Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Hearing loss and psychosocial outcomes: Influences of social emotional aspects and personality

  • Kayla Cormier,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Speech Language and Hearing Sciences, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, United States of America

  • Christine Brennan,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Speech Language and Hearing Sciences, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, United States of America

  • Anu Sharma

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    anu.sharma@colorado.edu

    Affiliation Department of Speech Language and Hearing Sciences, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, United States of America

Abstract

The aim of this study was to examine the effects of social isolation, loneliness, anxiety, depression, higher stress, and memory complaints interacting with personality traits as a function of hearing loss. Personality traits have previously been shown to correlate with anxiety and depression, as well as hearing loss, suggesting an effect of personality on the relationship between social emotional outcomes and hearing loss. A cross-sectional survey including validated screening measures was anonymously administered. Measures included personality (Big Five Index-2 Extra-Short Form), stress (Perceived Stress Scale), anxiety and depression (Patient Health Questionnaire for Depression and Anxiety), loneliness (Three-Item Loneliness Scale), social engagement (Lubben Social Network Scale-6), and self-perceived memory complaints (Subjective Cognitive Function). Eight hundred and ninety-one responses were obtained from adults between the ages of 18 and 90 years old (M = 50 years old). Hearing loss was positively correlated with subjective memory complaints only when not accounting for demographic variables, personality traits, psychosocial outcomes, and social emotional outcomes. There were additive effects of hearing loss and negative emotionality on increases in memory complaints in those who self-identified as maybe having hearing loss. Higher degrees of hearing loss also increased loneliness, with greater hearing loss negating the positive correlation between extraversion and social engagement. Overall, our results suggest that hearing loss significantly interacts with personality traits and other social emotional measures. Our results suggest that the impact of hearing loss on memory complaints, social isolation, and loneliness may differ across patients with hearing loss in comparison with those who think they maybe have hearing loss. Information from this study may provide insights for hearing healthcare and mental healthcare professionals to better serve persons with hearing loss who may require additional support or interventions.

Introduction

Age related hearing loss affects an estimated 1.5 billion persons globally, making it one of the most common chronic health conditions in adults [1]. Untreated hearing loss is associated with cognitive decline and has recently been identified as a major independent and possibly modifiable risk factor for dementia [2, 3]. Although the relationship between hearing loss and cognitive decline has been demonstrated, the mechanism behind this relationship is still in question. Three of the theories that have been proposed have potentially differing implications for the expected outcomes of hearing treatment. A current theory suggests that listening effort due to degraded speech input from hearing loss increases a person’s cognitive load, as the individual is constantly utilizing top-down cognitive processing to make up for the poor sensory input. As a result, the cognitive load theory (see also the information degradation theory or Framework for Understanding Effortful Listening) suggests listening effort secondary to hearing loss results in a chronic taxing of cognition, specifically attention, memory, and executive functioning, resulting in lower global cognitive abilities in people with hearing loss compared to age matched peers [48]. This theory posits that the effects of hearing loss on cognition are temporary and may resolve with hearing treatments [7, 8]. The common cause theory suggests that the association between hearing loss and cognition is the result of another factor that causes both decreases in hearing and decreases in cognition simultaneously [79]. For example, cerebrovascular disease could result in changes in both cognition and hearing abilities [2]. This theory would not predict that hearing treatments would influence cognition [7, 8]. Finally, the cascade hypothesis suggests that auditory deprivation occurring not only due to the hearing loss, but also other hearing loss consequences such as social isolation, results in changes in the brain such as cortical re-allocation, deafferentation, or atrophy [710]. These cortical changes in turn can have a negative impact on cognition. However, reversal of these cortical changes by hearing treatments should predict increases in cognition with hearing treatments [8].

Hearing loss has not only cognitive consequences but may result in various physical and social emotional changes [1114]. Therefore the relationship between hearing loss and cognitive decline may be mediated by a decline in social emotional wellbeing [9]. Research has shown that hearing loss is correlated with a higher risk of both social isolation and loneliness [15, 16]. Recent meta-analyses have concluded that hearing loss is associated with depression [17, 18]. Hearing loss has also been found to be correlated with increases in stress and anxiety in addition to depression [12, 14, 19, 20]. Jayakody et al. [12] found that while hearing loss severity was associated with increases in emotional loneliness, depression and stress were associated with increases in social loneliness while hearing loss severity was positively associated with depression and stress. Therefore, a complex relationship may be present between the effects of hearing loss on psychosocial outcomes and the down-stream effects of those psychosocial outcomes on other mental health outcomes. Furthermore, Kiely et al. [21] demonstrated that while hearing loss severity was associated with increases in depression, accounting for activity engagement fully explained the relationship between hearing loss and depression. These studies suggest that the presence of hearing loss with changes in psychosocial well-being could interact in an intricate manner. Additionally, the COVID-19 pandemic highlighted the association between hearing loss and social emotional well-being. A recent survey during the COVID-19 pandemic found that patients with hearing loss fit with cochlear implants (CI) reported less diversity in listening environments, less interaction with people, and more loneliness than before the pandemic [22]. Considering these findings, Timmer et al. [23] has called for hearing healthcare providers to adopt individualized treatment plans for hearing loss patients that address social-emotional well-being. Specifically, these authors suggest the use of anxiety, depression, and social isolation questionnaires to tailor hearing loss interventions.

In fact, treatments for hearing loss could positively impact social emotional wellbeing. The use of hearing aids is reported to improve social support, decrease the odds of reporting psychological distress, and decrease odds of reporting depression in comparison to those with untreated hearing loss [2426]. Cochlear implantation also results in decreases in depression [26]. However, not all studies have demonstrated improvements in all areas of wellbeing with hearing aid use and researchers have suggested that these inconsistent findings confirm a need for more research into the associations of hearing treatments on social emotional wellbeing [9, 25]. Additionally, treatment with hearing aids and CI may vary in impact. In studies that have compared CI users and hearing aid users, CI users demonstrated significantly higher depression and loneliness scores pre-treatment than hearing aid users [26, 27]. After treatment, depression scores remained significantly lower for CI users throughout the first year of CI use but hearing aid users only showed significantly lower depression scores over the first 6 months of hearing aid usage [26]. Therefore, measures of social-emotional well-being (anxiety, depression, and social isolation) could be helpful for clinicians if utilized pre-and post-hearing loss treatment to identify the need for new interventions and/or goals [23]. In terms of cognition, both hearing aid and CI use have been associated with improvements in multiple aspects of cognition, including executive functioning, working and spatial memory, processing speed, visual learning, and visual attention in individuals with hearing loss [2832]. Yet, other research findings suggest no significant improvements in cognition with hearing treatments [3335]. Overall, the impacts of hearing treatments on psychosocial and social-emotional outcomes are currently mixed and warrant further investigation.

Personality traits can also affect both social emotional outcomes and cognition. Nikčević et al. [36] found that higher levels of extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness as measured by the Big Five Inventory-10 [37], negatively correlated with general anxiety and depression scores. The Big Five Inventory-10 is an established tool to examine personality traits focused on five factors including neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. This inventory was created by abbreviating a previous personality inventory known as the standard BFI (Big Five inventory) which had 44 items [38]. In general, studies suggest higher scores in neuroticism have been associated with worse cognitive performance, while higher scores on openness and conscientiousness are related to better cognitive performance [39, 40]. Sutin et al. [39] found that greater agreeableness was also related to better cognitive performance, while extraversion did not seem to have much of an impact on cognition. Kendler and Myers [41] established a negative correlation between conscientiousness and depression, as well as a positive correlation between neuroticism and depression.

