Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Herding-like behaviour in medical decision making: An experimental study investigating general practitioners’ prescription behaviour

  • Sandro Tiziano Stoffel ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    s.stoffel@ucl.ac.uk

    Affiliations Research Department of Behavioural Science and Health, UCL, London, United Kingdom, Institute of Pharmaceutical Medicine (ECPM), University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland, Department of Public Health, Health Economics Facility, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland

  • Rachel Spencer,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Unit of Academic Primary Care, Warwick Medical School, University of Warwick, Coventry, United Kingdom

  • Judit Konya,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Exeter Collaboration for Academic Primary Care (APEx), University of Exeter, Exeter, United Kingdom

  • Ivo Vlaev,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Warwick Business School, University of Warwick, Coventry, United Kingdom

  • Matthias Schwenkglenks

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Institute of Pharmaceutical Medicine (ECPM), University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland, Department of Public Health, Health Economics Facility, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland

Abstract

Previous observational studies have indicated that social influences, such as arising from herding-like behaviour, can contribute to medical errors. In this study, we experimentally examined whether general practitioners (GPs) would follow incorrect prescription recommendations from fellow GP or specialists. To investigate this, we conducted an online survey with 475 GPs practicing in England that included two case vignettes. Case vignette 1 focused on sleeping tablets, and case vignette 2 was centred around antibiotics. The vignettes were presented in random order, and within each vignette, study participants were assigned to one of three experimental conditions: control condition (lacking peer recommendation), fellow GP condition (including a recommendation from a fellow GP not aligned with best practice clinical guidelines), or specialist condition (including a recommendation from a specialist not aligned with best practice clinical guidelines). The primary outcome measure was the proportion of GPs who prescribed medication that deviated from best practice clinical guidelines. We found that, in both case vignettes, the percentage of respondents prescribing such medication was highest in those assigned to the specialist condition, followed by those assigned to the control condition. It was lowest in those assigned to the fellow GP condition (case vignette 1: 73.8% vs. 55.6% vs. 36.6% and case vignette 2: 24.0% vs. 12.4% vs. 10.1%). In the case of vignette 1, the difference between the fellow GP condition and the control condition is statistically significant, suggesting that GPs are less likely to prescribe sleeping tablets when recommended by a fellow GP. This implies that GPs are more inclined to prescribe non-guideline-recommended medication when advised by specialists. This study is the first to experimentally demonstrate that physician herding behaviour can result in prescription errors. Future research could extend this inquiry to diverse contexts, including diagnosis.

Introduction

The majority of individuals who interact with healthcare providers typically receive high-quality care. However, some may encounter medical errors, such as incorrect diagnoses, treatments, and prescriptions, which can be detrimental to patients and result in significant financial consequences for health systems [13]. Systematic literature reviews have revealed the frequent occurrence of medical errors across various healthcare settings, including prescribing and prescription errors [4,5]. These errors can account for up to 11% of all prescriptions, with dosage errors being a primary concern [5,6]. Prescription-making is a complex task influenced by various factors, including physicians’ therapeutic training, drug knowledge, experience, understanding of the patient, perception of risk, and their own physical and emotional well-being [7,8]. While unsurprisingly, especially newly graduated physicians are prone to making prescription errors, senior doctors make errors too, albeit at a lower rate [911]. Recent studies have suggested that social influences, including social norms [12], informational social influence [13], and compliance [14], can influence physicians’ decision-making, potentially resulting in prescription errors [15]. These influences may lead to herding-like behaviour, where physicians follow recommendations from colleagues rather than making their own informed decisions based on available information. In some cases, this behaviour can lead to physicians adopting wrong recommendations. Social influences have been vastly studied in the context of stock markets [1620]. There experiments have shown that even in the ideal environment of perfect knowledge, the study participants followed the decision of others, creating bubbles and market crashes [19]. Herding behaviour has been described as response to uncertainty [20].

