Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Mother-infant interaction characteristics associate with infant falling reactivity and child peer problems at pre-school age

Abstract

This longitudinal study investigated the associations between mother-infant interaction characteristics at 9 months of age, maternal mental health, infant temperament in the first year postpartum, and child behaviour at 3 years of age. The infants (N = 54, 22 females) mainly had White British ethnic backgrounds (85.7%). Results showed that i) mother-infant dyadic affective mutuality positively correlated with infant falling reactivity, suggesting that better infant regulatory skills are associated with the dyad’s ability to share and understand each other’s emotions; and ii) maternal respect for infant autonomy predicted fewer child peer problems at 3 years of age, suggesting that maternal respect for the validity of the infant’s individuality promotes better social and emotional development in early childhood.

Introduction

The quality of caregiver-infant interaction is of fundamental importance for optimal cognitive, social and emotional infant development (e.g., [13]), as well as for later behavioural and academic outcomes (e.g., [4,5]). In fact, primary caregivers, mostly mothers in western societies, are key in promoting their infant’s development by introducing them to new experiences, supporting emerging skills, and providing opportunities for learning [6]. While mother-infant interactions have been studied extensively in the past decades, it is still unclear which specific components of these interactions positively impact on infant and child development. Similarly, the question of how individual infant characteristics and the wellbeing of the mother interact during their exchanges is still open. In the past decade, the study of mother-infant interactions has increased the use of dyadic measures to capture the nature of such inherently social, bi-directional and reciprocal exchanges more comprehensively. In the present mother-infant interaction study, we employed a coding scheme that comprised of six codes, two relating to maternal behaviour, two to infant behaviour, and two to dyadic interaction behaviour. This approach allowed us to investigate which characteristics of an observed interaction between a mother and her 9-month-old infant were significantly associated with maternal mental health and infant temperament measured throughout the first year postpartum, and which ones predicted child behaviour measured at preschool age.

Historically, researchers have primarily focused on the mother’s sensitive response to infant cues during mother-infant interactions. Ainsworth and colleagues originally defined maternal sensitivity as the ability to perceive and accurately interpret the signals and communications implicit in infant behaviour, and to respond to them appropriately and promptly [7]. Indeed, maternal sensitivity has been found to predict a range of child outcomes, from better emotional regulation [8] to lower levels of aggression [9] and behavioural problems [10]. In addition, attachment theory (e.g., [11]) has traditionally focused on the construct of maternal sensitivity as a main precursor of child secure attachment. However, it is now well-accepted that maternal sensitivity is not the only factor promoting optimal development in young children [12]. The infant’s ability to regulate affect (e.g., [13]) as well as mother-child dyadic synchrony during interaction (e.g., [14]) play an important mediating role between maternal sensitivity and infant-mother attachment.

Similarly, another key factor that needs to be considered when observing mother-infant interaction is maternal mental health. Critically, a line of research focusing on the impact of maternal mental health on mother-infant interactions shows how these interactions are affected in clinical as well as sub-clinical samples [1518]. For example, Cohn et al. [19] reported that depressed mothers have longer latencies in terms of maternal responsivity towards the infant, indicating a lack of synchrony during interactions. In a sample of mothers who were not diagnosed with or treated for depression, Moehler and colleagues (2006) [16] found that depressive symptoms in the first 4 months postpartum were predictive of bonding impairments in the first 14 months of life, with a particularly pronounced impact of maternal depressive symptoms on bonding at 6 weeks postpartum. Another study, conducted in a general population sample with a relatively low number of high-risk mother-infant dyads [20], found an association between depressive symptoms at 6 months postpartum and lower maternal structuring and decreased infant involvement in interaction observed at 8 months (see also [21,22]). The authors concluded that these results may either indicate disruption of interaction patterns that infants of depressed mothers develop early on [23,24] or bidirectional associations between the individual characteristics of the child and maternal caregiving behaviour [25].

Existent literature suggests that maternal and infant characteristics are confounded in parental reports and that these are reflected in the parent-infant relationship [26]. In line with this, Seifer et al. (2004) [27] reported that the mother-infant relationship affects the way the mother perceives her infant’s behaviour. Similarly, mothers’ expectations and representations of the relationship with their infant may also impact on the way they behave during mother-infant interactions [28]. Indeed, studies have shown that those infants who were perceived as more difficult by their mothers received less responsive mothering during the interaction [29,30]. At the same time, it is also the case that infant characteristics, such as temperament, influence the way an infant takes part in interactions [31]. In a study with 6-month-old infants, parental rating of infant difficult temperament was related to the infant’s less responsive behaviour during an interaction with their mother [32]. In addition, a study with mothers and their infants aged between 4 and 11 weeks found that both the mother’s intrusiveness and the infant’s poor interactive behaviour (i.e., avoidance and lack of active communication and positive vocalisations) significantly increased the infant’s likelihood of being perceived as difficult [33]. Another study investigated how maternal and infant characteristics are reflected in the parent-child relationship during the first 3 months of life and showed that maternal unresponsiveness and inflexibility predicted the amount of time an infant fussed and cried at 3 months of age during an observation session [34].

In an interesting investigation by Costa and Figueiredo (2012) [35], infants’ behavioural and physiological profiles (i.e., neurobehavioral organization, social withdrawal behaviour, and endocrine reactivity to stress) were analysed in relation to the quality of mother-infant interactions. The authors concluded that early behavioural characteristics may influence social interaction development, that the mother’s behaviour plays a critical role in the modulation of the infant’s regulation of biological responses to stressors, and that withdrawn infants may be at risk of developmental difficulties due to both the infants’ and mothers’ less optimal interaction behaviours.

Taken together, this body of evidence highlights the relevance of studying mother-infant interactions for a better understanding of the dynamic, and likely transactional, associations with maternal mental health and with infant development. Crucially, more recent studies have demonstrated how the behaviours observed in early mother-infant interaction are also related to the child’s emotional and behavioural outcomes (e.g., [36,37]) and even to brain development (e.g., [3840]). For example, in a behavioural study, Mäntymaa et al. (2006) [33] investigated how shared pleasure (defined as the parent and child sharing positive affect in synchrony) in early mother–infant interaction at 2 months was related to later child emotional and behavioural symptoms at 2 years of age. Findings indicated that the duration of shared pleasure was negatively associated with socio-emotional difficulties, i.e., shorter shared pleasure moments were associated with higher infant internalizing and externalizing problem scores at 2 years. Additionally, shared pleasure in early mother–infant interaction was found to be a protective factor in the presence of parental psychopathology. Specifically, children with longer shared pleasure moments in early interaction were more protected against family mental health problems. In children with short shared pleasure moments, internalizing symptoms increased with more psychopathology in the family (both mother’s and father’s mental health problems), and the risk for higher externalizing problems increased with father’s mental health problems. In agreement with Siegel’s integrative view of how human development occurs within a social world (2001, 2012) [41,42], Mäntymaa et al. (2006) [33] concluded that children with more optimal dyadic emotion regulation in early infancy are more likely to internalize adaptive emotion regulation models and may be more protected against risks or may show more flexibility in adapting to various demands in the environment. More generally, and in accordance with a transactional perspective, during the transition to toddlerhood, mother-infant interactions lay the foundation for future developmental outcomes (e.g. [43]).

