Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Mindfully missing myself: Induced mindfulness causes alienation among poor self-regulators

Abstract

Mindfulness is a popular technique that helps people to get closer to their self. However, recent findings indicate that mindfulness may not benefit everybody. In the present research, we hypothesized that mindfulness promotes alienation from the self among individuals with low abilities to self-regulate affect (state-oriented individuals) but not among individuals with high abilities to self-regulate affect (action-oriented individuals). In two studies with participants who were mostly naïve to mindfulness practices (70% indicated no experience; N1 = 126, 42 men, 84 women, 0 diverse, aged 17–86 years, Mage = 31.87; N2 = 108, 30 men, 75 women, 3 diverse, aged 17–69 years, Mage = 28.00), we tested a mindfulness group (five-minute mindfulness exercise) against a control group (five-minute text reading). We operationalized alienation as lower consistency in repeated preference judgments and a lower tendency to adopt intrinsic over extrinsic goal recommendations. Results showed that, among state-oriented participants, mindfulness led to significantly lower consistency of preference judgments (Study 1) and lower adoption of intrinsic over extrinsic goals (Study 2) compared to text reading. The alienating effect was absent among action-oriented participants. Thus, mindfulness practice may alienate psychologically vulnerable people from their self and hamper access to preferences and intrinsic goals. We discuss our findings within Personality-Systems-Interactions (PSI) theory.

“If you know yourself, then you’ll not be harmed by what is said about you”

[1]

1. Introduction

Aristotle’s quote “knowing yourself is the beginning of all wisdom” highlights that ideas related to knowing oneself are quite pertinent to well-being. However, people’s conscious self-knowledge usually does not cover the full spectrum of experience and has to be distinguished from the ‘true’ self [2]. The self can be defined as a complex knowledge structure comprising personal needs, emotions, preferences, goals, and values which operates largely at an implicit level [3,4]. The self is a vital source of psychological functioning [5]. However, access to the self gets blocked, for example, if people are overwhelmed by external pressure, stress, and negative affect [6]. This makes the ability to self-regulate negative affect an important resource against alienation from the self. However, not everybody has equal resources.

The disposition of action versus state orientation explains individual differences in the ability to self-regulate affect [3,7,8]. A plethora of findings show that, when confronted with stressful conditions, action-oriented people are able to down-regulate negative affect and maintain volitional control over their thoughts and actions, whereas state-oriented people are unable to down-regulate negative affect and get stuck in uncontrollable rumination [911]. This unattenuated negative affect makes state-oriented people more vulnerable towards losing touch with their self [6,12]. For example, under stressful conditions, state-oriented people have been found to show lower consistency in preference judgments and confuse external assignments as self-selected goals (for an overview, see 9). Since the self represents personal needs, emotions, preferences, and goals [3,4], lower consistency in preference judgments and confusing external assignments as self-selected goals indicates that state-oriented individuals lost access to their self. Therefore, an important question is how self-access can be restored among state-oriented people.

Mindfulness seems like a promising remedy as mindfulness has shown positive effects especially in terms of better emotion regulation [13,14] and stress regulation [15,16]. However, the idea that mindfulness always acts as a remedy is a “false myth”. Recent findings show that mindfulness can even be harmful and promote alienation among state-oriented people [17]. In the upcoming paragraphs, we elaborate on state-oriented people’s vulnerability towards alienation, review findings on mindfulness, and explain our hypothesis that mindfulness might not help but even hurt state-oriented people.

1.1. State-oriented people are vulnerable towards alienation

The theoretical foundation of action-state orientation is elaborated in the theory of Personality-Systems-Interactions (PSI; 603,4,11). PSI theory defines central cognitive systems and elaborates in detail how the regulation of affect influences their flexible interplay that is needed for adaptive personality functioning. Furthermore, PSI theory elaborates how action- and state-oriented individuals differ in their dispositional ability to self-regulate affect. Action versus state orientation is an intuitive, self-confrontational, and efficient type of coping with negative affect that is distinct from reappraisal [12], self-esteem [18,19], and self-efficacy [20,21].

Many findings support the assumption that state- compared to action-oriented people are indeed less able to self-regulate affect [22, 2018; 8,10,12]. For example, state-oriented students were unable to down-regulate negative affect when exams came closer, whereas action-oriented students successfully did [23]. This self-regulatory deficit has been shown to influence a broad range of life domains like psychological well-being [24], sleep quality [25], relationship satisfaction [26], and educational achievement [27].

Importantly, the range of adverse effects extends to indicators of self-access. Previous findings show that, under negative affect, state-oriented people became inconsistent in judging their preferences (e.g., for abstract art and soft drinks; 28), confused assigned and self-selected goals [6,29], and chose goals that did not match their implicit motives [23,24]. Action-oriented individuals, in contrast, did not show these signs of alienation and maintained self-access [12,30,31]. It is important to note that state-oriented individuals’ judgment inconsistencies, goal confusions, and motive-goal incongruences are markers of a broader tendency towards alienation from the self. Although they may seem like trivial outcomes in themselves, they do have profound consequences for psychological well-being and health such as higher psychosomatic symptoms [24], unhealthy eating behavior [32], and lower identity status [33].

Therefore, state-oriented people’s vulnerability towards alienation makes it imperative to find tools that can help to restore self-access. One promising resort is mindfulness. A huge body of research shows that practicing mindfulness has many beneficial effects and positively influences one’s self-view and self-regulation [34,35]. However, other findings indicate that mindfulness does not help everybody and may even hurt some people [3639].

1.2. Does mindfulness help or hurt?

Mindfulness is a specific quality of awareness where people pay attention to all body and mind experiences in an open, non-judgmental, and accepting manner [4042]. Mindfulness is associated with diverse benefits for psychological functioning. For example, practicing mindfulness has been found to improve emotion regulation [43], attention [44], emotional and psychological well-being [34,45], stress, depressive symptoms, anxiety, and rumination [16,46]. Furthermore, mindfulness practices induce a brain state that improves the resolution of conflict [47], helps coping with pain [48], improves relationship quality [49], and enhances social quality of life [50]. These findings suggest that mindfulness practices may also benefit state-oriented people.

