Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Child support grant expansion and cognitive function among women in rural South Africa: Findings from a natural experiment in the HAALSI cohort

  • Rishika Chakraborty ,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    rchakra@iu.edu

    Affiliation Department of Environmental and Occupational Health, Indiana University School of Public Health-Bloomington, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America

  • Lindsay C. Kobayashi,

    Roles Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Epidemiology, University of Michigan School of Public Health, Ann Arbor, Michigan, United States of America

  • Janet Jock,

    Roles Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation O’Neill School of Public and Environmental Affairs, Indiana University-Bloomington, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America

  • Coady Wing,

    Roles Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation O’Neill School of Public and Environmental Affairs, Indiana University-Bloomington, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America

  • Xiwei Chen,

    Roles Validation, Visualization

    Affiliation Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Indiana University School of Public Health-Bloomington, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America

  • Meredith Phillips,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Indiana University School of Public Health-Bloomington, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America

  • Lisa Berkman,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Harvard Center for Population and Development Studies, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Kathleen Kahn,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations MRC/Wits Rural Public Health and Health Transitions Research Unit (Agincourt), School of Public Health, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa, Department of Epidemiology and Global Health, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden

  • Chodziwadziwa Whiteson Kabudula,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation MRC/Wits Rural Public Health and Health Transitions Research Unit (Agincourt), School of Public Health, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa

  • Molly Rosenberg

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Indiana University School of Public Health-Bloomington, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America, MRC/Wits Rural Public Health and Health Transitions Research Unit (Agincourt), School of Public Health, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa

Abstract

Background

Cash transfers are a promising but understudied intervention that may protect cognitive function in adults. Although South Africa has a rapidly ageing population, little is known about the nature of association between cash transfers and cognitive function in this setting.

Objectives

We leveraged age-eligibility expansions to South Africa’s Child Support Grant (CSG) to investigate the association between duration of CSG eligibility and cognitive function of biological mothers of child beneficiaries in South Africa.

Methods

We analysed 2014/2015 baseline data from 944 women, aged 40–59 years with at least one CSG-eligible child, enrolled in the population-representative HAALSI cohort in Agincourt, South Africa. Duration of CSG eligibility for each mother was calculated based on the birth dates of all their children and the CSG age-eligibility expansion years (2003–2012). Cognitive function was measured using a cognitive battery administered at the HAALSI baseline interview. Linear regression was used to estimate the association between duration of CSG eligibility, dichotomized as low (≤10 years) and high (>10 years) eligibility, and cognitive function z-scores of the mothers.

Results

High vs. low duration of CSG eligibility, was associated with higher cognitive function z-scores in the full sample [β: 0.15 SD units; 95% CI: 0.04, 0.26; p-value = 0.01]. In mothers with one to four lifetime children, but not five or more, high vs. low duration of CSG eligibility, was associated with higher cognitive function z-scores [β: 0.19 SD units; 95% CI: 0.05, 0.34, p-value = 0.02].

Conclusion

Government cash transfers given to support raising children may confer substantial protective effects on the subsequent cognitive function of mothers. Further studies are needed to understand how parity may influence this relationship. Our findings bring evidence to policymakers for designing income supplementation programmes to promote healthy cognitive ageing in low-income settings.

Introduction

In 2019, about 57 million people worldwide were living with dementia and this number is projected to increase to 83 million in 2030, 116 million in 2040 and 153 million by 2050 [1, 2]. Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias (ADRD) is one of the leading causes of disabilities, poor health, and decreased quality of life [3]. The burden of ADRD falls disproportionately on people living in low- and middle-income countries, where two-thirds of cases currently occur [4]. Largely due to changes in demographic patterns and population ageing, it is estimated that by 2050 there will be about 7.62 million people living with ADRD in sub-Saharan Africa [5]. Despite this growing public health concern, limited research exists on ADRD risk and prevention in sub-Saharan African countries like South Africa [6, 7].

Prior evidence suggests that socioeconomic conditions throughout the life course shape ADRD risk [812]. A diagnosis of ADRD is typically preceded by a prodromal period of at least five years of accelerated decline in cognitive function [13]. Socioeconomic status indicators across the life course such as education, occupation and income have been linked to cognitive outcomes in older adults [14]. These socioeconomic status indicators may be associated with later life cognitive function by providing individuals with assets or tools to attain resources that can promote healthy cognitive ageing [9]. For instance, higher income allows adults to access nutritious food and healthy housing, which can positively influence their later life cognitive outcomes. In contrast, low income or lack of wealth may lead to chronic stress, which in turn can negatively affect their cognitive health [9].

The cumulative presence of socioeconomic advantages and thus cognitively stimulating exposures across the life course may have a protective effect on cognitive function and ADRD risk by promoting cognitive reserve [15, 16]. Cognitive reserve is a theoretical construct that is thought to improve the brain’s ability to cope with pathological damage and can be accrued across one’s life course [15]. Individuals with higher cognitive reserve may use their pre-existing neural networks more efficiently or may recruit compensatory mechanisms to perform a cognitive task, consequently coping better with brain pathology [17]. Socioeconomic status indicators, such as income, are considered proxy measures for cognitive reserve [18]. However, little is known about the nature of association between income and cognitive function in low-income settings, such as in rural South Africa [6, 19].