In the current longitudinal study, we aimed to further our understanding of whether and how specific behaviours and dyadic patterns that can be observed when mothers and infants interact are associated with maternal wellbeing and infant characteristics. We specifically focused on associations with maternal depressive symptoms in the first 9 months postpartum, infant temperament in the first 9 months of life, and child behaviour at 3 years of age.

Methods

Participants

A total of 81 families were recruited from the University of Essex database and through events for expectant parents at Colchester General Hospital and other local venues in Colchester (UK). The inclusion criteria were that the potential participant was female, pregnant, and had not yet entered the 37th week of pregnancy. There were no exclusion criteria. Ethical approval was received from the National Health Service’s National Research Ethics Service Committee, London-Hampstead branch, United Kingdom (15/LO/0478) and from the University of Essex Faculty of Science subcommittee. The approved protocol included a procedure for guiding mothers at high risk of clinical depression to further support and treatment options.

The current study reports on observed mother-infant interaction data (coded from video recordings) and questionnaire data from a longitudinal study investigating attention and social skills in the first year of life, and consisting of one pre- and four post-natal assessment points (36 weeks’ gestation, 2 weeks, 4 months, 6 months, 9 months). The prenatal assessment consisted of a foetal heart rate and movement assessment (data point not included in this article). At each postnatal assessment point, infants took part in behavioural and EEG studies and mothers completed questionnaires relating to their own mental health and to infant temperament. In addition to these measures, at 9 months infants took part in a free-play interaction session with their mothers. A final time point was added at 3 years of age to assess the child’s behavioural outcome via questionnaire. While previously published articles include further details on the participant sample and other longitudinal data from these infants [4446], none includes data from the mother-infant interaction session reported in this article.

Of the 81 recruited families, 16 mothers participated only in the prenatal assessment and 2 infants suffered from complications at birth and were excluded from the final sample. The sample reported on here consisted of 54 infants (22 females) and their mothers who came to the lab to take part in the mother-infant interaction session when the infant was 9 months old. No formal sample size calculation was used due to the lack of previous longitudinal studies with similar measures to base the calculation on. There were also financial and time constraints. However, a sample size of 54 infants is larger than most infant studies involving extensive behavioural testing. The majority of participating infants were born full-term (at 37 weeks of gestation or later; M = 40 weeks, 3 days), and only two infants were born between 36 and 37 weeks of gestation. Infants in the sample had normal birth weight (M(SD) = 3.5(0.4) Kg; Range = 2.44–4.82 Kg), no complications at birth, and no known health issues (pregnancy, birth and health information was missing for 4 infants). At the time of the prenatal assessment, which occurred around 36 weeks of gestation, mothers were on average 31.2 years of age (SD = 4.6) and fathers were on average 33.6 years of age (SD = 6.1). Mothers had spent an average of 16.8 years in education (SD = 3.6) and fathers had spent an average of 15.2 years in education (SD = 3.7). Participating families had an average income of £54,483 (SD = £28,606). The infants’ ethnic background was mainly White British (N = 54; 85.7%). Three infants were of Other White background (4.8%), four infants had mixed ethnic background but did not provide further details (6.4%), one infant was White British mixed with another ethnic background (1.6%), and one infant had a Hispanic background (1.6%).

Questionnaires

Participating mothers were asked to complete well-established questionnaires relating to infant temperament as well as symptoms of depression. Details on each of the questionnaires are provided below. The links to the online questionnaires were sent via email to those families who had confirmed attendance for a scheduled lab test session for the following day in an effort to keep the time difference between test session and questionnaire completion as short as practically possible.

The Beck Depression Inventory, 2nd Edition.

To assess mothers’ level of depressive symptoms over the course of their infant’s first year, we asked them to complete the Beck Depression Inventory, 2nd Edition (BDI-II; [47]). The BDI-II is a standardized questionnaire containing 21 items. Each question asks to what extent a certain state does or does not apply to the respondent (e.g., Sadness; response options: 0 –I do not feel sad, 1 –I feel sad much of the time, 2 –I am sad all the time, 3 –I am so sad or unhappy that I can’t stand it). A higher score indicates a higher level of depressive symptoms. A review of 118 studies established high internal consistency of the BDI-II, with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .83 to .96 across both clinical and non-clinical samples [48]. In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha for BDI-II scores at the four assessment time points ranged from .87 to .94 (see S1 Table in S1 File), with a mean alpha of .92. BDI-II questionnaires were scored by adding up the ratings for all items with the first option always scoring zero and the last option always scoring three. The sum of scores indicates four different levels of depression. A score between 0 and 13 is classified as “minimal depression”. A score between 14 and 19 is classified as “mild depression”. A score between 20 and 28 is classified as “moderate depression”, and a score between 29 and 63 is classified as “severe depression” [47].

The Infant Behavior Questionnaire–Revised – Very Short Form.

Mothers completed the Infant Behavior Questionnaire–Revised–Very Short Form (IBQ-R VSF; [49]), as well as two additional scales from the Infant Behavior Questionnaire–Revised (IBQ-R; [50]). The IBQ-R VSF consists of 37 items in total, and can be separated into three broad scales: Surgency (13 items; e.g., “When tossed around playfully, how often did the baby laugh?”), Negative Affect (12 items; e.g., “At the end of an exciting day, how often did your baby become tearful?”), and Orienting/Regulatory Capacity (12 items; e.g., “How often during the last week did the baby enjoy being read to?”) [49]. In addition to these scales, we also included the scales for Distress (12 items; e.g., “After sleeping, how often did the baby fuss or cry immediately?”) and Falling Reactivity (13 items; e.g., “When put down for a nap, how often did your baby settle down quickly?”) from the IBQ-R [50]. For each question, mothers were asked to rate how often their infant had shown a specific behaviour in a given situation during the past week by selecting a score between 1 (“Never”) and 7 (“Always”). Additionally, an item could be scored as “NA–Does not apply” should the situation described in the question not have occurred over the previous seven days. Average scores were calculated for each scale. Items rated as “NA–Does not apply” were excluded from analysis.