In contrast, several studies also point to a negative side of mindfulness. For example, mindfulness training and high levels of self-focused attention have been associated with psychopathology [51], negative affect [52], and increased psychophysiological stress responses [35]. Other findings show that mindfulness also has negative effects on the self as indicated by cognitive anomalies [53], dissociation [54], and loss of emotions [37; 2019 38,55]. One could argue that such negative effects are a matter of practice time and only long-term practices of mindfulness yield enduring positive effects [34,56]. However, Parsons and colleagues [57] found a significant relationship between the amount of long-term practice and negative outcomes such as poorer sleep and cortical arousal. Sahdra and colleagues [58] argue to look at mindfulness not in absolute terms of “more is better, less is worse.rather, it is how the different mindfulness skills combine in a person that may be most important for his/her mental health.” Therefore, the overall impact of mindfulness may depend on personality dispositions and bad effects may be more pronounced for some people but not for others [59].

1.3. Mindfulness hurts state-oriented people

Recent findings by Kaufmann and colleagues [17] show that induced mindfulness alienated state-oriented people from the self. More specifically, among state-oriented people, a 90-minute mindfulness training promoted alienation as indicated by a higher tendency to adopt (i.e., falsely self-ascribe) extrinsic goals that were recommended by an expert. Ironically, mindfulness exacerbated the very effect that it was supposed to ameliorate. This supports the idea that “there is no such thing as an unmitigated good” [60].

Our current studies conceptually replicate and extend the research by Kaufmann and colleagues [17] by testing the effects of a 5-minute mindfulness induction, varying stress and no-stress conditions, and applying two different measures of alienation that tap into participants’ preferences and goals [9]. As the self is defined as an extended network of implicit representations of own needs, preferences, and goals, we cover two important aspects of the self: preferences (Study 1) and goals (Study 2).

2. Study 1

In Study 1, we tested whether a 5-minute mindfulness exercise leads to alienation from the self among state-oriented participants. As an inverse measure of alienation, we assessed the consistency of preference judgments. If people can reliably access own preferences, they should show some kind of consistency across repeated judgments. However, if people’s access to own preferences is reduced, their judgments should become unreliable and inconsistent [10,61]. According to PSI theory, access to the self is not permanently on or off but changes with different conditions [3,4]. Under adverse compared to neutral conditions, for example, state-oriented participants become more inconsistent when rating art paintings, soft drinks, taste of jam, wallpaper patterns, and Chinese symbols [9,28,62].

In the present study, we used Chinese symbols. We expected state-oriented participants to show lower consistency in repeated judgments of Chinese symbols after the mindfulness exercise compared to the control condition. We did not expect mindfulness to alienate action-oriented participants from their preferences.

2.1. Participants

The required number of participants was calculated prior to data collection using G*Power [63]. Kaufmann et al. [17] found small to intermediate interaction effects of ΔR2 = .07 and .05 in Studies 1 and 2 (f = .27 and .23), respectively. To detect a small to intermediate within-between interaction effect of f = .20 with a power of .90 and α = .05 (ANOVA, 4 groups, 2 measurements), G*Power suggested a minimum of 96 participants. Our final sample consisted of 126 participants (42 men, 84 women) aged 17–86 years (M = 31.87, SD = 14.84). We recruited participants (42 undergraduates, 48 others) online at a German University.

2.2. Methods

The ethics committee of the University waived the need for consent. Participants took part in an online experiment. Before starting the experiment, participants gave their consent through a standardized consent form. By clicking the consent field and the ‘continue’ button, participants agreed to participate in the experiment. Additionally, they were informed that they could cancel and revoke their consent and leave the experiment any time as they pleased. As illustrated in Fig 1, participants first completed questionnaires measuring action-state orientation, implicit affect, explicit affect, and state mindfulness (Pre). Next, they rated the attractiveness of 12 Chinese symbols based on their gut feeling on a scale from -9 to +9 (T0). We presented the same 12 Chinese symbols in a new order and asked participants to rate the attractiveness again (T1). Then, we randomly assigned participants to experimental conditions. In the mindfulness group (n = 62), participants listened to a 5-minute audio recorded by a local certified mindfulness trainer. The exercise contained steps to draw attention to body sensations of breathing and stay in the present. In the control group (n = 64), participants read a paragraph about attention from a textbook by Zimbardo and Gerrig [64] and answered some questions afterwards. Finally, all participants rated their affect and state mindfulness (Post), the Chinese symbols (T2), and some post-experimental questions (e.g., prior experience with mindfulness exercises and Chinese symbols). We thanked and debriefed participants and compensated undergraduate students with course credit. The experiment lasted roughly 30 minutes.

2.3. Materials

2.3.1. Action-state orientation.

We measured action versus state orientation with the 12 failure-related items of the Action Control Scale (ACS; 8). An example item is: „If I’ve been working on something for four weeks and then everything goes wrong, then (A) it takes a long time to come to terms with it or (B) I don’t think about it much longer”. Option (A) reflects the state-oriented and option (B) the action-oriented response alternative. We counted the number of action-oriented response alternatives (range 0–12). Cronbach’s α = .67 was sufficient for research purposes [65]. Applying the norms [8], participants with values from 0 to 4 were assigned to the state-oriented group (n = 56) and participants with values from 5 to 12 to the action-oriented group (n = 70). In the absence of multicollinearity (e.g., in experimental designs), a median split is completely legitimate and does not create misleading results [66].

2.3.2. Affect.

For the present research question, affect was not relevant (for measures and results, see supplementary material).

2.3.3. State mindfulness.

Consistent with Kaufmann and colleagues [17], we adapted five items from the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS-K; [34,67]) to measure state rather than trait mindfulness (e.g., “Right now I find it difficult to stay focused on what is happening”, “Right now I find myself doing things without paying attention to them”). Participants rated items on a Likert-scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very strong). We reversed items so that higher scores indicate higher state mindfulness (Cronbach’s α = .80/.87 for pre/post measures).

2.3.4. Mindfulness intervention.

For the mindfulness intervention, we requested a local certified MBSR trainer to record a 5-minute audio for our experiment. The audio began and ended with a gong. Throughout the exercise, participants were instructed to pay close attention to their breath and to perceive it without changing it and just being in sync with the present moment.

2.3.5. Consistency.

We calculated consistency as within-person correlations across repeated preference judgments. For consistency at T1, we correlated participants’ ratings at T0 and T1. For T2, we correlated T0 and T2. Higher consistency indicates reliable access to the self, whereas lower consistency indicates alienation from the self [9,10].

