Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Investigating gene-environment interaction on attention in a double-hit model for Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Melvin Maroon,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Neuroscience Graduate Program, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada

  • Faraj Haddad,

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Neuroscience Graduate Program, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada

  • Ella Doornaert,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation

    Affiliation Neuroscience Graduate Program, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada

  • Brian Allman,

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision

    Affiliations Neuroscience Graduate Program, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada, Anatomy & Cell Biology, Schulich School of Medicine & Dentistry, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada

  • Susanne Schmid

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Susanne.schmid@schulich.uwo.ca

    Current address: Schulich School of Medicine & Dentistry, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada

    Affiliations Neuroscience Graduate Program, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada, Anatomy & Cell Biology, Schulich School of Medicine & Dentistry, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada, Psychology, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada

Abstract

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental behavioral disorder characterized by social, communicative, and motor deficits. There is no single etiological cause for ASD, rather, there are various genetic and environmental factors that increase the risk for ASD. It is thought that some of these factors influence the same underlying neural mechanisms, and that an interplay of both genetic and environmental factors would better explain the pathogenesis of ASD. To better appreciate the influence of genetic-environment interaction on ASD-related behaviours, rats lacking a functional copy of the ASD-linked gene Cntnap2 were exposed to maternal immune activation (MIA) during pregnancy and assessed in adolescence and adulthood. We hypothesized that Cntnap2 deficiency interacts with poly I:C MIA to aggravate ASD-like symptoms in the offspring. In this double-hit model, we assessed attention, a core deficit in ASD due to prefrontal cortical dysfunction. We employed a well-established attentional paradigm known as the 5-choice serial reaction time task (5CSRTT). Cntnap2-/- rats exhibited greater perseverative responses which is indicative of repetitive behaviors. Additionally, rats exposed to poly I:C MIA exhibited premature responses, a marker of impulsivity. The rats exposed to both the genetic and environmental challenge displayed an increase in impulsive activity; however, this response was only elicited in the presence of an auditory distractor. This implies that exacerbated symptomatology in the double-hit model may situation-dependent and not generally expressed.

Introduction

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental behavioral disorder affecting approximately one in 100 children worldwide [1]. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) criterion for ASD focuses on deficits in social behavior and communication, as well as restricted interests and repetitive actions [2]. Intellectual disabilities and abnormal processing of sensory input are also seen in individuals with ASD [2]. Accordingly, past research has reliably shown that individuals with ASD demonstrate difficulty accurately perceiving social cues and exhibit inflexible behavior [35]. Inflexible behavior is commonly seen through repetitive motor actions and strict custom routines [6, 7]. Presently, ASD is predominantly diagnosed through behavioral assessment [2]. Due to the number of potential risk factors, both genetic and environmental, it is valuable to investigate their roles in the emergence of ASD-like symptomatology.

The emergence of genetic sequencing technology has helped researchers elucidate the bases of neurobiological disorders that were previously categorized as idiopathic [8]. Through large-scale sequencing cohorts, researchers have discovered that the genetic basis of ASD is widely heterogenous. ASD can be caused by single-gene (monogenic) mutations. For instance, a mutation in the X-linked FMR1 gene, commonly known as Fragile X Syndrome, accounts for approximately 2% of identified cases [9]. Contrarily, rare and common variants are not necessarily pathogenic [10, 11]. The impact of such risk factors is dependent on the type of variant, the modified genes, and the compounded effect of additional genetic and environmental impacts [12, 13]. For instance, a large-scale genome-wide association study discovered 102 common variants associated with ASD; however, they only account for around 3.5% of the total heritability of the disorder [14].

Beyond genetics, many sources discuss the influence of environmental factors on the etiological basis of ASD; these include parental age, maternal medication use, and post-natal family environment [15, 16]. More recently, the maternal inflammatory response, triggered by a viral infection, has been found to harmfully affect fetal development [17]. While certain rare pathogens pose distinctive threats to the brain, the infection caused by several viruses result in a similar risk for ASD development. This implies that the induced maternal immune activation (MIA) is the core mechanism impacting the fetus, as opposed to the virus itself [1820]. The MIA hypothesis has been validated by activating the immune system with a range of pathogens during pregnancy. Subsequently, the offspring are assessed for corresponding symptomatology that is attributed to human neurodevelopmental disorders [19, 21]. A greater understanding of the effects of maternal immune reaction on fetal brain development is imperative, especially with the ongoing after-effects of the COVID-19 pandemic.

With the growing repertoire of genetic and environmental risk factors attributed to ASD, more studies have begun characterizing double-hit gene-environment models for neurodevelopmental disorders [22, 23]. For example, a national surveying study in Sweden revealed that ASD heritability is at approximately 50%, reinforcing the proportionally impactful role of the environment [24]. Additionally, in a study centered around maternal metabolic and inflammatory conditions, children with diabetic mothers had an increased risk of ASD onset compared to children with a genetic predisposition alone [25]. Nonetheless, each human is genetically and behaviorally dissimilar, undermining potential risk-factor interactions [26]. Animal models alone can substantiate the claim that environmental risk factors exacerbate genetic predispositions [26]. The present study aimed to characterize a double-hit gene-environment rodent model for ASD, particularly focusing on the process of attention.

The genetic factor in our present model is the loss-of-function of the Contactin-associated protein-like 2 (CNTNAP2) gene. CNTNAP2 is highly expressed in brain areas implicated in ASD and poses a risk through both common and rare variations [27, 28]. CNTNAP2 codes for a cell adhesion molecule involved in synaptic formation, cortex organization and neuronal migratory function [29, 30]. Consequences of CNTNAP2 loss-of-function mutations were uncovered by Strauss et al. [31] in a population of Old Order Amish children. Out of the affected population, 70% exhibited ASD-associated symptoms [31]. The preclinical Sprague-Dawley rodent model with a homozygous gene knockout (Cntnap2 KO) displays endophenotypes including hyperactivity, repetitive behaviors, and reduced vocalizations [32, 33]. For the environmental challenge, we used polyinosinic polycytidylic acid (poly I:C) to trigger an antiviral-like immune response in pregnant dams. Poly I:C elicits its effects through toll-like 3 receptors (TLR3), a highly conserved innate immune receptor [34, 35]. Due to the proven efficacy of the Sprague-Dawley poly I:C model, we studied the effect on attention, an ASD related behavior, in combination with the Cntnap2 risk factor.

