Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Socioeconomic disparities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in Sweden: An intersectional ecological niches analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy (IEN-AIHDA)

  • Christoffer Hornborg ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Christoffer.hornborg@vastervik.se

    Affiliations Department of Sociology and Work Science, University of Gothenburg, Göteborg, Sweden, Unit for Social Epidemiology, Faculty of Medicine, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden, Centre for Welfare, Social Innovation and Sustainability in Rural Areas, Campus Västervik, Västervik, Sweden

  • Rebecca Axrud,

    Roles Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Unit for Social Epidemiology, Faculty of Medicine, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden

  • Raquel Pérez Vicente,

    Roles Data curation, Methodology

    Affiliation Unit for Social Epidemiology, Faculty of Medicine, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden

  • Juan Merlo

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Unit for Social Epidemiology, Faculty of Medicine, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden, Centre for Primary Health Care Research, Region Skåne, Malmö, Sweden

Abstract

We aimed (i) to gain a better understanding of the demographic and socioeconomical distribution of ADHD risk in Sweden; and (ii) to contribute to the critical discussion on medicalization, i.e., the tendency to define and treat behavioural and social problems as medical entities. For this purpose, we analysed the risk of suffering from ADHD in the whole Swedish population aged between 5 and 60 years, across 96 different strata defined by combining categories of gender, age, income, and country of birth. The stratified analysis evidenced considerable risk heterogeneity, with prevalence values ranging from 0.03% in high income immigrant women aged 50–59, to 6.18% in middle income immigrant boys aged 10–14. Our study questions the established idea that behavioural difficulties conceptualized as ADHD should be primarily perceived as a neurological abnormality. Rather, our findings suggest that there is a strong sociological component behind how some individuals become impaired and subject to medicalization.

Introduction

In August 2022, The Moderate Party of Sweden proposed that children in vulnerable suburbs around the capital Stockholm should be offered rapid tests for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), as a preventive measure against exclusion and gang crime [1]. Without questioning the intentions of the proposal, there is an all the more important observation to make. Rather than addressing the social fabric surrounding children that grow up under adverse psychosocial circumstances, the proposal illustrated how the discourse of ‘neuropsychiatry’ has become more and more hegemonic in how to make sense of human behavior and social problems.

According to meta-analyses, the number of ADHD diagnoses has increased considerably all over the world [2]. Figs 1 and 2 summarizes data from the Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare [3], illustrating that from 2001 to 2018, the prevalence of ADHD diagnoses in open specialized care increased 36 times in boys and 112 times in girls between 5 and 19 years of age. This massive increase in the number of ADHD diagnoses during the last two decades is a notable epidemiological phenomenon [4]. Arguably, the threshold of who are considered to meet the criteria for a medical disorder has become lower, where parents, schools, and society in general seem to have become more inclined to use ADHD as an explanatory model when children display problematic or undesirable behaviour. In the dominant discourse on ADHD symptoms, the general emphasis is put on an underlying biological disturbance [5]. This is despite the fact that a tangible number of children who receive an ADHD diagnosis are subject to social disadvantage and stressful environments that may impact their development and well-being [6].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Prevalence (%) of ADHD diagnosis in the population 5 to 60 year-old by sex and calender year.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294741.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Prevalence (%) of ADHD diagnosis in the population 5 to 19 year-old by sex and calender year.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294741.g002

The ontology of ADHD

Historically, the debate about ADHD has been rather polarized. In psychiatry and the natural sciences, ADHD has been conceptualized as a universally occurring medical condition that exists regardless of societal conditions and cultural settings [7]. Within this paradigm, researchers have sought to investigate neurobiological components associated with possible functional [8, 9], structural [10, 11], and neurotransmitter [1214] alterations in various regions of the brain. Accordingly, ADHD has been treated as a natural kind, i.e., as a universal entity that exists independently of human systems of classification [15]. On the other hand, sociologists [16] and critical psychiatrists [17] have emphasised how societal and cultural processes have led to an expanded use of the diagnosis, and the scientific integrity of ADHD has also been questioned [18]. Others [19] have nuanced the tension by emphasizing how considering a diagnosis as a societal and cultural process, does not suggest that the diagnosis is not real in a specific context. Furthermore, the controversies around ADHD have been addressed as a problem of reification, i.e., the belief that if something is given a name, we have acquired an explanation [20]. Te Meerman and coworkers thus reminds us that “The descriptive classification Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is often mistaken for a disease entity that explains the causes of inattentive and hyperactive behaviors, rather than merely describing the existence of such behaviors”. Accordingly, there is a risk that not only scientists, but also laymen and politicians tend to regard ADHD as an entity, rather than a mere name for observed behaviors. Such a point of departure does not diminish the suffering and seriousness of the symptoms, but when it comes to scientific understanding, problems of reification can illustrate the fallacies that systematically run through psychiatric thinking. As an example, in an analysis of the increased prescription of pharmacological treatment of ADHD, The Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare [21] stated that the proportion of people receiving a diagnosis eventually will be reflected in how many that actually have ADHD in the population, and when that happens, the curve for new users of medications will flatten out. The fallacy in this sort of thinking is that the diagnosis is conceptualized as a thing one simply has or not. There is a risk that an excessively narrow perspective on epidemiology will end up in a discussion about whether ADHD is over- or underdiagnosed. Such a focus assumes that there exists an objective prevalence, but since most traits and behaviours, including those applied to define ADHD, are continuously distributed within the population, the prevalence is ultimately dependent on social consensus regarding the boundaries of what fits into the diagnosis and what does not. In other words, there is no ‘natural’ prevalence to be found, regardless of scientific rigour. ADHD as a category is so heterogenous, multi-factorial, and even ambiguous [22], that it could implicate an all too broad spectrum of idiopathic symptoms of suffering, depending on the intentions and bias of its advocates.

According to meta-analyses [23, 24], there has been no evidence to suggest an increase in the number of individuals who meet criteria for ADHD in the past three decades. Within the general Swedish population, there are no indications that ‘ADHD traits’ have increased, despite the obvious rise in the number of diagnoses. This discrepancy of course raises questions. Has society become better in discovering more cases of a valid disease entity, among individuals who have previously been undiagnosed? Or has it changed the view on what should be considered a disorder [25, 26]? Psychiatric diagnoses are flexible objects that arise, evolve, and even disappear, in line with historical processes [27, 28]. Something that started out as a label for hyperactive children, nowadays serve as a hypothetical cause for a tremendously broad range of behavioral and cognitive features, in both children and adults: difficulties with concentration, aggression, carelessness, burn-out syndrome, addiction, apathy, impulsivity, antisocial behavior, and so on–basically a majority of psychological attributes associated with impaired functioning. To treat ADHD as a more or less biological disease entity causing an impairment, can be a logical fallacy of reification, prohibiting a more nuanced understanding of human functioning. Therefore, it is important to map the contextual factors that are associated with individuals that become diagnosed. This is where sociological and philosophical perspectives have the potential to enrich the understanding of psychiatric epidemics such as ADHD [16, 27, 2931].