South Africa has a rapidly ageing population of people whose lives have been affected by structural racism during Apartheid from 1948 to 1994 [20]. Since the end of Apartheid, South Africa has implemented several social protection programmes, the largest being the Child Support Grant (CSG) [21, 22]. Implemented in 1998, the CSG is an unconditional cash transfer program designed to alleviate child poverty, offset the costs of raising children and improve their health and nutrition by providing financial assistance that supplements household income [23]. While the CSG is intended for the primary caregivers of the child beneficiaries, nearly 90 per cent of recipients are their biological mothers [24, 25]. When the CSG was first rolled out, only children under seven years were eligible. However, between 2003 and 2012, CSG eligibility was progressively expanded over time and, since 2012, children under the age of 18 have been eligible [22]. Prior research has shown that cash transfers like the CSG can raise living standards, improve health outcomes, and empower women within their households and communities [26]. CSG has been associated with several positive outcomes in child beneficiaries including alleviating food insecurity, improving education, school attendance, and anthropometric outcomes [22]. However, few studies have investigated the impacts of CSG transfers on the caregivers of child beneficiaries. One study found that CSG recipient mothers had a higher probability of being in the labor market and being employed [27], while another reported adults in CSG recipient households had improved mental health [28]. Mothers of CSG recipients have also reported having higher financial independence [29], decision making powers [29, 30], lower worry and stress [30], and improved social relationships within households and communities [30].

What remains unclear is whether cash transfers like the CSG can impact cognitive function and ADRD risk among the caregivers of child beneficiaries. Since cash transfers supplement income, they may help accrue cognitive reserve, however, few studies have investigated the causal effects of cash transfers on ADRD risk. Only one study in Mexico [31], one in United States [32], and another in South Africa [33] used natural experiments to demonstrate the causal effects of cash transfers in improving cognitive function in older adults. These studies found that higher social protection income led to substantial improvements in memory and other domains of cognitive function in older adults [31, 32]. Thus, the causal relationship between cash transfers and mid-to-later life cognitive function in adults from low-resource settings such as rural South Africa remain understudied.

We thus aimed to exploit the exogenous variation in CSG eligibility, due to iterative age eligibility expansion policies, as a natural experiment to estimate the causal effect of cash transfers on cognitive function among biological mothers in South Africa. Based on the cognitive reserve theory, we hypothesized that a greater duration of CSG eligibility would improve mid- life cognitive function among mothers, since such exposure increases income, thereby promoting cognitive reserve. This study will contribute to the literature linking social protection policies and cognitive function in an underrepresented and rapidly ageing population in a middle-income country.

Materials and methods

Study setting and design

This study is set in the rural Agincourt area of Bushbuckridge sub-district in Mpumalanga province, South Africa. Since 1992, the study area, currently consisting of 31 villages and approximately 116,000 inhabitants, has been under continuous surveillance through the Agincourt Health and Socio-Demographic Surveillance System (HDSS) run by the Medical Research Council/Wits University Rural Public Health and Health Transitions Research Unit [20, 34]. While the socioeconomic situation of the Agincourt population has improved post-Apartheid, the region still faces difficulties in accessing basic services, such as electricity and piped water [20]. This population is Human Immunodeficiency Virus hyperendemic (~20 per cent prevalence in 2010) [35], and has high unemployment rates [20].

Cash transfers from social protection programmes, such as the CSG, are an important source of household income in the study region [20]. Primary caregivers of age-eligible children whose income falls below a means-tested level (about 10 times the grant amount for single caregivers) are eligible to apply for the CSG [22, 25]. When the CSG was first introduced, uptake rates were low, with only 28 per cent of CSG-eligible households in Agincourt submitting a CSG application in 2001 [25]. However, uptake rates and eligibility have increased over time and the CSG currently reaches over 1.1 million children in Mpumalanga province every month [36]. Since its implementation in 1998, the age eligibility was expanded from children under seven to under age nine in 2003, under age 11 in 2004, under age 14 in 2005, under age 15 in 2009, and through the age of 16 in 2010. Since 2012, children under the age of 18 have been eligible [21, 22]. The grant payment has also increased over time to match inflation, from 100 Rand (~$7 USD) in 1998 to 510 Rand (~$27 USD) in October 2023 per month for each child [37].

Using these CSG age eligibility expansions as natural experiments, we analysed baseline data collected in the 2014/15 ‘Health and Ageing in Africa: A Longitudinal Study of an INDEPTH Community in South Africa (HAALSI)’ [34]. HAALSI is a longitudinal cohort study of 5059 adults (2345 men and 2714 women) aged ≥40 years, representative of the Agincourt HDSS as its sampling frame (~86 per cent response rate). Trained, local fieldworkers collected household and individual data using computer assisted personal interviews [34]. All interviews were conducted in the local Xitsonga language with interview materials translated from English and back-translated for reliability.

Ethical approval for HAALSI was obtained from the Wits University Human Ethics Committee (#M141159), the Harvard T. H. Chan School of Public Health Office of Human Research Administration (#13–1608), and the Mpumalanga Provincial Research and Ethics Committee. Each HAALSI participant provided written informed consent prior to being interviewed. Participants unable to read had a witness and used inked fingerprint to sign consent form. This current study was deemed to be a ‘Not Human Subjects Research’ by the Institutional Review Board of Indiana University Bloomington (protocol #2002584956) since the data was de-identified prior to analysis.

Study sample

Study participants were CSG-eligible women, defined as HAALSI women between the ages of 40 to 59 years who had at least one CSG-eligible biological child. We restricted our sample to only biological mothers (excluding other female relatives/fathers/male relatives) since nearly 90 per cent of primary caregivers receiving the CSG are biological mothers of eligible children [25]. After removing women who did not have any children (n = 224), who did not have CSG-eligible children (n = 1363), and with implausible childbirth ages (≤10 or ≥60 years old) (n = 2), there were 1125 CSG eligible women in the HAALSI cohort. We then excluded women who were ≥ 60 years of age (n = 181) since this group may not be representative of other women in their later-life in this rural South African setting and due to their small sample size. The older women would have been at least 38 years old at the time of childbirth in order to have CSG-eligible children, making their reproductive history fall outside the typical childbearing age which was between 20–34 years in South Africa in 1998 [38]. Therefore, the final analytic sample included 944 CSG-eligible mothers (Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Flowchart describing the cohort construction of 944 child support grant eligible mothers enrolled in health and ageing in Africa: A longitudinal study of an INDEPTH community in South Africa (HAALSI), 2014/15.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297673.g001

Study measures

Exposure variable: Child Support Grant (CSG) eligibility.