Visuospatial attention is an integral characteristic when assessing social-communicative deficits attributed to ASD [36]. Selective attention and orienting have a critical role in cognitive development and can even play a role in regulating emotional states in humans [37]. Prior studies have found that children with ASD have lessened attention, particularly to facial cues; however, most rodent models focus on non-salient visual cues [38, 39]. Based on previous reports on attentional deficits in ASD, we hypothesized that Cntnap2 deficiency interacts with poly I:C immune activation to exacerbate ASD related attentional changes exhibited by each model alone. We employed the 5-choice serial reaction time task (5CSRTT), a standard rodent behavioral task used to assess attention [40]. The 5-CSRTT was developed as the rodent equivalent to Leonard’s choice reaction time task in the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery [41]. We predicted that the double-hit model displays poorer performance on the 5CSRT task compared to either single-hit model.

Materials and methods

Animals

The study was conducted with 19 wildtype (WT) and 22 homozygous knockout (Cntnap2 KO) male Sprague-Dawley rats. The n-value may differ in the displayed test results as certain rats were seizure prone and exhibited atypical baseline behavior. Horizon Discovery (Boyertown, PA; originated at SAGE Laboratories, Inc. with Autism Speaks; the line is presently upheld by Envigo) provided heterozygous breeders with a five base-pair deletion at exon six in the Cntnap2 gene, created by zinc-finger nuclease target site CAGCATTTCCGCACC|aatgga|GAGTTTGACTACCTG. All genotypes tested in this experiment were littermates acquired from heterozygous crossings.

Pregnant dams were exposed to either saline or poly I:C on gestation day (GD) 9.5. This timepoint corresponds to the first trimester in humans, where the risk of MIA on ASD onset in the offspring is highest [42]. On GD 9.5, pregnant females (n = 16; npoly I:C = 8, nsaline = 9) underwent brief isoflurane anesthesia. The dams were injected with either 0.9% saline or 4 mg/kg poly I:C (Sigma Lot#037M4011V) into the tail vein. From weaning (PD 21), the offspring were placed in open cages, provided with ad libitum food and water, and were put on a 12-hour light– 12-hour dark cycle. Polycarbonate huts and crinkled paper supplemented the cage environment. The offspring were housed in same-sex pairs prior to and during behavioral testing. All behavioral testing was conducted in the light-cycle (7:00 a.m. to 7:00 p.m.).

5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT)

At PD 100, the rats were food restricted to a target weight of 90% of their normal weight. Through operant conditioning, the rats were trained to locate and report a passing visual stimulus shown pseudo-randomly in one of five sites on a horizontal mask of apertures. The detailed protocol for pretraining and baseline acquisition are well documented in a priorly published protocol by Mar and colleagues [43].

Task outline for 5-CSRTT protocol.

The default house-light setting in the chamber is off for all training and testing sessions. At the beginning of every session, a pellet (50 food: 50 sugar) is dropped in the food tray to motivate the rat to begin the task. To initiate the trial, the rat must nose poke inside the food tray and activate the sensor. There is a 5 s delay prior to stimulus presentation. The rat must then nose poke the illuminated stimulus panel to collect a food reward at the tray. If the correct panel is selected, a pellet is dropped with a pure tone. If an incorrect panel is selected, the house light is turned on with a white tone as punishment. After a 5 s intertrial interval, the tray illuminates to instigate the start of the subsequent trial. The stimulus duration is dependent on the training or testing stage. When the rat does not interact with the screen during stimulus presentation, the trial is considered an omission. If the rat nose pokes the screen during the delay period, the trial is labelled as a premature response. Lastly, if the rat continuously nose pokes the panel after a correct choice, its is labelled as a perseverative response.

Pretraining and baseline training period.

Pretraining allowed the rat to acclimate to the touchscreen chamber and screen. The rats also learned the fundamentals of the system; this includes habituation, initiating the task, and associating a correct response with a sugar-pellet reward [43]. The training period was divided across 13 baseline levels. The session length for all levels was set at 60 minutes, with a total of 60 trials per session. The intertrial interval was set at five seconds across all training stages. The latency or delay prior to the stimulus was also set at five seconds across all training stages. The stimulus duration was set at 60 seconds and was gradually decreased to 0.5 seconds at Baseline Stage 13. To move past a stage, the rat had to achieve greater than 80% accuracy, and omit less than 20% of trials. The results display the findings from Baseline 13 (0.5 second delay) as it corresponds to the duration value used in the test paradigms.

This study employed a fixed training period structure. Once the rats completed pretraining, they had 40 days to complete the training phase prior to testing. If the rat completed the training in less than 40 days, the testing paradigms were introduced earlier. This structure ensured that the rats showing no improvement did not overtrain at lower baseline levels indefinitely. Additionally, this structure allowed fast learners to complete the training phase without being overtrained, which is a limitation that can be seen in a group training structure where all rats would be trained until the very last rat completed their baseline training. Rats unable to complete baseline training independently were moved to the final baseline training stage on day 40. Extreme outliers were removed prior to the initiation of the testing protocol.

Test paradigms.

The first test was a Short-Delay variation. This test randomized the delay prior to stimulus onset at values less than the standard 5-second delay. This variation assessed global attention, providing the rat with less time to prepare for stimulus onset. The second test paradigm was a Long-Delay variation. The delays prior to stimulus onset were set at values greater than the standard 5-second delay. This test assessed inhibition and impulsivity because the rat was provided more opportunity to prematurely respond prior to stimulus onset. The third test paradigm was the Distraction variation. This test employed an auditory distractor–noise of 105dB at randomized time points between initiation and stimulus presentation. This test measured selective attention. Each of the four test paradigms were set at 100 trials to be completed within 60 minutes or less.

Measures for attention processing.

The following measures were used to assess the rats’ executive function: response accuracy (number of correct over all trials) to measure of attentional selectivity, omissions (trials with no response) to measure sustained attention, and premature responses (nose poke prior to stimulus) or perseverative correct responses (continued response after correct action feedback) as measures of impulsivity and compulsion, respectively.

Statistics

All data are mean ± standard deviations unless otherwise stated. Data was analyzed using a two-way analysis of variance ANOVA for the baseline task acquisition stage. The two between-subjects factors for the two-way ANOVA were genotype (WT, Cntnap2 KO) and prenatal exposure (saline, poly I:C). A three-way mixed analysis of variance ANOVA (two between-subject factors and one within-subjects factor) was used for the three test paradigms: Short Delay, Long Delay, and Distraction. The three-way ANOVA was followed by the Bonferroni pairwise comparison post hoc, utilizing the IBM SPSS statistics software. The statistical significance was placed at p < .05.