Very few studies have examined the effect of personality traits in hearing loss patients. In terms of self-rated hearing loss, the rate of decline in extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness is faster in those reporting worse hearing abilities measured across four years [42]. In fact, only self-reported hearing loss was found to be associated with increased rates of decline in extraversion in a study of older adults over six years even when including self-reported general health or number of diagnoses in the analysis of this change [43]. The rate of increases in neuroticism were also faster in those noting worse hearing [42]. However, among hearing aid users, greater degrees of hearing loss were not related to differences in personality traits [44]. Lower scores in extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness, with higher scores in neuroticism, predicted worsening hearing four years later, as well [42]. On the other hand, higher scores on extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness were associated with lower risk of hearing loss progression [45]. In terms of hearing aid treatment, individuals with lower scores in neuroticism and openness were more likely to pursue hearing aids [44]. Additionally, higher scores in neuroticism were found to be related to poorer hearing aid outcomes including reporting residual difficulties with hearing aid use [46, 47]. Cox et al. [47] found personality affects self-reported outcomes on commonly used hearing aid questionnaires; however, following the creation of three factors from an exploratory factor analysis of these hearing aid questionnaires and inclusion of pre-fitting reports about hearing problems, expectations for hearing aids, and sound aversion, personality was no longer significantly or independently related to these measures. This underscores the importance of controlling variables when examining hearing loss, hearing treatments, and personality.

Largely, there is very little information regarding the relationship between personality, social wellbeing, and cognition in hearing loss patients. Such information may be valuable in allowing us to better understand the association between hearing loss and cognition, such as memory complaints, as well as better inform practices of both mental health professionals and hearing healthcare professionals to possibly deliver more customizable experiences to patients with hearing loss. To this end, our exploratory study aimed to examine the relationships between hearing loss, personality, and wellbeing. Given that clinical audiology patients present with limited social engagement, loneliness, anxiety, depression, stress, and memory concerns, we expected these variables to be related to hearing loss in a complex manner. Furthermore, previous studies have demonstrated differences in personality traits of those who obtain and benefit from hearing loss treatments [44, 47]. Yet, few studies have explored how all these factors interact in individuals with hearing loss. Therefore, this study aimed as a first step to identify and better understand the relationships between hearing loss, loneliness, social isolation, stress, anxiety, depression, and memory complaints.

Materials and methods

Study design

A cross-sectional, anonymous survey design was utilized for this study allowing for an investigation of a wide array of individuals. Inclusion criteria included residence within the United States and being 18 years of age or older. The survey was administered online via Qualtrics from February 8, 2021, to June 12, 2021. This data was collected during the COVID-19 pandemic, as such a question regarding COVID-19 vaccination status was added on March 22, 2021, with the majority (61%) of the survey respondents answering this question. The survey was distributed through Research Match and via social media, which included both group sites (hearing aid tracker, agency on aging, local city/town pages, etc.) and personal social media pages. No personally identifiable information was collected, questions within the survey were not randomized, and participants could only submit one response. Two validation questions were utilized in which the participant was instructed to select a particular response. Participants who did not select the indicated response for these questions were excluded from analysis. This survey was approved by the University of Colorado Boulder Institutional Review Board (IRB). All participants included in this study were first presented with an explanation of the study, after which they were asked to indicate if they consented to continue the study. Those who selected not to consent to the study terminated the questionnaire at that time and were thanked for their participation.

Closed ended questions were utilized for a demographics section including age, gender, employment status, education, marital status, number of individuals in the household, pets in the household, COVID-19 vaccination status, frequency of social media use, news consumption, the presence of tinnitus, self-reported hearing loss, use of amplification, and severity of hearing loss. Questions relating to COVID-19 vaccination status and pets in the household were included in this study due to the potential impact these factors could have on psychosocial and social emotional measures during the pandemic. Therefore, both of these measures were included in this analysis. For self-reported hearing loss, respondents could select that they did not have hearing loss, maybe had hearing loss, or had hearing loss. Previous research has shown self-reported hearing loss to be sensitive to objective measures of hearing thresholds [4850]. For those who self-reported having a hearing loss they answered more detailed questions about the hearing loss such as which ear and/or ears had hearing loss, the use of hearing loss treatment devices, duration of device use, and severity of hearing loss. Validated questionnaires were employed in this study to cover the domains of psychosocial measures (stress, loneliness, memory complaints), social emotional measures (anxiety, depression, and social isolation), and personality. The questions used in this survey can be viewed in S1 Appendix.

Psychosocial measures

The Three-Item Loneliness Scale was utilized to capture loneliness in this study. The Three-Item Loneliness scale is measured on a Likert scale from 1 (hardly ever) to 3 (often) indicating greater loneliness for higher scores [51]. This questionnaire was adapted from the longer Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale by selecting the three questions from Revised UCLA Loneliness scale that loaded onto the first factor of both an exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses. The Three-Item Loneliness scale is strongly, positively correlated with the Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale and the internal reliability, as measured with Cronbach’s Alpha, of this scale has been noted to be 0.72. Thus, the Three-Item Loneliness Scale has demonstrated good internal consistency, as well as good discriminant and convergent validity as a measure of loneliness [51].

Stress was measured using the short four item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-4) using a Likert scale from 0 (never) to 4 (very often) [52]. The PSS-4 was adapted from a longer version with 14 questions. The four questions selected for the PSS-4 have been shown to load onto one factor and demonstrate an internal reliability alpha coefficient of 0.72 [52].

To measure memory complaints, we employed the Subjective Cognitive Function (SCF) questionnaire using six yes/no questions with 1-point given for each yes response to the six included memory concerns. These questions are the same as those use by previous investigations of cognitive function and decline [53]. A study that examined whether physical activity was associated with late-life cognitive function measured subjective cognitive function using six yes/no questions about recent changes in the ability to remember recent events, remember a short list, remember things from one second to the next, understand or follow spoken instructions, follow a group conversation or plot of a show, and find one’s way on familiar streets. Two other studies [54, 55] examined the relationship between cognitive function and hearing also utilizing the same SCF questions as Fondell et al. [53]. These previous studies demonstrated a relationship between severity of hearing loss and SCF in both men and women [54, 55].

Social emotional measures

A 6-item Lubben Social Network Scale (LSNS-6) was administered with a Likert scale from 0 (none) to 5 (nine or more). Lower LSNS-6 scores indicate more social isolation, with the highest possible score being 30. The LSNS-6 includes three social engagement questions related to family and three questions related to friends [56]. Factor analysis of the LSNS-6 has demonstrated that each of these three items load heavily on to their respective subscales. The LSNS-6 also has been shown to have good discriminate validity and internal reliability with Cronbach’s Alpha coefficients of 0.83 having been reported for this scale indicating satisfactory reliability for this outcome measure [57]. In this study the subscales were not analyzed separately with only the total score being employed as both an independent and dependent variable.

The Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4), which is also a 4-item form, was used to examine anxiety (first 2 questions) and depression (last 2 questions). The PHQ-4 utilizes a Likert scale from 0 (never) to 3 (nearly every day) with a score greater than or equal to three on the two questions in each domain suggestive of anxiety and/or depression [58]. Factor analysis has confirmed that the respective items load on the two subscales of anxiety and depression. Additionally, a good internal reliability with a Cronbach’s Alpha of 0.78 has been noted for the PHQ-4, confirming the PHQ-4’s validity and reliability as a measure [59].