Putnam et al. (2014) [49] reported an average Cronbach’s alpha of .75 or above for the three broad IBQ-R VSF scales. In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha for the Surgency scale ranged from .70 to .91 across ages, with a mean alpha of .80; Cronbach’s alpha for the Negative Affect scale ranged from .79 to .88 across ages, with a mean alpha of .82; Cronbach’s alpha for the Orienting/Regulatory Capacity scale ranged from .62 to .80 across ages, with a mean alpha of .71. Montirosso et al. (2011) [51] reported average Cronbach’s alphas of .83 and .84 across two samples for the variables of Distress and Falling Reactivity, respectively. An overview of a number of studies using different versions of the IBQ-R revealed that Cronbach’s alpha for Distress and Falling Reactivity ranged between .74 and .79 (M = .76) and between .76 and .84 (M = .80), respectively, when using the short version of the IBQ-R [47]. In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha for the Distress scale ranged from .59 to .70 across ages, with a mean alpha of .64, and Falling Reactivity ranged from .63 to .85 across ages, with a mean alpha of .78 (see S2 Table in S1 File).

The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ).

To investigate the child’s behaviours, emotions and relationships, mothers were asked to complete the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ, [52]) when the child was 36 months old. This consists of 25 items separated into 5 subscales. In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha for the five SDQ subscales were as follows: .65 for Emotional Symptoms, .43 for Conduct Problems, .71 for Hyperactivity-Inattention, .52 for Peer Problems, and .74 for Prosocial Behavior. It should be noted that Cronbach’s alpha was acceptable for all subscales in the large validation sample of 5- to 15-year-olds in Goodman (2001) [53]. The lower alpha for some SDQ scales in our sample could be due to younger children showing less differentiation of behavioural problems. Furthermore, given that all the subscales, except the Hyperactivity-Inattention scale, were not normally distributed (please refer to Supplementary Material in [45]), the lower alphas could also reflect low variance of symptoms in the population sampled, although Goodman et al. (2001) [53] also assessed a general population sample.

Lab observation of mother-infant interaction

Mothers were invited to the lab when their infants were 9 months old. They were shown a play area with age-appropriate toys and books on a floor mat, and asked to play with their infants for a short while (about 10 minutes) as they would normally do at home. All experimenters left the room during this playtime, and the dyads were video recorded with two cameras focusing on the mother and infant, respectively.

Coding of interaction behaviours.

Six consecutive minutes of interaction were coded in one-minute intervals on a 7-point Likert scale. The coding was started from the moment the experimenter was heard to have left the room. The coding system used for this study was adapted from scales previously used [5458]. It comprised six codes, two relating to maternal behaviour, two to infant behaviour, and two to dyadic interaction behaviour. The maternal rating scales included maternal supportive presence (MSP) and maternal respect for infant autonomy (MRIA). The MSP scale concerns the mother’s level of emotional support towards the infant; a high score on this scale would be characterised by expressions of encouragement or positive regard as well as through physical proximity. The MRIA scale reflects the degree to which the parent recognizes and respects the validity of the infant’s individuality, motives, and perspectives. For example, a parent scoring high on this scale would be verbalizing her acknowledgement of the infant’s intentions and negotiating the course of the interactions. The infant rating scales included infant agency / autonomy (IA)–the degree to which the infant shows agency and enthusiasm in the interaction, takes an active interest in his or her activities, invests effort in them, and appreciates successes–and infant negativity (IN)–the amount of negative affect and emotions toward the mother displayed during the interaction. Infants scoring high in IA would show confidence and eagerness to play, while infants high in IN would show anger, dislike, or hostility toward the parent. The dyadic interaction scales included dyadic affective mutuality (DAM), i.e. how strongly the mother-infant dyad shared and understood each other’s emotions, and dyadic reciprocity (DREC), i.e. how strongly the mother’s and infant’s behaviour and verbalisations during the interaction were reciprocated in a conversation-like manner. Specifically, DAM assesses the availability and mutuality of emotion between infant and parent as well as how secure the infant feels with the parent. Dyads high on this scale almost always have a moment of shared pleasure. DREC is characterised by contingent responsiveness, i.e., well-timed behavioural or verbal responses to comments, questions, suggestions or non-verbal cues from the parent and/or the infant. A detailed description of the coding scheme specifically adapted for this study, with examples for each code, can be found in S1 File.

Six undergraduate students, unaware of the questionnaire data, were trained in coding using 10% of the mother-infant interaction videos. They subsequently coded the remaining 90% of videos, with an additional 10% of these videos coded by at least two students to enable continuous interrater reliability monitoring. The final interrater reliability was then calculated for the complete set of videos (i.e., 20%) that were double coded, and yielded the following intraclass correlation (ICC) values: MSP = .581, MRIA = .681, IA = .724, IN = .748, DAM = .741, DREC = .764. Accordingly, apart from the MSP code, all codes either reached acceptable or good interrater reliability. We used all codes for subsequent analyses, but acknowledge that results involving maternal codes, and particularly MSP, should be regarded with some caution due to their relatively lower interrater reliability.

Results

Maternal depression symptom scores obtained from the BDI-II [47], infant temperament scale scores obtained from the IBQ-R VSF [49] and IBQ-R [50], the child behavioural outcome scores obtained from the SDQ [52], and the interaction behaviours obtained from the interaction coding scales are summarised in Table 1 below. All relevant variables were checked for distribution and outliers. Many variables did not meet normality criteria due to (left or right) skew. For subsequent simple correlation analyses, we therefore used Spearman’s r, and multiple regression analyses were always checked for normality (e.g., residual distributions). As several variables included extreme outliers (values beyond 3 standard deviations from the mean), these outliers were winsorised up or down to the respective upper or lower 3 standard deviation boundary before analysis. Each analysis was initially run once with and once without including sociodemographic variables (family annual income, maternal age, maternal years in education, and child gestational age). Since both analyses yielded comparable results, here we only report the analyses without the sociodemographic variables. The analyses including the sociodemographic variables can be found in S8-S11 Tables in S1 File.

thumbnail
Table 1. Average (SD) scores of the measures used in this study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0302661.t001

Associations between interaction behaviours

Simple correlations, Bonferroni-corrected for multiple comparisons (adjusted p-value q< .003), were run to investigate the associations between the interaction coding scales of maternal behaviour (MSP and MRIA), infant behaviour (IA and IN), and dyadic behaviour (DAM and DREC). Results revealed a significant positive correlation between the two maternal behaviour scales MSP and MRIA (r = .518, p < .001), and between the two dyadic behaviour scales DAM and DREC (r = .568, p < .001). Furthermore, the infant behaviour scale IA correlated positively with the two dyadic behaviour scales DAM (r = .417, p = .002) and DREC (r = .454, p < .001). Results are summarised in S3 Table in S1 File.