Results

5CSRTT training: Baseline stage 13 (0.5s stimulus)

Animals needed between 30 and 40 days of training to reach testing criterion independently of experimental group (see S1 Fig in S1 File). A two-way ANOVA was conducted to examine the effects of genotype (Cntnap2 +/+ [Cntnap2 WT] or Cntnap2 -/- [Cntnap2 KO]) and prenatal exposure (saline or poly I:C [MIA]) on each measure of attention for the last baseline training stage (baseline 13). During training, average accuracy on baseline performance was used to assess attentional selectivity. Neither genotype nor prenatal exposure significantly influenced performance accuracy for baseline training (genotype main effect: F(1, 42) = 0.338, p = 0.564, partial η2 = 0.009; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 42) = 3.550, p = 0.067, partial η2 = 0.085; Fig 1A). There was also no significant interaction effect between genotype and prenatal exposure on average accuracy for baseline stage 13 (F(1, 42) = 0.420, p = 0.521, partial η2 = 0.011). The measure of omission was used to assess sustained attention during task acquisition. There was no main effect seen by either risk factor alone (genotype main effect: F(1, 42) = 2.489, p = 0.123, partial η2 = 0.061; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 42) = 0.037, p = 0.849, partial η2 = 0.001; Fig 1B). No significant interaction between the models was seen (F(1, 42) = 0.238, p = 0.628, partial η2 = 0.006).

thumbnail
Fig 1.

A) During the last stage of baseline training, the rats did not exhibit model-dependent accuracy changes; B) changes in omission; C) or significant differences in premature count. D) Cntnap2 KO rats exhibited increased perseverative correct responses regardless of poly I:C MIA. *p < .05. Results are shown as mean ± SEM. NWT/SALINE = 10; NWT/POLY I:C = 9; NKO/SALINE = 13; NKO/POLY I:C = 10.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0299380.g001

Premature responses, which were used to measure motor impulsivity, did not reveal any main effect for either model type (genotype main effect: F(1, 42) = 0.013, p = 0.911, partial η2 = 0.000; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 42) = 1.698, p = 0.200, partial η2 = 0.043; Fig 1C), nor a significant interaction between genotype and prenatal exposure (F(1, 42) = 0.361, p = 0.552, partial η2 = 0.009). However, Cntnap2 KO rats exhibited greater perseverative correct responses, a measure of repetitive behaviors, regardless of prenatal exposure (genotype main effect: F(1, 42) = 6,236, p = 0.017, partial η2 = 0.141; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 42) = 1.350, p = 0.252, partial η2 = 0.034; Fig 1D; interaction effect: F(1, 42) = 0.180, p = 0.674, partial η2 = 0.005).

5CSRTT baseline training revealed a significant increase of perseveration in Cntnap2 KO rats, but no significant differences in the other measures. This indicates that neither Cntnap2 KO, nor MIA, has a severe impact on learning in the 5CSRTT task.

5CSRTT testing: Short-delay paradigm

The Short-Delay paradigm altered the onset of the stimulus presentation after trial initiation. Specifically, the stimulus was presented either 0.5, 1.5, 3.0, or 4.5 seconds after trial initiation pseudo randomly; the standard delay/latency period on all baseline stages was a 5.0 second delay.

A three-way mixed ANOVA was run to investigate the effects of genotype, prenatal exposure and short-delay value on each respective measure of attention. Rats prenatally exposed to poly I:C exhibited poorer accuracy at the 0.5 second delay, regardless of genotype (prenatal exposure* 0.5 timepoint simple-main effect: F(3, 111) = 4.681, p = 0.044; genotype main effect: F(1, 37) = 0.970, p = 0.331; Fig 2A). Cntnap2 KO rats exhibited a lower rate of omissions regardless of prenatal exposure (genotype main effect: F(1, 37) = 13.610, p = 0.001; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 37) = 0.529, p = 0.472). There was a three-way interaction effect between genotype and prenatal exposure across the delay levels (F(3, 111) = 5.842, p = 0.001). There was a significant two-way interaction between delay and genotype, but not delay and injection (delay*genotype: F(3, 111) = 6.461, p = 0.000; delay*injection: F(1, 37) = 0.873, p = 0.457). Furthermore, there was a two-way interaction between genotype and injection (genotype*injection main effect: F(1, 37) = 5.168, p = 0.029; Fig 2B).). All bonferroni-adjusted pairwise comparisons were performed for statistically significant simple simple main effects. Saline-exposed Cntnap2 KO rats exhibited a lower rate of omissions than saline-exposed wildtypes across all four delays (delay timepoint 0.5 seconds: p < 0.001; delay timepoint 1.5 seconds: p < 0.001; delay timepoint 3.0 seconds: p = 0.008; delay timepoint 4.5 seconds: p = 0.035). Contrarily, prenatally exposed Cntnap2 KO rats did not exhibit a lower omission rate than prenatally exposed wildtypes across all delays (delay timepoint 0.5 seconds: p = 0.468; delay timepoint 1.5 seconds: p = 0.479; delay timepoint 3.0 seconds: p = 0.272; delay timepoint 4.5 seconds: p = 0.339; S2A Fig in S1 File). For a summary of all detailed statistical results per different delays or distraction parameters, please see S1 Table in S1 File.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Short-delay 5CSRTT testing.

A) There was a decrease in accuracy in offspring of poly I:C injected rats at the shortest delay of 0.5 s, regardless of genotype. B) Cntnap2 KO rats exhibited a decreased omission rate compared to wildtype. There was no simple main effect of genotype on rats exposed to poly I:C; however, there was a simple main effect of genotype on non-exposed rats. C) There was no significant effect of genotype or prenatal exposure, nor any interaction, on premature response. D) Cntnap2 KO rats exhibited increased perseverative correct responses regardless of prenatal exposure. *p < .05. Results are shown as mean ± SEM. NWT/SALINE = 10; NWT/POLY I:C = 9; NKO/SALINE = 12; NKO/POLY I:C = 10. For detailed data on different short delay intervals, please see S2 Fig in S1 File.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0299380.g002

The analysis of premature responses did not reveal any main effect in either model type (genotype main effect: F(1, 37) = 1.629, p = 0.210; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 37) = 0.747, p = 0.393; Fig 2C), nor any significant three-way interaction between genotype and prenatal exposure across the delay levels(F(3, 111) = 0.141, p = 0.935). However, like in Baseline Training, Cntnap2 KO rats exhibited greater perseverative correct responses regardless of prenatal exposure (genotype main effect: F(1, 37) = 4.929, p = 0.033; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 37) = 0.612, p = 0.439; interaction effect: F(3, 111) = 0.562, p = 0.641; Fig 2D, for detailed data per delay, please see S2 Fig in S1 File).