Personality

The Big Five Inventory–2 Extra-Short Form (BFI-2-XS) was used to examine personality across the categories of extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, negative emotionality, and open-mindedness. The BFI-2-XS is a 15-question measure that is scored using a Likert scale from 1 (disagree strongly) to 5 (agree strongly). Personality traits are measured separately and the separate totals from each personality trait were used in all analyses in this study [60]. The Big Five personality traits are well established in psychology. Persons with high scores in extraversion are more likely to be outgoing and enjoy social gatherings. Higher scores on agreeableness are related to being more trusting and helpful. Individuals who have higher scores in conscientiousness tend to plan out activities, be more organized, and methodical. Greater negative emotionality is related to experiencing more negative emotions such as anger or guilt. Those who show more open-mindedness tend to be curious and are open to trying new things [61]. In validating the BFI-2-XS all questions loaded strongly onto their respective five components of personality. Internal reliability Cronbach’s Alpha coefficients ranged from 0.83 to 0.90 for the subdomains, establishing adequate reliability and validity [60].

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were completed using R Studio version 4.2.1 [62]. Serial ordinary least squares regression models were utilized to examine each psychosocial outcome: loneliness, stress, and memory complaints, and each social emotional outcome: social isolation, anxiety, and depression, as outlined in Macià et al. [63]. In addition to this analysis technique being completed in the literature exploring loneliness, a similar approach has been used to examine personality and self-reported hearing aid outcomes [47].

Therefore, regressions were first completed without controlling for any other factors for both hearing loss and age (see uncontrolled regression models Fig 1A). In the uncontrolled hearing loss models, hearing loss factors included comparisons for self-reported hearing loss versus no hearing loss, self-report of maybe having hearing loss in comparison to the other hearing ability options (no hearing loss and hearing loss), hearing loss versus maybe having hearing loss, and no hearing loss versus maybe having hearing loss. The hearing loss models were completed twice once with orthogonal contrast codes and dummy codes to fully examine the differences across individuals who selected having hearing loss, maybe having hearing loss and not having hearing loss.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Regression models.

This data was analyzed using a consecutive ordinary least squares regression approach. (A) First uncontrolled regression models were run with only a few variables. These included hearing loss only and age only models. The hearing loss only model was completed twice with orthogonal contrast codes and dummy codes to fully examine the differences across individuals who selected having hearing loss, maybe having hearing loss, and not having hearing loss. To further investigate hearing loss, a hearing loss characteristics model was completed, which divided hearing loss into several reported factors. (B) Additional demographic, psychosocial, social emotional, and personality trait variables were added to the controlled regression. Interaction terms were included between hearing loss variables and age, psychosocial, social emotional, personality traits, and tinnitus. (C) The age and hearing loss only regression utilized the same controlled regression for comparison. This model was completed twice to account for the different hearing loss codes. The controlled regression for the hearing loss characteristics model also included these covariates and interactions, but exchanged the hearing loss variable itself with the numerous hearing loss factors of interest. In these model formulas Yi is the outcome variable of interest, β0 refers to the mean, and β1-x correspond to the slope of each additional variable. βx *Covariatexi…… implies the addition of all covariates listed in B. to the linear regression. βx *Interactionxi…… implies the addition of all moderation terms with hearing loss factors listed in B. to the linear regression.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0304428.g001

Next a regression controlling for multiple possible covariates, as shown in Fig 1: General Models, were completed, and compared to the unadjusted regressions. In the controlled regression models, interaction variables were included for all hearing loss variables with age, psychosocial outcomes, social emotional outcomes, and personality traits. Furthermore, due to the high co-occurrence of tinnitus and hearing loss interaction variables between the presence of tinnitus and hearing loss variables were also included. Given that the variables of interest for this study were the moderation effects, main effects for psychosocial outcomes, social emotional outcomes, personality traits, and tinnitus are only discussed in relation to significant interactions but are reported in the tables in the results sections. In this larger regression, the employment options of retirement and disabled were combined due to a perfect correlation between these two categories.

To address the question of hearing loss characteristics the same method was utilized with a non-controlled and controlled regression for each outcome, which can be seen in Fig 1: Hearing Loss Characteristics. In these analyses, duration of device usage was not included due to the occurrence of multicollinearity between this variable and device type. Additionally, when examining memory complaints, a subset analysis was conducted in the same manner with participants aged 55 years or older due to the high incidence of memory concerns in this population [64].

Furthermore, controlling contrast coded variables with a Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) greater than five were removed from analysis, which included two contrast codes accounting for education and two contrast codes representing employment status. Histograms, Q-Q plots, Residuals vs Leverage plots, Scale-Location plots, and Residuals vs Fitted plots were first computed to assess potential violations in assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance for all variables. Although variables with high VIF values were excluded, Residuals vs Fitted plots continued to demonstrate a patterned response. Visual inspection and outlier analyses were also performed.

P-values were adjusted to account for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini–Hochberg False Discovery Rate procedure to reduce the risk of type I errors [65]. Adjusted p-values are reported with an alpha of ≤0.05 utilized to signify statistical significance.

Results

Participant characteristics

One thousand and ninety-seven participants accessed the survey. Of these subjects 0.91% decided not to participate in the study, 9% did not correctly complete the verification questions within the survey, 18% had only partial survey responses, which resulted in a total sample of 891 participants utilized in the analysis of this survey. Participants were able to finish the survey over multiple days with one participant completing the survey in four days; however, among those who completed the survey in one day the average response time was 12 minutes. Using a power analysis with power (1 - β) set at 0.80 and α equal to 0.05 (two-tailed) indicated that 219 participants would be sufficient to detect a small effect size (d = 0.2). The mean age of participants in this study was approximately 50 years of age with a standard deviation of 18 years. Females accounted for the majority of respondents (76%), with the remaining sample being approximately 24% male and 0.78% identifying as other. Over half of the respondents reported having a college degree with 42.76% of the participants having a graduate degree, 31.51% of the participants having a bachelor’s degree, and 6.46% having an associate degree. Furthermore, 0.56% of the sample did not complete high school, 4.34% had a high school diploma, 2.12% received a technical degree, and 12.25% completed some college coursework. 53.25% of respondents indicated they were married not separated, while 46.75% of participants were not married. Two hundred and sixty-seven (30%) respondents self-reported having a hearing loss and 123 (14%) individuals indicated that they may have a hearing loss. For those with hearing loss, moderate hearing loss was most often indicated (41%), followed by severe hearing loss (34%), mild hearing loss (23%), and 2% of the 267 respondents with self-reported hearing loss did not know the severity of their hearing loss. Additionally, 22 CI users and 139 hearing aid users completed this survey. One hundred and fourteen individuals in this survey with self-reported hearing loss did not utilize a hearing device.

Loneliness

Hearing loss was not associated with greater loneliness (F(1, 889) = 6.89, CI [0.05, 0.33], p = 0.097) in an uncontrolled model. In terms of age, a younger age was correlated with greater loneliness (F(1,890) = 35.61, CI [-0.03,-0.01], p<0.001) without the inclusion of hearing loss in the model.