Ecological niches as a conceptual framework

Rather than seeking to establish the objective prevalence of ADHD, Hornborg and Merlo [25] have advocated that the focus should be to identify the way diagnoses and prevalence are moulded by how societal, economic, and ideological currents change the assumptions, conceptual apparatus and explanatory models of clinicians and researchers. What type of social and cultural forces may have increased the prevalence and in which settings do ADHD occur? One way of understanding social and psychiatric epidemiology on the basis of these issues is by applying the philosophy of Ian Hacking [27]. Instead of perceiving controversial diagnoses as either medically valid or merely socially constructed, Hacking sought to understand the contextual factors that allowed the diagnosis to arise and thrive over time and space. To do this he used the concept of ecological niche, a metaphor derived from biology. Just as biological organisms only will survive in a niche with the right conditions, the same applies to certain types of diseases. For example, the diagnosis of dyslexia would not flourish in a society without a written language. It is possible to argue that the symptoms of ADHD have always existed, but the idea of an ecological niche helps to illuminate in which conditions they become classified as a medical diagnosis. Hacking’s metaphor is epidemiologically relevant since it acknowledges both a disorder (analogous to the organism) and its context (defined by the factors that forms the ecological niche in which the organism thrives), and it can be particularly useful in social epidemiology. In drastic epidemiological changes, it is of great importance for research to study the contextual forces that underly the act of diagnosing. This can be done by investigating vehicles on a macro level, such as the influence of the pharmaceutical industry, an increased biological focus within psychiatry, transitions from ICD to DSM and the role of the Internet [32], i.e., factors that affect the entire way research is conceptualized, as well as the general perception of laymen and media. But it can also be done by examining how different micro niches, domains within society, is affected differently by the changing perceptions of what constitutes a disorder, acknowledging how the act of turning human behaviour into disease always takes place within a particular context. If the national ADHD prevalence has a considerable heterogeneity in the distribution of diagnoses, this could be framed as an existence of smaller ecological niches in which demographical and socioeconomic factors condition the setting where diagnoses occur. This approach is analogous to the traditional study of inequalities in health where socioeconomic differences are investigated with an intersectional approach [33].

Intersectional analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy (AIHDA)

Intersectionality was originally a theory that mainly applied to qualitative methodology. However, an intersectional quantitative approach is being increasingly utilized in epidemiology and public health [3337] as it provides an ideal framework for understanding the demographic and socioeconomic heterogeneity in diagnoses existing within a community. The Oxford English Dictionary 2015 defines intersectionality as “The interconnected nature of social categorizations such as race, class, and gender, regarded as creating overlapping and interdependent systems of discrimination or disadvantage”. Thereby, the demographic, and socioeconomic dimensions that condition health and disease risk need to be considered as interlocked rather than as unidimensional. By using intersectionality, numerous intersectional groups (i.e., strata) can be defined by a combination of several demographic and socioeconomic dimensions (e.g., age, gender, income, and country of birth). In this way, the intersectional perspective provides an improved mapping of disadvantage that better illustrates the heterogeneous socioeconomic distribution of health in the community. Such intersectional strata could be conceptualized as micro niches that facilitate or hinder the construction of ADHD diagnoses by providing different thresholds for dichotomizing traits and behaviours. For instance, many of the questions included in self-assessment forms for ADHD such as ASRS-v1.1 [38] can be interpreted differently in different intersectional contexts. Also, schoolteachers and legal guardians from different niches may have different expectations that condition the evaluation of the symptomatology [3941]. The influence of intersectional niches could be perceived as an addition of influences from separate social dimensions or as an interaction where the propensity of receiving an ADHD diagnosis is larger or lower than the sum of the influences of separate social dimensions.

The present study develops an intersectional perspective into an intersectional ecological niche (IEN) framework. The IEN framework benefits from intersectionality by allowing an improved use of interlocked socioeconomic dimensions, but simultaneously allows the considerations of whatever other relevant ecological factors that would not traditionally be included in a formal intersectional analysis. Thereby, the IE framework gains flexibility when it comes to investigating overlapping and interdependent systems of discrimination or disadvantage. Utilizing the concept of ecological niches is a way of counteracting essentialist narratives and developing a more constructionist framework on how disorders such as ADHD flourish due to contextual factors. Instead of conceptualizing ADHD as a universally occurring entity, it is approached as something that emerges more or less in a given context. Accordingly, the IEN-AIHDA framework that we propose may be a way of addressing the emergence of mental illnesses both in societies and in individuals [cf. 42].

By applying a methodology denominated multilevel analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy (MAIHDA) [36], the individual is considered as nested within intersectional ecological niches. Such a methodology can be applied by using both formal multilevel regression [43, 44] as well as traditional regression analyses [33]. MAIHDA provides a detailed mapping of the risk of ADHD diagnosis in the population and allows an evaluation to which degree micro niches are with accuracy able to discriminate individuals with ADHD from individuals without ADHD [36, 45]. Traditional methodologies assign the same average value (i.e., probability) to all the members in the group (i.e., intersectional niche) without considering the individual heterogeneity around the group average and the overlapping between categories [46]. If the discriminatory accuracy is low, the benefit of focusing on specific intersectional niches for understanding differences in ADHD diagnosis can be discussed.

Aim

The aim of this study is twofold. Firstly, we want to obtain a better understanding of the demographic and socioeconomic distribution of ADHD diagnosis in the Swedish population. We hypothesised that different intersectional niches (i.e., contexts, strata) may promote or hinder the risk of getting an ADHD diagnose in Sweden. That is, the same individual would receive or not receive the diagnosis if he/she was exposed to a different niche. If this is true, differences between the intersectional niches would be high. Secondly, we want to contribute to the ongoing critical discussion on medicalization, i.e., the tendency to increasingly define and treat behavioural and social problems as medical entities. Our analysis of intersectional niches is therefore grounded in a nominalist point of departure, highlighting the importance of critically examining current discourses on disease categories such as ADHD.

  • How is the prevalence of ADHD diagnoses distributed across intersectional niches defined by gender, age, country of birth, and income?
  • What is the discriminatory accuracy of the intersectional niches for classifying individuals regarding the existence of an ADHD diagnosis?
  • In what way can the findings contribute to the ongoing discussion about ADHD as a historically situated diagnostic trend of epidemic proportions?