The cumulative duration of CSG eligibility at HAALSI baseline, in years, for each CSG-eligible mother was the exposure of interest. This was calculated based on the birth dates (month and year) of all their children and the CSG age-eligibility expansion years. We aggregated the potential years of CSG eligibility for each child beneficiary to calculate the total duration of CSG eligibility for the mothers. Take, for instance, a woman with two children, one born in January 1994 and the other born in January 1995. Based on their birth dates and the expansions implemented between 2003 and 2012, the child born in 1994 will contribute 7.5 years of CSG eligibility, while the child born in 1995 will contribute 12.5 years of CSG eligibility by January 2016. Thus, for the mother, we would calculate a total CSG eligibility of 20 years at HAALSI baseline. An excel workbook showing our CSG eligibility calculation is included in our Supplementary Materials. Among women of the same age and with similar number of children, the variation in CSG eligibility duration plausibly leads to exogenous differences in CSG exposure over time. In contrast, the actual uptake of CSG benefits may be associated with individual risk factors or early outcomes such as education or high early life cognitive functioning that could confound the relationship between CSG and later outcomes [39]. Thus, in this paper, we focus on the “intent to treat” relationship between exogenous variation in years of CSG eligibility (adjusted for age, education, and number of children) and cognitive functioning.

For current analysis, we categorized the duration of CSG eligibility into a binary variable: ≤10 years (“low”) versus >10 years (“high”). This choice of categorisation was informed by a data driven decision making, where we observed a threshold of higher cognitive function scores for mothers who had greater than 10 years of CSG eligibility compared to those who had less. We first mapped the relationship between all CSG eligibility years (as indicator variables) and cognitive function z-scores. To better understand the trend, we then grouped the CSG eligibility years into five-year intervals to visualize the same relationship. We observed that mothers with 1 to 5 years and 5.5 to 10 years of CSG eligibility duration had similar cognitive function scores while those with greater than 10 years had relatively higher cognitive function scores (S3 Fig in S1 File). Given this observation, we decided to use 10 years of CSG eligibility as our threshold for analysis (the CSG variable construction process has been described in further details under Child Support Grant Variable Construction in our Supplementary Materials). We also conducted several sensitivity analyses using CSG as a continuous variable and with different cut-offs for duration of CSG eligibility to assess the robustness of this threshold (Please see Sensitivity Analysis, S1 to S3 Tables in S1 File).

Outcome variable: Cognitive function.

A cognitive battery was administered in the HAALSI study interview, which included measures of orientation (ability to correctly state the date, month, year, and South African President; 4 points total) and episodic memory (immediate and delayed recall of ten words that were read out loud by the interviewer; 20 points total) [40]. These measures were selected for use as the inability to orient oneself in time and place and loss of episodic memory are hallmark symptoms of ADRD, and are highly sensitive to ageing-related change [41, 42]. This cognitive battery was adapted from the United States Health and Retirement Study (US HRS) and has been used in other HRS International Partner Studies around the world [43, 44] including South Africa [33, 40, 45]. The cognitive battery also had 2 numeracy measures (forward count and number skip pattern), which we did not include in the present analysis. This is because numeracy is a skill learned through education and given the limited educational opportunities that our study sample faced during Apartheid, those who are not numerate may be particularly disadvantaged for the numeracy tests [40, 41]. For present analysis, we used the continuous z-standardized score of the 24-point composite score (with mean 0 and standard deviation 1) [40].

Sociodemographic variables.

The sample of CSG-eligible mothers was described according to their age at the time of the HAALSI baseline interview and CSG implementation (years), number of CSG-eligible children, lifetime number of children (one to four, and five and more), education (none, some primary, and some secondary or more), marital status (never married/ separated/divorced/widowed, and currently married), religion (none, Christianity, and African traditional), country of origin (South Africa, and Mozambique/other), and wealth index quintiles (based on household characteristics and ownership of vehicles, livestock and household items) [34].

Statistical analysis

CSG-eligible mothers were stratified into two groups according to their number of lifetime children, one to four and five and more children, since the number of lifetime children is structurally linked to higher duration of CSG eligibility. This categorization was chosen to distinguish those with grand multiparity of five and more births [46]. For descriptive analysis, we used Chi-square tests and Mann Whitney U tests for categorical and continuous variables, respectively. Ordinary Least Square regression models were used to estimate the association between duration of CSG eligibility and cognitive function z-score, first in the full sample, and then stratified by lifetime number of children subgroups. All models were adjusted for age of mother (as a linear and a quadratic term), education (as none, some primary, some secondary or more) and lifetime number of children indicator variables. Statistical analyses were conducted using R statistical software (version 3.6.0, Vienna, Austria). Threshold for significance was defined as p value < 0.05.

Results

Characteristics of the sample have been described in Table 1. In the full sample, the median duration of CSG eligibility for the mothers was 26.25 years (IQR: 16 to 41 years, Range: 1 to 98.5 years). These years of eligibility could be simultaneously accrued over multiple children, with the median number of CSG eligible children in the full sample being 2 (IQR: 1 to 3 children, Range: 1 to 9 children). The median age of the mothers at the time of the study interview was 50 years (IQR: 44 to 54 years, Range: 40 to 59 years) while at the time of CSG implementation in 1998, the median age was 34 years (IQR: 28 to 38 years, Range: 24 years to 43 years). Over four-fifths (86 per cent, 808/944) of the full sample had high duration of CSG eligibility (>10 years). About four-fifths (84 per cent, 328/390) of the mothers with one to four lifetime children and 87 per cent (480/554) of the mothers with five and more lifetime children had high duration of CSG eligibility. Approximately one-third of the mothers (~59 per cent, 554/944) had five or more lifetime number of children. Around one-third (~35 per cent, 329/944) of the mothers had received some secondary or higher education and over half (~55 per cent, 515/944) were currently married. About two-thirds of the mothers (69 per cent, 651/944) were originally from South Africa, and majority (95 per cent, 901/944) were Christian (Table 1). Some sociodemographic characteristics significantly differed across the mothers according to their CSG eligibility status (Table 1). Mothers with high duration of CSG eligibility were younger and had more CSG eligible children than mothers with low duration of CSG eligibility. Mothers with high duration of CSG eligibility were marginally more likely to have achieved some secondary or higher education compared to their counterparts with low duration of CSG eligibility. No meaningful differences by duration of CSG eligibility were observed for lifetime number of children, marital status, country of origin, religion, and wealth index.