When adding covariates and additional moderators (as noted in Fig 1B) to the model, hearing loss significantly interacted with another psychosocial measure. As demonstrated in Table 1, although there were no main effects of anxiety and hearing loss on loneliness, a significant additive interaction was found between hearing loss and anxiety (F(1,829) = 8.63, CI [0.06, 0.30], p = 0.044), to this extent having hearing loss and anxiety increased loneliness levels in comparison to people without hearing loss.

thumbnail
Table 1. Predictors effects on loneliness in a controlled model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0304428.t001

In examining specific hearing loss characteristics in an uncontrolled model, loneliness increased as a function of severity of hearing loss (F(1, 878) = 10.89, CI [0.24, 0.96], p = 0.018), such that as hearing loss severity increased reported loneliness scores increased as well. There was a trend such that, not having a hearing device was associated with greater loneliness scores (F(1, 878) = 8.34, CI [0.10, 0.54], p = 0.053) in comparison to those with hearing aids and CIs. Additionally, there was a significant difference in loneliness between those who reported utilizing a hearing aid and a CI (F(1,878) = 9.92, CI [0.26, 1.11], p = 0.030), such that hearing aid users on average scored 0.68 points higher on loneliness. There was a significant negative interaction between hearing loss severity and using a hearing aid, as well (F(1,878) = 13.89, CI [-0.55, -0.17], p = 0.005). This suggests that as hearing loss severity increased there was less of a difference in reported loneliness between hearing aid users and CI users.

In the hearing loss characteristics model after the addition of the covariates only the negative interaction between hearing loss severity and hearing aid use remained significant (F(1,818) = 12.97, CI [-0.44, -0.13], p = 0.007), while there was a trend for increases in hearing loss severity being associated with increases in loneliness scores (F(1,818) = 8.21, CI [0.13, 0.72], p = 0.053).

Social isolation

Surprisingly there was no significant effect of hearing loss on social isolation (F(1, 889) = 4.41, CI [-0.85, -0.03], p = 0.252) in an uncontrolled model. However, there was a trend towards a significant difference between maybe having hearing loss and not having hearing loss, such that those who selected maybe having hearing loss indicated less social engagement (F(1, 889) = 7.34, CI [0.41, 2.58], p = 0.078). Advancing age was negatively correlated with social engagement when not taking hearing loss or other covariates into account, such that older individuals reported higher levels of social isolation (F(1,890) = 10.28, CI [-0.05, -0.01], p = 0.018).

Table 2 demonstrates that upon adding covariates and additional moderator variables (see Fig 1B), age remained a significant predictor of social isolation (F(1, 829) = 23.21, CI [-0.11, -0.05], p<0.001), such that older age was associated with more social isolation. There was a trend towards an interaction between maybe having hearing loss and stress (F(1, 829) = 7.43, CI [-1.22, -0.20], p = 0.079). As stress levels increased, maybe having hearing loss resulted in greater social isolation than that exhibited by those self-identifying as having hearing loss.

thumbnail
Table 2. Predictors effects on social isolation in a controlled model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0304428.t002

In the controlled hearing loss characteristics model, as extraversion increased, social engagement was predicted to significantly increase (F(1, 818) = 49.80, CI [0.32, 0.57], p<0.001), with a trend towards a significant negative interaction between hearing loss severity and extraversion (F(1, 818) = 7.92, CI [-0.27, -0.05], p = 0.062).This trend results in extraversion no longer resulting in greater social engagement above a mild degree of hearing loss.

Stress

In the overall uncontrolled and controlled hearing loss and hearing loss characteristics models, hearing loss was not a significant predictor of stress nor were there significant interactions with hearing loss on stress (see Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Predictors effects on stress in a controlled model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0304428.t003

In a regression model only including age, age was negatively correlated with stress (F(1,890) = 133.63, CI [-0.08, -0.06], p<0.001), consequently younger respondents reported greater stress on average. In the controlled regression, there was no longer a significant relationship between age and stress (F(1, 829) = 0.741, CI [-0.02, 0.01], p = 0.720).

Anxiety

There was a trend towards a relationship between anxiety and hearing loss (F(1,889) = 8.15, CI [-0.27, -0.05], p = 0.053), such that those without hearing loss reported higher anxiety levels on average in an uncontrolled regression model. Age was negatively correlated with increasing anxiety levels (F(1,890) = 141.13, CI [-0.03, -0.02], p<0.001) in an uncontrolled model, and was trending towards a negative correlation with anxiety in the larger model (F(1,829) = 7.91, CI [-0.02, 0.00], p = 0.062), when covariates and additional moderating variables (see Fig 1B) were added to the model.

As shown in Table 4, when covariates were added to the model, there was a significant additive relationship between loneliness and hearing loss (F(1,829) = 9.61, CI[0.03, 0.13], p = 0.030). It is important to note that this significant interaction was also noted above when examining loneliness as an outcome measure.

thumbnail
Table 4. Predictors effects on anxiety in a controlled model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0304428.t004

The uncontrolled and controlled hearing loss characteristics model overall were not significantly related to anxiety scores.

Depression

Depression was negatively correlated with age (F(1,890) = 60.09, CI [-0.02, -0.01], p<0.001) when not controlling for any covariates. No relationship between hearing loss and depression (F(2,889) = 0.91, p = 0.730) or hearing loss characteristics and depression (F(13,878) = 0.58, p = 0.966) were noted in the omnibus uncontrolled regressions (see Table 5).

thumbnail
Table 5. Predictors effects on depression in a controlled model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0304428.t005

In the controlled regression model, age was no longer a significant predictor of depression (F(1,829) = 0.09, CI [-0.01, 0.01], p = 0.924). In the controlled hearing loss characteristics regression, there was a trend such that as hearing loss severity increased depression scores increased indicating the presence of a linear relationship (F(1,818) = 8.43, CI [-0.49, -0.09], p = 0.053). There was a trend towards an interaction between hearing loss severity and not having a hearing device (F(1,818) = 7.94, CI [0.03, 0.14],p = 0.062), indicating that at higher levels of hearing loss severity individuals without a hearing device had higher depression scores.

Memory complaints

In our overall uncontrolled regression model, age was not related to memory complaints as measured by the SCF questions (F(1,890) = 3.12, p = 0.390). However, hearing loss in an uncontrolled model was associated with greater memory complaints (F(1,889) = 18.05, CI [0.14, 0.38], p<0.001).

Surprisingly in the controlled regression model, as demonstrated in Table 6, age remained unrelated to memory complaints (F(1,829) = 3.01, CI [0.00, 0.02], p = 0.540). There was a trending relationship between hearing loss and maybe having hearing loss interacting with negative emotionality (F(1,829) = 8.38, CI [0.07, 0.37], p = 0.053). At higher levels of negative emotionality, having hearing loss resulted in more memory complaints than those who maybe have a hearing loss.

thumbnail
Table 6. Predictors effects on memory complaints in a controlled model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0304428.t006

In a deeper analysis of hearing loss characteristics in an uncontrolled model, there was a trend that respondents who indicated that the ear with hearing loss was unknown were more likely to report memory difficulties via their responses to the SCF questions (F(1, 878) = 8.50, CI [0.14, 0.73], p = 0.053). In the controlled hearing loss characteristics model, not knowing which ear had hearing loss was associated with more memory difficulties (F(1,818) = 11.40, CI [0.20, 0.74], p = 0.018). There was a trend such that not using a hearing device interacted with depression (F(1,818) = 7.97, CI [-0.23, -0.04], p = 0.062). At higher levels of depression, having a hearing device was associated with increased memory complaints in comparison to not having a hearing device.