Associations between interaction behaviours and maternal depressive symptoms

Correlational analyses were conducted to test for associations between maternal depressive symptom scores derived from the BDI-II [47], averaged across the first 9 months, and behaviours obtained from the interaction coding scales. Because there was only one independent variable in this analysis (i.e., BDI-II scores), we used a simple Spearman correlation. No significant associations were found. Results are summarised in S4 Table in S1 File. However, when socio-economic status variables (family annual income, maternal age, maternal years in education, and child gestational age) were included in the analyses, we observed a negative association between maternal depressive symptoms and MRIA (r = -.297, p = .047) and a positive association between maternal depressive symptoms and IN (r = .335, p = .024). Neither of these correlations, however, remained significant after multiple comparisons correction. Results are summarised in S9 Table in S1 File.

Associations between interaction behaviours and infant temperament

Regression analyses were conducted to explore associations between the 6 interaction behaviour coding scales and the 5 infant temperament scales from the IBQ-R VSF [49] and IBQ-R [50]. The scores of the temperament scales were averaged across the 4 assessment time points in the first 9 months of life. We conducted one multiple regression analysis for each interaction coding scale and subsequently Bonferroni-corrected all significance values for multiple comparisons–i.e., the number of interaction coding scales (adjusted p-value q < .0083). The overall multiple regression model was significant for the two dyadic interaction codes DAM, p < .007, and DREC, p < .001, but not for any of the maternal or infant codes. DAM was positively associated with Falling Reactivity (p = .004), and no other significant association was found with any of the other temperament scales. DREC was associated with Orienting / Regulatory Capacity (p = .04) and with Falling Reactivity (p = .05) but after controlling for multiple comparisons, these associations were no longer significant. Results of the significant multiple regression models are summarised in Table 2, and the significant association between DAM and Falling Reactivity is illustrated in Fig 1. Results of all the multiple regression models are summarised in S5 Table in S1 File.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Illustration of the significant association between Dyadic Affective Mutuality (DAM) and infant falling reactivity.

Note. Dyadic Affective Mutuality (DAM) obtained from the interaction coding scales and infant Falling Reactivity measured with the Infant Behavior Questionnaire–Revised (IBQ-R; [50]).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0302661.g001

thumbnail
Table 2. Associations between dyadic interaction behaviours and infant temperament.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0302661.t002

An additional correlation analysis, conducted to better understand at what time point(s) in the first months of life infant Falling Reactivity was associated with DAM (observed during the interaction at 9 months) showed that all correlations were significant except the one involving Falling Reactivity at 2 weeks (see S6 Table in S1 File).

Associations between interaction behaviours and child behavioural outcomes

Regression analyses were conducted to explore associations between the 6 interaction behaviour coding scales and the 5 child behavioural outcomes as assessed with the SDQ [52] at 3 years of age. We conducted one multiple regression analysis for each interaction coding scale. Only the overall multiple regression model for MRIA was significant (p = .03) (Table 3), although it did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (adjusted p-value q < .0083). This revealed a significant association between MRIA at 9 months and Peer Problems at 3 years (p = .003) (Fig 2). None of the other multiple regression models was significant (See S7 Table in S1 File).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Illustration of the significant association between Maternal Respect for Infant Autonomy (MRIA) and child peer problems.

Note. Maternal Respect for Infant Autonomy (MRIA) obtained from the interaction coding scales and Peer Problems as measured by the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ, [52]).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0302661.g002

thumbnail
Table 3. Associations between Maternal Respect for Infant Autonomy (MRIA) and child behavioural outcomes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0302661.t003

Discussion

Existent literature suggests that the behaviours observed in mother-infant interactions are associated with both maternal mental health and infant individual characteristics, and they also predict later socio-emotional child outcomes. However, it is unclear which components and dyadic patterns of the observed behaviours associate with each of these characteristics and outcomes. In this longitudinal study, we observed mothers interacting with their 9-month-old infants and employed an interaction coding scheme that comprised codes specific to behaviours of each interaction partner as well as to the dyadic interaction behaviour. This allowed us to investigate how each of these behaviours associated with i) maternal depressive symptoms in the first 9 months postpartum, ii) infant temperament in the first 9 months of life and iii) child behaviour at 3 years of age. Our results indicated 1) a lack of association between interaction behaviours and maternal depressive symptoms, 2) a positive association between infant falling reactivity and dyadic affective mutuality, and 3) a negative association between maternal respect for infant autonomy and child peer problems at 3 years of age. We will discuss each of these findings below.

Our analyses focusing on maternal depressive symptoms did not reveal any significant association with the behaviours observed during the interaction session when socio-economic status (SES) variables (family annual income, maternal age, maternal years in education, and child gestational age) were excluded from the analyses. This is interesting as previous literature shows that mothers’ mental health difficulties may profoundly affect their ability to engage in dyadic interactions with their infants. For example, mothers suffering from depressive symptoms might show less sensitivity or responsiveness, and more difficulties in reading their infant’s facial expressions, affecting their ability to attune to their infant’s internal states [5961]. Previous studies have also found that dyads where the mother is depressed spend more time in negative behavioural states and that the mother tends to react more negatively to their infant’s negative behaviour [23,62]. Our own previous findings from this sample, based on questionnaire data alone [44], showed cascading associations between maternal depressive symptoms and infant negative affect across the first year of life, and primarily during the first 6 months of life, with most of these effects being maternally driven (i.e., earlier maternal depression predicted later infant negative affect, but not the other way round). Other previous studies have also found that infants of depressed mothers are characterized by more negative affect [e.g., 60,63,64], and a recent study replicated the association between maternal depression and infant negative affect at 10 months, whereas this association had disappeared by 16 months [65]. Thus, this specific association between maternal depressive symptoms and the construct of infant negative affect might be stronger in early infancy and start weakening by the end of the first year of life. It is possible that the coding scales employed in this study were not focused on the same elements that defined maternal sensitivity or responsiveness in previous studies, nor infant negative affect as measured in the IBQ-R VSF [49]. For example, infant negativity in the interaction coding scheme employed here was assessed as the degree to which the infant showed anger, dislike, or hostility toward the parent. This is indeed different from what is measured in the IBQ-R VSF [49] where infant negative affect relates to infants’ reactions in unfamiliar or potentially stressful situations. Another key difference is that the IBQ-R VSF [49] measure is based on observations across multiple different situations summarised by a single score. The mother-infant interaction observed in this study, on the other hand, provides a snapshot of behaviours in a very specific situation and may therefore not tap into the full range of negative affect behaviours that would be observed across a range of circumstances.