thumbnail
Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of child support grant eligible mothers according to low (≤10 years) and high (>10 years) duration of child support grant eligibility, health and ageing in Africa: A longitudinal study of an INDEPTH community in South Africa, 2014/15.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297673.t001

In the full sample, in unadjusted analysis, high duration of CSG eligibility was associated with higher cognitive function z-scores in the mothers (β = 0.27 SD units, 95 per cent CI: 0.11, 0.43). After adjustment for mother’s age, education and lifetime number of children, the magnitude of the observed association was attenuated (adjusted β = 0.15 SD units, 95 per cent CI: 0.04, 0.26; Table 2, Fig 2). In subgroup analyses, high duration of CSG eligibility was associated with higher cognitive function z-scores only among mothers with one to four children (β = 0.37 SD units, 95 per cent CI: 0.12, 0.62). The magnitude of this association was also attenuated after covariate adjustment (adjusted β = 0.19 SD units, 95 per cent CI: 0.05, 0.34; Table 2, Fig 2). In mothers with five or more children, unadjusted analysis indicated that high duration of CSG eligibility was marginally associated with higher cognitive function (β = 0.21 SD units, 95 per cent CI: 0.003, 0.41), but no meaningful associations between duration of CSG eligibility and cognitive function z-scores were observed in adjusted analyses (Table 2).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Adjusteda estimates from ordinary least squares regression models of the cognitive function in child support grant (CSG)-eligible mothers who had high versus low duration of CSG eligibility in the three analytic samples.

aAdjusted for mother’s age, mother’s age squared, mother’s education and lifetime number of children (indicator variables).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297673.g002

thumbnail
Table 2. Estimated effects of high child support grant eligibility (>10 years) compared to low child support grant eligibility (≤10 years) on cognitive function in child support grant-eligible mothers, health and ageing in africa: A longitudinal study of an INDEPTH community in South Africa, 2014/15.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297673.t002

In our sensitivity analyses, we analysed CSG in three ways: a) as a continuous variable and dichotomized the years of CSG eligibility using two different thresholds near the 10-year cut-off, b) one at 8 years and c) another at 12 years (Supplementary Materials: Sensitivity Analysis). As a continuous variable, duration of CSG eligibility was not associated with cognitive function z-scores after adjusting for mother’s age, education, and lifetime number of children (S1 Table in S1 File). When dichotomizing duration of CSG eligibility as ≤8 years (“low”) versus >8 years (“high”), the association between duration of CSG eligibility and cognitive function z-scores for the full sample and subgroups were similar in magnitude and precision (β = 0.27; 95% CI: 0.14, 0.39; p value = 0.007, for mothers with 1–4 children) to the main analysis conducted with the 10-year cut-off (S2 Table in S1 File). When dichotomizing duration of CSG eligibility as ≤12 years (“low”) versus >12 years (“high”), no association was observed between duration of CSG eligibility and cognitive function z scores in the full sample and in the lifetime number of children subgroups (β = 0.05; 95% CI: -0.09, 0.19; p value = 0.33, for mothers with 1–4 children) in adjusted analyses.

Discussion

In this study of mothers in a rural, low-income South African population, we found that a higher duration of eligibility for Child Support Grant was associated with higher subsequent cognitive function z-scores among mothers between ages 40–59 years. We also found that in mothers with one to four children in their lifetimes, CSG eligibility duration was associated with higher subsequent cognitive function z-scores. The magnitude of these associations was substantial. In our full sample, for every 5-year increase in age, the cognitive function z score lowered by an average of 0.18 SD units. Therefore, our study results suggest that over 10 years of cumulative eligibility for regular government cash transfers for child support in this group of mothers in their mid-life was equivalent to a nearly 5-year reduction in their cognitive ageing.

Only three other studies that we are aware of have used natural experiments to evaluate the causal effects of cash transfers on cognitive function in older adults [3133]. In Mexico, cash transfers given to adults aged ≥70 years improved immediate and delayed recall scores with magnitudes that were equivalent to reductions in cognitive ageing of 5.5 and 12.4 years, respectively [31]. In the United States, cash transfers from social security income were associated with clinically meaningful improvements in multiple domains of cognitive function in older adults [32]. In South Africa, men who were exposed to expanded pension eligibility had higher cognitive function [33]. While our sample comprised women from a low-income, rural South African setting, our findings are consistent with the results from Mexico, United States and South Africa. We considered cognitive function across an earlier age range than had been examined in prior work by focusing on South African women in their mid-life. Cognitive decline can start during the middle years of adulthood and progressively worsen in later life [47, 48], potentially leading to ADRD [13]. Early detection of cognitive function changes in mid-life may help support interventions to delay or prevent ADRD onset.