The same analyses as described above were also completed in a subset of respondents aged 55 years or older. In the uncontrolled models, hearing loss was still associated with greater memory concerns (F(1, 394) = 21.31, CI[0.22, 0.55], p<0.001). Age continued to not demonstrate a relationship with memory complaints (F(1, 395) = 4.45, CI[-0.04, 0.00], p = 0.250). In the controlled model, there was a trend for greater memory concerns to be reported in those with hearing loss (F(1,335) = 727, CI[0.08, 0.52], p = 0.079).

In terms of hearing loss characteristics, there was a trend that having hearing loss and no device was associated with greater memory complaints (F(1, 383) = 8.31, CI[0.10, 0.55], p = 0.053). Additionally there was a trend, such that having a hearing aid was associated with greater memory complaints than having a CI (F(1, 383) = 7.90, CI[0.18, 1.03], p = 0.062). However, when covariates were added to the models, there were no significant relationships between hearing loss, age, or hearing loss characteristics nor any significant interactions with these variables of interest.

Discussion

The results from this study aid in the understanding of the complex relationship between hearing loss, psychosocial outcomes, and personality traits. This study found that increases in memory complaints were associated with hearing loss. Hearing loss was positively correlated with recent difficulty with memory-based tasks, as measured by the SCF questions, when examining all adults and older adults (>54 years of age) as has been noted in previous studies [54, 55, 6669]. However, the addition of covariates (Fig 1B) and additional predictors (personality traits, psychosocial outcomes, and social emotional outcomes) resulted in hearing loss no longer being associated with increased memory complaints. A recent study by Lin et al. [70] found differences in rates of cognitive decline were affected by hearing aids only in a subpopulation of individuals with hearing loss and cardiac risk factors. Taken together with our findings, it may suggest that the link between hearing loss, hearing treatments, and cognition is only present or stronger in certain individuals [70]. Future research is needed in this area to determine which individual or personality characteristics may place hearing patients at increased risk for cognitive decline. Furthermore, researchers would benefit from carefully considering possible covarying factors when examining hearing loss and cognition [71]. For instance, while this study did not find hearing loss to remain a significant predictor of memory complaints when controlling for many other factors such as demographics, social emotional outcomes and personality traits, our other psychosocial outcomes (stress and loneliness) showed a significant positive relationship between stress and loneliness to memory concerns. This may suggest that caution should be taken when examining cognition to also account for other psychosocial outcomes.

Consistent with Dawes et al. [9], the results of this study did not find significant interactions between social isolation and depression with hearing loss on the effects of cognition. Dawes et al. [9] also examined if the relationship between cognition and hearing was mediated by social isolation and depression and noted it was unlikely that the effect of hearing and hearing treatment on cognition is mediated by these factors. However, personality traits did interact with the effects of hearing loss on memory concerns. Previous research has revealed a link between higher negative emotionality with greater cognitive decline [39, 40]. Our results further demonstrate this, in that, having a more negative personality and hearing loss rather than ‘maybe’ having hearing loss created an additive effect in increasing reports of memory complaints.

Furthermore, this study demonstrated that the presence and severity of hearing loss can have differential effects on known relationships between personality and psychosocial outcomes. Interestingly, greater hearing loss negated the relationship between extraversion and social engagement. These outcomes may be seen as complementary to Stephan et al.’s [42] findings of greater rates of decline in extraversion for individuals with hearing loss. Further, Cox et al. [47] noted that higher neuroticism and lower extraversion were correlated with greater activity limitations and participation restrictions in people obtaining hearing aids. These significant interactions suggest that it may be useful to consider personality traits in those with hearing loss when planning intervention programs. For instance, it may be that our extraverted patients need more support as their social engagement and levels of loneliness could be more affected than introverted patients.

In the controlled regression models, which added numerous variables (see Fig 1B), hearing loss significantly interacted with other outcome factors such as anxiety, loneliness, and stress. The presence of anxiety and hearing loss had an additive effect on increased loneliness, while the presence of loneliness and hearing loss had an additive effect on increased anxiety. This three-way interaction was present regardless of the outcome measure of interest (i.e., loneliness or anxiety) suggesting this is a robust finding. Previous studies have shown increases in anxiety with increases in hearing loss severity [12, 14, 72]. In examining the relationship between stress and social isolation it was found that as social isolation increased, stress increased with those who selected ‘maybe’ having hearing loss showing the greatest amount of stress linked to social isolation. Previous research has found high levels of social loneliness in individuals who self-report hearing loss, and it has been noted that those with less acceptance of their hearing loss and more stress reported greater social loneliness [73]. Given that we separately examined those who indicated maybe having hearing loss, our findings could be reflecting a lack of acceptance of hearing loss interacting with stress. Furthermore, Jayakody et al. [14] found that even with only high frequency hearing loss at 6000 and 8000 Hz there was an increased risk of anxiety and stress. These respondents, who often would not be considered clinically for hearing treatments, may also be the same individuals that would self-identify as maybe having hearing loss. These findings suggest that future research should examine the effects of loneliness and social isolation in the ‘maybe’ hearing loss population separate from the non-hearing loss and hearing loss populations.

Overall, the results from this study replicate results from previous studies on hearing loss and personality relationships [42, 47] as well as hearing loss and wellbeing [9, 12, 14, 39, 40, 72]. However, this study expands on those findings by including both relationships of personality and wellbeing and their interaction with hearing loss. Given that this study found interactions between personality and memory outcomes, future research may wish to explore if targeted interventions could improve memory benefits obtained from hearing loss treatments. Furthermore, research could explore if audiological rehabilitation programs are more effective for certain personality traits. For instance, social support groups may be particularly effective for those with higher levels of extroversion given that this study found greater decreases in social engagement in these individuals with increasing levels of hearing loss. Lastly, findings in this study indicate that that those who think they maybe have a hearing loss have differences in their wellbeing between those with and without hearing loss. This may be a population of interest for future researchers as over-the-counter hearing aids are intended for perceived hearing loss rather than diagnosed hearing loss.

It is important to note that while the above findings are compelling, there were limitations with the study. The results of this study are correlational and therefore causation cannot be determined. This study utilized validated questionnaires; however, many of these questionnaires employ Likert-scales, which provide limited choice possibilities and are prone to acquiescence bias [74]. Furthermore, the short form versions of the anxiety and depression screener may have resulted in floor effects being obtained on these measures. This study was completed with a convenience sample, therefore results on some of the outcomes did not vary greatly between respondents. Those individuals who choose to participate in survey research when convenience samples are utilized may differ from those who choose not to partake in research [75]. Future studies should attempt a random sampling approach to better reflect the general population.

The current study focused on self-reported performance on memory-based tasks as a measure of Subjective Cognitive Function. In a large longitudinal data set of 454 participants, it was found that higher responses to the SCF questions at baseline were related to increased odds of progression to mild cognitive impairment measured using an objective neurocognitive test battery. The authors suggest that the presence of subjective cognitive complaints prior to a measurable mild cognitive impairment diagnosis may represent a transitional phase to mild cognitive impairment [76]. While our methodology focused on memory, a method employed by others to evaluate cognition and cognitive decline [5355, 76], it is important to recognize that cognition also involves attention, sensory processing, perception, and processes that regulate, retain, manipulate, and integrate information [77]. As previous studies have done, the current study focused on memory as our cognitive aspect of interest, rather than other aspects of cognition. Given our interest in potential cognitive changes associated with hearing loss, an emphasis on memory, which is relatively easy to evaluate via self-report, is justified. Given the design of the current study, self-reported measures of perception, attention, sensation, and other cognitive processes (such as the ability to integrate information) would be more difficult for subjects to self-evaluate and report accurately, increasing the risk of self-reporting errors. Future studies that employ direct measures of cognition and hearing should consider including additional measures of other aspects cognition than were used here.