So far little is known about herding-like behaviour in medical decision making. Only three non-experimental studies investigated the social influence of peers on physicians’ decision making [15,21,22]. The first study used a cross-sectional survey to investigate the prevalence and associated factors of social influences in multiple sclerosis (MS) care in Spain [15]. In the survey, neurologists with expertise in MS care were presented with a case vignette that featured a treatment recommendation of a peer, which was not supported by best practice clinical guidelines. Note that while the authors suggest that around 78% of the neurologists showed herding-like behaviour, in that they followed the wrong recommendation, the study did not feature a control condition without a treatment recommendation. In multivariate analyses, experience in the form of the number of patients seen per week was found to be positively associated with herding-like behaviour. Neurologists’ personality traits, such as risk aversion, aversion to ambiguity, and low tolerance to uncertainty, however, showed no association with herding-like behaviour.

The second study utilized longitudinal data on prescribed antipsychotic drugs for schizophrenia patients in Taiwan and found that physicians’ prescriptions of newly introduced drugs were influenced by the observed behaviour of their colleagues [21]. Peer influence on acceptance of a risky new prescription drug was similarly identified by Iyengar and colleagues [22] in an extensive survey. In their study, peer influence had an impact on both initial trial prescriptions and subsequent repeat orders for a novel and risky drug, particularly among physicians who did not view themselves as opinion leaders.

Study overview, objectives and hypothesis

Despite this observational evidence indicating social influences on physicians’ decision-making, there is a lack of experimental studies comparing decision-making with and without input from peers. In the current study, we conducted an experiment to determine whether general practitioners (GPs) follow prescription recommendations made by other medical professionals. Specifically, we employed a randomised, web-based behavioural experiment to assess whether incorrect recommendations from other physicians could lead to potential prescription errors in a primary care setting, which is a highly relevant area. In England, the cost of medicines prescribed in primary care in 2022 and 2023 was £9.59 billion, 50.0% of total expenditure [23]. To prescribe medications optimally GPs must assess the appropriateness of medication for each patient, including considerations such as dosage, frequency, and type of medication to be used [24]. Our study seeks to address the following research question (RQ):

RQ: To what extent do GPs follow prescription recommendations of other physicians?

Methods

To address this research question, we formulated three working hypotheses (WH). The first hypothesis builds on the focus theory of normative conduct [25] and assumes that the GP or consultant’s recommendation is a social norm of the descriptive type that refers to perceptions of how others would behave in this situation [26]. Focus theory predicts that the peer’s recommendation provides the decision-makers with a standard from which they do not want to deviate in the given situation [27]. By simply registering what others have done in the same situation and imitating their actions, one can usually choose efficiently and well how to behave [26]. Thus, rather than making their own informed decision based on available information, GPs will follow the prescription recommendation of other medical professionals.

WH1: GPs in the treatment conditions are more likely to select the prescription option that is not supported by best practice clinical guidelines than those in the control condition.

The second hypothesis addresses that GPs may believe the expertise of specialists is better than that of fellow GPs, as specialists are senior physicians, trained and certified in specific areas of medical practice.

WH2: A recommendation from a specialist will further increase the likelihood of selecting the prescription that is not supported by best practice clinical guidelines than a recommendation from a fellow GP.

The third hypothesis assumes, in line with previous studies on reputation-based herding, that the herding-like behaviour is accentuated in GPs with more work experience [15,28].

WH3: GP’s working experience is positively correlated with herding-like behaviour.

We tested the hypotheses through a randomized online experiment involving General Practitioners (GPs). The experiment was run in the first week of July 2023 and utilized a three-condition between-subjects design, comprising a control condition without a recommendation from a peer and two treatment conditions. Both treatment conditions involved a prescription recommendation that is not supported by best practice clinical guidelines. In the first condition, the recommendation originated from a fellow GP, while the second treatment condition included a recommendation from a specialist (geriatrician/respiratory consultant).