Methods

Databases

We used a database composed of three linked registers covering the whole country of Sweden: The Register of the Total Population (TPR) [47], the Longitudinal Integration Database for Health Insurance and Labour Market Studies (LISA) administered by Statistics Sweden [48], and The National Patient Register (NPR) [49] administered by the National Board of Health and Welfare [50]. We performed a record linkage using the unique 12-digit social security number (SSN) assigned to each individual residing in Sweden. In order to pseudonymize the research database, Statistics Sweden in coordination with The National Board of Health and Welfare replaced the SSN with an arbitrary serial number before delivering files. The information is registered without formal individual consent. However, while the data is confidential, the Swedish authorities delivers pseudonymize information for research purposes after evaluation.

The TPR [47] registers data on all Swedish residents regarding socioeconomic information about gender, civil status, migration, country of birth and family relationship as well as general demographics for those who stayed in Sweden for at least 12 months. The LISA database is administered by Statistics Sweden [48] and combines information on all Swedish residents above the age of 15 from several registers, including the labour market, social sectors, and educational sectors. It includes status on living conditions, demographics, and socioeconomic factors, like income, education, and employment. The NPR [50, 51] records more than 99% of inpatient and approximately 80% of specialized outpatient diagnoses from hospitals in Sweden. At the time of the study, all diagnoses were coded according to the International Classification of Diseases, 10th version (ICD-10). The NPR collects data from both private and public health care providers with an 85–95% positive predictive value on inpatient diagnosis [49]. Data from primary care is not included in the NPR.

Study population

According to the TPR, 9.420.128 individuals resided in Sweden at the baseline date of December 31st, 2010 (Fig 3). When comparing data with the Death Register, we excluded 5.630 individuals that had passed away but with delayed registration, making the correct number of individuals 9.414.498. Based on the TPR, 444.596 individuals that had stayed in Sweden for less than 5 years before baseline were excluded, since recently arrived immigrants might have incomplete register information. Finally, we excluded 2.882.109 individuals older than 60 or younger than 5 years, leaving the database containing information on 6.093.423 individuals.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Flow chart informing on inclusion and exclusion criteria, as well as number of individuals included in the study population.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294741.g003

Assessment of the outcome variable

The outcome variables used were presence or absence of ADHD diagnosis between 2005 and 2010. We defined ADHD diagnosis as any variant of International Classification of Diseases, 10th version (ICD-10) codes F90.0-F90.8.

Assessment of demographic and socioeconomic variables

Gender was binary coded as either man or woman according to legal status. We categorized age at the baseline date into eight intervals: 5–9, 10–14, 15–19, 20–24, 25–29, 30–39, 40–49 and 50–59 years of age. It is known that an ADHD diagnosis is more common in boys than girls [5257] and studies have also concluded that it is more common amongst 10–14-year-old children [58, 59]. Furthermore, we used information from Statistics Sweden to classify country of birth into a binary variable. We labelled individuals born in Sweden as ‘natives’ and individuals born outside of Sweden as ‘immigrants’.

In order to achieve accurate measures of the individuals’ income, the income variable was created using the individualized cumulative disposable income of the household. The disposable income is the income that remains after taxes has been accounted for. We computed a variable for the years 2000, 2005, and 2010, and divided the total disposable income of a family by the number of individuals in that family. The different consumption weights of adults and children of different ages were used according to a specific formula [60]. We sorted groups by 25th quantiles using the TPR in the years 2000, 2005, and 2010. Thereafter, were added the scores from the three years ranging from 1 to 25 and obtained values from three (lowest cumulative income) to 75 (highest cumulative income). Finally, we categorized the cumulative income into three groups (high, middle, and low) by tertiles. The 1002 individuals with missing values of income during 2000 or 2005 were assigned the tertile values of the year 2010. No individuals in our study population had missing income data in that particular year.

Assessment of intersectional niches

As a way of including the intersectional niches, we created a multi-categorical variable consisting of 96 strata. The strata were created by combining the two categories of gender, the eight categories of age, the two categories of country of birth and the three categories of cumulative income. We used a reference stratum constructed of 10-14-year-old women born in Sweden with high income for comparison. This choice was somewhat arbitrary but based on previous studies indicating the characteristics of the individuals with the lowest ADHD risk.

Statistical analysis

The first step in our analysis was to map the prevalence (i.e., absolute risk) of ADHD across the intersectional niches by graphical stratified analyses. Thereafter, we modelled the binary ADHD variable as the dependent variable. We applied Cox proportional hazard regressions with a constant follow-up time equal to one. In this way, we formally attained prevalence ratios (PR) or relative risks [61].

We performed six different models. Model 1 included only age, model 2 only gender, model 3 only income, and model 4 only country of birth. In model 5 all four single variables were entered simultaneously. Finally, in model 6, we included the same information as in model 5 but with the multi-categorical variable with the stratum of 10–14-year-old native women with high income as reference. In all models we calculated PR with 99% instead of 95% confidence intervals (CI) to counteract the multiple comparisons problem.

For each model, we quantified its discriminatory accuracy (DA) by means of the area under the receiver operator characteristics curve (AUC) [45]. The AUC is calculated by plotting the true positive fraction (i.e., sensitivity) against the false positive fraction (i.e., 1 –specificity) for different binary classification thresholds of the predicted probability of ADHD diagnosis. Thus, the AUC measures the accuracy of the information provided by the variables in the model used to discriminate individuals with ADHD from those without ADHD. The AUC obtains a value between 0.5 and 1, where 1 indicates perfect discrimination and 0.5 means that the studied variables have no discriminatory accuracy at all. Rather than only evaluating the differences between absolute risks of strata values, the DA also assessed the overlapping of the individual risk predictions (based on the intersectional niches) between individuals with and without ADHD. There is no fully established practical guideline for the interpretation of the size of the AUC as a measure of discriminatory accuracy when analysing IEN inequalities. However, based on the classification provided by Hosmer and Lemeshow [62] we defined the discriminatory accuracy as (i) ‘absent or very low’ (AUC = 0.5–0.6) corresponding with ‘absent or very low’ strata inequalities, (ii)‘poor’ (AUC >0.6–≤ 0.7), corresponding with ‘small’ strata inequalities, (iii) ‘acceptable’ (AUC >0.7– ≤ 0.8), corresponding with ‘large’ strata inequalities, (iv) ‘excellent’ (AUC >0.8–≤ 0.9) or (v) ‘outstanding’ (AUC > 0.9–1) corresponding with ‘very large’ strata inequalities.

We further calculated the gradual change in the AUC value (Δ-AUC) between the models. The Δ-AUC quantifies the improvement in the discriminatory accuracy obtained by a model, in relation to a reference model [63]. The categorical intersectional variable in model 6 allows us to capture possible interaction of effects between the variables that define the strata so if any interaction exists, the discriminatory accuracy of model 6 in comparison with model 5 will increase and the Δ-AUC be positive [33].

All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences) version 24 and Stata v15 (StataCorp, College Station, TX).