2.4. Results

2.4.1. Prior experience.

Only 30% of the participants indicated experience with mindfulness exercises (mindfulness condition: 29% vs. control condition: 31%). One participant was a native Chinese speaker, three had some experience with Chinese. The pattern of results was the same when excluding these four participants from the analyses.

2.4.2 Manipulation check.

To test the effectiveness of the mindfulness induction, we analyzed state mindfulness using a 2 (Time: pre, post) x 2 (state vs. action orientation) x 2 (mindfulness vs. control condition) ANOVA with repeated measures on the first factor. The significant main effect of Time, F(1, 122) = 4.46, p < .04, = .035, was qualified by a significant Time x Condition interaction, F(1, 122) = 6.19, p < .02, = .048. In the mindfulness condition, there was a significant increase in state mindfulness after the exercise: Mpre = 4.71 (1.34) vs. Mpost = 5.01 (1.38), t(62) = -2.77, p < .01. In the control condition, state mindfulness did not change over time: Mpre = 4.65 (1.40) vs. Mpost = 4.64 (1.41); t(64) = 0.09, p = .93. There were no differential effects, indicating that the mindfulness induction was equally effective for both state- and action-oriented participants.

2.4.3. Consistency.

To test our hypothesis, we analyzed consistency scores using a 2 (Time: pre, post) x 2 (state vs. action orientation) x 2 (mindfulness vs. control condition) ANOVA with repeated measures on the first factor. There was a significant main effect of Time, F(1, 122) = 6.99, p < .01, = .054, indicating that consistency declined over time: MT1 = .66 (.27) vs. MT2 = .62 (.28). Consistent with expectations, there was a significant Time x Orientation x Condition interaction, F(1, 122) = 5.01, p = .027, = .039. Note that the three-way interaction remained significant when excluding participants experienced in Chinese, F(1, 118) = 5.29, p = .023, = .043, and when converting correlation coefficients r into z scores using Fisher transformation to better ensure normality of the dependent variable, F(1, 122) = 5.79, p = .018, = .045. As listed in Table 1, in the mindfulness condition, state-oriented participants showed a significant decrease in consistency, t(23) = 3.10, p < .01, whereas action-oriented participants did not, t(37) = 0.18, p = .86. In the control condition, in contrast, state-oriented participants did not change in consistency, t(31) = 0.19, p = .85, whereas action-oriented participants showed a significant decrease, t(31) = 2.83, p < .01.

thumbnail
Table 1. Means (and Standard Deviations) of Consistency (Within-Person Correlations) in Preference Judgments in Study 1 (N = 126).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0303505.t001

To illustrate the effect (see Fig 2), we calculated change scores (rpostrpre). State-oriented participants in the mindfulness condition showed a marginally stronger decrease in consistency compared to state-oriented participants in the control condition, t(54) = 1.76, p = .084, and a significantly stronger decrease compared to action-oriented participants in the mindfulness condition, t(60) = -2.08, p = .042.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Pre-post changes in the consistency of preference judgments (within-person correlations: rPostrPre) in Study 1 (N = 126).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0303505.g002

2.5. Discussion

In line with our hypothesis, state-oriented participants showed lower consistency across preference judgments in the mindfulness compared to the neutral condition and in comparison, with their action-oriented counterparts. The finding indicates that state-oriented people became alienated from their preferences after the mindfulness induction. One could question whether the alienating effect for state-oriented people extends beyond preferences. In Study 2, we therefore broadened our scope from preferences to goals. In addition, we included a stress induction because people often seek mindfulness as a stress reduction tool [68,69].

3. Study 2

In Study 2, we aimed to replicate and extend the findings of Study 1. First, we induced stress in all participants. Second, we used a different measure of alienation that taps into the adoption of goal recommendations. People have to deal with social expectations on a daily basis and may adopt recommendations wholeheartedly, halfheartedly, or not at all—indicating the degree of integration into the self [70]. Intrinsic goals can be easily integrated into the self because they fulfill inner needs. Extrinsic goals, in contrast, are diametrically opposed to inner needs and often introjected rather than integrated [71]. However, extrinsic goals are not bad per se but a lower tendency to adopt intrinsic over extrinsic goals is detrimental to psychological health [72].

In the present study, we used false self-ascriptions of recommended goals to measure goal adoption [17] and looked at the relation between intrinsic and extrinsic goals rather than just extrinsic goals [72]. We expected state-oriented participants to show a lower tendency to adopt (falsely self-ascribe) intrinsic over extrinsic goal recommendations after the mindfulness exercise compared to the control condition. We did not expect mindfulness to alienate action-oriented participants from their goals.

3.1. Participants

The required number of participants was calculated prior to data collection using G*Power [63]. Consistent with Kaufmann et al. [17], we observed a small to intermediate interaction effect in Study 1 ( = .039; f = .20). To detect a small to intermediate within-between interaction effect of f = .20 with a power of .90 and α = .05 (ANOVA, 4 groups, 2 measurements), the result suggested a minimum of 96 participants. The sample consisted of 108 people (30 men, 75 women, 3 diverse) aged 17–69 years (M = 28.00, SD = 9.84). We recruited participants online at a German University (21 undergraduates, 87 others).

3.2. Methods

The ethics committee of the University waived the need for consent. Participants took part in an online assessment. As in Study 1, before starting the experiment, participants gave their consent to participate in the experiment by clicking the consent field and the ‘continue’ button. Additionally, they were informed that they could cancel and revoke their consent and leave the experiment at any time. As illustrated in Fig 3, participants first completed measures of action-state orientation, implicit/explicit affect, and state mindfulness (T1).

3.2.1. Stress induction.

Next, we induced stress in all participants through an imagery exercise which has been successfully used in previous work (24, Exp. 3; 51): In a 4-minute audio, participants were instructed to revitalize an experience with a threatening person, the person’s tone of voice and words, and their own feelings in that situation. Afterwards, we measured implicit/explicit affect and state mindfulness again (T2).