Despite these differences, it is worth noting that when demographics of SES (family annual income, maternal age, maternal years in education, and child gestational age) were considered in the analyses, we found a positive association between maternal depressive symptoms reported in the first 9 months post-partum and infant negativity during the interaction observed in this study. We also found a negative association between maternal depressive symptoms and maternal respect for infant autonomy. While neither of these correlations remained significant after multiple comparisons correction, existent literature suggests that SES is an important factor that impacts on both maternal mental health, generally reporting higher SES levels associated with better maternal mental health (e.g. [66]), and on child development [67,68]. For example, high parental educational level and family income are key protective components of the parent–child relationship [69], and are also related to developmental outcomes, such as language and literacy skills [70], fine motor development [71] and school readiness [72]. SES can impact child development also indirectly by influencing proximal factors, such as parenting attitudes. For example, research suggests that higher maternal education level has a positive influence on parenting strategies because children are more likely to be exposed to complex vocabulary and books, and mothers are more likely to have greater knowledge of child development and higher educational expectations for her own children [7375]. Our findings suggest that when SES variables, such as family annual income, maternal age, maternal education, and child gestational age, are included in the correlational analyses, higher maternal depressive symptoms are associated with higher infant negative affectivity and also with fewer occurrences of maternal respect of the validity of the infant’s individuality, desires and perspectives during the interaction.

Our second set of analyses focused on associations between observed behaviours in the interaction and infant temperament. The results suggest that the dyad’s ability to share and understand each other’s emotions is associated with higher infant falling reactivity in the first year of life. Falling reactivity describes to what extent an infant is capable of recovering from a high level of distress or even positive excitement and, therefore, the infant’s ability to self-regulate [50]. Feldman (2003, 2007) [76,77] emphasized the importance of sharing positive affect in parent–infant interaction, and claimed that synchronized positive affect is essential for the development of self-regulation. The dyad’s ability to share and communicate emotions to each other, alongside the caregiver’s empathic understanding of the infant’s affective state, are indeed recognised steps in the process of emotion regulation in the literature [e.g., 78,79]. While it is difficult to define a sole directionality of the association between infant temperament and the dyad’s behaviour, our data hint to an infant-driven effect where infants with high self-regulation skills from an early age engage with the parent in a more positive way. Indeed, our additional correlational analyses (reported in S1 File) revealed that infant falling reactivity predicts later dyadic affective mutuality (here observed at 9 months) from 4 months of age. Critically though, in order to establish the directionality of these associations, future studies should include mother-infant observations also at earlier time points. The dyadic affective mutuality code employed in this study specifically assessed availability and mutuality of emotion between mother and infant, how secure the child felt with the parent, and verbal and non-verbal communication of the two interaction partners. In dyads high on this scale, both emotion and communication flew freely, and smiles, eye contact, proximity seeking and help seeking behaviours were frequently observed. There was at least one shared occurrence of reciprocally communicated positive emotion, however, both positive and negative emotions were shared. With this study, we show that infants who have better abilities to cope with distress and/or overwhelming emotions, and are therefore better able to self-regulate, experience more of this kind of positive interaction with their caregiver.

Finally, our findings suggest that maternal respect for infant autonomy promotes a better social and emotional development in early childhood. Specifically, we found that infants whose mothers recognized and respected the validity of the infant’s individuality, desires and perspectives during the interaction, go on to develop fewer peer problems at preschool age. One interpretation of this finding is that infants learn through the interactions with their mothers how to respect other people’s autonomy and identity, which in turn may help them make friends, be liked by others and negotiate challenging social situations. Autonomy is considered a fundamental child need and parental support for autonomy is arguably one of the most important aspects of parenting for the development of child independence [80]. Requests for autonomy are often found among the most frequent cues in toddlers’ interactions with both their mother and father (e.g. [81]), highlighting the emerging need for autonomy in the transition from infancy to toddlerhood. Similar to Mäntymaa et al.’s (2006) [33] finding that shared pleasure in early mother–infant interaction was a protective factor against internalizing and externalizing problems at 2 years, our results suggest that maternal respect for infant autonomy in the first year of life acts as a protective factor against child peer problems at 3 years of age. A study with older children also reported that a longer duration of positive interactions between parent and child at 3 years was associated with fewer conduct problems at 4 years [82], therefore supporting the view that more positive early interactions are associated with lower levels of socio-emotional and behavioural problems later in development. These conclusions are also supported by a meta-analysis by Pinquart (2017) [83] who examined the relations between parent-child interactions and child externalizing behaviours. This work reports unidirectional longitudinal associations between increases in parental support and lower levels of externalizing behaviours [83]. Interestingly, Hughes et al. [84] also report an inverse association between maternal autonomy support at 14 months and externalizing behaviour at 2 years but only for those infants who showed low levels of negative affect at 4 months. The authors discuss their findings in relation to models of vantage sensitivity [e.g., 85], i.e., infants with easy temperaments may gain most from high quality maternal support. However, they also suggest the possibility that their findings could reflect child-driven effects [86], i.e., mothers of infants prone to distress may have attempted to minimize frustration by providing greater assistance during the interaction and thereby inadvertently limited their child’s autonomy. In addition, another recent study [87] reported differential patterns of associations between toddler temperament, parental stress and support for autonomy depending on the temperamental characteristics. Parent stress was found to fully account for the negative association between toddler negative affectivity and parent autonomy support, suggesting that toddler temperament affects parent wellbeing, which in turn is related to parenting practices. While our sample size does not allow for testing these interpretations, taken together the evidence shows that parental autonomy support is crucial for healthy child adjustment [88], and in particular during toddlerhood [81,87].