Results

Out of all 6.093.423 individuals registered in our database, 54.181 (0.9%) were diagnosed with ADHD during the study period. Table 1 shows that the prevalence of ADHD diagnoses increases with age from 0.72% among 5–9-year-old children until the age of 15–19 when it reaches its maximum of 2.32%, and thereafter decreases with age reaching its lowest value of 0.2% among people 50–59 years old. Overall, ADHD is more frequent in men and in native Swedes than in women and immigrants. Also, there is a clear income gradient with the highest prevalence in the low-income group. Figs 4 and 5 state the prevalence in the different strata using two different approaches, in order to improve our understanding of the underlying heterogeneity in the distribution of ADHD diagnoses across different IE niches. The principal finding is that such heterogeneity is considerable. When observing the stratified analysis of the 96 groups, the risk of ADHD is higher in most immigrant strata than in native strata, especially in males. However, Table 2, which does not contain stratified information, indicates that immigrants have a lower ADHD risk than natives. This could illustrate the so-called Simpsons paradox [64], which describes that when combining strata, a trend can disappear, or show the opposite outcome in comparison to unidimensional analysis.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Prevalence (absolute risk) of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) by gender, age, country of birth and low (L), middle (M) and high (H) income in the study cohort.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294741.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Prevalence (absolute risk) of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), black circles across the 96 intersectional strata defined by gender, country of birth, age and low (L), middle (M) and high (H) income.

The lines crossing the circles are 99% confidence intervals. The lines between the strata show the income gradient across each age interval.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294741.g005

thumbnail
Table 1. Number of individuals (N), number of individuals with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) (n) and prevalence of ADHD in Sweden between 2005 and 2010.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294741.t001

The tables and graphs demonstrating the strata or IE niches, present a similar pattern as in Table 1 concerning the prevalence of different age intervals as well as a higher prevalence in men than women. However, according to the strata seen in Figs 4 and 5, the income gradient appears to show a different pattern for immigrants compared to native Swedes. The immigrants with low income show the lowest prevalence amongst the income groups until the age of 24, thereafter the income gradients disappear, and the prevalence appears to be low in all three income groups. In contrast, the native Swedish groups present an income gradient across all age intervals and show a lower prevalence amongst individuals with high income. Observing the native Swedes, the absolute risk of having an ADHD diagnosis amongst young boys (10–19 years old) with a low income compared to the boys in the high-income groups is almost three times higher. The outcome is similarly much higher in immigrant boys when comparing them to native Swedish boys, in the high- and middle-income groups.

Table 2 displays the PR indicating the association of ADHD and age (Model 1), gender (Model 2), income (Model 3) and country of birth (Model 4) in separate analyses. It also displays the variables stated above put together as one specific variable (Model 5), and as multi-categorical variable with 96 strata (Model 6). The results of model 6 are displayed in Table 3 and show the prevalence or absolute risk for the 10 highest and 10 lowest strata out of all 96 that are seen in the supplementary information (S1 Table). These strata are compared to the reference 10-14-year-old women born in Sweden with high income.

When comparing the 96 strata, the difference in absolute risk, AR, is striking. 10-14-year-old immigrant boys had an AR of 6.18, which is more than 200 times as high as that of 50-59-year-old immigrant women (AR = 0.03). As illustrated in Table 3, the risk was highest among middle- and high-income immigrant boys between the age of 10 and 19, followed by low-income native boys in the same age group. For full information, see S1 and S2 Tables.

thumbnail
Table 3. Number of individuals (N) and prevalence or Absolute Risk (AR) of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in the 96 intercategorical strata as well as prevalence ratios (PR), obtained in model 6.

Only the 10 lowest and 10 highest PR are shown.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294741.t003

The AUC-value, hence the DA, was calculated to 0.76 when all four variables were included in Model 5. Comparing this value with the models including only one variable at time showed no difference in age (Δ-AUC = 0.00; 0.76–0.76) but a difference compared to gender (Δ-AUC = 0.02), income (Δ-AUC = 0.09) and country of birth (Δ-AUC = 0.23). The DA for model 6 was calculated to be 0.77 which indicates a difference and a small interaction effect comparing it to model 5 (Δ-AUC = 0.01). Thus, the variables used in our study present mainly additive effects and only a small interactional effect.

Discussion

Adopting an IEN approach, this study provides a detailed stratified analysis that offers a better understanding of the ADHD ‘epidemic’ in Sweden in 2010. It shows that behind the overall national prevalence there is considerable demographic and socioeconomic heterogeneity in the distribution of ADHD diagnoses in the Swedish population. As expected, the risk of receiving a diagnosis had a peak in younger age groups and was higher amongst males. However, the stratified analysis illustrates how this risk is strikingly higher in young immigrants than in natives, especially in males. It is noteworthy that, overall, natives had a higher absolute risk of ADHD than immigrants, yet when performing the stratified analysis, many of the immigrant strata had a higher prevalence than the native strata. Also, while there is a typical income gradient in native Swedes (with a higher ADHD risk in low-income individuals), a different pattern is observed among immigrants, where the low-income group has lower risk than middle- and high-income groups.

A map of the distribution of ADHD diagnoses across numerous IE strata is useful for identifying contexts with a potential higher risk. However, a disadvantage is that this detailed information may promote the stigmatization of concrete IEN, such as 10–14 years old male immigrants from a middle-income background. An important reminder is therefore to interpret strata information from the viewpoint of discriminatory accuracy, i.e., the capacity of an IEN to classify with accuracy cases and non-cases within the population. The average immigrant teenage boy is obviously not likely to have behavioural problems associated with an ADHD diagnosis. In the same way, a diagnosis may occur in any strata of the population. However, some subjects are at much greater risk, and the intersectional grouping in this study provides a detailed picture of this uneven distribution.

Since observability is a crucial vector in the terminology of Hacking [65], Brossard [42] has proposed that behaviours associated with mental disorders are primarily role disturbances. A diagnosis therefore requires identification of how social roles are disturbed and such an observation is contingent on social networks in the specific context. This might explain why our study illustrates a thorough heterogeneity between different strata. Furthermore, prevalence discrepancies between natives and immigrants within the low-income-group may depend on differences between countries of origin, regarding attitudes and access to psychiatric treatment. In some countries, psychiatric care is not utilized to the same extent, and parents from other regions of the world could be expected to have differing expectations of the health care system. It is possible that psychiatric diagnoses such as ADHD may be considered more stigmatising in some cultures [66], and this could be particularly evident among people with less resources, i.e., low income. In addition, there could be quite some cultural variation in what is considered acceptable or standard childhood behaviour [67]. However, our study uses a very rough classification of country of birth, grouping all immigrants together, even though this is a highly heterogenous category. It is a limitation of this study that it was not possible to analyse how coming from a war afflicted country, trauma, education level, cultural differences, and so on, relates to our results. In future studies, we propose an analysis using a more granulated classification of country of birth. Since the study illustrated a strong overall link between ADHD and socioeconomic/demographic variables, future research could also benefit from expanding the analysis by incorporating other variables in the construction of strata, i.e., geographic area and/or specific care facility. The impact of the clinician can for example be a key factor behind varying prevalence. As commented in the introduction, the IEN-AIHDA framework is more flexible than an analysis formally based on intersectional theory only. An important limitation with the application of IEN-AIHDA in this study is that we were unable to choose the variables of interest a priori, since the information in the registers was limited. Future studies can contribute to the IEAN-AIHDA approach by adding new variables hypothesized to be relevant in the understanding of ADHD risk distribution.