A potential mechanism through which cash transfers may exert protective effects on mid- life cognitive function is via promoting cognitive reserve [17]. Accrual of cognitive reserve is thought to occur over the life course through cumulative cognitively stimulating experiences from childhood through adulthood [17, 49, 50]. People with high cognitive reserve are thought to be able to use compensatory behavioral or neural brain mechanisms to stave off the functional impacts of ageing-related brain pathology [17, 49]. In South Africa, CSG benefits account for nearly 40 per cent of income in the poorest recipient households and are accumulated over time in early- to mid-adulthood for mothers while they are raising children [21, 23]. Increased income, especially when sustained over time, can alleviate poverty, improve living conditions, reduce stress, and improve nutrition and health [22, 23], all of which may build cognitive reserve [51]. Prior research has shown that since the mothers were primary CSG recipients, they had a strong role in making financial decisions regarding the use of CSG benefits [23, 52]. It is possible that mother’s economic empowerment within the household may be protective of their cognitive health. Furthermore, the CSG has been found to strengthen social support for the mothers via improved relationships with neighbors in the community and enabling them to participate in stokvels, which is a rotating savings and credit association [30]. High social support and social engagement is protective against cognitive ageing, particularly in adults in their mid-life [53, 54]. This may be due to them engaging in complex relationships and a feeling a sense of belonging and dignity in the community [55]. Another potential pathway to accrue cognitive reserve might have been expanding access to education in adolescent mothers who might still be in school at the time of CSG implementation. However, in our study sample, the youngest mothers were at least 24 years old in 1998, when CSG was first implemented. Therefore, their opportunities for expanding education might have closed by the time their children were CSG eligible. So, higher education levels might not plausibly explain the observed association between CSG and cognitive function in this group of mothers. Future research is needed to better understand the mechanisms through which improving the financial circumstances and independence of low-income middle-aged mothers may improve their cognitive health as they age.

We did not observe an association between higher duration of CSG eligibility and cognitive function in mothers with grand multiparity (with five or more children). Prior studies have found grand multiparity to be strongly associated with cognitive impairment and ADRD risk, which may overwhelm any protective effects of CSG eligibility for these women [56, 57]. Grand multiparity may result in decreased estrogen levels [58], long-term parenting stress [59], and increased risk of cardiovascular diseases [60], all of which can contribute to cognitive decline [59]. Our sample of rural South African women with a median of five lifetime number of children represented a relatively high fertility setting. There is scarce research on the association between parity and cognitive ageing in this setting and there may be unknown sociodemographic, socioeconomic, or psychosocial factors that mediate or moderate this relationship. Prior work using data from Agincourt HDSS found that the receipt of CSG had not motivated women to have more children [61]. Future studies are planned to investigate the relationships between reproductive history and cognitive ageing outcomes in the HAALSI cohort.

The findings from our sensitivity analysis suggest the use of the 10-year threshold may be adequate. As a continuous variable, duration of CSG eligibility was not associated with cognitive function. In South Africa, the inflation-adjusted food poverty line in April 2021 was 624 Rand (~33 USD) per person per month, while the CSG amount in April 2021 was only 480 Rand (~ 25 dollars) per child per month [62]. Therefore, the CSG benefit was of a magnitude smaller than that needed to cover the cost of the child’s minimum required daily energy intake. However, CSG income is predominantly used to buy food for the child and the household [52], and sometimes is the only source of income in the households [63]. Therefore, it is plausible that a difference of one year of CSG eligibility is likely too little in duration and financial amounts to have had any meaningful impacts on the cognitive function of the mothers. We also tested the sensitivity of our threshold cut point at 10 years of CSG eligibility. When using the 8-year cut-off, our results were similar to the main analysis, suggesting that higher duration of CSG eligibility may be protective of the cognitive health of mothers with fewer children but not for those with grand multiparity. However, when we used the 12-year cut-off, we observed no association between CSG eligibility and cognitive function for mothers with 1–4 children, suggesting that the cut-off greater than 10 may be too high to detect any relationships between CSG eligibility and cognitive function. It is also possible that for mothers with 1–4 children, to be eligible for longer durations of CSG means having multiple children within short birth spacing intervals, which may neutralize any effect of CSG on mother’s cognitive function.

Limitations and strengths

Several aspects of the experimental design and data structure warrant careful interpretation of the results. We utilized duration of CSG eligibility and not actual CSG receipt in our analyses. The true relationship between CSG receipt and cognitive function may be different from our findings. However, given that not all mothers with CSG-eligible children applied for the grant [22], our exposed group included eligible mothers who did not receive the full CSG income we attributed to her. Therefore, our study findings plausibly yield more conservative estimates than we would expect if we used the actual amount of CSG received as our exposure. Furthermore, this method of exposure calculation has been previously used in South Africa to assess the relationship between years of Old Age Pension eligibility and health outcomes in older adults [64]. Cognitive function was measured at a single point in time, and we could not assess within-person cognitive change over time or incidence of cognitive impairment or ADRD. As the current cohort ages, studies using extended follow-up in HAALSI have been planned which will allow us to track cognitive change during ageing among the middle-aged women in the current sample. Cognitive function was assessed using measures for orientation and episodic memory, which does not include all possible domains of cognitive function. To address this issue, we ran a sensitivity analysis using cognitive function outcome that included numeracy measures in addition to orientation, and episodic memory measures (S4 Table in S1 File). Our results were similar, with higher CSG eligibility duration associated with higher cognitive function scores in mothers with 1–4 children. Future studies should investigate other domains, such as executive function, language, or processing speed to gain a better understanding of whether and how cash transfers affect different domains of cognitive function. Longitudinal studies with large sample sizes and actual CSG receipt data are necessary to elucidate the relationship between cash transfers and cognitive decline in South African women in their later-life. It is critical to know how long any cognitive health benefits of the CSG received in early- to mid-adulthood may last as beneficiaries age. It is also possible that pre-HAALSI baseline mortality patterns in women may have biased our findings, as aging cohorts are vulnerable to selective survival bias [65]. However, women who died prior to enrollment and women who were enrolled in HAALSI had very similar household asset indices (2.2 vs 2.3, respectively, from unpublished analyses using underlying vital statistics data collected by the Agincourt HDSS [20], covering the source population of the HAALSI cohort). Therefore, we believe that differences in household wealth between those enrolled in HAALSI and those not enrolled may not be large in magnitude to severely bias our results. That said, our findings should be interpreted as conditioned on survival until age 40 years and older in rural South Africa. Finally, our study findings may have limited generalizability outside of a rural South African setting.