Ethical approval and preregistration

The protocols for the study received ethics approval from the Humanities and Social Sciences Research Ethics Committee (HSSREC) of University Warwick (approval number HSSREC 155/22-23) and was preregistered on Open Science Framework before data collection.

Vignettes

In order to test the working hypotheses, we conducted a web-based experiment that presented GPs two case vignettes in random order (screenshots of the vignettes are presented in S1 and S2 Figs). After consenting to participate in the study, participants were asked some basic questions about their work as GP, including the number of patients they see, their work experience, and the number of other GPs practicing in their clinic. They were then presented with two case vignettes, which were adapted by medical experts from previous literature and tested in pilot studies with experts in the field of academic primary care as well as practicing primary care physicians before the study was conducted. One case vignette described a situation of potentially inappropriate prescribing of sleeping tablets to a 70-year-old patient who was having trouble sleeping [29]. The second case vignette focused on antibiotic prescription and described a 27-year-old woman with no known underlying lung disease. She presented with a 10-day history of cough, producing yellow non-bloody sputum [30]. For each case vignette, participants had to select one of two prescription options, of which one was supported by best practice clinical guidelines (no medication) and the other was not (sleeping tablets in the first case vignette and antibiotics in the second case vignette)

Outcome measures

The primary outcome of the study was the proportion of GPs who prescribed the medication. Specifically, participants were asked which of the two available prescriptions they would recommend in each vignette, 1) the prescription of a sleeping tablet/antibiotics or 2) no medication. Both prescription options were presented in random order. For each vignette, we compared the proportion of participants who recommended the incorrect prescription across the experimental conditions.

Secondary outcomes included findings on perceived decision difficulty and decision effort. Study participants were asked after each vignette how difficult it was for them to decide, using a fully labelled five-point Likert scale (‘Not at all’, ‘Slightly’, ‘Moderately’, ‘Very’ and ‘Extremely’) in response to the question ‘How difficult was it for you to make the recommendation?’ They were also asked to indicate their decision effort on a similar fully labelled five-point Likert scale (‘None’, ‘Little’, ‘Some’, ‘Considerate’, and ‘Great’) for the question ‘How much effort did you put into making the recommendation?’ Both questions were adapted and simplified from a 12-item subjective measurement of mental load and mental effort [31].

Additionally, we included ten questions from the decision style scale (DSS), which capture a broad range of rational and intuitive styles [32]. The scale contains five items on rational and five on intuitive dimensions. The response options featured fully labelled five-point ratings Likert scales (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). The global scores of the two decision styles are calculated as the sum of the corresponding five items and range between 5 and 25.

Finally, individual risk preferences were assessed by asking: “Generally, in the different domains of your daily life, would you describe yourself as someone who tries to avoid risks (risk-averse) or as someone who is fully prepared to take risks (risk-prone). Please answer on a scale from 0 to 10, where 0 means “risk-averse” and 10 means “risk-prone”. The question was adapted from previous studies [33,34].

Statistical analysis

To test the first two working hypotheses regarding the influence of peer recommendations on the proportion of participants prescribing a medication, we employed Chi-square tests of independence and binary logistic regressions. These analyses were adjusted for experimental conditions, decision style, risk preferences, and sociodemographic variables. Regarding the third working hypothesis, we used binary logistic regressions with an interaction term between experimental condition and work experience. The impact on perceived decision difficulty and cognitive effort was analysed using unadjusted and adjusted ordered logistic regressions. Additionally, we used Chi-square tests of independence, Fisher’s exact test, and ANOVAs to compare the characteristics of the study participants across the experimental conditions. Bonferroni adjusted significance levels were used for multiple comparisons between the three experimental conditions.

Sample size

The sample size was calculated before data collection using estimates from previous studies for the control condition [29,30]. A sample size calculation suggested minimum group sizes of 150 participants per experimental condition to be able to detect differences of at least 17% in the proportion of participants choosing to prescribe medication between two conditions [35]).