3.2.2. Selection phase.

We presented 24 intrinsic and 24 extrinsic goals (see S2 and S3 Tables in S1 File). Expert recommendations: Twelve intrinsic and twelve extrinsic goals were marked with an asterisk to indicate that experts recommended these goals because they allegedly matched participants’ initial personality screening. We asked participants to read each goal aloud and confirm the recommendation status by pressing a corresponding key. Self-selection: We presented eight lists each with six goals that were homogeneous with respect to goal type and recommendation status. We asked participants to select three goals of each list they would like to pursue. Thus, there were six intrinsic and six extrinsic goals in each of the following categories: both (recommended and self-selected), recommended, self-selected, remaining (see S1 Table in S1 File).

3.2.3. Experimental induction.

We randomly assigned participants to the mindfulness group (n = 56) versus control group (n = 52). The mindfulness exercise and text-reading task were the same as in Study 1. Next, we measured implicit/explicit affect and state mindfulness (T3).

3.2.4. Classification phase.

In two separate tasks, participants needed to classify goals as recommended (yes vs. no) and as self-selected (yes vs. no). Finally, participants answered some post-experimental questions. We thanked and debriefed participants and compensated undergraduate students with course credit. The experiment lasted roughly 45 minutes.

3.3. Materials

We used the same measures as in Study 1 for action orientation (Cronbach’s α = .74), affective states (explicit negative affect: .75/.86/.74), and state mindfulness (.82/.83/.86). For the dependent variable, we calculated the percentage of recommended goals that participants falsely ascribed as self-selected, separately for intrinsic and extrinsic goals. For example, an intrinsic goal adoption rate of 33% would indicate that participants falsely ascribed two out of six recommended intrinsic goals as self-selected. Finally, we calculated differences scores: intrinsic minus extrinsic goal adoption rates. High values indicate a healthy tendency to adopt recommended goals more often when they were intrinsic rather than extrinsic (i.e., low alienation), whereas low values indicated that people did not consider the goal type when adopting recommended goals (i.e., high alienation).

3.4. Results

3.4.1. Prior experience.

Only 29% of the participants indicated experience with mindfulness exercises (mindfulness condition: 23% vs. control condition: 35%; t(106) = 1.31, p = .194).

3.4.2. Manipulation check.

To test the effectiveness of our inductions, we conducted two separate 3 (time: T1, T2, T3) x 2 (state vs. action orientation) x 2 (mindfulness vs. control condition) ANOVAs with repeated measures on the first factor.

The analysis of explicit negative affect yielded only a significant main effect of Time, F(2, 208) = 39.97, p < .001, = .278, indicating that explicit negative affect significantly increased after the stress induction (MT1 = 1.49 (0.58) vs. MT2 = 1.99 (0.82); p < .001) and returned to baseline afterwards (MT3 = 1.40 (0.54)). There were no differential effects indicating that the stress induction was equally effective for both state- and action-oriented participants (see also S4 and S5 Tables in S1 File).

The analysis of state mindfulness yielded a significant main effect of Time, F(2, 208) = 4.21, p < .02, = .039, that was qualified by a significant Time x Condition interaction, F(2, 208) = 7.44, p < .001, = .067. As listed in Table 2, in the mindfulness condition, state mindfulness was significantly higher at T3 compared to T1 and T2. In the control condition, in contrast, state mindfulness was not significantly higher at T3 compared to T1 and T2. There were no main or interaction effects of state orientation indicating that the mindfulness induction was equally effective for both state- and action-oriented participants.

thumbnail
Table 2. Means (and Standard Deviations) of State Mindfulness in Study 2 (N = 108).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0303505.t002

3.4.3. False self-ascriptions.

To test our hypothesis, we used false self-ascription rates of recommended goals as a dependent variable and conducted a 2 (intrinsic vs. extrinsic goals) x 2 (state vs. action orientation) x 2 (mindfulness vs. control condition) ANOVA with repeated measures on the first factor. The highly significant main effect of Goal Type, F(1, 104) = F(1, 104) = 19.69, p < .001, = .159, indicated that participants adopted (falsely self-ascribed) more recommendations of intrinsic compared to extrinsic goals: Mintrinsic = 46.45% (24.65) vs. Mextrinsic = 32.87% (26.13). More importantly, the Goal Type x Orientation x Condition interaction was significant, F(1, 104) = 4.89, p = .029, = .045 (see Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Means (and Standard Deviations) of the Tendency to Adopt (Falsely Self-Ascribe) Recommended Goals (in %) in Study 2 (N = 108).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0303505.t003

To facilitate interpretation of the three-way interaction, we analyzed both experimental conditions separately. In the mindfulness condition, the significant main effect of Goal Type, F(1, 54) = 12.68, p < .001, = .190, was qualified by a significant Goal Type x Orientation interaction, F(1, 54) = 6.52, p < .015, = .108. In contrast to action-oriented participants, state-oriented participants did not adopt more recommendations of intrinsic compared to extrinsic goals. In the control condition, the Goal Type x Orientation ANOVA yielded only a significant main effect of Goal Type, F(1, 50) = 7.72, p < .008, = .134, but no significant Goal Type x Orientation interaction, F(1, 50) = 0.54, p = .466, = .011, indicating that both state- and action-oriented participants adopted more recommendations of intrinsic compared to extrinsic goals.

To illustrate the finding, we used difference scores of intrinsic minus extrinsic goal adoption (see Fig 4). In the mindfulness condition, state- compared to action-oriented participants showed a significantly lower tendency to adopt intrinsic over extrinsic goals: MState = 3.85% vs. MAction = 23.33%, t(54) = -2.55, p = .014. In the control condition, state- and action-oriented participants did not differ in the tendency to adopt intrinsic over extrinsic goals: MState = 16.05% vs. Action = 9.33%, t(50) = 0.74, p = .466.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Alienation as assessed by a lower tendency to adopt (falsely self-ascribe) intrinsic over extrinsic goal recommendations in Study 2 (N = 108).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0303505.g004

3.5. Discussion

In Study 2, state-oriented participants showed a lower tendency to adopt intrinsic over extrinsic goal recommendations in mindfulness compared to the control condition. This means that state-oriented people showed alienation from self-congruent goals after the mindfulness induction. Thus, our results replicated with a different measure of alienation and with the addition of a stress induction. This further supports our assumption that practicing mindfulness alienated state-oriented people from the self with and without stress. In both studies, we did not find alienating effects among action-oriented participants.