This study has several strengths. We used data from a population-representative sample in a setting that is under-represented in the global dementia evidence base [34]. We used a quasi-randomized exposure variable for duration of CSG eligibility based on the CSG age-eligibility expansions and children’s birth timings. This approach has also been used in previous research [28, 66]. This study data, which reports CSG eligibility duration since its inception until January 2016, extends prior work on CSG receipt that have used self-reported data over a shorter follow-up period and/or used self-reported data to identify thresholds of CSG receipt duration [67, 68]. Since the increases in CSG amount over time was to match inflation and not to increase in value, the duration of CSG eligibility was considered an adequate proxy for CSG receipt. This approach allowed us to avoid recall or social desirability bias that can occur when self-reports are used or confounding by individual uptake patterns. This design strengthens causal inference for the association between CSG eligibility and mid- life cognitive function. We used validated and reliable measures of cognitive function, which are harmonized with those used in the US HRS and its International Partner Studies around the world and are meaningful for ADRD risk [43]. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study examining the causal effects of cash transfer eligibility on mid- life cognitive function of mothers in a rural South African setting. Our findings bring evidence to policymakers for designing social protection plans that promote healthy cognitive ageing in mothers from rural, low-income settings.

Conclusion

Our findings suggest that eligibility for cash transfers to support young children may protect the cognitive health of maternal recipients. This finding is critical since it indicates cash transfers can make promising interventions to protect cognitive health; and that CSG confers long-term cognitive health benefits to mothers in addition to children and households at the time of transfer. Future research should investigate how parity affects this relationship, how this relationship may vary by the actual financial value of cumulative cash transfer receipt, and whether the protective effects of cash transfers like the CSG extend to later-life cognitive outcomes as beneficiaries age. Better understanding of the relationship between cash transfers and cognitive ageing is crucial, as this evidence would allow for policy recommendations, change in cost-benefit arguments and development of income supplementation programmes to promote healthy cognitive ageing in low-income settings.

Supporting information

S1 File. Child support grant variable construction and sensitivity analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297673.s001

(DOCX)

S2 File. Mapping child support grant exposure.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297673.s002

(XLS)