Short Delay 5CSRTT testing revealed a deficiency in accuracy in MIA offspring at the shortest delay and confirmed higher perseverative responses in Cntnap2 KO animals–as also observed in the final baseline training session. Furthermore, there was a lower omission rate in Cntnap2 Ko animals that was observed in saline animals, but not in Poly I:C injected animals.

5CSRTT testing: Long-delay paradigm

The Long-Delay paradigm altered the onset of the stimulus presentation after trial initiation. Specifically, the stimulus was presented either 4.5, 6, 7, or 9.5 seconds after trial initiation in a pseudo random fashion. During Long-Delay testing, neither genotype nor prenatal exposure significantly influenced performance accuracy (genotype main effect: F(1, 37) = 0 .053, p = 0.820; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 37) = 2.077, p = 0.158; Fig 3A). Also, there was no significant interaction effect between genotype, prenatal exposure and long-delay (F(3, 111) = 0.400, p = 0.753). Likewise, there was no main effect on the measure of omission by either risk factor alone (genotype main effect: F(1, 37) = 1.330, p = 0.256; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 37) = 0.036, p = 0.851; Fig 3B) nor any interaction between both risk factors (F(3, 111) = 0.192, p = 0.902).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Long delay 5CSRTT.

A) When tested under the long-delay paradigm, the rats did not exhibit model-dependent accuracy changes; B) or changes in omission. C) There was no significant risk factor effect on the perseverative response measure in this testing paradigm. D)WT rats prenatally exposed to poly I:C did not show a significant increase in premature responses compared to their saline counterpart. E) However, Cntnap2 KO rats prenatally exposed to poly I:C showed an increase in premature responses compared to their saline counterpart. F) Poly I:C treated Cntnap2 KO rats exhibited higher premature responses than poly I:C treated WT rats. *p < .05. Results are shown as mean ± SEM. NWT/SALINE = 10; NWT/POLY I:C = 9; NKO/SALINE = 12; NKO/POLY I:C = 10. For detailed data on different short delay intervals, please see S3 Fig in S1 File.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0299380.g003

No main effects of genotype or prenatal exposure on perseverative responses were found, nor was there a three-way interaction (genotype main effect: F(1, 37) = 2.755, p = 0.105; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 37) = 0.343, p = 0.561; interaction effect: F(3, 111) = 0.433, p = 0.730; Fig 3C). There was no main effect on premature responses in either model type alone (genotype main effect: F(1, 37) = 2.217, p = 0.14; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 37) = 3.714, p = 0.062), however, there was a significant interaction between genotype and prenatal exposure regardless of timepoint (F(1, 37) = 4.911, p = 0.033). Specifically, WT rats prenatally exposed to poly I:C did not exhibit increased premature responses compared to the WT saline group; however, Cntnap2 KO rats prenatally exposed to poly I:C exhibited significantly increased premature responses compared to their saline-exposed counterparts (prenatal exposure simple main effect: mean difference [9.051 +/- 3.575], p = 0.016; Fig 3D and 3E). Additionally, Cntnap2 KO rats prenatally exposed to poly I:C showed higher premature responses in comparison to WT rats prenatally exposed to poly I:C (genotype simple-main effect: mean difference 10.123 +/- 3.331, p = 0.004; Fig 3F).

The Long Delay 5CSRTT revealed an interaction of genotype and prenatal exposure, with significantly increased premature responses in poly I:C offspring with Cntnap2 KO, indicating an accumulating effect of a genetic deletion and MIA on this measure of impulsivity.

5CSRTT testing: Distraction paradigm

The Distraction paradigm altered the onset of a distractive auditory stimulus presentation after trial initiation. Specifically, a distracting noise of 105dB was presented either at 0.0, 0.5, 2.5, 4.5 or 5.0 seconds after trial initiation pseudo randomly. The latency time to stimulus onset was fixed to 5.0 seconds, in contrast to the short and long delay paradigms.

During Distraction testing, neither genotype nor prenatal exposure significantly influenced performance accuracy (genotype main effect: F(1, 37) = 0.284, p = 0.597; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 37) = 2.389, p = 0.131; Fig 4A). There was also no three-way interaction effect between genotype, prenatal exposure and distraction timepoint (F(3, 111) = 1.533, p = 0.195). However, Cntnap2 KO rats exhibited reduced omitted trials (genotype main effect: F(1, 37) = 13.982, p = 0.001; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 37) = 0.835, p = 0.367; Fig 4B) with no three-way interaction effect (interaction effect: F(3, 111) = 0.301, p = 0.877). Rats prenatally exposed to poly I:C exhibited significantly increased premature responses at distraction timepoint 0.5 seconds, regardless of genotype (genotype main effect: F(1, 37) = 0.003 p = 0.960; prenatal exposure*distraction effect: F(3, 111) = 4.506, p = .002; prenatal exposure simple main effect: mean difference [3.271 +/- 1.324], p = 0.018; Fig 4C). There was no significant three-way interaction between both risk factors at any distraction level (F(3, 111) = 0.785, p = 0.537). As reported in other 5CSRTT testing paradigms, Cntnap2 KO rats exhibited greater perseverative correct responses (genotype main effect: F(1, 37) = 4.504, p = 0.041; prenatal exposure main effect: F(1, 37) = 0.001, p = 0.977; Fig 4D), while there were no three-way interaction effects (interaction effect: F(3, 111) = 0.437, p = 0.781).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Distractor 5CSRTT.

A) When tested under the Distractor paradigm, the rats did not exhibit model-dependent accuracy changes. B) Rats with Cntnap2 deficiency exhibited lower omission rates, regardless of prenatal exposure. C) Rats with Cntnap2 deficiency exhibited increased perseverative correct responses regardless of injection type. D) Rats prenatally exposed to poly I:C exhibited increased premature responses at a distractor timepoint of 0.5 seconds. *p < .05. Results are shown as mean ± SEM. NWT/SALINE = 10; NWT/POLY I:C = 9; NKO/SALINE = 12; NKO/POLY I:C = 10. For detailed data on different short delay intervals, please see S4 Fig in S1 File.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0299380.g004

The Distractor 5CSRRT paradigm revealed a lower rate of omissions in Cntnap2 deficient rats, while once more confirming their higher perseveration rate. Furthermore, the premature responses in prenatally exposed poly I:C animals seemed to be enhanced in Cntnap2 KO animals only, but this could only be observed at one specific distraction timepoint.

