4dJCAAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PT11&dq=krumhansl+cognitive+foundations&ots=PF47VC1-Cj&sig=DjBgMy-284NnR8pLJ3cb6AHMuZI.
  57. 57. Mikumo M. Encoding Strategies for Tonal and Atonal Melodies. Music Percept. 1992;10: 73–81.
  58. 58. Radvansky GA, Fleming KJ, Simmons JA. Timbre Reliance in Nonmusicians’ and Musicians’ Memory for Melodies. Music Percept Interdiscip J. 1995;13: 127–140.
  59. 59. Cuddy LL, Lyons HI. Musical pattern recognition: A comparison of listening to and studying tonal structures and tonal ambiguities. Psychomusicology J Res Music Cogn. 1981;1: 15–33.
  60. 60. Demorest SM, Morrison SJ, Jungbluth D, Beken MN. Lost in Translation: An Enculturation Effect in Music Memory Performance. Music Percept. 2008;25: 213–223.
  61. 61. Yurgil KA, Velasquez MA, Winston JL, Reichman NB, Colombo PJ. Music Training, Working Memory, and Neural Oscillations: A Review. Front Psychol. 2020;11. Available: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00266.
  62. 62. Ding Y, Gray K, Forrence A, Wang X, Huang J. A behavioral study on tonal working memory in musicians and non-musicians. PLOS ONE. 2018;13: e0201765. pmid:30071003
  63. 63. Amer T, Kalender B, Hasher L, Trehub SE, Wong Y. Do older professional musicians have cognitive advantages? PloS One. 2013;8: e71630. pmid:23940774
  64. 64. Grassi M, Meneghetti C, Toffalini E, Borella E. Correction: Auditory and cognitive performance in elderly musicians and nonmusicians. PLOS ONE. 2018;13: e0192918. pmid:29420627
  65. 65. Parbery-Clark A, Strait DL, Anderson S, Hittner E, Kraus N. Musical experience and the aging auditory system: implications for cognitive abilities and hearing speech in noise. PloS One. 2011;6: e18082. pmid:21589653
  66. 66. Beauvois MW, Meddis R. Time decay of auditory stream biasing. Percept Psychophys. 1997;59: 81–86. pmid:9038410
  67. 67. Borella E, Carbone E, Pastore M, De Beni R, Carretti B. Working Memory Training for Healthy Older Adults: The Role of Individual Characteristics in Explaining Short- and Long-Term Gains. Front Hum Neurosci. 2017;11. Available: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00099 pmid:28381995
  68. 68. Ong JH, Chan AHD. Working memory modulates the effect of music on word learning. Lang Cogn. 2023;15: 131–147.
  69. 69. Pichora-Fuller MK, Kramer SE, Eckert MA, Edwards B, Hornsby BW, Humes LE, et al. Hearing impairment and cognitive energy: The framework for understanding effortful listening (FUEL). Ear Hear. 2016;37: 5S–27S. pmid:27355771
  70. 70. National Seniors Strategy. Ageism, Elder Abuse and Social Isolation. Canada: National Seniors Strategy; Available: http://nationalseniorsstrategy.ca/the-four-pillars/pillar-1/addressing-ageism-elder-abuse-and-social-isolation/.
  71. 71. Lin FR, Yaffe K, Xia J, Xue Q-L, Harris TB, Purchase-Helzner E, et al. Hearing loss and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA Intern Med. 2013;173: 293–299. pmid:23337978
  72. 72. Laukka P. Uses of music and psychological well-being among the elderly. J Happiness Stud. 2007;8: 215.
  73. 73. Sloboda J, Ford B. Why do classical musicians need to build audiences? Perform Arts. 1982;1992: 2002–2008.
  74. 74. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Park DC. How does it STAC up? Revisiting the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Neuropsychol Rev. 2014;24: 355–370. pmid:25143069
  75. 75. Zendel BR, Sauvé SA. Towards music-based rehabilitation for older adults. In: Cuddy LL, Belleville S, Moussard A, editors. Music and the Aging Brain. Academic Press; 2020. pp. 293–313. Available: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128174227000110.
  76. 76. Sauvé SA, Marozeau J, Zendel BR. Age and experience-related use of auditory streaming cues. PLOS ONE. 2022. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0274631.
  77. 77. Snyder JS, Alain C. Sequential auditory scene analysis is preserved in normal aging adults. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17: 501–512. pmid:16581981
  78. 78. Coren S, Hakstian AR. The development and cross-validation of a self-report inventory to assess pure-tone threshold hearing sensitivity. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1992;35: 921–928. pmid:1405547
  79. 79. Dowling WJ, Kwak S, Andrews MW. The time course of recognition of novel melodies. Percept Psychophys. 1995;57: 136–149. pmid:7885812
  80. 80. Tucker AM, Stern Y. COGNITIVE RESERVE IN AGING. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2011;8: 354–360. pmid:21222591
  81. 81. Government of Canada SC. A generational portrait of Canada’s aging population from the 2021 Census. 27 Apr 2022 [cited 15 Sep 2022]. Available: https://census.gc.ca/census-recensement/2021/as-sa/98-200-X/2021003/98-200-X2021003-eng.cfm.