Results

Study participants

Fig 1 depicts the flow of the survey. In total, 1195 GPs practicing in England and registered on a survey panel (M3 Global Research) received invitations to participate in the study. Among the 547 (45.8%) who accessed the survey, 517 (94.5%) provided their consent to participate. A total of 37 study participants (7.2%) were excluded due to not having worked as a general practitioner for at least 2 years, and 5 respondents (1.0%) did not complete the survey, resulting in a final sample of 475 responders.

The characteristics of the participating GPs are summarized in Table 1. The most frequent categories among respondents were as follows: up to 39 years old (44.0%), female (54.1%), with 11 to 20 years of work experience (34.3%), practicing in a facility with 6 to 10 practitioners (41.7%), and serving more than 10,000 patients (54.1%). The distribution of the risk preferences, rational decision-making score and intuitive decision-making score are presented in S3S5 Figs. On average, participating GPs exhibited tendencies toward risk aversion (Mean 4.34, Standard Deviation (SD) 2.00) and displayed a rational decision-making style (Mean 21.12, SD 2.52), with only a somewhat intuitive approach (Mean 14.25, SD 3.24). The characteristics were balanced across the three experimental conditions in the two case vignettes (see S1 and S2 Tables).

Effect on prescription behaviour

In line with working hypothesis 2, Table 2 and Fig 2 indicate that GPs were more inclined to prescribe a medication when they received a comparable recommendation from a specialist. In both case vignettes, the percentage of respondents who prescribed the medication was highest among those exposed to the specialist condition, followed by individuals in the control condition, and finally, those in the fellow GP condition (case vignette 1: 73.8% vs. 55.6% vs. 36.6%, χ2(2, N = 475) = 48.37, p<0.001 and case vignette 2: 24.0% vs. 12.4% vs. 10.1%, χ2(2, N = 475) = 13.19, p = 0.001).

thumbnail
Table 2. Binary logistic regressions on prescribing a medication that deviates from best practice clinical guidelines in the two case vignettes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.t002

The adjusted regressions confirmed that receiving a recommendation from a specialist increased the probability of prescribing sleeping tablets (adjusted odds ratio (aOR) 2.17, 95% Confidence Interval (CI): 1.33–3.55, p = 0.002) and antibiotics (aOR 2.40, 95%CI: 1.30–4.47, p = 0.005). The recommendation of the fellow GP only influenced the prescription decision in the sleeping tablet vignette (aOR 0.46 95%CI: 0.26–0.68, p<0.001) but not in the antibiotics vignette (aOR 0.89 95%CI: 0.42–1.88, p = 0.751).

We found limited evidence for the third working hypothesis (see S4 Fig). The regression models for the sleeping tablet case vignette, which incorporated an interaction term for working experience and experimental condition, demonstrated similar patterns for GPs with both less than 10 years of experience and those with more than 10 years (see S5 and S6 Tables and S6 Fig). In both groups, the highest percentage of respondents prescribed the medication in the specialist condition, followed by the control condition, and then the fellow GP condition (up to 10 years: 71.1% vs. 55.8% vs. 30.6%, χ2(2, N = 241) = 25.74, p<0.001; and more than 10 years: 76.6% vs. 55.3% vs. 38.3%, χ2(2, N = 223) = 23.67, p<0.001). While the patterns were similar in the antibiotics case vignette, there was only a statistically significant influence of the recommendations on the prescription decisions in those with more than 10 years of work experience (up to 10 years: 18.2% vs. 13.7% vs. 5.8%, χ2(2, N = 241) = 5.07, p = 0.079; and more than 10 years: 28.9% vs. 10.7% vs. 14.5%, χ2(2, N = 223) = 10.09, p = 0.006)

Effect on perceived decision difficulty and decision effort

Fig 3 shows the perceived decision difficulty in the two case vignettes.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Perceived decision difficulty regarding the prescription decisions in the two case vignettes [1;5].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.g003