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Audrey Pettifor and Erica Beidelman for their valuable contributions towards this manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Nichols E, Steinmetz JD, Vollset SE, Fukutaki K, Chalek J, Abd-Allah F, et al. Estimation of the global prevalence of dementia in 2019 and forecasted prevalence in 2050: an analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. The Lancet Public Health. 2022;7(2):e105–e25. pmid:34998485
  2. 2. Long S, Benoist C, Weidner W. World Alzheimer Report 2023: Reducing dementia risk: never too early, never too late. London, England: Alzheimer’s Disease International., 2023.
  3. 3. Qiu C, Kivipelto M, von Strauss E. Epidemiology of Alzheimer’s disease: occurrence, determinants, and strategies toward intervention. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2009;11(2):111–28. Epub 2009/07/10. pmid:19585947.
  4. 4. Guerchet M, Prince M, Prina M. Numbers of people with dementia worldwide: An update to the estimates in the World Alzheimer Report 2015. 2020.
  5. 5. Guerchet MM, Mayston R, Lloyd-Sherlock P, Prince MJ, Aboderin I, Akinyemi R, et al. Dementia in sub-Saharan Africa: challenges and opportunities. London: Alzheimer’s Disease International; 2017.
  6. 6. Mavrodaris A, Powell J, Thorogood M. Prevalences of dementia and cognitive impairment among older people in sub-Saharan Africa: a systematic review. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 2013;91:773–83. pmid:24115801
  7. 7. Akinyemi RO, Yaria J, Ojagbemi A, Guerchet M, Okubadejo N, Njamnshi AK, et al. Dementia in Africa: Current evidence, knowledge gaps, and future directions. Alzheimers Dement. 2021. Epub 2021/09/28. pmid:34569714.
  8. 8. Whalley LJ, Dick FD, McNeill G. A life-course approach to the aetiology of late-onset dementias. The Lancet Neurology. 2006;5(1):87–96. pmid:16361026
  9. 9. Glymour MM, Manly JJ. Lifecourse social conditions and racial and ethnic patterns of cognitive aging. Neuropsychology review. 2008;18(3):223–54. pmid:18815889
  10. 10. Dekhtyar S, Wang HX, Scott K, Goodman A, Koupil I, Herlitz A. A Life-Course Study of Cognitive Reserve in Dementia—From Childhood to Old Age. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2015;23(9):885–96. Epub 2015/03/10. pmid:25746486.
  11. 11. Scazufca M, Menezes PR, Araya R, Di Rienzo VD, Almeida OP, Gunnell D, et al. Risk factors across the life course and dementia in a Brazilian population: results from the Sao Paulo Ageing & Health Study (SPAH). Int J Epidemiol. 2008;37(4):879–90. Epub 2008/06/28. pmid:18583392.
  12. 12. Ma J, Yang Y, Wan Y, Shen C, Qiu P. The influence of childhood adversities on mid to late cognitive function: From the perspective of life course. PLoS One. 2021;16(8):e0256297. Epub 2021/08/17. pmid:34398901.
  13. 13. Wilson RS, Leurgans SE, Boyle PA, Bennett DA. Cognitive decline in prodromal Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Archives of neurology. 2011;68(3):351–6. pmid:21403020
  14. 14. Turrell G, Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Everson SA, Helkala E-L, Kauhanen J, et al. Socioeconomic position across the lifecourse and cognitive function in late middle age. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2002;57(1):S43–S51. pmid:11773232
  15. 15. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. The Lancet Neurology. 2012;11(11):1006–12. pmid:23079557
  16. 16. Wilson RS, Scherr PA, Hoganson G, Bienias JL, Evans DA, Bennett DA. Early life socioeconomic status and late life risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroepidemiology. 2005;25(1):8–14. pmid:15855799
  17. 17. Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the international neuropsychological society. 2002;8(3):448–60. pmid:11939702
  18. 18. Jefferson AL, Gibbons LE, Rentz DM, Carvalho JO, Manly J, Bennett DA, et al. A life course model of cognitive activities, socioeconomic status, education, reading ability, and cognition. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2011;59(8):1403–11. pmid:21797830
  19. 19. de Jager CA, Joska JA, Hoffman M, Borochowitz KE, Combrinck MI. Dementia in rural South Africa: A pressing need for epidemiological studies. S Afr Med J. 2015;105(3):189–90. Epub 2015/08/22. pmid:26294824.
  20. 20. Kahn K, Collinson MA, Gómez-Olivé FX, Mokoena O, Twine R, Mee P, et al. Profile: Agincourt health and socio-demographic surveillance system. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;41(4):988–1001. Epub 2012/08/31. pmid:22933647.
  21. 21. McEwen H, Kannemeyer C, Woolard I. Social assistance grants: analysis of the NIDS Wave 1 dataset. 2009.
  22. 22. DSD, SASSA, UNICEF. The South African Child Support Grant Impact Assessment: Evidence from a survey of children, adolescents and their households. Pretoria: UNICEF South Africa. 2012.
  23. 23. Delany A, Ismail Z, Graham L, Ramkissoon Y. Review of the child support grant: Uses, implementation and obstacles. Johannesburg: Community Agency for Social Enquiry. 2008.
  24. 24. Case A, Hosegood V, Lund F. The reach and impact of Child Support Grants: evidence from KwaZulu-Natal. Development Southern Africa. 2005;22(4):467–82.
  25. 25. Twine R, Collinson MA, Polzer TJ, Kahn K. Evaluating access to a child-oriented poverty alleviation intervention in rural South Africa. Scand J Public Health Suppl. 2007;69:118–27. Epub 2007/09/06. pmid:17676512.
  26. 26. Handa S, Otchere F, Sirma P, Team TES. More Evidence on the Impact of Government Social Protection in Sub Saharan Africa: Ghana, Malawi and Zimbabwe. Development Policy Review. 2021;n/a(n/a).
  27. 27. Eyal K, Woolard I. Throwing the Book at the CSG, South African Labour and Development Research Unit. Working Paper series 53, 2011.
  28. 28. Ohrnberger J, Fichera E, Sutton M, Anselmi L. The effect of cash transfers on mental health–new evidence from South Africa. BMC public health. 2020;20(1):1–13.
  29. 29. Patel L., Poverty Gender and Social Protection: Child Support Grants in Soweto, South Africa. Journal of Policy Practice. 2012;11(1–2):106–20.
  30. 30. Granlund S, Hochfeld T. ‘That Child Support Grant Gives Me Powers’–Exploring Social and Relational Aspects of Cash Transfers in South Africa in Times of Livelihood Change. The Journal of Development Studies. 2020;56(6):1230–44.
  31. 31. Aguila E, Kapteyn A, Smith JP. Effects of income supplementation on health of the poor elderly: The case of Mexico. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015;112(1):70–5. pmid:25535388
  32. 32. Ayyagari P, Frisvold D. The impact of social security income on cognitive function at older ages. American Journal of Health Economics. 2016;2(4):463–88.
  33. 33. Jock J, Kobayashi L, Chakraborty R, Chen X, Wing C, Berkman L, et al. Effects of Pension Eligibility Expansion on Men’s Cognitive Function: Findings from Rural South Africa. J Aging Soc Policy. 2023:1–20. Epub 20230328. pmid:36975023.
  34. 34. Gómez-Olivé FX, Montana L, Wagner RG, Kabudula CW, Rohr JK, Kahn K, et al. Cohort Profile: Health and Ageing in Africa: A Longitudinal Study of an INDEPTH Community in South Africa (HAALSI). Int J Epidemiol. 2018;47(3):689–90j. Epub 2018/01/13. pmid:29325152.
  35. 35. Gómez-Olivé FX, Angotti N, Houle B, Klipstein-Grobusch K, Kabudula C, Menken J, et al. Prevalence of HIV among those 15 and older in rural South Africa. AIDS Care. 2013;25(9):1122–8. Epub 2013/01/15. pmid:23311396.