Discussion

This study explored the separate and combined effects of Cntnap2 deficiency and poly I:C maternal immune activation on relevant measures of attentional processing. The research aimed to better assess the impact of genetic-environment interaction on ASD associated impairments of cognitive function, using the 5-CSRTT. The 5-CSRTT quantifies attention, impulsivity, and cognitive flexibility in preclinical models for ASD. Several previous studies have shown the construct validity of the 5-CSRTT as a model for attention [41, 44]. Due to the equalized effects of genetic and environmental risk factors, it was initially hypothesized that Cntnap2 deficiency would interact with poly I:C MIA to exacerbate ASD-related attentional alterations seen in each model individually. However, the presented results indicated only one incidence of such an interaction across all relevant measures in the 5-CSRTT.

Baseline training

In the baseline training stage, Cntnap2 -/- and poly I:C MIA did not interact to exacerbate ASD-associated symptoms across the measures of accuracy, omission, premature and perseverative correct responses. Cntnap2 -/- rats exhibited increased perseverative response in comparison to their wildtype counterpart; however, no other measures were influenced by either model alone. The baseline data raised several questions, including whether both risk factors influenced typical attentional processing during training, or if task acquisition posed a sufficient cognitive challenge to differentiate between the experimental groups.

Prior 5-CSRTT studies with separate ASD models provide varying results. In a 5-CSRTT study by Anshu and colleagues [38], valproic acid was administered prenatally at GD 12.5. The valproic acid model is consistently used in the field of ASD research due to its construct and face validity [45]. Male Sprague-Dawley rats prenatally exposed to valproic acid showed poorer performance at the first and last stages of baseline acquisition [38]. Contrastingly, a neonatal white-matter injury (WMI) study did not find a significant difference in accuracy between treatment and wildtype counterparts during baseline training. WMI can lead to hindered cognitive processing and an increased risk of ASD [46]. These disagreeing results suggest that baseline acquisition is dependent on the ASD risk-factor models’ ability to learn a novel task, or different baseline protocols that can be employed in the 5-CSRTT [47]. The double-hit Cntnap2 KO and prenatally exposed rats did not exhibit any deficit in learning and performing the task at baseline.

Cntnap2 KO rats showed an increase in perseverative correct responses in the baseline training paradigm. Perseverative responses are defined as continuous pokes after the correct stimuli has been selected. The continuous response is a marker of compulsiveness–a tendency to perform repetitive adverse behaviors [48]. The rat’s maladaptive tendency to continually nose poke a previously rewarded aperture can also act as a marker of cognitive inflexibility [49].

Variable short and long delay

The delay and distraction paradigms highlighted key differences between treatment groups in the test stage. Firstly, decreased accuracy in poly I:C exposed rats was seen at the shortest delay value alone. There are no direct models of ASD that display variable Short-Delay outcomes; however, prenatal exposure to poly I:C can lead to cognitive inflexibility [50]. Rats prenatally exposed to poly I:C show persistence in latent inhibition (LI), a process by which introduction to a nonreinforced stimulus affects the subsequent learning of a matching reinforced stimulus [50]. LI is also a key process for assessing cognitive inflexibility. Although the 5-CSRTT does not assess LI, novel delays may challenge their preconceived understanding of the task protocol, requiring adaptive mechanisms to maintain high performance. Similarly, the study investigating white matter insult discovered poorer accuracy performance with a variable intertrial interval [46].

In the short-delay paradigm, the Cntnap2 KO rats had a lower omission rate than their wildtype counterparts; however, this behavioral difference was only apparent in non-poly I:C exposed rats. Cntnap2 KO rats prenatally exposed to poly I:C did not have significantly lower omission rates in comparison to WT rats also exposed to poly I:C. At the same time, Cntnap2 KO rats showed greater perseverative correct responses in the Short-Delay paradigm [49]. Previous 5-CSRTT studies did not employ the Cntnap2 model; nonetheless, other behavioral paradigms were used to assess repetitive behavior. A study by Wang et al. [51] found that Cntnap2 KO mice exhibited increased grooming, a robust phenotype for repetitive behaviors. A recent paper by Scott et al. [32] identified that Cntnap2 KO rats showed increased full body rotations and self grooming compared to the wildtype counterpart. These measures of repetitive behavior are based on instinctive and uncontrolled actions. The 5-CSRTT provides insight into repetitive behaviors that are newly developed after exposure to a novel task; nonetheless, there could be a neural mechanism that links repetitive and perseverative actions across behavioral paradigms. A 5-CSRTT study that lesioned an important region in the dopaminergic system (core subregion of the nucleus accumbens) found comparable perseverative behaviors and lack of inhibitory control [52]. Remarkably, antagonizing dopaminergic receptor D2 decreased repetitive self grooming and perseverative behaviors in Cntnap2 KO mice [53]. Consequently, dopaminergic signalling may play a large role in modulating repetitive behaviors seen in instinctive and learned behaviors [53].

The Long-Delay paradigm elicited an interactive effect for premature responses between genotype and MIA. To elaborate, Cntnap2 -/- rats exposed to MIA had a greater number of premature responses to their saline treated counterparts. Contrarily, wildtype rats exposed to MIA did not have greater premature tendencies compared to the saline treated wildtypes. Premature responses are a staple measure of impulsivity in preclinical rodent models [54, 55]. Impulsivity arises as a phenotype for atypical inhibitory response control [56]. Additionally, the rats may have exhibited impulsive tendencies due to a phenomenon known as delay-discounting [57]. With the increased stimulus delay, the value of the reward can depreciate. Therefore, the rat prematurely prompts the screen as opposed to waiting for the stimulus. Delay-discounting relies on the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, a region involved in attention and inhibitory control [58].

Distraction effect on 5-CSRTT performance

The distraction paradigm elicited three separate parameter effects. A distractor was played pseudo randomly at four different timepoints preceding the stimulus onset. Similarly to the short-delay paradigm, Cntnap2 -/- rats had less omissions than their wildtype counterparts. This finding is not consistent across previous ASD 5-CSRTT studies [38, 55]; however, the study that looked at WMI found a decrease in omission rate with a fixed visual distractor [46]. Despite this, the present study utilized an auditory distractor. The rise in premature and perseverative responses may act as an alternative explanation in the distraction protocol. Due to increased interaction with the screen, the rats are less likely to omit a trial entirely.