4. General discussion

Mindfulness is a means to self-reflection and awakening and many findings support this claim [73,74]. Contrarily, in the current research, we empirically demonstrated that practicing mindfulness has adverse effects and alienates some people from their self. In two studies, we showed that, among state-oriented participants, mindfulness promoted alienation as measured by lower consistency of preference judgments and a lower tendency to adopt intrinsic over extrinsic goals. This alienating effect of mindfulness was absent in action-oriented participants.

Mindfulness not only detached state-oriented people from their preferences but also from their intrinsic goals. This could render into poor psychological functioning for state-oriented people affecting their everyday life choices and decisions and augmenting their self-regulatory deficit [11]. Although higher false self-ascriptions of assigned goals and lower consistency in repeated preference judgments do not seem like a big problem in themselves, they indicate a tendency towards alienation that has a real-life impact. For example, such markers of alienation from the self have been associated with higher depression and anxiety [75], higher cortisol reactivity [76], higher medication intake [77], lower flow experience [78], and lower work performance [79]. Therefore, our finding that mindfulness increases the tendency towards alienation among state-oriented people has profound consequences for their psychological functioning across many life domains. Note that our pattern of findings was consistent across two different samples, two different and well-established measures of alienation, and two different conditions of induced stress versus no stress. This methodological convergence increases confidence in our findings.

Our findings extend previous research on mindfulness and the self in at least three important ways. First, we took a differential view and tested for whom the practice is effective, ineffective, or even counter-indicated [58,80,81]. This is important because state-oriented people are the ones who may actually need and seek mindfulness the most in order to gain better self-access [29]. Previous findings suggest that depressed people and ruminators, for example, do indeed profit from mindfulness interventions [8284]. In contrast, our studies showed the opposite effect. State-oriented people did not only not profit but incurred harm. These detrimental effects on self-related variables may have gone unnoticed in previous studies because research relies heavily on self-report [8587].

Second, we measured self-access beyond self-report. Although self-report measures contribute well to the overall understanding of psychological constructs, for a mechanism like access to the implicit self, results are more credible when applying non-reactive measures [88,89]. Positive effects of mindfulness often emerge when measuring self-related variables by self-report [90]. However, the present findings show that the effects reverse when measuring self-access non-reactively (see also 17). Hence, self-reports should be supplemented with more ‘objective’ behavioral methods.

Third, Kaufmann and colleagues [17] tested the alienating effect for state-oriented people only under stress. The present findings show that mindfulness alienated state-oriented people across stress and no stress conditions. This has important consequences for understanding why mindfulness has alienating effects. On the basis of PSI theory [3,4,11], Kaufmann and colleagues [17] argued that practicing mindfulness might activate a basic processing system –called object recognition–which is associated with attentional focus to isolated objects and sensations (e.g., breathing, heartbeat; [91]) and antagonistically related to the self. This means that any activation of object recognition will reduce access to the self if it is not actively regulated. The current findings suggest that mindfulness might also activate another basic processing system–called intuitive behavior control–that is responsible for behavioral routines and staying in the ‘here and now’. Because it is antagonistically related to the self, any activation of intuitive behavior control will also reduce access to the self. Intuitive behavior control can be activated through non-conscious, indirect cues (e.g., priming; [92]), which is similar to the instructions given in mindfulness training for being in the present. Whereas object recognition is associated with negative affect, intuitive behavior control is associated with positive affect [3]. Activating this basic processing system might lead state-oriented people to ‘mindfully and happily miss themselves’ without conscious realization. However, the possible routes into alienation need further investigation.

5. Limitations and future directions

The present research is not free from shortcomings. First, both studies had short mindfulness interventions as even short exercises have positive impacts on psychological functioning [9395]. However, the outcomes could differ with longer mindfulness inductions. Long-term practice of mindfulness, for example, might help state-oriented people to develop the very self-regulatory abilities whose lack resulted in alienation.

Second, our participants are mostly amateur practitioners of mindfulness and might not have the “right” motivation to practice mindfulness. The short-term challenges of mindfulness for naïve practitioners are well-known (e.g., [37]) as well as the effects of motivation to practice on the practice outcomes [96]. Trying mindfulness without desire to practice might not have the same effects as a genuine mindfulness practice. Therefore, the outcomes might differ for advanced practitioners [97,98].

Finally, our findings draw attention towards a need to adapt mindfulness trainings in accordance with individual differences to enable state-oriented people to profit from its well-documented benefits. For example, state-oriented people profit from social support—even if it is imagined [12,24,99]. In future research, participants could imagine practicing the mindfulness exercise together with a close friend to integrate the positive effects of social support.

6. Conclusion

Mindfulness meditation is in vogue as a self-help tool for its profound psychological benefits across different populations and conditions [100,101]. One of the great potentials of mindfulness is that it helps people to get in touch with themselves [86]. However, whether mindfulness is beneficial or not depends on people’s personality dispositions. In the present studies, we show that mindfulness had the exact opposite effect and caused some people to get out of touch from their self. Although participants reported being mindful, on a deeper level, the practice alienated state-oriented people from their self. In conclusion, state-oriented people may mindfully miss themselves.

Supporting information

S1 File. All the supporting information can be found in S1 File.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0303505.s001

(DOCX)