  82. 82. Weschler D. WAIS-III: Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.
  83. 83. Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Administration and interpretation of the Trail Making Test. Nat Protoc. 2006;1: 2277–2281. pmid:17406468
  84. 84. Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19: 203–214. pmid:15010086
  85. 85. Makowski D. The psycho Package: an Efficient and Publishing-Oriented Workflow for Psychological Science. J Open Source Softw. 2018;3: 470.
  86. 86. Grolemund G, Wickham H. Dates and Times Made Easy with lubridate. J Stat Softw. 2011;40: 1–25.
  87. 87. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67.
  88. 88. Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB. lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. J Stat Softw. 2017;82: 1–26.
  89. 89. Dowling WJ, Tillman B, Ayers DF. Memory and the Experience of Hearing Music. Music Percept. 2001;19: 249–276.
  90. 90. McFarland DJ, Cacace AT. Aspects of Short-Term Acoustic Recognition Memory: Modality and Serial Position Effects. Audiology. 1992;31: 342–352. pmid:1492818
  91. 91. Wingfield A. Effects of serial position and set size in auditory recognition memory. Mem Cognit. 1973;1: 53–55. pmid:24214475
  92. 92. Chang A, Livingstone SR, Bosnyak DJ, Trainor LJ. Body sway reflects leadership in joint music performance. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2017;114: E4134–E4141. pmid:28484007
  93. 93. Kayser D, Egermann H, Barraclough NE. Audience facial expressions detected by automated face analysis software reflect emotions in music. Behav Res Methods. 2022;54: 1493–1507. pmid:34508286
  94. 94. O’Neill K, Egermann H. Development of the Social Experience of a Concert Scales (SECS): The Social Experience of a Live Western Art Music Concert Influences People’s Overall Enjoyment of an Event but not Their Emotional Response to the Music. Music Sci. 2022;5.
  95. 95. Swarbrick D, Bosnyak D, Livingstone SR, Bansal J, Marsh-Rollo S, Woolhouse MH, et al. How Live Music Moves Us: Head Movement Differences in Audiences to Live Versus Recorded Music. Front Psychol. 2019;9. Available: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02682.
  96. 96. Upham F, Memis AE, Hansen NC, Rosas FE, Clim M-A, Jensenius AR. Participatory applause. Conf Interdiscip Musicol. 2022 [cited 31 Aug 2023].
  97. 97. Du Y, Zatorre RJ. Musical training sharpens and bonds ears and tongue to hear speech better. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2017;114: 13579–13584. pmid:29203648
  98. 98. Zendel BR, West GL, Belleville S, Peretz I. Musical training improves the ability to understand speech-in-noise in older adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2019;81: 102–115. pmid:31280114
  99. 99. Prickett CA. Music and the special challenges of aging: A new frontier. Int J Music Educ. 1998;os-31: 25–36.
  100. 100. Särkämö T., Cognitive emotional, and neural benefits of musical leisure activities in aging and neurological rehabilitation: A critical review. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2018;61: 414–418. pmid:28461128
  101. 101. StatCan. Number of immigrants in Newfoundland and Labrador 2022. In: Statista [Internet]. 28 Sep 2022 [cited 31 Aug 2023]. Available: https://www.statista.com/statistics/609051/number-of-immigrants-in-newfoundland-and-labrador/.
  102. 102. Elsherif M, Middleton S, Phan JM, Azevedo F, Iley B, Grose-Hodge M, et al. Bridging Neurodiversity and Open Scholarship: How Shared Values Can Guide Best Practices for Research Integrity, Social Justice, and Principled Education. MetaArXiv; 2022.
  103. 103. Palmater P., Genocide Indian policy, and legislated elimination of Indians in Canada. Aborig Policy Stud. 2014;3.
  104. 104. Truth and Reconciliation Commission of Canada. Canada’s Residential Schools: The Final Report of the Truth and Reconciliation Commission of Canada. McGill-Queen’s Press-MQUP; 2015.
  105. 105. Meissner CA, Brigham JC. Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review. Psychol Public Policy Law. 2001;7: 3–35.
  106. 106. Stevens CJ. Music perception and cognition: A review of recent cross-cultural research. Top Cogn Sci. 2012;4: 653–667. pmid:22811369
  107. 107. Maynard R. Policing Black lives: State violence in Canada from slavery to the present. Fernwood Publishing; 2017.
  108. 108. Toth SL, Cicchetti D. Remembering, forgetting, and the effects of trauma on memory:A developmental psychopathology perspective. Dev Psychopathol. 1998;10: 589–605. pmid:9886217