The ordered logistic regressions, as presented in Table 3, indicate that recommendations from peers led to an increase in the perceived decision difficulty in the antibiotics case vignette (fellow GP vs control: aOR 1.55 95%CI: 1.00–2.39, p = 0.046, specialist vs: control condition: aOR 2.64 95%CI: 1.75–3.98, p<0.001).

thumbnail
Table 3. Ordered logistic regression on perceived decision difficulty [1;5].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.t003

The results in Table 4 demonstrate that the experimental manipulation did not exert an impact on decisional effort.

thumbnail
Table 4. Ordered logistic regression on perceived decision effort [1;5].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.t004

a Covariates included in the adjusted models are GP’s age, gender, work experience, number of GPs working in the practice, number of patients, region in which GP practices, risk preferences, rational decision-making score and intuitive decision-making score. The full models are presented in the S8 Table.

Discussion

This study represents the first experimental investigation into herding-like behaviour among GPs. Utilizing a web-based survey with case vignettes adapted from previous literature, we have demonstrated that the prescription decisions of GPs regarding sleeping tables and antibiotics may be influenced by recommendations. Notably, GPs were more willing to prescribe when receiving advice from a specialist. Conversely, a similar recommendation from a fellow GP influenced prescription decisions only in the sleeping tablet scenario, where GPs were less inclined to prescribe it. The results of this study partially validate earlier observational research which indicated that recommendations from other healthcare professionals can lead GPs into making medical errors [15,21,22]. While our research found evidence suggesting that herding-like behaviour could lead to prescription errors and in some cases protect against them, it offered limited insight into the mechanisms and motivations behind this phenomenon. Survey participants indicated that the decision whether to prescribe antibiotics became more challenging when they received recommendations to do so from a fellow GP or specialist. While this observation suggests that the introduction of additional information may have caused some confusion, the results do not indicate an increase in cognitive effort. The different frequency of choice of the control condition between the two case vignettes might be attributed to the GPs’ awareness of antibiotic resistance. GPs may be more reluctant to prescribe antibiotics due to their concerns about the broader issue of antimicrobial resistance [36]. Possible explanations for the influence of the specialists’ recommendation, but not the recommendation from the fellow GP, could include reputation effects or asymmetric information effects [3739]. The reputation effect is rooted in injunctive social norms, wherein individuals are influenced by the behaviour or opinions of those with high reputation or perceived status [27]. In our study, GPs might have adhered to specialists’ recommendations due to specialists’ status as opinion leaders and their standing as reputable, successful, or credible figures in the medical field. Studies investigating stock market behaviour found a positive correlation between the reputation and credibility of forecasts and herding-like behaviour [39]. Furthermore, the asymmetric information effect suggests that GPs assume specialists possessed more comprehensive information when making the recommendations [37,38]. Asymmetric information can create situations where GPs fear making the wrong decision, particularly if their peers seem to have better information. To avoid potential negative outcomes, they might follow the recommendation of those who appear to be better informed. Future studies could attempt to decipher the mechanism behind the observed herding-like behaviour by conducting a similar experiment, but in which the fellow GP condition is substituted with an alternative specialist condition, incorporating an additional sentence clarifying that the specialist had access to the same information when making the recommendation. The finding that GPs do not follow or even contradict the recommendations of fellow GPs may imply a lack of trust. Prior literature on clinicians’ trust in their colleagues indicates that this trust is influenced not only by perceived competence but also by contracts and communication [40]. Future studies should incorporate trust measures as potential explanatory variables for herding-like behaviour.

The study has several limitations. Firstly, it featured a web-based experimental approach with hypothetical case vignettes and treatment intentions as outcomes [41], lacking behavioural validation in the real-world setting. Secondly, our study did not contain any questions about the perception of the recommendations or their source. Thirdly, there could have been limited contextual Information: The web-based format might not have provided GPs with the full clinical context, which could impact their prescription decisions and the study’s conclusions.

Our findings have practical ethical and legal implications as they confirmed that social information may be a possible underlying cause for medical errors leading to complications (i.e. a wrong recommendation by a physician could trigger a series of false diagnostic and treatment decisions of other physicians) [42,43]. In order to prevent these with educational interventions, future research is needed to understand the underlying mechanisms and motivations.