References

  1. 1. al-Thawri S. (2015). The Biographies of the elite lives of the scholars, Imams & Hadith masters (Haddad S. F., Ed.). Zulfiqar Ayub.
  2. 2. Koole S.L., & DeHart T. (2007). Self-affection without self-reflection: Origins, models, and consequences of implicit self-esteem. In Sedikides C.& Spencer S. J.(Eds.), The self (pp. 21–49). Psychology Press.
  3. 3. Kuhl J. (2000). The volitional basis of personality systems interaction theory: Applications in learning and treatment contexts. International Journal of Educational Research, 33, 665–703. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0883-0355(00)00045-8.
  4. 4. Kuhl, J. (2001). Motivation und Persönlichkeit. Interaktionen psychischer systeme [Motivation and personality: Interactions of mental systems]. Hogrefe.
  5. 5. Quirin M., Jais M., Di Domenico S.I., Kuhl J., & Ryan R.M. (2021). Effortless Willpower? The integrative self and self-determined goal pursuit. Frontiers in Psychology. pmid:33815234
  6. 6. Baumann N., & Kuhl J. (2003). Self-infiltration: Confusing assigned tasks as self-selected in memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(4), 487–497. pmid:15281191
  7. 7. Koole S.L., Jostmann N.B., & Baumann N. (in press). From intentions to action: An integrative review of four decades of action control theory and research. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.
  8. 8. Kuhl J. (1994). Action versus state orientation: Psychometric properties of the action control scale (ACS-90). In Kuhl J. & Beckmann J.(Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation, 47–59. Hogrefe.
  9. 9. Baumann N., Kazén M., & Quirin M. (2018). How do we know if you know yourself: Measures, causes, and consequences of self-access. In Baumann N., Kazén M., Quirin M., & Koole S.(Eds.), Why people do the things they do: Building on Julius Kuhl’s contributions to the psychology of motivation and volition (pp. 259–280). Hogrefe.
  10. 10. Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1994). Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation. Hogrefe & Huber.
  11. 11. Kuhl J., Quirin M., & Koole S.L. (2021). The functional architecture of human motivation: Personality systems interactions theory. Advances in Motivation Science, Vol. 8, 0–62. Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.adms.2020.06.001
  12. 12. Koole S.L., & Jostmann N.B. (2004). Getting a grip on your feelings: Effects of action orientation and external demands on intuitive affect regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(6), 974–990. pmid:15598118
  13. 13. Roemer L., Williston S.K., & Rollins L.G. (2015). Mindfulness and emotion regulation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 52–57.
  14. 14. Teper R., Segal Z.V., & Inzlicht M. (2013). Inside the mindful mind: How mindfulness enhances emotion regulation through improvements in executive control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(6), 449–454.
  15. 15. Brand S., Holsboer-Trachsler E., Naranjo J. R., & Schmidt S. (2012). Influence of mindfulness practice on cortisol and sleep in long-term and short-term meditators. Neuropsychobiology, 65(3), 109–118. pmid:22377965
  16. 16. Norouzi E., Gerber M., Masrour F. F., Vaezmosavi M., Pühse U., & Brand S. (2020). Implementation of a mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) program to reduce stress, anxiety, and depression and to improve psychological well-being among retired Iranian football players. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 47, 101636.
  17. 17. Kaufmann M., Rosing K., & Baumann N. (2020). Being mindful does not always benefit everyone: Mindfulness-based practices may promote alienation among psychologically vulnerable people. Cognition and Emotion, 35(2), 241–255. pmid:32993426
  18. 18. Koole S. L. (2004). Volitional shielding of the self: Effects of action orientation and external demands on implicit self-evaluation. Social Cognition, 22(1).
  19. 19. Kuehner C., & Huffziger S. (2013). Factors predicting the long-term illness course in a cohort of depressed inpatients. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 263(5), 413–423. pmid:23108435
  20. 20. Sniehotta F. F., Scholz U., & Schwarzer R. (2005). Bridging the intention–behaviour gap: Planning, self-efficacy, and action control in the adoption and maintenance of physical exercise. Psychology & Health, 20(2), 143–160.
  21. 21. Wolf B. M., Herrmann M., & Brandstätter V. (2018). Self-efficacy vs. action orientation: comparing and contrasting two determinants of goal setting and goal striving. Journal of Research in Personality, 73, 35–45.
  22. 22. Baumann N., Kaschel R., & Kuhl J. (2007). Affect sensitivity and affect regulation in dealing with positive and negative affect. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1), 239–248.
  23. 23. Brunstein J.C. (2001). Persönliche Ziele und Handlungs-versus Lageorientierung. Wer bindet sich an realistische und bedürfniskongruente Ziele? Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 22, 1–12.
  24. 24. Baumann N., Kaschel R., & Kuhl J. (2005). Striving for unwanted goals: stress-dependent discrepancies between explicit and implicit achievement motives reduce subjective well-being and increase psychosomatic symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(5), 781. pmid:16351368
  25. 25. Gieselmann A., Ophey M., de Jong-Meyer R., & Pietrowsky R. (2012). An induced emotional stressor differentially decreases subjective sleep quality in state-oriented but not in action-oriented individuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(8), 1007–1011.
  26. 26. Backes S., Brandstätter V., Kuster M., Nussbeck F.W., Bradbury T.N., Bodenmann G., et al. (2017). Who suffers from stress? Action-state orientation moderates the effect of external stress on relationship satisfaction. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 34(6), 894–914.
  27. 27. Schlüter C., Fraenz C., Pinnow M., Voelkle M.C., Güntürkün O., & Genç E. (2018). Volition and academic achievement: Interindividual differences in action control mediate the effects of conscientiousness and sex on secondary school grading. Motivation Science, 4(3), 262–273.
  28. 28. Koole S.L., Tops M., Strübin S., Bouw J., Schneider I.K., & Jostmann N.B. (2014). The ego fixation hypothesis: Involuntary persistence of self-control. In Forgas J. P. & Harmon-Jones E. (Eds.), Motivation and its regulation: The control within, 95–112. Psychology Press.
  29. 29. Kazén M., Baumann N., & Kuhl J. (2003). Self-Infiltration vs. Self-Compatibility Checking in dealing with Unattractive tasks: The Moderating influence of state vs. Action orientation. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 157–197.
  30. 30. Herrmann M., & Brandstätter V. (2013). Overcoming action crises in personal goals–Longitudinal evidence on a mediating mechanism between action orientation and well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 47(6), 881–893.
  31. 31. Thakur N., & Baumann N. (2022). Breaking the anxious cycle of self-criticism: Action orientation buffers the detrimental effects of self-critical personality style. Journal of Affective Disorders, 301, 30–35. pmid:34995706