So how should the massive heterogeneity in this study be interpreted in relation to other studies? Previous research has shown that lower family income is associated with stressful events [68], parenting style [69, 70], and family conflict [71]. Regarding ADHD, population-based studies have confirmed that there is a link between an increased risk of receiving a diagnosis and social problems such as childhood adversities and low family income [72, 73]. Such relationships between psychiatric disorders and social factors can be explained by either (i) a social drift hypothesis, i.e., that biologically caused illness increases the risk of drifting into a lower social class and decreases the chances for upward social mobility, or (ii) a social causation hypothesis, i.e., that social disadvantage increases the risk of developing psychiatric disorders [74]. When it comes to these socioeconomic disparities in ADHD, there is a risk that the social drift hypothesis will continue to appeal, since parent traits then can explain both social disadvantage and the child’s symptoms, without compromising the narrative of a neurological entity, characterized by ‘neural abnormalities’ and a strong genetic association [75]. The biological discourse associated with behaviors labeled as ADHD, risks portraying social conditions as a dependent variable rather than a cause of illness, i.e., that individuals to a greater extend end up in socioeconomically unfavorable conditions due to neuropsychiatric characteristics, rather than vice versa. However, our large study, utilizing stratified analysis showed that in the middle and high-income groups, immigrant boys have a much higher risk of ADHD than native boys. These findings support the notion that psychosocial factors should be more highlighted since it is unlikely that children to a greater extent have an immigrant background as a result of neuropsychiatric characteristics (cf. [76]). This opens for a more thorough discussion on how social factors needs to be recognized in the understanding of behaviours and symptoms associated with receiving an ADHD diagnosis [77, 78]. Accordingly, Miller and coworkers [78] have pointed out that if socioeconomic disadvantage causes disorders such as ADHD, explanations and interventions should not only consider individual characteristics. The tendency to address social problems by seeking to detect undiagnosed biological dysfunction among socially disadvantaged youth, as noted in the introduction, is therefor problematic.

While researchers have proposed to move beyond the dichotomy of genes versus environment, and instead develop models based on interplay and epigenetics [79], the aetiologic narrative of ‘ADHD symptoms’ arguably continues to be unbalanced, explaining a wide range of heterogeneous behaviors as a biological disease entity [6, 20, 80]. Te Meerman and coworkers [80], for example, found that academic textbooks were rather biased, overstating the results of twin studies, and not addressing the implications of disappointing molecular research. Such an emphasis on genetical coherence through twin studies tends to disguise the fact that environmental factors can be of great importance even for traits with a very high heritability (i.e., human height, which in Europe increased by 11 centimeters in just 100 years [81]). This is not to say that the different strata in this study provide a more coherent explanation on why individuals become diagnosed. Rather, it is important to acknowledge the complexity of diagnoses entirely based on behavioral problems, and to highlight that some psychiatric categories should be approached with care, as blunt clinical tools. In clinical practise, there is a risk that ADHD comes to serve as an umbrella term for people that suffer and display a wide variety of maladaptive symptoms: A trend that might be accentuated due to a reorientation from attention and hyperactivity, towards a broader clinical discourse on ‘executive difficulties’ or ‘self-regulation’. In line with how medicalization processes operate, this leads to an all too wide range of behavioral difficulties in everyday life residing within the same explanatory category.

It is highly important to consider to which extent ADHD is utilized as an explanation rather than a heterogenous description [20]. In an understanding of the ontology of ADHD, the core question is what it actually means to have ADHD. Is it to have been diagnosed? Is it to be positioned at the outskirts of a bell-shaped curve for a certain trait? Or is it to have difficulties in everyday life, i.e., stated as ‘functional impairment’? And perhaps most importantly, what are the relationships between these parameters? High-functioning adults can display tangible features of chaos, hyperactivity, inattentiveness, and impulsivity in their everyday life, without experiencing much suffering. People who have such traits to a lesser extent (but who also have childhood trauma, financial stress, low status in society, low self-esteem) arguably have a higher risk of fulfilling criteria D–problems with academic, social, and/or occupational functioning–and therefore being diagnosed with ADHD. In the end, a diagnosis is not a result of how much ‘ADHD traits’ a person has, but how well his or her life is working out. This functioning over time, i.e., adaptation to life, is apparently crucially intertwined with social variables. Such an awareness does not dismiss the fact that ADHD, as well as every other psychiatric category, contains a certain biological predisposition. But when it comes to understanding the increasing presence of ADHD in clinical discourse, in academic research, and in media, it is vital to distinguish between people’s behavioral characteristics on the one hand, and why something becomes a disability, on the other. Even if a trait such as activity level might have a substantial genetic basis, this is not the same thing as having a disease [80]. The genetic findings from twin studies may simply display that children at risk of receiving an ADHD diagnosis more often have certain temperament factors. However, this should not be conceptualized as an explanation, since the disorder is ultimately contingent on an environment that is not sufficiently adapted to the developmental needs of the specific individual. Such an interpretation is in line with the findings of this study, where context–intersectional ecological niche–had a large impact on prevalence.

There is an abundance of research that has investigated correlates between subjects diagnosed with ADHD and potential biological abnormalities. Studies have reported an association with inflammatory processes [82], hypothesized a connection to foods and inhalants [83], or highlighted ADHD as part of a broader biological dysfunction in stress-relevant mechanisms [84]. As a nuance to this research paradigm, it is an important reminder that a person’s psychiatric symptoms seldom can be categorized into those for which we can find a biological cause and those for which we can not. This is why the term idiopathic is not even included in the DSM. When trying to imitate somatic care by utilizing assessments and explanatory narratives centered around the body in order to find a biomedical explanation behind behavioral symptoms, it is not only a misunderstanding of the very nature of mental disorders, but also a way of shifting focus and resources from other modes of understanding. In a time when the number of ADHD diagnoses are hitting epidemic proportions, parallel to growing economic gaps, it is important to be sociologically observant of which people that are receiving these diagnoses, and what kind of distress is being subject to ‘psychiatrization’ [85, 86].