  36. 36. Hall K. Child Support Grants South Africa2021 [cited 2021 December 5th]. http://childrencount.uct.ac.za/indicator.php?domain=2&indicator=10.
  37. 37. South African Social Security Agency. Child Support Grant South Africa2023 [cited 2023]. https://www.gov.za/services/child-care-social-benefits/child-support-gran.
  38. 38. Department of Health/South Africa, Macro International. South Africa Demographic and Health Survey 1998. Pretoria, South Africa: Department of Health/South Africa, 2002.
  39. 39. Zembe-Mkabile W, Doherty T, Sanders D, Jackson D, Chopra M, Swanevelder S, et al. Why do families still not receive the child support grant in South Africa? A longitudinal analysis of a cohort of families across South Africa. BMC international health and human rights. 2012;12:24-. pmid:23088827.
  40. 40. Kobayashi LC, Mateen FJ, Montana L, Wagner RG, Kahn K, Tollman SM, et al. Cognitive function and impairment in older, rural South African adults: evidence from “health and aging in Africa: a longitudinal study of an INDEPTH community in rural South Africa”. Neuroepidemiology. 2019;52(1–2):32–40.
  41. 41. Bäckman L, Small BJ, Fratiglioni L. Stability of the preclinical episodic memory deficit in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2001;124(1):96–102. pmid:11133790
  42. 42. Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging. 2009;30(4):507–14. pmid:19231028
  43. 43. Kobayashi LC, Gross AL, Gibbons LE, Tommet D, Sanders RE, Choi S-E, et al. You say tomato, I say radish: can brief cognitive assessments in the US Health Retirement Study be harmonized with its International Partner Studies? The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 2020.
  44. 44. Wu Q, Tchetgen Tchetgen EJ, Osypuk TL, White K, Mujahid M, Maria Glymour M. Combining direct and proxy assessments to reduce attrition bias in a longitudinal study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2013;27(3):207–12. Epub 2012/09/21. pmid:22992720.
  45. 45. Kobayashi LC, Glymour MM, Kahn K, Payne CF, Wagner RG, Montana L, et al. Childhood deprivation and later-life cognitive function in a population-based study of older rural South Africans. Soc Sci Med. 2017;190:20–8. Epub 20170814. pmid:28837862.
  46. 46. Mgaya AH, Massawe SN, Kidanto HL, Mgaya HN. Grand multiparity: is it still a risk in pregnancy? BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth. 2013;13(1):241. pmid:24365087
  47. 47. Hughes ML, Agrigoroaei S, Jeon M, Bruzzese M, Lachman ME. Change in cognitive performance from midlife into old age: Findings from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2018;24(8):805–20. pmid:30019663
  48. 48. Ferreira D, Correia R, Nieto A, Machado A, Molina Y, Barroso J. Cognitive decline before the age of 50 can be detected with sensitive cognitive measures. Psicothema. 2015;27(3):216–22. pmid:26260927
  49. 49. Whalley LJ, Deary IJ, Appleton CL, Starr JM. Cognitive reserve and the neurobiology of cognitive aging. Ageing research reviews. 2004;3(4):369–82. pmid:15541707
  50. 50. Berkman LF. The association between educational attainment and mental status examinations: of etiologic significance for senile dementias or not? J Chronic Dis. 1986;39(3):171–5. Epub 1986/01/01. pmid:3949941.
  51. 51. Richards M, Deary IJ. A life course approach to cognitive reserve: a model for cognitive aging and development? Annals of Neurology: Official Journal of the American Neurological Association and the Child Neurology Society. 2005;58(4):617–22.
  52. 52. DSD, SASSA, UNICEF. Child Support Grant Evaluation 2010: Qualitative Research Report. Pretoria: UNICEF South Africa. 2011.
  53. 53. Kats D, Patel MD, Palta P, Meyer ML, Gross AL, Whitsel EA, et al. Social support and cognition in a community-based cohort: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Age Ageing. 2016;45(4):475–80. Epub 20160421. pmid:27107128.
  54. 54. Harling G, Kobayashi LC, Farrell MT, Wagner RG, Tollman S, Berkman L. Social contact, social support, and cognitive health in a population-based study of middle-aged and older men and women in rural South Africa. Soc Sci Med. 2020;260:113167. Epub 20200706. pmid:32688161.
  55. 55. Berkman LF. Which influences cognitive function: living alone or being alone? The Lancet. 2000;355(9212):1291–2. pmid:10776738
  56. 56. Bae JB, Lipnicki DM, Han JW, Sachdev PS, Kim TH, Kwak KP, et al. Does parity matter in women’s risk of dementia? A COSMIC collaboration cohort study. BMC Med. 2020;18(1):210. Epub 2020/08/06. pmid:32753059.
  57. 57. Bae JB, Lipnicki DM, Han JW, Sachdev PS, Kim TH, Kwak KP, et al. Parity and the risk of incident dementia: a COSMIC study. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2020;29:e176. Epub 2020/10/21. pmid:33077022.
  58. 58. Li FD, He F, Chen TR, Xiao YY, Lin ST, Shen W, et al. Reproductive history and risk of cognitive impairment in elderly women: a cross-sectional study in eastern China. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;49(1):139–47. Epub 2015/10/08. pmid:26444784.
  59. 59. Saenz JL, Díaz-Venegas C, Crimmins EM. Fertility history and cognitive function in late life: The case of Mexico. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 2021;76(4):e140–e52.
  60. 60. Parikh NI, Cnattingius S, Dickman PW, Mittleman MA, Ludvigsson JF, Ingelsson E. Parity and risk of later-life maternal cardiovascular disease. Am Heart J. 2010;159(2):215–21.e6. Epub 2010/02/16. pmid:20152219.
  61. 61. Rosenberg M, Pettifor A, Nguyen N, Westreich D, Bor J, Bärnighausen T, et al. Relationship between Receipt of a Social Protection Grant for a Child and Second Pregnancy Rates among South African Women: A Cohort Study. PLoS One. 2015;10(9):e0137352. Epub 2015/09/24. pmid:26398678.
  62. 62. Statistics South Africa. National Poverty Lines 2021. Pretoria, South Africa: 2022.
  63. 63. Luthuli S, Haskins L, Mapumulo S, Horwood C. Does the unconditional cash transfer program in South Africa provide support for women after child birth? Barriers to accessing the child support grant among women in informal work in Durban, South Africa. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):112. Epub 20220116. pmid:35034606.
  64. 64. Riumallo Herl C, Kabudula C, Kahn K, Tollman S, Canning D. Pension exposure and health: Evidence from a longitudinal study in South Africa. The Journal of the Economics of Ageing. 2022;23:100411. pmid:36505964
  65. 65. Weuve J, Proust-Lima C, Power MC, Gross AL, Hofer SM, Thiébaut R, et al. Guidelines for reporting methodological challenges and evaluating potential bias in dementia research. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11(9):1098–109. pmid:26397878.
  66. 66. Garman EC, Eyal K, Avendano M, Evans-Lacko S, Lund C. Cash transfers and the mental health of young people: Evidence from South Africa’s child support grant. Soc Sci Med. 2022;292:114631. Epub 20211204. pmid:34891031.
  67. 67. Cluver L, Boyes M, Orkin M, Pantelic M, Molwena T, Sherr L. Child-focused state cash transfers and adolescent risk of HIV infection in South Africa: a propensity-score-matched case-control study. The Lancet Global Health. 2013;1(6):e362–e70. pmid:25104601
  68. 68. Zembe-Mkabile W, Ramokolo V, Sanders D, Jackson D, Doherty T. The dynamic relationship between cash transfers and child health: can the child support grant in South Africa make a difference to child nutrition? Public Health Nutr. 2016;19(2):356–62. Epub 20150608. pmid:26050975.