  32. 32. Job V., Oertig D., Brandstätter V., & Allemand M. (2010). Discrepancies between implicit and explicit motives and unhealthy eating behavior. Journal of Personality, 78, 1209–1238. pmid:20545817
  33. 33. Hofer J., Busch H., Chasiotis A., & Kiessling F. (2006). Motive congruence and interpersonal identity status. Journal of Personality, 74, 511–541. pmid:16529585
  34. 34. Brown K.W., & Ryan R.M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822. pmid:12703651
  35. 35. Creswell J.D., Pacilio L.E., Lindsay E.K., & Brown K.W. (2014). Brief mindfulness meditation training alters psychological and neuroendocrine responses to social evaluative stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 44, 1–12. pmid:24767614
  36. 36. Kuijpers H.J., Van der Heijden F.M.M.A., Tuinier S., & Verhoeven W.M.A. (2007). Meditation-induced psychosis. Psychopathology, 40(6), 461–464. pmid:17848828
  37. 37. Lindahl J.R., Fisher N.E., Cooper D.J., Rosen R.K., & Britton W.B. (2017). The varieties of contemplative experience: A mixed-methods study of meditation-related challenges in western Buddhists. PLOS ONE, 12(5). pmid:28542181
  38. 38. Lindahl J.R., Britton W.B., Cooper D.J., & Kirmayer L.J. (2019). Challenging and adverse meditation experiences: Toward a person-centered approach. The Oxford Handbook of Meditation. Oxford University Press.
  39. 39. Lustyk M.K., Chawla N., Nolan R.S., & Marlatt G.A. (2009). Mindfulness meditation research: issues of participant screening, safety procedures, and researcher training. Advances in Mind-Body Medicine, 24(1), 20–30. pmid:20671334
  40. 40. Kabat-Zinn, J., & University of Massachusetts Medical Center/ Worcester. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. Delacorte Press.
  41. 41. Kabat-Zinn, J. (2005). Coming to our senses: Healing ourselves and the world through mindfulness. Hachette.
  42. 42. Kabat-Zinn, J., & Hanh, T.N. (2009). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. Delta.
  43. 43. Gotink R.A., Meijboom R., Vernooij M.W., Smits M., & Hunink M.M. (2016). 8-week mindfulness based stress reduction induces brain changes similar to traditional long-term meditation practice–a systematic review. Brain and Cognition, 108, 32–41. pmid:27429096
  44. 44. Verhaeghen P. (2020). Mindfulness as attention training: Meta-analyses on the links between attention performance and mindfulness interventions, long-term meditation practice, and Trait Mindfulness. Mindfulness, 12(3), 564–581.
  45. 45. Anderson N.D., Lau M.A., Segal Z.V., & Bishop S.R. (2007). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and attentional control. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 14(6), 449–463.
  46. 46. Chambers R., Lo B.C.Y., & Allen N.B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 303–322.
  47. 47. Fan Y., Tang Y.Y., Tang R., & Posner M.I. (2015). Time course of conflict processing modulated by brief meditation training. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 911. pmid:26191022
  48. 48. Liu X., Wang S., Chang S., Chen W., & Si M. (2012). Effect of brief mindfulness intervention on tolerance and distress of pain induced by cold-pressor task. Stress and Health, 29(3), 199–204. pmid:22961992
  49. 49. Kappen G., Karremans J. C., & Burk W. J. (2019). Effects of a short online mindfulness intervention on relationship satisfaction and partner acceptance: The moderating role of Trait mindfulness. Mindfulness, 10(10), 2186–2199.
  50. 50. Nyklíček I., Hoogwegt F., &Westgeest T. (2015). Psychological distress across twelve months in patients with rheumatoid arthritis: The role of disease activity, disability, and mindfulness. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 78(2), 162–167. pmid:25260860
  51. 51. Ingram R.E. (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: review and a conceptual model. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 156–176. pmid:2181521
  52. 52. Mor N., & Winquist J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 638–662. pmid:12081086
  53. 53. Farias M., Maraldi E., Wallenkampf K. C., & Lucchetti G. (2020). Adverse events in meditation practices and meditation-based therapies: A systematic review. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 142(5), 374–393. pmid:32820538
  54. 54. Sierra M. (2009). Depersonalization: A new look at a neglected syndrome. Cambridge University Press.
  55. 55. Cebolla A., Demarzo M., Martins P., Soler J., & Garcia-Campayo J. (2017). Unwanted effects: Is there a negative side of meditation? A multicentre survey. PloS One, 12(9), e0183137. pmid:28873417
  56. 56. Kabat-Zinn J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical Considerations and preliminary results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4(1), 33–47. pmid:7042457
  57. 57. Parsons C.E., Crane C., Parsons L.J., Fjorback L.O., & Kuyken W. (2017). Home practice in mindfulness-based cognitive therapy and mindfulness-based stress reduction: A systematic review and meta-analysis of participants’ mindfulness practice and its association with outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 95, 29–41. pmid:28527330
  58. 58. Sahdra B.K., Ciarrochi J., Parker P.D., Basarkod G., Bradshaw E.L., & Baer R. (2017). Are people mindful in different ways? Disentangling the quantity and quality of mindfulness in latent profiles and exploring their links to mental health and life effectiveness. European Journal of Personality, 31(4), 347–365.
  59. 59. Buric I., Farias M., Driessen J. M., & Brazil I. A. (2022). Individual differences in meditation interventions: A Meta-analytic study. British Journal of Health Psychology, 27(3), 1043–1076. pmid:35224829
  60. 60. Grant A.M., & Schwartz B. (2011). Too much of a good thing: The challenge and opportunity of the inverted U. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 61–76. pmid:26162116
  61. 61. Nordgren L.F., & Dijksterhuis A.P. (2009). The devil is in the deliberation: Thinking too much reduces preference consistency. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(1), 39–46.
  62. 62. Guevara, M. (1994). Alienation und Selbstkontrolle: Das Ignorieren eigener Gefühle [Alienation and self-control: Ignoring one’s feelings]. Peter Lang.
  63. 63. Faul F., Erdfelder E., Buchner A., & Lang A.G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), pmid:19897823
  64. 64. Zimbardo, P.G., & Gerrig, R.J. (1999). Psychology and Life. Longman.
  65. 65. Ellis J.L. (2013). A standard for test reliability in group research. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 16–24. pmid:22736454
  66. 66. Iacobucci D., Posavac S.S., Kardes F.R., Schneider M.J., & Popovich D.L. (2015). Toward a more nuanced understanding of the statistical properties of a median split. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(4), 652–665.
  67. 67. Michalak J., Heidenreich T., Ströhle G., & Nachtigall C. (2008). Die deutsche Version der Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale (MAAS): Psychometrische Befunde zu einem Achtsamkeitsfragebogen. Zeitschrift Für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 37(3), 200–208.