Conclusion

Our study analyses a large record linkage and nationwide database, which minimises the risk of selection bias that can be prominent in smaller studies. In summary, using IE-AIHDA we mapped prevalence of ADHD across numerous strata defined by demographics and socioeconomic categories. As far as we know, our study is pioneering in providing such information. We found a large heterogeneity in the risk for an ADHD diagnosis across those IE strata, and there is a need for further studies to understand the reason for these large differences in prevalence. For this purpose, the use of intersectional contexts and thinking of these contexts as ‘ecological niches’ provides a promising approach for illuminating the diagnostic trend as a process that is historically, sociologically, and culturally situated. The large differences between different social groups raises important questions. It is our hope that future studies aim to focus on identifying factors that increases the risk of labelling behavioural and social issues as ADHD. Research could also benefit from addressing contextual interpretation of symptoms and diagnosis, contradictions between different paradigms, and practical applications of the above insights.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Number of individuals (N) and prevalence or Absolute Risk (AR) of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in the 96 intercategorical strata as well as prevalence ratios (PR).

Ranked.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294741.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Number of individuals (N) and prevalence or Absolute Risk (AR) of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in the 96 intercategorical strata as well as prevalence ratios (PR).

Not ranked.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294741.s002

(XLS)

References

  1. 1. Moderaterna. (2022, 12 aug). Förslag: Snabbtest för ADHD i utsatta områden. https://moderaterna.se/stockholm/nyhet/forslag-snabbtest-for-adhd-i-utsatta-omraden/
  2. 2. Thomas R., Sanders S., Doust J., Beller E., & Glasziou P. (2015). Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 135(4):e994–1001. pmid:25733754
  3. 3. National Board of Health and Welfare. (2019). Welfare SNBoHa. Statistical databases. https://www.socialstyrelsen.se/en/statistics-and-data/statistics/statistical-databases/
  4. 4. Polyzoi M., Ahnemark E., Medin E., & Ginsberg Y. (2018). Estimated prevalence and incidence of diagnosed ADHD and health care utilization in adults in Sweden—a longitudinal population-based register study. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment, 14, 1149–61. pmid:29765219
  5. 5. Faraone S. V., Banaschewski T., Coghill D., Zheng Y., Biederman J., Bellgrove M. A., et al. (2021). The World Federation of ADHD international consensus statement: 208 evidence-based conclusions about the disorder. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews, 128, 789–818. pmid:33549739
  6. 6. Erlandsson S., Hornborg C., Sorbring E., & Dauman N. (2022). Is ADHD a way of conceptualizing long-term emotional stress and social disadvantage? Frontiers in Public Health, 10, 1–14. pmid:36424967
  7. 7. Barkley R. A. (2002). International consensus statement on ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 5, 89–111.
  8. 8. Dickstein S. G., Bannon K., Castellanos F. X., & Milham M. P. (2006). The neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an ALE meta-analysis. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 47(10), 1051–62. pmid:17073984
  9. 9. Cortese S., Kelly C., Chabernaud C., Proal E., Di Martino A., Milham M.P., et al. (2012). Toward systems neuroscience of ADHD: a meta-analysis of 55 fMRI studies. American journal of psychiatry, 169(10), 1038–55. pmid:22983386
  10. 10. Valera E. M., Faraone S. V., Murray K. E., & Seidman L. J. (2007). Meta-analysis of structural imaging findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological psychiatry, 61(12), 1361–9. pmid:16950217
  11. 11. Ellison-Wright I., Ellison-Wright Z., & Bullmore E. (2008). Structural brain change in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder identified by meta-analysis. BMC psychiatry, 8:51. pmid:18590567
  12. 12. Del Campo N., Chamberlain S.R., Sahakian B.J., & Robbins T. W. (2011). The roles of dopamine and noradrenaline in the pathophysiology and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological psychiatry, 69(12), e145–57. pmid:21550021
  13. 13. Economidou D., Theobald D. E., Robbins T. W., Everitt B. J., & Dalley J. W. (2012). Norepinephrine and dopamine modulate impulsivity on the five-choice serial reaction time task through opponent actions in the shell and core sub-regions of the nucleus accumbens. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37(9), 2057–66. pmid:22510726
  14. 14. Maltezos S., Horder J., Coghlan S., Skirrow C., O’Gorman R., Lavender T. J., et al. (2014). Glutamate/glutamine and neuronal integrity in adults with ADHD: a proton MRS study. Transl Psychiatry, 4, e373. pmid:24643164
  15. 15. Brinkmann S. (2014). “Natural and human kinds”, in: Teo T. (ed.), Encyclopedia of Critical Psychology. Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 1220–1225.
  16. 16. Conrad P., & Potter D. (2000). From hyperactive children to ADHD adults: observations on the expansion of medical categories. Social Problems, 47(4), 559–582.
  17. 17. Timimi S., & Leo J. (Eds.). (2009). Rethinking ADHD: From brain to culture. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
  18. 18. Mills S. (2022). The scientific integrity of ADHD: A critical examination of the underpinning theoretical constructs. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13. pmid:36620696
  19. 19. Brinkmann S. (2016). Toward a cultural psychology of mental disorder: The case of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Culture & Psychology, 22(1), 80–93.
  20. 20. Te Meerman S., Freedman J. E., & Batstra L. (2022). ADHD and reification: Four ways a psychiatric construct is portrayed as a disease. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13. pmid:36590613
  21. 21. National Board of Health and Welfare. (2021). Förskrivningen av adhd-läkemedel fortsätter att öka. https://www.socialstyrelsen.se/globalassets/sharepoint-dokument/artikelkatalog/ovrigt /2021-6-7436.pdf
  22. 22. Honkasilta J. & Koutsoklenis A. (2022). The (un)real existence of ADHD–criteria, functions, and forms of the diagnostic entity. Frontiers in Sociology. pmid:35707639
  23. 23. Polanczyk G. V., Willcutt E. G., Salum G. A., Kieling C., & Rohde L. A. (2014). ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: an updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. International journal of epidemiology, 43(2), 434–42. pmid:24464188
  24. 24. Polanczyk G. V., Salum G. A., Sugaya L. S., Caye A., & Rohde L. A. (2015). Annual research review: A meta-analysis of the worldwide prevalence of mental disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 56(3), 345–65. pmid:25649325