Inhibitory response control and cognitive inflexibility can explain the rise in perseverative and premature responses. Like the short delay paradigm, the rats performed repetitive behaviors as a maladaptive response to a novel stimulus. On the other hand, the premature responses present a novel outcome in the distractor task. Different from the Long-Delay paradigm, there is no interactive effect between genotype and MIA. Rather, poly I:C MIA triggered premature responses at the earliest distractor timepoint alone. Poly I:C may potentially affect an inhibitory response mechanism that Cntnap2-/- did not. Rodents utilize temporal strategies to ‘time’ when they should attend to the oncoming stimulus [55]. It is a mediating behavior developed because of training. The distractor can interfere with the rat’s temporal strategy, triggering a premature response [55]. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is involved in both temporal strategies, and sensory hypersensitivity to increased stimuli [59, 60]. Poly I:C has been found to increase dendritic branching in the pyramidal cells of the DLPFC, potentially explaining an increase in impulsivity due to an auditory distractor [61].

Limitations

Although the findings provide insight into the attentional processing of a gene-environment model, future studies must address respective changes in neurobiological function to better understand the behavioral phenotype. One potential neurophysiological confound to our findings is motor planning and execution. The prefrontal cortex is known to have an inhibiting modulatory control over the premotor cortex, the area most associated with motor planning [62]. In fact, several studies on this cortico-cortical connection state that altered prefrontal activity results in motor impulsivity [63, 64]. Specifically, response cells in the premotor cortex may fire prematurely because of poor prefrontal cortex inhibition [63]. Future 5-CSRTT studies must assess if there is a significant correlation between involuntary motor impulsivity and attentional impulsivity because of premature anticipatory neuronal firing. Moreover, the 5-CSRTT task cannot act as a standalone measure of attention. For example, the continuous performance task requires the rodent to discriminate between difference stimuli based on changes in color, brightness, contrast, etc [65]. Employing additional paradigms may provide a more holistic understanding of the impact of gene-environment risk model.

Future directions and conclusion

In conjunction with behavioral assessment, subsequent studies must look at the neurobiological explanation behind the behavioral impairments demonstrated in this study. Regarding attentional involvement of Cntnap2 and poly I:C, a more causative link between behavioral output and mechanistic explanations can be made. Utilizing deep brain stimulation to attenuate ASD-phenotypes can directly look at the causative impact of risk factors. A study by Bekovsky et al. [66], found that the use of deep brain stimulation attenuated ASD-like sensorimotor and latent inhibition dysfunction in a poly I:C rat model. Potentially using the targets such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the ventral striatum, we can better appreciate the causal link behind atypical brain development and atypical attentional processing.

The present investigation showed that both MIA and genetic knockout can affect the cognitive process of attention separately; The Cntnap2 KO model showed conserved changes in repetitive behavior, while the MIA model exacerbated impulsivity and lack of inhibitory control. Although neural alterations may converge across both models, a double-hit model does not certainly result in additive behavioral effects. However, attention deficits can be exacerbated in the double-hit model when presented with specific external stimuli, e.g., an auditory distractor. The parameters of this touchscreen task may also negate certain environmental influences that are generally present in a more natural environment. Due to the heterogeneity of ASD phenotypes, continually characterizing double-hit models will facilitate our understanding about the etiological basis of the disorder.