  68. 68. Astin J.A. (1997). Stress reduction through mindfulness meditation. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 66(2), 97–106. pmid:9097338
  69. 69. Chiesa A., & Serretti A. (2009). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for stress management in healthy people: a review and meta-analysis. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 15(5), 593–600. pmid:19432513
  70. 70. Kuhl J., & Kazén M. (1994). Self-discrimination and memory: State orientation and false self-ascription of assigned activities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(6), 1103–1115. pmid:8046579
  71. 71. Deci E.L., & Ryan R.M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self‐determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.
  72. 72. Sheldon K.M., Ryan R.M., Deci E.L. & Kasser T. (2004). The independent effects of goal contents and motives in well-being: It’s both what you pursue and why you pursue it. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 475–486. pmid:15070476
  73. 73. Anālayo B. (2020). The qualities pertinent to awakening: Bringing mindfulness home. Mindfulness.
  74. 74. van Wietmarschen H., Tjaden B., Van Vliet M., Battjes-Fries M., & Jong M. (2018). Effects of mindfulness training on perceived stress, self-compassion, and self-reflection of Primary Care Physicians: A mixed-methods study. BJGP Open, 2(4). pmid:30723806
  75. 75. Baumann, N. (1998). Selbst- versus Fremdbestimmung: Zum Einfluss von Stimmung, Bewusstheit und Persönlichkeit [Self-determination versus heteronomy: On the impact of mood, consciousness, and personality]. Unpublished Dissertation. University of Osnabrück, Germany.
  76. 76. Quirin M., Koole S. L., Baumann N., Kazén M., & Kuhl J. (2009). You can’t always remember what you want: The role of cortisol in self-ascription of assigned goals. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 1026–1032.
  77. 77. Schüler J., Job V., Fröhlich S. M., & Brandstätter V. (2009). Dealing with a ‘hidden stressor’: Emotional disclosure as a coping strategy to overcome the negative effects of motive incongruence on health. Stress and Health, 25, 221–233.
  78. 78. Schüler J. (2010). Achievement incentives determine the effects of achievement-motive incongruence on flow experience. Motivation and Emotion, 34, 2–14.
  79. 79. Lang J. W. B., Zettler I., Ewen C., & Hülsheger U. R. (2012). Implicit Motives, Explicit Traits, and Task and Contextual Performance at Work. Journal of Applied Psychology. Advance online publication. pmid:22867444
  80. 80. Roth, A., Fonagy, P. (2005). What works for whom?: A critical review of psychotherapy research. Guilford.
  81. 81. Shapiro S.L., Brown K.W., Thoresen C., & Plante T.G. (2011). The moderation of mindfulness-based stress reduction effects by trait mindfulness: Results from a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67(3), 267–277. pmid:21254055
  82. 82. Deyo M., Wilson K.A., Ong J., & Koopman C. (2009). Mindfulness and rumination: does mindfulness training lead to reductions in the ruminative thinking associated with depression? Explore, 5(5), 265–271. pmid:19733812
  83. 83. Gilbert K., & Gruber J. (2014). Emotion regulation of goals in bipolar disorder and major depression: A comparison of rumination and mindfulness. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38(4), 375–388.
  84. 84. Schroevers M.J., Tovote K.A., Snippe E., & Fleer J. (2016). Group and individual mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) are both effective: A pilot randomized controlled trial in depressed people with a somatic disease. Mindfulness, 7(6), 1339–1346. pmid:27909465
  85. 85. Baumeister R.F., Vohs K.D., & Funder D.C. (2007). Psychology as the science of self-reports and finger movements: Whatever happened to actual behavior? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(4), 396–403. pmid:26151975
  86. 86. Carlson E.N. (2013). Overcoming the barriers to self-knowledge: Mindfulness as a path to seeing yourself as you really are. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(2), 173–186. pmid:26172498
  87. 87. McDonald J.D. (2008). Measuring personality constructs: The advantages and disadvantages of self-reports, informant reports and behavioural assessments. Enquire, 1(1), 1–19.
  88. 88. Baer R. (2019). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report. Current Opinion in Psychology, 28, 42–48. pmid:30423507
  89. 89. Greenwald A.G., & Banaji M.R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27. pmid:7878162
  90. 90. Röthlin P., Horvath S., Trösch S., Holtforth M.G., & Birrer D. (2020). Differential and shared effects of psychological skills training and mindfulness training on performance-relevant psychological factors in sport: A randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychology, 8(1). 1–13. pmid:32762736
  91. 91. Schlinkert C, Herbert B., Baumann N., & Koole S.L. (2020). Preoccupied with the body: Mild stress amplifies the relation between rumination and interoception. Cognition and Emotion, 34(7), 1382–1394. pmid:32223508
  92. 92. Bargh J.A., Chen M., & Burrows L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 230. pmid:8765481
  93. 93. Fazia T., Bubbico F., Iliakis I., Salvato G., Berzuini G., Bruno S., & Bernardinelli L. (2020). Short-Term Meditation Training Fosters Mindfulness and Emotion Regulation: A Pilot Study. Frontiers in Psychology, 2828. pmid:33192816
  94. 94. Tang Y.Y., Ma Y., Wang J., Fan Y., Feng S., Lu Q., et al. (2007). Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104(43), 17152–17156. pmid:17940025
  95. 95. Zeidan F., Johnson S.K., Diamond B.J., David Z., & Goolkasian P. (2010). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2), 597–605. pmid:20363650
  96. 96. Rosenkranz M. A., Dunne J. D., & Davidson R. J. (2019). The next generation of mindfulness-based intervention research: What have we learned and where are we headed? Current Opinion in Psychology, 28, 179–183. pmid:30739006
  97. 97. Baer R.A., Lykins E.L., & Peters J.R. (2012). Mindfulness and self-compassion as predictors of psychological wellbeing in long-term meditators and matched non-meditators. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 7(3), 230–238.
  98. 98. Carmody J., & Baer R.A. (2008). Relationships between mindfulness practice and levels of mindfulness, medical and psychological symptoms and well-being in a mindfulness-based stress reduction program. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 31(1), 23–33. pmid:17899351
  99. 99. Amlinger-Chatterjee M.B., & Baumann N. (2018). The we helps me: Poor emotion-regulators benefit from relatedness. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 49(1).
  100. 100. Chiesa A., & Serretti A. (2011). Mindfulness based cognitive therapy for psychiatric disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 187(3), 441–453. pmid:20846726
  101. 101. Hofmann S.G., Sawyer A.T., Witt A.A., & Oh D. (2010). The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78(2), 169–183. pmid:20350028