Conclusion

This is the first experimental study providing evidence that herding-like behaviour among GPs may result in prescription errors. The findings indicate that GPs are more inclined to prescribe medication that diverges from best practice clinical guidelines when they receive comparable recommendations from specialists. As our study only investigated hypothetical scenarios, subsequent studies should explore herding-like behaviour in real-world settings.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Description of study sample in first case vignette (N = 475).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Description of study sample in second case vignette (N = 475).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Binary logistic regression on prescribing sleeping tablets in case vignette 1 (N = 475).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s003

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Binary logistic regression on prescribing antibiotics in case vignette 2 (N = 475).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s004

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Binary logistic regression with interaction terms on prescribing sleeping tablets in case vignette 1 (N = 475).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s005

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Binary logistic regression with interaction terms on prescribing antibiotics in case vignette 2 (N = 475).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s006

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Ordered logistic regression on perceived decision difficulty (N = 475).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s007

(DOCX)

S8 Table. Ordered logistic regression on cognitive effort (N = 475).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s008

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Screenshots of the sleeping table case vignette.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s009

(DOCX)

S2 Fig. Screenshots of the antibiotics case vignette.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s010

(DOCX)

S3 Fig. Distribution of risk preferences [0;10].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s011

(DOCX)

S4 Fig. Distribution of rational decision-making score [5;25].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s012

(DOCX)

S5 Fig. Distribution of intuitive decision-making score [5;25].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s013

(DOCX)

S6 Fig. Individual prescription decisions in the two case vignettes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0297019.s014

(DOCX)