Self-report bias is another caution of this study. While some research has demonstrated that self-reported hearing loss methods are sensitive to objective measures of hearing loss thresholds [4850], self-reported hearing loss has also been shown to result in many people under or over reporting their hearing loss [78]. Specifically, younger respondents tend to overestimate while older respondents underestimate their hearing loss [78]. Responses to the SCF questions are also self-reported evaluations of memory/cognition and are also vulnerable to either over or underestimation.

Finally, this study was conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic, which may have impacted our results. In examining those with hearing loss, Dunn et al. [22] found less social isolation and anxiety in CI users during the pandemic. In surveys examining the general population, high stress was reported by participants [79, 80]. These studies demonstrate that that the COVID-19 pandemic may have differentially impacted the hearing loss and non-hearing loss populations. Furthermore, a recent review suggests that in older individuals, feelings of loneliness increased during the pandemic [81]. Shrira et al. [82] also noted that the link between feelings of loneliness and mental health outcomes during the COVID-19 pandemic was stronger in those who perceived themselves as being older. Additionally, in a meta-analysis examining the link between prolonged feelings of loneliness and social isolation, it was shown that older adults experiencing these were at greater risk for developing dementia [83]. Since this study included older adults (mean age = 50 years) and examined psychosocial outcomes that could affect each other as well as have been affected by the COVID-19 pandemic, the results reported from this study could have been inflated by the COVID-19 pandemic. Research on this topic should continue and be extended beyond the COVID-19 pandemic to fully understand the impact of COVID-19 on these results.

Conclusion

To summarize the main findings of our study, hearing loss is related in a complex fashion to many psychosocial outcomes and social emotional factors. In addition to the known relationships between hearing loss, loneliness, and cognition, our study demonstrates that other social emotional factors such as anxiety, and stress can strengthen these relationships as a function of hearing loss. These interactions were different for those who thought they may have hearing loss in comparison to those with hearing loss or without hearing loss. Furthermore, our study suggests that hearing loss interacts with personality in complex ways. For example, individuals with greater negative emotionality and hearing loss tended to report more memory complaints. The association between hearing loss and memory complaints was also found to change as a function of controlling variables, such as controlling for social emotional and psychosocial outcomes. In reviewing the associations between hearing loss, cognitive impairment, depression, anxiety, and quality of life, Blazer & Tucci [84] noted that mental health professionals required a greater knowledge of hearing loss to allow for enhanced psychotherapies in this population. Our results provide useful information for mental health professionals and hearing health care professionals who treat patients with hearing loss regarding psychosocial and social emotional influences of hearing loss, and how personality may shape these relationships. A deeper insight into these relationships may produce better treatment outcomes and allow for customizable aural rehabilitation programs.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix. Study questionnaire.

All questionnaires (Three-Item Loneliness, PSS-4, SCF, LSNS-6, PHQ-4, BFI-2-XS) utilized in this study are included. In addition, all demographic information collected from respondents are presented.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0304428.s001

(DOCX)