Our work is not without limitations. For instance, a larger sample size would have allowed for deeper investigation of the complex inter-relations among the interaction elements and the individual temperamental characteristics of infants and their mothers’ wellbeing. Collecting information from additional observers of the child (e.g., other family members, educators, etc.) would have allowed a more objective assessment of the infant temperament as well as the child’s social and emotional outcome at 3 years of age. Further, our study only included a single observation of mother-infant interaction, therefore limiting the generalizability of these findings to a specific point in time. The dynamics in play during mother-infant interactions are likely to change with infant age, making it important to track how these changes are related to more stable temperamental traits and maternal mental health. Future research should therefore include a larger sample and a mother-infant interaction protocol at multiple ages. This would also allow for investigation of potential bi-directional and transactional effects over time. Relatedly, collecting measures of prenatal maternal depressive symptoms as well as considering other (prenatal) biological factors will also be important in future studies. Finally, while the focus of this study was on the interactions infants experience with their mother, it would also be valuable to further our understanding of how interactions with fathers and other caregivers influence infant and child development.

Despite these limitations, with this study we show important links between infant falling reactivity and the exchanges of emotion between mother and infant, and between maternal respect for infant autonomy and child social adjustment at 3 years of age. This type of information is important for defining and implementing intervention strategies that will facilitate and promote a healthy mother-infant relationship from the very beginning. For example, given the association between the mother’s respect for infant autonomy and her child’s later social development, interventions could aim to enhance mothers’ attention to the acknowledgement of her infant’s intentions and desires. Similarly, mothers could benefit from being aware of the importance of sharing their affective states with their infants and of the association with infant self-regulatory abilities. Altogether, a better understanding of the transactional associations between maternal wellbeing, infant temperament, and interaction behaviours could benefit the development of healthier mutual relationships.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the families who took part in the study and thereby made this research possible.