References

  1. 1. Zeidan J, Fombonne E, Scorah J, Ibrahim A, Durkin MS, Saxena S, et al. Global prevalence of autism: A systematic review update. Autism Research. 2022;15: 778–790. pmid:35238171
  2. 2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Fifth Edition. American Psychiatric Association; 2013. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
  3. 3. Vivanti G, Hamner T, Lee NR. Neurodevelopmental Disorders Affecting Sociability: Recent Research Advances and Future Directions in Autism Spectrum Disorder and Williams Syndrome. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2018;18: 94. pmid:30328520
  4. 4. Weitlauf AS, Gotham KO, Vehorn AC, Warren ZE. Brief Report: DSM-5 “Levels of Support:” A Comment on Discrepant Conceptualizations of Severity in ASD. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44: 471–476. pmid:23812664
  5. 5. Wetherby AM, Guthrie W, Woods J, Schatschneider C, Holland RD, Morgan L, et al. Parent-Implemented Social Intervention for Toddlers With Autism: An RCT. Pediatrics. 2014;134: 1084–1093. pmid:25367544
  6. 6. Pathways in ASD Study Team, Georgiades S, Szatmari P, Duku E, Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, et al. Phenotypic Overlap Between Core Diagnostic Features and Emotional/Behavioral Problems in Preschool Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41: 1321–1329. pmid:21161575
  7. 7. Chaxiong P, Dimian AF, Wolff JJ. Restricted and repetitive behavior in children with autism during the first three years of life: A systematic review. Front Psychol. 2022;13: 986876. pmid:36389482
  8. 8. Rylaarsdam L, Guemez-Gamboa A. Genetic Causes and Modifiers of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front Cell Neurosci. 2019;13: 385. pmid:31481879
  9. 9. Betancur C. Etiological heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorders: More than 100 genetic and genomic disorders and still counting. Brain Research. 2011;1380: 42–77. pmid:21129364
  10. 10. Gaugler T, Klei L, Sanders SJ, Bodea CA, Goldberg AP, Lee AB, et al. Most genetic risk for autism resides with common variation. Nat Genet. 2014;46: 881–885. pmid:25038753
  11. 11. Krumm N, Turner TN, Baker C, Vives L, Mohajeri K, Witherspoon K, et al. Excess of rare, inherited truncating mutations in autism. Nat Genet. 2015;47: 582–588. pmid:25961944
  12. 12. Sanders SJ, He X, Willsey AJ, Ercan-Sencicek AG, Samocha KE, Cicek AE, et al. Insights into Autism Spectrum Disorder Genomic Architecture and Biology from 71 Risk Loci. Neuron. 2015;87: 1215–1233. pmid:26402605
  13. 13. Girirajan S, Rosenfeld JA, Cooper GM, Antonacci F, Siswara P, Itsara A, et al. A recurrent 16p12.1 microdeletion supports a two-hit model for severe developmental delay. Nat Genet. 2010;42: 203–209. pmid:20154674
  14. 14. Autism Spectrum Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, BUPGEN, Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 23andMe Research Team, Grove J, Ripke S, et al. Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nat Genet. 2019;51: 431–444. pmid:30804558
  15. 15. Karimi P, Kamali E, Mousavi S, Karahmadi M. Environmental factors influencing the risk of autism. J Res Med Sci. 2017;22: 27. pmid:28413424
  16. 16. Sandin S, Schendel D, Magnusson P, Hultman C, Suren P, Susser E, et al. Autism risk associated with parental age and with increasing difference in age between the parents. Mol Psychiatry. 2015. pmid:26055426
  17. 17. Massrali A, Adhya D, Srivastava DP, Baron-Cohen S, Kotter MR. Virus-Induced Maternal Immune Activation as an Environmental Factor in the Etiology of Autism and Schizophrenia. Front Neurosci. 2022;16: 834058. pmid:35495047
  18. 18. Abdallah MW, Larsen N, Grove J, Nørgaard-Pedersen B, Thorsen P, Mortensen EL, et al. Amniotic fluid inflammatory cytokines: Potential markers of immunologic dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry. 2013;14: 528–538. pmid:22175527
  19. 19. Careaga M, Murai T, Bauman MD. Maternal Immune Activation and Autism Spectrum Disorder: From Rodents to Nonhuman and Human Primates. Biological Psychiatry. 2017;81: 391–401. pmid:28137374
  20. 20. Shi L, Fatemi SH, Sidwell RW, Patterson PH. Maternal influenza infection causes marked behavioral and pharmacological changes in the offspring. J Neurosci. 2003;23: 297–302. pmid:12514227
  21. 21. Meyer U, Feldon J, Fatemi SH. In-vivo rodent models for the experimental investigation of prenatal immune activation effects in neurodevelopmental brain disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2009;33: 1061–1079. pmid:19442688
  22. 22. Brown AS, Derkits EJ. Prenatal Infection and Schizophrenia: A Review of Epidemiologic and Translational Studies. AJP. 2010;167: 261–280. pmid:20123911
  23. 23. Tordjman S, Somogyi E, Coulon N, Kermarrec S, Cohen D, Bronsard G, et al. Gene × Environment Interactions in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Role of Epigenetic Mechanisms. Front Psychiatry. 2014;5. pmid:25136320
  24. 24. Sandin S, Lichtenstein P, Kuja-Halkola R, Larsson H, Hultman CM, Reichenberg A. The Familial Risk of Autism. JAMA. 2014;311: 1770. pmid:24794370
  25. 25. Krakowiak P, Walker CK, Bremer AA, Baker AS, Ozonoff S, Hansen RL, et al. Maternal Metabolic Conditions and Risk for Autism and Other Neurodevelopmental Disorders. PEDIATRICS. 2012;129: e1121–e1128. pmid:22492772
  26. 26. Estes ML, McAllister AK. Maternal immune activation: Implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Science. 2016;353: 772–777. pmid:27540164
  27. 27. Abrahams BS, Tentler D, Perederiy JV, Oldham MC, Coppola G, Geschwind DH. Genome-wide analyses of human perisylvian cerebral cortical patterning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104: 17849–17854. pmid:17978184
  28. 28. Alarcón M, Abrahams BS, Stone JL, Duvall JA, Perederiy JV, Bomar JM, et al. Linkage, Association, and Gene-Expression Analyses Identify CNTNAP2 as an Autism-Susceptibility Gene. The American Journal of Human Genetics. 2008;82: 150–159. pmid:18179893
  29. 29. Baig DN, Yanagawa T, Tabuchi K. Distortion of the normal function of synaptic cell adhesion molecules by genetic variants as a risk for autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research Bulletin. 2017;129: 82–90. pmid:27743928
  30. 30. Poot M. Connecting the CNTNAP2 Networks with Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Mol Syndromol. 2015;6: 7–22. pmid:25852443
  31. 31. Strauss KA, Puffenberger EG, Huentelman MJ, Gottlieb S, Dobrin SE, Parod JM, et al. Recessive Symptomatic Focal Epilepsy and Mutant Contactin-Associated Protein-like 2. N Engl J Med. 2006;354: 1370–1377. pmid:16571880
  32. 32. Scott KE, Kazazian K, Mann RS, Möhrle D, Schormans AL, Schmid S, et al. Loss of Cntnap2 in the Rat Causes Autism‐Related Alterations in Social Interactions, Stereotypic Behavior, and Sensory Processing. Autism Research. 2020;13: 1698–1717. pmid:32918359
  33. 33. Möhrle D, Wang W, Whitehead SN, Schmid S. GABAB Receptor Agonist R-Baclofen Reverses Altered Auditory Reactivity and Filtering in the Cntnap2 Knock-Out Rat. Front Integr Neurosci. 2021;15: 710593. pmid:34489651
  34. 34. Lombardo MV, Moon HM, Su J, Palmer TD, Courchesne E, Pramparo T. Maternal immune activation dysregulation of the fetal brain transcriptome and relevance to the pathophysiology of autism spectrum disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2018;23: 1001–1013. pmid:28322282