  25. 25. Hornborg C., & Merlo J. (2018). Finns det en ADHD-epidemi i Sverige? Antropologiska och socialepidemiologiska reflektioner. Läkartidningen, 2018, 115.
  26. 26. Hornborg C., & Erlandsson S. (2022).”The Ontology of ADHD: Discovery or Invention?”, in Maisel E. (ed.), Deconstructing ADHD: Mental disorder or social construct, 330–353. Cambridge: Ethics International Press.
  27. 27. Hacking I. (1999). The social construction of what? Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.
  28. 28. Johannisson K. (2006).”Hur skapas en diagnos? Ett historiskt perspektiv”, in: Hallerstedt G. (Ed.), Diagnosens makt. Om kunskap, pengar och lidande, 29–41. Göteborg: Daidalos.
  29. 29. Rhodes L. (1990). “Studying biomedicine as a cultural system”, in Johnson T. M. & Sargent C. (Eds.), Medical Anthropology: Contemporary Theory and Method (pp. 159–173). New York: Praeger.
  30. 30. Pérez-Álvarez M. (2017). The four causes of ADHD: Aristotle in the classroom. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 928. pmid:28649208
  31. 31. Nilsson Sjöberg M., & Dahlbeck J. (2018). The inadequacy of ADHD: a philosophical contribution. Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties, 23(1), 97–108.
  32. 32. Conrad P, & Bergey M. R. (2014). The impending globalization of ADHD: notes on the expansion and growth of a medicalized disorder. Social Science & Medicine, 122, 31–43. pmid:25441315
  33. 33. Wemrell M., Bennet L., & Merlo J. (2019). Understanding the complexity of socioeconomic disparities in type 2 diabetes risk: a study of 4.3 million people in Sweden. MJ Open Diabetes Research and Care, 97, e000749. pmid:31798898
  34. 34. Bauer G. (2014). Incorporating intersectionality theory into population health research methodology: challenges and the potential to advance health equity. Social Science & Medicine, 110, 10–17. pmid:24704889
  35. 35. Evans C. R., Williams D.R., Onnela J.P., & Subramanian S. V. (2017). A multilevel approach to modelling health inequalities at the intersection of multiple social identities. Social Science & Medicine. pmid:29199054
  36. 36. Merlo J. (2018). Multilevel analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy (MAIHDA) within an intersectional framework. Social Science & Medicine, 203, 74–80. pmid:29305018
  37. 37. Persmark A., Wemrell M., Zettermark S., Leckie G., Subramanian S. V., & Merlo J. (2019). Precision public health: Mapping socioeconomic disparities in opioid dispensations at Swedish pharmacies by Multilevel Analysis of Individual Heterogeneity and Discriminatory Accuracy (MAIHDA). PLoS One, 14(8), e0220322. pmid:31454361
  38. 38. Kessler R., Adler L., Ames M., Demler O., Faraone S., Hiripi E., et al. (2005). The World Health Organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): A short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychological Medicine, 35(2), 245–256. pmid:15841682
  39. 39. Harvey E. A., Fischer C., Weieneth J. L., Hurwitz S. D., & Sayer A. G. (2013). Predictors of discrepancies between informants’ ratings of preschool-aged children`s behavior: An examination of ethnicity, child characteristics and family functioning. Early Childhood Research, 28(4), 668–682. pmid:23935240
  40. 40. DuPaul G. J., Reid R., Anastopoulos A. D., Lambert M. C., Watkins M. W., & Power T. J. (2015). Parent and teacher ratings of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms: factors structure and normative data. Psychological Assessment, 28(2) 214–225. pmid:26011476
  41. 41. Lawson G. M., Nissley-Tisiopinis J., Nahmais A., McConaughy S. H., & Eiraldi R. (2017). Do parent and teacher report of ADHD symptoms in children differ by SES and racial status? Psychopathology & Behavioral Assessments, 39,
  42. 42. Brossard B. (2019). Why mental disorders flourish and wither: Extending the theory of ecological niches. Social Science & Medicine, 237. pmid:31376530
  43. 43. Axelsson Fisk S., Mulinari S., Wemrell M., Leckie G., Perez Vicente R., & Merlo J. (2018). Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease in Sweden: An intersectional multilevel analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy. SSM Popul Health, 4, 334–346. pmid:29854918
  44. 44. Evans C. R., & Erickson N. (2019). Intersectionality and depression in adolescence and early adulthood: A MAIHDA analysis of the national longitudinal study of adolescent to adult health, 1995–2008. Social Science & Medicine, 220, 1–11. pmid:30390469
  45. 45. Pepe M. S., Janes H., Longton G., Leisenring W., & Newcomb P. (2004). Limitations of the odds ratio in gauging the performance of a diagnostic, prognostic, or screening marker. American Journal of Epidemiology, 159(9), 882–90. pmid:15105181
  46. 46. Merlo J., Mulinari S., Wemrell M., Subramanian S. V., & Hedblad B. (2017). The tyranny of the averages and the indiscriminate use of risk factors in public health: The case of coronary heart disease. SSM Popul Health, 3, 684–98. pmid:29349257
  47. 47. Ludvigsson J. F., Almqvist C., Edstedt Bonamy A.-K., Ljung R., Michaëlsson K., Neovius M., et al. (2016). Registers of the Swedish total population and their use in medical research. European journal of epidemiology, 31(2), 125–36. pmid:26769609
  48. 48. Statistics Sweden (2016a). Longitudinell integrationsdatabas för sjukförsäkrings- och arbetsmarknadsstudier (LISA). Available from: https://www.scb.se/LISA/
  49. 49. Ludvigsson J. F., Andersson E., Ekbom A., Feychting M., Kim J.-L., Reuterwall C., et al. (2011). External review and validation of the Swedish national inpatient register. BMC public health, 11:450. pmid:21658213
  50. 50. National Board of Health and Welfare. (2022). The National Patient Register. https://www.socialstyrelsen.se/statistik-och-data/register/patientregistret/
  51. 51. National Board of Health and Welfare. (2009). Kvalitet och innehåll i patientregistret. Utskrivningar från slutenvården 1964–2007 och besök i specialiserad öppenvård (exklusive primårvårdsbesök) 1997–2007. https://www.socialstyrelsen.se/globalassets/sharepoint-dokument/artikelkatalog/statistik/2009-125-15_200912515_rev2.pdf
  52. 52. Scahill L., Schwab-Stone M., Merikangas K. R., Leckman J. F., Zhang H., & Kasl S. (1999). Psychosocial and clinical correlates of ADHD in a community sample of school-age children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(8), 976–84. pmid:10434489
  53. 53. Anselmi L., Menezes A. M. B., Barros F.C., Hallal P. C., Araújo C.L., Domingues M.R., et al. (2010). Early determinants of attention and hyperactivity problems in adolescents: the 11-year follow-up of the 1993 Pelotas (Brazil) birth cohort study. Cadernos de saude publica, 26(10), 1954–62. pmid:20963293
  54. 54. Getahun D., Jacobsen S. J., Fassett M. J., Chen W., Demissie K., & Rhoads G. G. (2013). Recent trends in childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA pediatrics, 167(3), 282–8. pmid:23338799