References

  1. 1. Rocha NA, dos Santos Silva FP, Dos Santos MM, Dusing SC. Impact of mother–infant interaction on development during the first year of life: A systematic review. Journal of Child Health Care. 2020 Sep;24(3):365–85. pmid:31337225
  2. 2. Saltaris C, Serbin L, Stack D, Karp J, Schwartzman A, Ledingham J. Nurturing cognitive competence in preschoolers: A longitudinal study of intergenerational continuity and risk. Int J Behav Dev. 2004 Mar 1;28(2):105–15.
  3. 3. Noll LM, Harding CG. The relationship of mother–child interaction and the child’s development of symbolic play. Infant Ment Health J: Official Publication of The World Association for Infant Mental Health. 2003 Nov;24(6):557–70.
  4. 4. Stams GJ, Juffer F, Van IJzendoorn MH. Maternal sensitivity, infant attachment, and temperament in early childhood predict adjustment in middle childhood: the case of adopted children and their biologically unrelated parents. Dev Psychol. 2002 Sep;38(5):806. pmid:12220057
  5. 5. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Mothers’ and fathers’ support for child autonomy and early school achievement. Dev Psychol. 2008; 44(4), 895–907.
  6. 6. Feldman R, Eidelman AI, Rotenberg N. Parenting stress, infant emotion regulation, maternal sensitivity, and the cognitive development of triplets: A model for parent and child influences in a unique ecology. Child Dev. 2004 Dec;75(6):1774–91. pmid:15566379
  7. 7. Ainsworth MD, Bell SM, Stayton DJ. Individual differences in strange-situation behaviour of one-year old: The Origins of Human Social Relations. New York: Academic Press; 1971.
  8. 8. Calkins SD, Hill A. Caregiver influences on emerging emotion regulation. Handbook of emotion regulation. 2007;229248:229–48.
  9. 9. Leerkes EM, Blankson AN, O’Brien M. Differential effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social‐emotional functioning. Child Dev. 2009 May;80(3):762–75. pmid:19489902
  10. 10. Bernier A, Carlson SM, Whipple N. From external regulation to self‐regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Dev. 2010 Jan;81(1):326–39. pmid:20331670
  11. 11. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of Attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
  12. 12. De Wolff MS, Van Ijzendoorn MH. Sensitivity and attachment: A meta‐analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Dev. 1997 Aug;68(4):571–91. pmid:9306636
  13. 13. Braungart‐Rieker JM, Garwood MM, Powers BP, Wang X. Parental sensitivity, infant affect, and affect regulation: Predictors of later attachment. Child Dev. 2001 Jan;72(1):252–70. pmid:11280483
  14. 14. Isabella RA, Belsky J, von Eye A. Origins of infant-mother attachment: An examination of interactional synchrony during the infant’s first year. Dev Psychol. 1989 Jan;25(1):12.
  15. 15. Campbell SB, Brownell CA, Hungerford A, Spieker SJ, Mohan R, Blessing JS. The course of maternal depressive symptoms and maternal sensitivity as predictors of attachment security at 36 months. Dev Psychopathol. 2004 Jun;16(2):231–52. pmid:15487594
  16. 16. Moehler E, Brunner R, Wiebel A, Reck C, Resch F. Maternal depressive symptoms in the postnatal period are associated with long-term impairment of mother–child bonding. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2006 Sep;9:273–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00737-006-0149-5 pmid:16937313
  17. 17. Ohoka H, Koide T, Goto S, Murase S, Kanai A, Masuda T, et al. Effects of maternal depressive symptomatology during pregnancy and the postpartum period on infant–mother attachment. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014 Aug;68(8):631–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12171 pmid:24521214
  18. 18. Righetti-Veltema M, Conne-Perréard E, Bousquet A, Manzano J. Postpartum depression and mother–infant relationship at 3 months old. Journal of affective disorders. 2002 Aug 1;70(3):291–306. pmid:12128241
  19. 19. Cohn JF, Campbell SB, Matias R, Hopkins J. Face-to-face interactions of postpartum depressed and nondepressed mother-infant pairs at 2 months. Dev Psychol. 1990 Jan;26(1):15.
  20. 20. Hakanen H, Flykt M, Sinervä E, Nolvi S, Kataja EL, Pelto J, et al. How maternal pre-and postnatal symptoms of depression and anxiety affect early mother-infant interaction? JAD. 2019 Oct 1;257:83–90. pmid:31299408
  21. 21. Parfitt Y, Pike A, Ayers S. The impact of parents’ mental health on parent–baby interaction: A prospective study. Infant Behav Dev. 2013 Dec 1;36(4):599–608. pmid:23850989
  22. 22. Stein A, Craske MG, Lehtonen A, Harvey A, Savage-McGlynn E, Davies B, et al. Maternal cognitions and mother–infant interaction in postnatal depression and generalized anxiety disorder. J Abnorm Psychology. 2012 Nov;121(4):795. pmid:22288906
  23. 23. Field T. Infants of depressed mothers. Dev Psychopathol. 1992 Jan;4(1):49–66.
  24. 24. Field T, Diego M, Hernandez-Reif M. Depressed mothers’ infants are less responsive to faces and voices. Infant Behav Dev. 2009 Jun 1;32(3):239–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.03.005 pmid:19439359
  25. 25. Stright AD, Gallagher KC, Kelley K. Infant temperament moderates relations between maternal parenting in early childhood and children’s adjustment in first grade. Child Dev. 2008 Jan;79(1):186–200. pmid:18269517
  26. 26. Foreman DM, Henshaw C. Objectivity and subjectivity in postnatally depressed mothers’ perceptions of their infants. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2002 Jun;32:263–75. pmid:12022770
  27. 27. Seifer R, Sameroff A, Dickstein S, Schiller M, Hayden LC. Your own children are special: Clues to the sources of reporting bias in temperament assessments. Infant Behav Dev. 2004 Oct 1;27(3):323–41.
  28. 28. Siddiqui A, Hägglöf B. Does maternal prenatal attachment predict postnatal mother–infant interaction? Early Hum Dev. 2000 Jul 1;59(1):13–25. pmid:10962164
  29. 29. Milliones J. Relationship between perceived child temperament and maternal behaviors. Child Dev. 1978 Dec 1:1255–7.
  30. 30. Campbell SB. Mother-infant interaction as a function of maternal ratings of temperament. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 1979 Dec;10(2):67–76. pmid:527392
  31. 31. Murray L, Stanley C, Hooper R, King F, Fiori‐Cowley A. The role of infant factors in postnatal depression and mother‐infant interactions. DMCN.1996 Feb;38(2):109–19. pmid:8603778
  32. 32. Zeanah CH, Keener MA, Anders TF. Developing perceptions of temperament and their children to mother and infant behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1986 Jul;27(4):499–512.
  33. 33. Mäntymaa M, Puura K, Luoma I, Salmelin RK, Tamminen T. Mother’s early perception of her infant’s difficult temperament, parenting stress and early mother–infant interaction. Nordic J Psychiatry. 2006 Jan 1;60(5):379–86. pmid:17050296
  34. 34. Crockenberg SB, Smith P. Antecedents of mother–infant interaction and infant irritability in the first 3 months of life. Infant Behav Dev. 2002 Jan 1;25(1):2–15.
  35. 35. Costa R, Figueiredo B. Infants’ behavioral and physiological profile and mother–infant interaction. Int J Behav Dev. 2012 May;36(3):205–14.
  36. 36. Mäntymaa M, Puura K, Luoma I, Latva R, Salmelin RK, Tamminen T. Shared pleasure in early mother–infant interaction: Predicting lower levels of emotional and behavioral problems in the child and protecting against the influence of parental psychopathology. Infant Ment Health J. 2015 Mar;36(2):223–37. pmid:25739800
  37. 37. Kemppinen K, Kumpulainen K, Raita‐Hasu J, Moilanen I, Ebeling H. The continuity of maternal sensitivity from infancy to toddler age. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology. 2006 Aug 1;24(3):199–212.
  38. 38. Bernier A, Calkins SD, Bell MA. Longitudinal associations between the quality of mother–infant interactions and brain development across infancy. Child Dev. 2016 Jul;87(4):1159–74. pmid:27040719
  39. 39. Bernier A, Dégeilh F, Leblanc É, Daneault V, Bailey HN, Beauchamp MH. Mother–infant interaction and child brain morphology: A multidimensional approach to maternal sensitivity. Infancy. 2019 Mar;24(2):120–38. pmid:32677197
  40. 40. Sethna V, Pote I, Wang S, Gudbrandsen M, Blasi A, McCusker C, et al. Mother–infant interactions and regional brain volumes in infancy: an MRI study. Brain Struct Funct. 2017 Jul;222:2379–88. pmid:27915378
  41. 41. Siegel DJ. Toward an interpersonal neurobiology of the developing mind: Attachment relationships,“mindsight,” and neural integration. Infant Ment Health J: official publication of the world association for infant mental health. 2001 Jan;22(1‐2):67–94.
  42. 42. Siegel DJ. The developing mind: How relationships and the brain interact to shape who we are. Guilford Publications; 2020 May 27.
  43. 43. Lecuyer-Maus EA. Maternal sensitivity and responsiveness, limit-setting style, and relationship history in the transition to toddlerhood. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing. 2000 Jan 1;23(2):117–39. pmid:11111501