References

  1. 1. Deafness and hearing loss [Internet]. World Health Organization. 2021 [cited 2023 Feb 23]. Available from: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/deafness-and-hearing-loss
  2. 2. Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, Costafreda SG, Huntley J, Ames D, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet Lond Engl. 2017 Dec 16;390(10113):2673–734. pmid:28735855
  3. 3. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet Lond Engl. 2020 Aug 8;396(10248):413–46. pmid:32738937
  4. 4. Pichora-Fuller MK, Kramer SE, Eckert MA, Edwards B, Hornsby BWY, Humes LE, et al. Hearing impairment and cognitive energy: The Framework for Understanding Effortful Listening (FUEL). Ear Hear. 2016;37 Suppl 1:5S–27S. pmid:27355771
  5. 5. Peelle JE. Listening effort: How the cognitive consequences of acoustic challenge are reflected in brain and behavior. Ear Hear. 2018;39(2):204–14. pmid:28938250
  6. 6. Wingfield A, Amichetti NM, Lash A. Cognitive aging and hearing acuity: Modeling spoken language comprehension. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2015 Jun 11 [cited 2023 Sep 21];6. Available from: http://journal.frontiersin.org/Article/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00684/abstract pmid:26124724
  7. 7. Powell DS, Oh ES, Lin FR, Deal JA. Hearing impairment and cognition in an aging world. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol JARO. 2021 Jul;22(4):387–403. pmid:34008037
  8. 8. Uchida Y, Sugiura S, Nishita Y, Saji N, Sone M, Ueda H. Age-related hearing loss and cognitive decline—The potential mechanisms linking the two. Auris Nasus Larynx. 2019 Feb;46(1):1–9. pmid:30177417
  9. 9. Dawes P, Emsley R, Cruickshanks KJ, Moore DR, Fortnum H, Edmondson-Jones M, et al. Hearing loss and cognition: the role of hearing aids, social isolation and depression. PloS One. 2015;10(3):e0119616. pmid:25760329
  10. 10. Lin FR, Yaffe K, Xia J, Xue QL, Harris TB, Purchase-Helzner E, et al. Hearing loss and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA Intern Med. 2013 Feb 25;173(4):293–9. pmid:23337978
  11. 11. Tian R, Almeida OP, Jayakody DMP, Ford AH. Association between hearing loss and frailty: A systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Geriatr. 2021 Dec;21(1):333. pmid:34034656
  12. 12. Jayakody DMP, Wishart J, Stegeman I, Eikelboom R, Moyle TC, Yiannos JM, et al. Is there an association between untreated hearing loss and psychosocial outcomes? Front Aging Neurosci. 2022 May 19;14:868673. pmid:35663574
  13. 13. Gispen FE, Chen DS, Genther DJ, Lin FR. Association between hearing impairment and lower levels of physical activity in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2014 Aug;62(8):1427–33.
  14. 14. Jayakody DMP, Almeida OP, Speelman CP, Bennett RJ, Moyle TC, Yiannos JM, et al. Association between speech and high-frequency hearing loss and depression, anxiety and stress in older adults. Maturitas. 2018 Apr;110:86–91. pmid:29563040
  15. 15. Shukla A, Harper M, Pedersen E, Goman A, Suen JJ, Price C, et al. Hearing loss, loneliness, and social isolation: A systematic review. Otolaryngol—Head Neck Surg. 2020 May;162(5):622–33. pmid:32151193
  16. 16. Bott A, Saunders G. A scoping review of studies investigating hearing loss, social isolation and/or loneliness in adults. Int J Audiol. 2021;60(sup2):30–46. pmid:34030565
  17. 17. Huang CQ, Dong BR, Lu ZC, Yue JR, Liu QX. Chronic diseases and risk for depression in old age: A meta-analysis of published literature. Ageing Res Rev. 2010 Apr;9(2):131–41. pmid:19524072
  18. 18. Lawrence BJ, Jayakody DMP, Bennett RJ, Eikelboom RH, Gasson N, Friedland PL. Hearing loss and depression in older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Gerontologist. 2020 Apr 2;60(3):e137–54. pmid:30835787
  19. 19. Jayakody DMP, Friedland PL, Eikelboom RH, Martins RN, Sohrabi HR. A novel study on association between untreated hearing loss and cognitive functions of older adults: Baseline non‐verbal cognitive assessment results. Clin Otolaryngol. 2018 Feb;43(1):182–91. pmid:28710824
  20. 20. Jayakody DMP, Friedland PL, Nel E, Martins RN, Atlas MD, Sohrabi HR. Impact of cochlear implantation on cognitive functions of older adults: Pilot test results. Otol Neurotol. 2017 Sep;38(8):e289–95. pmid:28806341
  21. 21. Kiely KM, Anstey KJ, Luszcz MA. Dual sensory loss and depressive symptoms: The importance of hearing, daily functioning, and activity engagement. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:837. pmid:24379769
  22. 22. Dunn CC, Stangl E, Oleson J, Smith M, Chipara O, Wu YH. The influence of forced social isolation on the auditory ecology and psychosocial functions of listeners with cochlear implants during COVID-19 mitigation efforts. Ear Hear. 2021;42(1):20–8. pmid:33369590
  23. 23. Timmer BHB, Bennett RJ, Montano J, Hickson L, Weinstein B, Wild J, et al. Social-emotional well-being and adult hearing loss: Clinical recommendations. Int J Audiol. 2023 Mar 24;1–12. pmid:36960799
  24. 24. Bigelow RT, Reed NS, Brewster KK, Huang A, Rebok G, Rutherford BR, et al. Association of hearing loss with psychological distress and utilization of mental health services among adults in the United States. JAMA Netw Open. 2020 Jul 1;3(7):e2010986. pmid:32687587
  25. 25. Wells TS, Nickels LD, Rush SR, Musich SA, Wu L, Bhattarai GR, et al. Characteristics and health outcomes associated with hearing loss and hearing aid use among older adults. J Aging Health. 2020;32(7–8):724–34. pmid:31092107
  26. 26. Choi JS, Betz J, Li L, Blake CR, Sung YK, Contrera KJ, et al. Association of using hearing aids or cochlear implants with changes in depressive symptoms in older adults. JAMA Otolaryngol—Head Neck Surg. 2016 Jul 1;142(7):652–7. pmid:27258813
  27. 27. Contrera KJ, Sung YK, Betz J, Li L, Lin FR. Change in loneliness after intervention with cochlear implants or hearing aids. The Laryngoscope. 2017 Aug;127(8):1885–9. pmid:28059448
  28. 28. Glick HA, Sharma A. Cortical neuroplasticity and cognitive function in early-stage, mild-moderate hearing loss: evidence of neurocognitive benefit from hearing aid use. Front Neurosci. 2020;14:93. pmid:32132893
  29. 29. Cuoco S, Cappiello A, Scarpa A, Troisi D, Autuori M, Ponticorvo S, et al. Neuropsychological profile of hearing-impaired patients and the effect of hearing aid on cognitive functions: An exploratory study. Sci Rep. 2021 Apr 30;11(1):9384. pmid:33931670
  30. 30. Sarant J, Harris D, Busby P, Maruff P, Schembri A, Lemke U, et al. The effect of hearing aid use on cognition in older adults: Can we delay decline or even improve cognitive function? J Clin Med. 2020 Jan 17;9(1):254. pmid:31963547
  31. 31. Mosnier I, Bebear JP, Marx M, Fraysse B, Truy E, Lina-Granade G, et al. Improvement of cognitive function after cochlear implantation in elderly patients. JAMA Otolaryngol—Head Neck Surg. 2015 May 1;141(5):442–50. pmid:25763680
  32. 32. Mosnier I, Vanier A, Bonnard D, Lina-Granade G, Truy E, Bordure P, et al. Long-term cognitive prognosis of profoundly deaf older adults after hearing rehabilitation using cochlear implants. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2018 Aug;66(8):1553–61. pmid:30091185
  33. 33. Kramer S, Vasil KJ, Adunka OF, Pisoni DB, Moberly AC. Cognitive functions in adult cochlear implant users, cochlear implant candidates, and normal-hearing listeners. Laryngoscope Investig Otolaryngol. 2018 Aug;3(4):304–10. pmid:30186962
  34. 34. Megha null, Maruthy S. Consequences of hearing aid acclimatization on ALLRs and its relationship with perceived benefit and speech perception abilities. Eur Arch Oto-Rhino-Laryngol. 2019 Apr;276(4):1001–10. pmid:30694378
  35. 35. van Hooren S a. H, Anteunis LJC, Valentijn S a. M, Bosma H, Ponds RWHM, Jolles J, et al. Does cognitive function in older adults with hearing impairment improve by hearing aid use? Int J Audiol. 2005 May;44(5):265–71. pmid:16028789
  36. 36. Nikčević AV, Marino C, Kolubinski DC, Leach D, Spada MM. Modelling the contribution of the Big Five personality traits, health anxiety, and COVID-19 psychological distress to generalised anxiety and depressive symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic. J Affect Disord. 2021 Jan 15;279:578–84. pmid:33152562
  37. 37. Rammstedt B, John OP. Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. J Res Personal. 2007 Feb;41(1):203–12.
  38. 38. John OP, Donahue EM, Kentle RL. Big Five Inventory [Internet]. American Psychological Association; 1991 [cited 2023 Feb 23]. Available from: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/t07550-000
  39. 39. Sutin AR, Stephan Y, Luchetti M, Terracciano A. Five-factor model personality traits and cognitive function in five domains in older adulthood. BMC Geriatr. 2019 Dec 5;19(1):343. pmid:31805866
  40. 40. Allen MS, Laborde S, Walter EE. Health-related behavior mediates the association between personality and memory performance in older adults. J Appl Gerontol. 2019 Feb;38(2):232–52. pmid:28380727