References

  1. 1. Avery AJ, Ghaleb M, Barber N, Franklin BD, Armstrong SJ, Serumaga B, et al. The prevalence and nature of prescribing and monitoring errors in English general practice: a retrospective case note review. British Journal of General Practice. 2013 Aug 1;63(613): e543–53. pmid:23972195
  2. 2. Chen YF, Avery AJ, Neil KE, Johnson C, Dewey ME, Stockley IH. Incidence and possible causes of prescribing potentially hazardous/contraindicated drug combinations in general practice. Drug Safety. 2005 Jan; 28:67–80. pmid:15649106
  3. 3. Van Den Bos J, Rustagi K, Gray T, Halford M, Ziemkiewicz E, Shreve J. The $17.1 billion problem: the annual cost of measurable medical errors. Health Affairs. 2011 Apr 1;30(4):596–603. pmid:21471478
  4. 4. Harrison R, Walton M, Manias E, Smith–Merry J, Kelly P, Iedema R, et al. The missing evidence: a systematic review of patients’ experiences of adverse events in health care. International Journal for Quality in Health Care. 2015 Dec 1;27(6):424–42. pmid:26424702
  5. 5. Avery AA, Barber N, Ghaleb M, Dean Franklin B, Armstrong S, Crowe S, et al. Investigating the prevalence and causes of prescribing errors in general practice: the PRACtICe study. Final report for the GMC, May 2012. [Available from: http://www.gmc-uk.org/Investigating_the_prevalence_and_causes_of_prescribing_errors_in_general_practice___The_PRACtICe_study_Reoprt_May_2012_48605085.pdf, accessed 06/10/2021].
  6. 6. Sandars J, Aneez E. The frequency and nature of medical error in primary care: understanding the diversity across studies. Family practice 2003; 20.3: 231–236. pmid:12738689
  7. 7. Slight SP, Howard R, Ghaleb M, Barber N, Franklin BD, Avery AJ. The causes of prescribing errors in English general practices: a qualitative study. British Journal of General Practice, 2013; 63(615), e713–e720. pmid:24152487
  8. 8. Leviatan I, Oberman B, Zimlichman E, Stein GY Associations of physicians’ prescribing experience, work hours, and workload with prescription errors. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association, 2021; 28(6), 1074–1080. pmid:33120412
  9. 9. Dornan T, Ashcroft D, Heathfield H, Lewis P, Miles J, Taylor D, et al. An in-depth investigation into causes of prescribing errors by foundation trainees in relation to their medical education: EQUIP study. London: General Medical Council. 2009 Dec:1–215.
  10. 10. Ryan C, Ross S, Davey P, Duncan EM, Francis JJ, Fielding S, et al. Prevalence and causes of prescribing errors: the PRescribing Outcomes for Trainee Doctors Engaged in Clinical Training (PROTECT) study. PloS one. 2014 Jan 3;9(1): e79802. pmid:24404122
  11. 11. Keers RN, Williams SD, Cooke J, Ashcroft DM. Causes of medication administration errors in hospitals: a systematic review of quantitative and qualitative evidence. Drug safety. 2013 Nov; 36:1045–67. pmid:23975331
  12. 12. Chung A, Rimal RN. Social norms: A review. Review of Communication Research. 2016; 4:1–28.
  13. 13. Schöbel M, Rieskamp J, Huber R. Social influences in sequential decision making. PloS one. 2016 Jan 19;11(1): e0146536. pmid:26784448
  14. 14. Wang W, Zhao X, Sun J, Zhou G. Exploring physicians’ extended use of electronic health records (EHRs) A social influence perspective. Health Information Management Journal. 2016 Dec;45(3):134–43. pmid:27246917
  15. 15. Saposnik G, Maurino J, Sempere AP, Ruff CC, Tobler PN. Herding: a new phenomenon affecting medical decision-making in multiple sclerosis care? Lessons learned from DIScUTIR MS. Patient preference and adherence. 2017 Jan 31:175–80.
  16. 16. Prechter RR Jr. Unconscious herding behavior as the psychological basis of financial market trends and patterns. The Journal of Psychology and Financial Markets. 2001 Sep 1;2(3):120–5.
  17. 17. Roider A, Drehmann M, Oechssler J. Herding and Contrarian Behavior in Financial Markets-An Internet Experiment. InRoyal Economic Society Annual Conference 2003 2003 Jun 4 (No. 177). Royal Economic Society.
  18. 18. Tan L, Chiang TC, Mason JR, Nelling E. Herding behavior in Chinese stock markets: An examination of A and B shares. Pacific-Basin finance journal. 2008 Jan 1;16(1–2):61–77.
  19. 19. Smith VL, Suchanek GL, Williams AW. Bubbles, crashes, and endogenous expectations in experimental spot asset markets. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society. 1988 Sep 1:1119–51.
  20. 20. Keynes JM. A treatise on money. (No Title). 1930 Aug 1.
  21. 21. Yang M, Lien HM, CHOU SY. Is there a physician peer effect? Evidence from new drug prescriptions. Economic Inquiry. 2014 Jan;52(1):116–37.
  22. 22. Iyengar R, Van den Bulte C, Lee JY. Social contagion in new product trial and repeat. Marketing Science. 2015 May;34(3):408–29.