  44. 44. Rigato S, Stets M, Bonneville‐Roussy A, Holmboe K. Impact of maternal depressive symptoms on the development of infant temperament: Cascading effects during the first year of life. Soc Dev. 2020 Nov;29(4):1115–33.
  45. 45. Rigato S, Charalambous S, Stets M, Holmboe K. Maternal depressive symptoms and infant temperament in the first year of life predict child behavior at 36 months of age. Infant Behav Dev. 2022 May 1;67:101717. pmid:35452976
  46. 46. Rigato S, Stets M, Charalambous S, Dvergsdal H, Holmboe K. Infant visual preference for the mother’s face and longitudinal associations with emotional reactivity in the first year of life. Sci Rep. 2023 Jun 24;13(1):10263. pmid:37355764
  47. 47. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1996
  48. 48. Wang YP, Gorenstein C. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory-II: a comprehensive review. Brazilian J Psychiatry. 2013 Oct;35:416–31. pmid:24402217
  49. 49. Putnam SP, Helbig AL, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK, Leerkes E. Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the Infant Behavior Questionnaire–Revised. J Pers Assess. 2014 Jul 4;96(4):445–58. pmid:24206185
  50. 50. Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav Dev. 2003 Feb 1;26(1):64–86.
  51. 51. Montirosso R, Cozzi P, Putnam SP, Gartstein MA, Borgatti R. Studying cross-cultural differences in temperament in the first year of life: United States and Italy. Int J Behav Dev. 2011 Jan;35(1):27–37.
  52. 52. Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. JCPP. 1997 Jul;38(5):581–6. pmid:9255702
  53. 53. Goodman R. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001 Nov 1;40(11):1337–45. pmid:11699809
  54. 54. Deater-Deckard K, Pylas MV, Petrill SA. The Parent-child interaction system (PARCHISY). London: Institute of Psychiatry. 1997.
  55. 55. Egeland B, Hiester M. Teaching task rating scales. Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota. 1993.
  56. 56. Owen MT, Vaughn A, Barfoot B, Ware A. The NICHD study of early child care parent-child interaction scales: Early childhood. University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX. 1996.
  57. 57. Pianta RC. Rating scales for parent-child interaction in preschoolers. University of Virginia. 1994.
  58. 58. Hirschmann N. INTAKT. University of Vienna. 2013.
  59. 59. Arteche A, Joormann J, Harvey A, Craske M, Gotlib IH, Lehtonen A, Counsell N, Stein A. The effects of postnatal maternal depression and anxiety on the processing of infant faces. JAD. 2011 Sep 1;133(1–2):197–203. pmid:21641652
  60. 60. Feldman R, Granat AD, Pariente C, Kanety H, Kuint J, Gilboa-Schechtman E. Maternal depression and anxiety across the postpartum year and infant social engagement, fear regulation, and stress reactivity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009 Sep 1;48(9):919–27. pmid:19625979
  61. 61. Murray L, Fiori‐Cowley A, Hooper R, Cooper P. The impact of postnatal depression and associated adversity on early mother‐infant interactions and later infant outcome. Child Dev. 1996 Oct;67(5):2512–26. pmid:9022253
  62. 62. Field T, Hernandez-Reif M, Diego M. Intrusive and withdrawn depressed mothers and their infants. Dev Review. 2006 Mar 1;26(1):15–30.
  63. 63. McMahon C, Barnett B, Kowalenko N, Tennant C, Don N. Postnatal depression, anxiety and unsettled infant behaviour. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2001 Oct;35(5):581–8. pmid:11551272
  64. 64. Whiffen VE, Gotlib IH. Infants of postpartum depressed mothers: temperament and cognitive status. J Abnorm Psychol. 1989 Aug;98(3):274. pmid:2768663
  65. 65. Fiske A, Scerif G, Holmboe K. Maternal depressive symptoms and early childhood temperament before and during the COVID‐19 pandemic in the United Kingdom. Infant Child Dev. 2022 Nov;31(6):e2354.
  66. 66. Smith TA, Kievit RA, Astle DE. Maternal mental health mediates links between socioeconomic status and child development. Current Psychology. 2023 Sep;42(25):21967–78. pmid:37692883
  67. 67. Poehlmann-Tynan J, Gerstein ED, Burnson C, Weymouth L, Bolt DM, Maleck S, et al. Risk and resilience in preterm children at age 6. Development and Psychopathology. 2015 Aug;27(3):843–58. pmid:25196017
  68. 68. Shah R, Sobotka SA, Chen YF, Msall ME. Positive parenting practices, health disparities, and developmental progress. Pediatrics. 2015 Aug 1;136(2):318–26. pmid:26216325
  69. 69. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Grantham-McGregor S, Black MM, Nelson CA, Huffman SL, et al. Inequality in early childhood: risk and protective factors for early child development. The Lancet. 2011 Oct 8;378(9799):1325–38. pmid:21944375
  70. 70. Rowe ML, Denmark N, Harden BJ, Stapleton LM. The role of parent education and parenting knowledge in children’s language and literacy skills among White, Black, and Latino families. Infant and Child Development. 2016 Mar;25(2):198–220.
  71. 71. Ye A, Yan S, Huang K, Mao L, Ge X, Weng T, et al. Maternal intelligence quotient and motor development in early childhood: The mediating role of mother’s education. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health. 2019 Jan;55(1):87–94. pmid:30051946
  72. 72. Wolf S, McCoy DC. Household socioeconomic status and parental investments: Direct and indirect relations with school readiness in Ghana. Child Development. 2019 Jan;90(1):260–78. pmid:28708296
  73. 73. Chin T, Phillips M. Social reproduction and child-rearing practices: Social class, children’s agency, and the summer activity gap. Sociology of Education. 2004 Jul;77(3):185–210.
  74. 74. Davis-Kean P. E. (2005). The influence of parent education and family income on child achievement: the indirect role of parental expectations and the home environment. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(2), 294. pmid:15982107
  75. 75. Ertem IO, Atay G, Dogan DG, Bayhan A, Bingoler BE, Gok CG, et al. Mothers’ knowledge of young child development in a developing country. Child: care, health and development. 2007 Nov;33(6):728–37. pmid:17944782
  76. 76. Feldman R. Infant–mother and infant–father synchrony: The coregulation of positive arousal. Infant Ment Health J: Official Publication of The World Association for Infant Mental Health. 2003 Jan;24(1):1–23.
  77. 77. Feldman R. Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. JCPP. 2007 Mar;48(3‐4):329–54. pmid:17355401
  78. 78. Manian N, Bornstein MH. Dynamics of emotion regulation in infants of clinically depressed and nondepressed mothers. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009 Nov;50(11):1410–8. pmid:19788549
  79. 79. Schore AN. Effects of a secure attachment relationship on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant Ment Health J: official publication of the world association for infant mental health. 2001 Jan;22(1-2):7–66.
  80. 80. Joussemet M, Landry R, Koestner R. A self-determination theory perspective on parenting. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne. 2008 Aug;49(3):194.
  81. 81. Ballarotto G, Murray L, Bozicevic L, Marzilli E, Cerniglia L, Cimino S, et al. Parental sensitivity to toddler’s need for autonomy: An empirical study on mother-toddler and father-toddler interactions during feeding and play. Infant Behavior and Development. 2023 Nov 1;73:101892. pmid:37839158
  82. 82. Gardner F, Ward S, Burton J, Wilson C. The role of mother–child joint play in the early development of children’s conduct problems: A longitudinal observational study. Soc Dev. 2003 Aug;12(3):361–78.
  83. 83. Pinquart M. Associations of parenting dimensions and styles with externalizing problems of children and adolescents: An updated meta-analysis. Dev Psychol. 2017 May;53(5):873. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000295 pmid:28459276
  84. 84. Hughes C, Devine RT, Mesman J, Blair C. Understanding the terrible twos: A longitudinal investigation of the impact of early executive function and parent–child interactions. Dev Sci. 2020 Nov;23(6):e12979. pmid:32353921
  85. 85. Pluess M, Belsky J. Vantage sensitivity: individual differences in response to positive experiences. Psychological bulletin. 2013 Jul;139(4):901. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030196 pmid:23025924
  86. 86. Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007 Dec;16(6):300–4.
  87. 87. Andreadakis E., Laurin J.C., Joussemet M. et al. Toddler Temperament, Parent Stress, and Autonomy Support. J Child Fam Stud 29, 3029–3043 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-020-01793-3.
  88. 88. Ryan RM, Deci EL, Vansteenkiste M. Autonomy and autonomy disturbances in self-development and psychopathology: Research on motivation, attachment, and clinical process. Developmental psychopathology. 2016 Feb 29;1:385–438.