  41. 41. Kendler KS, Myers J. The genetic and environmental relationship between major depression and the five-factor model of personality. Psychol Med. 2010 May;40(5):801–6. pmid:19732485
  42. 42. Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Bosselut G, Terracciano A. Sensory functioning and personality development among older adults. Psychol Aging. 2017 Mar;32(2):139–47. pmid:28287784
  43. 43. Berg AI, Johansson B. Personality change in the oldest-old: Is it a matter of compromised health and functioning? J Pers. 2014 Feb;82(1):25–31. pmid:23336670
  44. 44. Cox RM, Alexander GC, Gray GA. Who wants a hearing aid? Personality profiles of hearing aid seekers. Ear Hear. 2005 Feb;26(1):12–26. pmid:15692301
  45. 45. Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Caille P, Terracciano A. Personality and hearing acuity: Evidence from the Health and Retirement Study and the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Psychosom Med. 2019;81(9):808–13. pmid:31335490
  46. 46. Wu YH, Dumanch K, Stangl E, Miller C, Tremblay K, Bentler R. Is the Device-Oriented Subjective Outcome (DOSO) independent of personality? J Am Acad Audiol. 2017;28(10):932–40. pmid:29130441
  47. 47. Cox RM, Alexander GC, Gray GA. Personality, hearing problems, and amplification characteristics: contributions to self-report hearing aid outcomes. Ear Hear. 2007 Apr;28(2):141–62. pmid:17496667
  48. 48. Choi JE, Moon IJ, Baek SY, Kim SW, Cho YS. Discrepancies between self-reported hearing difficulty and hearing loss diagnosed by audiometry: Prevalence and associated factors in a national survey. BMJ Open. 2019 May 1;9(4):e022440. pmid:31048419
  49. 49. Brennan-Jones CG, Taljaard DS, Brennan-Jones SEF, Bennett RJ, Swanepoel DW, Eikelboom RH. Self-reported hearing loss and manual audiometry: A rural versus urban comparison. Aust J Rural Health. 2016 Apr;24(2):130–5. pmid:26311193
  50. 50. Zadeh LM, Brennan V, Swanepoel DW, Lin L, Moore DR. Remote self-report and speech-in-noise measures predict clinical audiometric thresholds. MedRxiv Prepr Serv Health Sci. 2023 Jul 17;2022.05.09.22274843. pmid:35821983
  51. 51. Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: Results from two population-based studies. Res Aging. 2004;26(6):655–72. pmid:18504506
  52. 52. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983 Dec;24(4):385–96. pmid:6668417
  53. 53. Fondell E, Townsend MK, Unger LD, Okereke OI, Grodstein F, Ascherio A, et al. Physical activity across adulthood and subjective cognitive function in older men. Eur J Epidemiol. 2018 Jan;33(1):79–87. pmid:29147949
  54. 54. Curhan SG, Willett WC, Grodstein F, Curhan GC. Longitudinal study of hearing loss and subjective cognitive function decline in men. Alzheimers Dement J Alzheimers Assoc. 2019 Apr;15(4):525–33. pmid:30709794
  55. 55. Curhan SG, Willett WC, Grodstein F, Curhan GC. Longitudinal study of self-reported hearing loss and subjective cognitive function decline in women. Alzheimers Dement J Alzheimers Assoc. 2020 Apr;16(4):610–20. pmid:31628050
  56. 56. Lubben JE. Gender differences in the relationship of widowhood and psychological well-being among low income elderly. Women Health. 1988;14(3–4):161–89. pmid:3266815
  57. 57. Lubben J, Blozik E, Gillmann G, Iliffe S, von Renteln Kruse W, Beck JC, et al. Performance of an abbreviated version of the Lubben Social Network Scale among three European community-dwelling older adult populations. The Gerontologist. 2006 Aug;46(4):503–13. pmid:16921004
  58. 58. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Löwe B. An ultra-brief screening scale for anxiety and depression: The PHQ-4. Psychosomatics. 2009;50(6):613–21. pmid:19996233
  59. 59. Löwe B, Wahl I, Rose M, Spitzer C, Glaesmer H, Wingenfeld K, et al. A 4-item measure of depression and anxiety: Validation and standardization of the Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4) in the general population. J Affect Disord. 2010 Apr;122(1–2):86–95. pmid:19616305
  60. 60. Soto CJ, John OP. The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2017 Jul;113(1):117–43. pmid:27055049
  61. 61. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Stability and change in personality assessment: The revised NEO Personality Inventory in the year 2000. J Pers Assess. 1997 Feb;68(1):86–94. pmid:9018844
  62. 62. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing [Internet]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2022. Available from: https://www.R-project.org/
  63. 63. Macià D, Cattaneo G, Solana J, Tormos JM, Pascual-Leone A, Bartrés-Faz D. Meaning in life: A major predictive factor for loneliness comparable to health status and social connectedness. Front Psychol. 2021;12:627547. pmid:33716892
  64. 64. Açikgöz M, Özen Baru T B, Emre U, Taşçilar N, Atalay A, Köktürk F. Assessment of Relation between subjectıve memory complaınts and objective cognitive performance of elderly over 55 years old age. Noro Psikiyatri Arsivi. 2014 Mar;51(1):57–62.
  65. 65. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B Methodol. 1995 Jan;57(1):289–300.
  66. 66. Lin FR, Ferrucci L, Metter EJ, An Y, Zonderman AB, Resnick SM. Hearing loss and cognition in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neuropsychology. 2011 Nov;25(6):763–70. pmid:21728425
  67. 67. Lin FR, Metter EJ, O’Brien RJ, Resnick SM, Zonderman AB, Ferrucci L. Hearing loss and incident dementia. Arch Neurol. 2011 Feb;68(2):214–20. pmid:21320988
  68. 68. Lin FR, Niparko JK, Ferrucci L. Hearing loss prevalence in the United States. Arch Intern Med. 2011 Nov 14;171(20):1851–2. pmid:22083573
  69. 69. Lin FR, Thorpe R, Gordon-Salant S, Ferrucci L. Hearing loss prevalence and risk factors among older adults in the United States. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2011 May;66(5):582–90. pmid:21357188
  70. 70. Lin FR, Pike JR, Albert MS, Arnold M, Burgard S, Chisolm T, et al. Hearing intervention versus health education control to reduce cognitive decline in older adults with hearing loss in the USA (ACHIEVE): A multicentre, randomised controlled trial. The Lancet. 2023 Sep;402(10404):786–97. pmid:37478886
  71. 71. Dawes P, Munro KJ. Hearing loss and dementia: Where to from here? Ear Hear. 2024 May;45(3):529–36. pmid:38379156
  72. 72. Contrera KJ, Betz J, Deal J, Choi JS, Ayonayon HN, Harris T, et al. Association of hearing impairment and anxiety in older adults. J Aging Health. 2017 Feb;29(1):172–84. pmid:26916793
  73. 73. Warringa LTL, Henke CE, Pronk M, Kramer SE, Stam M. Relationships between coping behaviors and social loneliness in adults with self-reported hearing problems. Ear Hear. 2020;41(4):1040–50. pmid:31977728
  74. 74. Yonnie Chyung SY, Kennedy M, Campbell I. Evidence-based survey design: The use of ascending or descending order of likert-type response options. Perform Improv. 2018 Oct;57(9):9–16.
  75. 75. Andrade C. The inconvenient truth about convenience and purposive samples. Indian J Psychol Med. 2021 Jan;43(1):86–8. pmid:34349313
  76. 76. Donovan NJ, Amariglio RE, Zoller AS, Rudel RK, Gomez-Isla T, Blacker D, et al. Subjective cognitive concerns and neuropsychiatric predictors of progression to the early clinical stages of Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2014 Dec;22(12):1642–51. pmid:24698445
  77. 77. Styles E. Attention, Perception and Memory [Internet]. 0 ed. Psychology Press; 2004 [cited 2023 Feb 23]. Available from: https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/9781135481346
  78. 78. Kamil RJ, Genther DJ, Lin FR. Factors associated with the accuracy of subjective assessments of hearing impairment. Ear Hear. 2015 Jan;36(1):164–7. pmid:25158982
  79. 79. Gloster AT, Lamnisos D, Lubenko J, Presti G, Squatrito V, Constantinou M, et al. Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on mental health: An international study. PloS One. 2020;15(12):e0244809. pmid:33382859
  80. 80. Wang X, Hegde S, Son C, Keller B, Smith A, Sasangohar F. Investigating mental health of us college students during the covid-19 pandemic: Cross-sectional survey study. J Med Internet Res. 2020 Sep 17;22(9):e22817. pmid:32897868
  81. 81. Mushtaq A, Khan MA. Social isolation, loneliness, and mental health among older adults during COVID-19: A scoping review. J Gerontol Soc Work. 2024;67(2):143–56. pmid:37501381
  82. 82. Shrira A, Hoffman Y, Bodner E, Palgi Y. COVID-19-related loneliness and psychiatric symptoms among older adults: The buffering role of subjective age. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2020 Nov;28(11):1200–4. pmid:32561276
  83. 83. Lazzari C, Rabottini M. COVID-19, loneliness, social isolation and risk of dementia in older people: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the relevant literature. Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract. 2022 Jun;26(2):196–207. pmid:34369248
  84. 84. Blazer DG, Tucci DL. Hearing loss and psychiatric disorders: A review. Psychol Med. 2019 Apr;49(6):891–7. pmid:30457063