  35. 35. Takeda K. Toll-like receptors in innate immunity. International Immunology. 2004;17: 1–14. pmid:15585605
  36. 36. Landry R, Bryson SE. Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Child Psychology Psychiatry. 2004;45: 1115–1122. pmid:15257668
  37. 37. Chevallier C, Parish‐Morris J, McVey A, Rump KM, Sasson NJ, Herrington JD, et al. Measuring social attention and motivation in autism spectrum disorder using eye‐tracking: Stimulus type matters. Autism Research. 2015;8: 620–628. pmid:26069030
  38. 38. Anshu K, Nair AK, Kumaresan UD, Kutty BM, Srinath S, Laxmi TR. Altered attentional processing in male and female rats in a prenatal valproic acid exposure model of autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research. 2017;10: 1929–1944. pmid:28851114
  39. 39. Hanley M, McPhillips M, Mulhern G, Riby DM. Spontaneous attention to faces in Asperger syndrome using ecologically valid static stimuli. Autism. 2013;17: 754–761. pmid:22987893
  40. 40. Asinof SK, Paine TA. The 5-Choice Serial Reaction Time Task: A Task of Attention and Impulse Control for Rodents. JoVE. 2014; 51574. pmid:25146934
  41. 41. Fizet J, Cassel J-C, Kelche C, Meunier H. A review of the 5-Choice Serial Reaction Time (5-CSRT) task in different vertebrate models. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016;71: 135–153. pmid:27587002
  42. 42. Haddad FL, Patel SV, Schmid S. Maternal Immune Activation by Poly I:C as a preclinical Model for Neurodevelopmental Disorders: A focus on Autism and Schizophrenia. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2020;113: 546–567. pmid:32320814
  43. 43. Mar AC, Horner AE, Nilsson SR, Alsio J, Kent BA, Kim CH, et al. The touchscreen operant platform for assessing executive function in rats and mice. Nat Protoc. 2013;8: 1985–2005. pmid:24051960
  44. 44. Birtalan E, Bánhidi A, Sanders JI, Balázsfi D, Hangya B. Rapid learning of the 5-choice serial reaction time task in an automated rodent training system. Neuroscience; 2020 Feb.
  45. 45. Mabunga DFN, Gonzales ELT, Kim J, Kim KC, Shin CY. Exploring the Validity of Valproic Acid Animal Model of Autism. Exp Neurobiol. 2015;24: 285–300. pmid:26713077
  46. 46. Achterberg EJM, Van Oldeniel RJ, Van Tilborg E, Verharen JPH, Nijboer CH, Vanderschuren LJMJ. Cognitive performance during adulthood in a rat model of neonatal diffuse white matter injury. Psychopharmacology. 2022;239: 745–764. pmid:35064798
  47. 47. Huang H, Guadagna S, Mereu M, Ciampoli M, Pruzzo G, Ballard T, et al. A schizophrenia relevant 5-Choice Serial Reaction Time Task for mice assessing broad monitoring, distractibility and impulsivity. Psychopharmacology. 2017;234: 2047–2062. pmid:28378204
  48. 48. Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Blackwell AD, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Motor Inhibition and Cognitive Flexibility in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder and Trichotillomania. AJP. 2006;163: 1282–1284. pmid:16816237
  49. 49. Amitai N, Markou A. Disruption of Performance in the Five-Choice Serial Reaction Time Task Induced By Administration of N-Methyl-D-Aspartate Receptor Antagonists: Relevance to Cognitive Dysfunction in Schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;68: 5–16. pmid:20488434
  50. 50. Bitanihirwe BK, Peleg-Raibstein D, Mouttet F, Feldon J, Meyer U. Late Prenatal Immune Activation in Mice Leads to Behavioral and Neurochemical Abnormalities Relevant to the Negative Symptoms of Schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;35: 2462–2478. pmid:20736993
  51. 51. Wang HB, Tahara Y, Luk SHC, Kim Y-S, Hitchcock ON, MacDowell Kaswan ZA, et al. Melatonin treatment of repetitive behavioral deficits in the Cntnap2 mouse model of autism spectrum disorder. Neurobiology of Disease. 2020;145: 105064. pmid:32889171
  52. 52. Christakou A, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Prefrontal Cortical–Ventral Striatal Interactions Involved in Affective Modulation of Attentional Performance: Implications for Corticostriatal Circuit Function. J Neurosci. 2004;24: 773–780. pmid:14749421
  53. 53. Gandhi T, Lee CC. Neural Mechanisms Underlying Repetitive Behaviors in Rodent Models of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Front Cell Neurosci. 2021;14: 592710. pmid:33519379
  54. 54. Ciampoli M, Contarini G, Mereu M, Papaleo F. Attentional Control in Adolescent Mice Assessed with a Modified Five Choice Serial Reaction Time Task. Sci Rep. 2017;7: 9936. pmid:28855580
  55. 55. Cope ZA, Halberstadt AL, Van Enkhuizen J, Flynn AD, Breier M, Swerdlow NR, et al. Premature responses in the five-choice serial reaction time task reflect rodents’ temporal strategies: evidence from no-light and pharmacological challenges. Psychopharmacology. 2016;233: 3513–3525. pmid:27534540
  56. 56. Chudasama Y, Robbins TW. Dissociable Contributions of the Orbitofrontal and Infralimbic Cortex to Pavlovian Autoshaping and Discrimination Reversal Learning: Further Evidence for the Functional Heterogeneity of the Rodent Frontal Cortex. J Neurosci. 2003;23: 8771–8780. pmid:14507977
  57. 57. Doremus-Fitzwater TL, Barreto M, Spear LP. Age-related differences in impulsivity among adolescent and adult Sprague-Dawley rats. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2012;126: 735–741. pmid:22889309
  58. 58. Ikegami M, Sorama M. Differential Neural Correlates in the Prefrontal Cortex during a Delay Discounting Task in Healthy Adults: An fNIRS Study. Brain Sciences. 2023;13: 758. pmid:37239230
  59. 59. Barbas H, Zikopoulos B. The Prefrontal Cortex and Flexible Behavior. Neuroscientist. 2007;13: 532–545. pmid:17901261
  60. 60. Zhao H-C, Lv R, Zhang G-Y, He L-M, Cai X-T, Sun Q, et al. Alterations of Prefrontal-Posterior Information Processing Patterns in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Front Neurosci. 2022;15: 768219. pmid:35173572
  61. 61. Hanson KL, Weir RK, Iosif A-M, Van De Water J, Carter CS, McAllister AK, et al. Altered dendritic morphology in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of nonhuman primates prenatally exposed to maternal immune activation. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2023;109: 92–101. pmid:36610487
  62. 62. Cisek P, Kalaska JF. Simultaneous Encoding of Multiple Potential Reach Directions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2002;87: 1149–1154. pmid:11826082
  63. 63. Duque J, Labruna L, Verset S, Olivier E, Ivry RB. Dissociating the Role of Prefrontal and Premotor Cortices in Controlling Inhibitory Mechanisms during Motor Preparation. J Neurosci. 2012;32: 806–816. pmid:22262879
  64. 64. Schiffer B, Müller BW, Scherbaum N, Forsting M, Wiltfang J, Leygraf N, et al. Impulsivity-related brain volume deficits in schizophrenia-addiction comorbidity. Brain. 2010;133: 3093–3103. pmid:20647266
  65. 65. Kim CH, Hvoslef-Eide M, Nilsson SRO, Johnson MR, Herbert BR, Robbins TW, et al. The continuous performance test (rCPT) for mice: a novel operant touchscreen test of attentional function. Psychopharmacology. 2015;232: 3947–3966. pmid:26415954
  66. 66. Bikovsky L, Hadar R, Soto-Montenegro ML, Klein J, Weiner I, Desco M, et al. Deep brain stimulation improves behavior and modulates neural circuits in a rodent model of schizophrenia. Experimental Neurology. 2016;283: 142–150. pmid:27302677