  23. 23. NHS Business Services Authority (UK). Prescribing Costs in Hospitals and Community in England 2018/19-2022/23. NHSBSA Statistics and Data Science. 2023 Nov. [Available from: https://www.nhsbsa.nhs.uk/statistical-collections/prescribing-costs-hospitals-and-community-england/prescribing-costs-hospitals-and-community-england-202122/prescribing-costs-hospitals-and-community-england-201819-202223, accessed 04/12/2023].
  24. 24. Medicines NI. Prescribing Centre (UK). Medicines optimisation: the safe and effective use of medicines to enable the best possible outcomes. Manchester: National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (UK). 2015 Mar. [Available from: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng5/resources/medicines-optimization-the-safe-and-effective-use-of-medicines-to-enable-the-best-possible-outcomes-pdf-51041805253, accessed 04/02/2022].
  25. 25. Cialdini RB, Kallgren CA, Reno RR. A focus theory of normative conduct: A theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behavior. In Advances in experimental social psychology 1991 Jan 1 (Vol. 24, pp. 201–234). Academic Press.
  26. 26. Reno RR, Cialdini RB, Kallgren CA. The transsituational influence of social norms. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1993 Jan;64(1):104.
  27. 27. Schultz PW, Nolan JM, Cialdini RB, Goldstein NJ, Griskevicius V. The constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychological science. 2007 May;18(5):429–34. pmid:17576283
  28. 28. Cueni R, Frey BS. How to Overcome Herding Behavior in Firms. Towards a New Theory of the Firm: Humanizing the Firm and the Management Profession. 2012.
  29. 29. Ramaswamy R, Maio V, Diamond JJ, Talati AR, Hartmann CW, Arenson C, et al. Potentially inappropriate prescribing in elderly: assessing doctor knowledge, confidence and barriers. Journal of evaluation in clinical practice. 2011 Dec;17(6):1153–9. pmid:20630004
  30. 30. Tannenbaum D, Doctor JN, Persell SD, Friedberg MW, Meeker D, Friesema EM, et al. Nudging physician prescription decisions by partitioning the order set: results of a vignette-based study. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2015 Mar; 30:298–304. pmid:25394536
  31. 31. Krell M. Evaluating an instrument to measure mental load and mental effort considering different sources of validity evidence. Cogent Education. 2017 Jan 1;4(1):1280256.
  32. 32. Hamilton K, Shih SI, Mohammed S. The development and validation of the rational and intuitive decision styles scale. Journal of personality assessment. 2016 Sep 2;98(5):523–35. pmid:26967981
  33. 33. Dohmen T, Falk A, Huffman D, Sunde U, Schupp J, Wagner GG. Individual risk atudes: Measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. Journal of the european economic association. 2011 Jun 1;9(3):522–50.
  34. 34. Nebout A, Cavillon M, Ventelou B. Comparing GPs’ risk attitudes for their own health and for their patients’: a troubling discrepancy? BMC Health Services Research. 2018 Dec;18(1):1–0.
  35. 35. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Academic press; 2013 Sep 3.
  36. 36. Simpson SA, Wood F, Butler CC. General practitioners’ perceptions of antimicrobial resistance: a qualitative study. Journal of Antimicrobial Chemotherapy. 2007 Feb 1;59(2):292–6. pmid:17110392
  37. 37. Auronen L. Asymmetric information: theory and applications. InSeminar of Strategy and International Business as Helsinki University of Technology 2003 May 21 (Vol. 167, pp. 14–18).
  38. 38. Robinson JC. The end of asymmetric information. Journal of Health Politics, Policy and Law. 2001;26(5):1045–53. pmid:11765255
  39. 39. Spyrou S. Herding in financial markets: a review of the literature. Review of Behavioral Finance. 2013 Nov 25;5(2):175–94.
  40. 40. Rushton CH, Reina ML, Francovich C, Naumann P, Reina DS. Application of the Reina Trust and Betrayal Model to the experience of pediatric critical care clinicians. Am J Crit Care. 2010; 19(4): e41–e51. pmid:20595211
  41. 41. Evans SC, Roberts MC, Keeley JW, Blossom JB, Amaro CM, Garcia AM, et al. Vignette methodologies for studying clinicians’ decision-making: Validity, utility, and application in ICD-11 field studies. International journal of clinical and health psychology. 2015 May 1;15(2):160–70. pmid:30487833
  42. 42. Woolf SH, Kuzel AJ, Dovey SM, Phillips RL. A string of mistakes: the importance of cascade analysis in describing, counting, and preventing medical errors. The Annals of Family Medicine. 2004 Jul 1;2(4):317–26. pmid:15335130
  43. 43. Ely JW, Levinson W, Elder NC, Mainous AG 3rd, Vinson DC. Perceived causes of family physicians’ errors. J Fam Pract. 1995 Apr;40(4):337–44. pmid:7699346.