  55. 55. Reyes N., Baumgardner D. J., Simmons D. H., & Buckingham W. (2013). The potential for sociocultural factors in the diagnosis of ADHD in children. WMJ: official publication of the State Medical Society of Wisconsin, 112(1), 13–7. pmid:23513308
  56. 56. Russell A. E., Ford T., & Russell G. (2015). Socioeconomic Associations with ADHD: Findings from a Mediation Analysis. PLoS One, 10(6), e0128248-e. pmid:26030626
  57. 57. Sun S., Kuja-Halkola R., Faraone S. V., D’Onofrio B. M., Dalsgaard S., Chang Z., et al. (2019). Association of psychiatric comorbidity with the risk of premature death among children and adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. JAMA psychiatry. pmid:31389973
  58. 58. Lindemann C., Langner I., Kraut A. A., Banaschewski T., Schad-Hansjosten T., Petermann U., et al. (2012). Age-specific prevalence, incidence of new diagnoses, and drug treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Germany. Journal of child and adolescent psychopharmacology, 22(4), 307–14. pmid:22856384
  59. 59. Vasiliadis H.-M., Diallo F. B., Rochette L., Smith M., Langille D., Lin E., et al. (2017). Temporal Trends in the Prevalence and Incidence of Diagnosed ADHD in Children and Young Adults between 1999 and 2012 in Canada: A Data Linkage Study. Canadian journal of psychiatry / Revue canadienne de psychiatrie, 62(12), 818–26. pmid:28616934
  60. 60. Statistics Sweden (2016b). Att jämföra inkomster för hushåll. Available from: https://www.scb.se/hitta-statistik/artiklar/2016/Att-jamfora-inkomster-for-hushall/
  61. 61. Barros A. J. D, & Hirakata V. N. (2003). Alternatives for logistic regression in cross-sectional studies: an empirical comparison of models that directly estimate the prevalence ratio. BMC medical research methodology, 3, 21. pmid:14567763
  62. 62. Hosmer D.W., & Lemeshow S. (2000). Applied logistic regression [2nd ed]. New York: Wiley.
  63. 63. Wemrell M., Mulinari S., & Merlo J. (2017). Intersectionality and risk for ischemic heart disease in Sweden: Categorical and anti-categorical approaches. Social Science & Medicine, 177, 213–22. pmid:28189024
  64. 64. Wagner C. H. (1982). Simpson’s Paradox in Real Life. The American Statistician, 36(1), 46–48.
  65. 65. Hacking I. (1998). Mad Travellers: Reflections on the Reality of Transient Mental Illnesses. London: Free Association Books.
  66. 66. Johnsdotter S. (2008).”Kulturella aspekter av psykisk ohälsa”, in: Östman M. (Ed.), Migration och psykisk ohälsa, 123–164. Malmö: Malmö högskola.
  67. 67. Due C., Riggs D., & Augoustinos M. (2014). Research with children of migrant and refugee backgrounds: A review of child-centered research methods. Child Indicators Research, 7(1), 209–227.
  68. 68. Chen E., Martin A. D., & Matthews K. A. (2007). Trajectories of socioeconomic status across children’s lifetime predict health. Pediatrics, 120(2), e297–303. pmid:17606533
  69. 69. Costello E. J., Compton S. N., Keeler G., & Angold A. (2003). Relationships between poverty and psychopathology: a natural experiment. Jama, 290(15), 2023–9. pmid:14559956
  70. 70. Rutter M. (2003). Poverty and child mental health: natural experiments and social causation. Jama, 290(15), 2063–4. pmid:14559963
  71. 71. Bradley R. H., & Corwyn R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual review of psychology, 53, 371–99. pmid:11752490
  72. 72. Björkenstam E., Björkenstam C., Jablonska B., & Kosidou K. (2018). Cumulative exposure to childhood adversity, and treated attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a cohort study of 543 650 adolescents and young adults in Sweden. Psychological medicine, 48(3), 498–507. pmid:28738913
  73. 73. Zhang N., Gao M., Yu J., Zhang Q., Wang W., Zhou C., et al. (2022). Understanding the association between adverse childhood experiences and subsequent attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Brain and Behavior, 12, e32748. pmid:36068993
  74. 74. Topor A., & Ljungqvist I. (2017). Money, social relationships and the sense of self: the consequences of an improved financial situation for persons suffering from serious mental illness. Community Mental Health Journal, 53, 823–31. pmid:28497399
  75. 75. Yadav S.K., Bhat A.A., Hashem S., Nisar S., Kamal M., Syed N., et al. (2021). Genetic variations influence brain changes in patients with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Transl Psychiatry 11, 349. pmid:34091591
  76. 76. Lehti V., Chudal R., Suominen A., Gissler M., Sourander A. (2016). Association between immigrant background and ADHD: a nationwide population-based case-control study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(8), 967–75. pmid:27133554
  77. 77. Larsson H., Sariaslan A., Langström N., D’Onofrio B., & Lichtenstein P. (2014). Family income in early childhood and subsequent attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A quasi-experimental study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(5), 428–435. pmid:24111650
  78. 78. Miller L. L., Gustafsson H. C., Tipsord J., Song M., Nousen E., Dieckmann N., et al. (2018). Is the association of ADHD with socio-economic disadvantage explained by child comorbid externalizing problems or parent ADHD? Journal of abnormal child psychology, 46, 951–963. pmid:29128953
  79. 79. Nigg J. T., & Craver L. (2014). Commentary: ADHD and social disadvantage: an inconvenient truth? A reflection on Russell et al. (2014) and Larsson et al. (2014). Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 55(5), 446–447. pmid:24708476
  80. 80. Te Meerman S., Batstra L., Hoekstra R., & Grietens H. (2017). Study books on ADHD genetics: balanced or biased? International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-being, 12, 1305590.
  81. 81. Hatton T. J. (2014). How have Europeans grown so tall? Oxford Economic Papers, 66(2), 349–372.
  82. 82. Anand D., Colpo G. D., Zeni G., Zeni C. P., & Teixeira A. L. (2017). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and inflammation: What does current knowledge tell us? A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8:228. pmid:29170646
  83. 83. Pelsser L. M. J., Buitelaar J. K., & Savelkoul H. F. J. (2009). ADHD as a (non) allergic hypersensitivity disorder: A hypothesis. Pediatr Allergy Immunol, 20, 107–112. pmid:18444966
  84. 84. Chang J. P., Mondelli V., Satyanarayanan S. K., Chiang Y., Chen H., Su K., et al. (2020). Cortisol, inflammatory biomarkers and neurotrophins in children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in Taiwan. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 88, 105–113. pmid:32418647
  85. 85. Mills C. (2015). The psychiatrization of poverty: rethinking the mental health-poverty nexus. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(5), 213–222.
  86. 86. Beeker T., Mills C., Bhugra D., te Meerman S., Thoma S., Heinze M., et al. (2021). Psychiatrization of society: a conceptual framework and call for transdisciplinary research. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 645556. pmid:34149474