Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

You must be myths-taken: Examining belief in falsehoods during the COVID-19 health crisis

  • May Oo Lwin ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: May Oo Lwin, Anita Sheldenkar, Pei Ling Tng

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Wee Kim Wee School of Communication and Information, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore

  • Anita Sheldenkar ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: May Oo Lwin, Anita Sheldenkar, Pei Ling Tng

    Roles Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Wee Kim Wee School of Communication and Information, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore

  • Pei Ling Tng

    Contributed equally to this work with: May Oo Lwin, Anita Sheldenkar, Pei Ling Tng

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    peiling.tng@ntu.edu.sg

    Affiliation Wee Kim Wee School of Communication and Information, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore

Abstract

The prevalence of health myths is increasing with the rise of Internet use. Left unaddressed, online falsehoods can lead to harmful behaviours. In times of crisis, such as the recent COVID-19 pandemic, the circulation of many myths is exacerbated, often to varying degrees among different cultures. Singapore is a multicultural hub in Asia with Western and Asian influences. Although several studies have examined health myths from a Western or Eastern perspective, little research has investigated online health falsehoods in a population that is culturally exposed to both. Furthermore, most studies examined myths cross-sectionally instead of capturing trends in myth prevalence over time, particularly during crisis situations. Given these literature gaps, we investigated popular myths surrounding the recent COVID-19 pandemic within the multicultural setting of Singapore, by examining its general population. We further examined changes in myth beliefs over the two-year period during the pandemic, and population demographic differences in myth beliefs. Using randomised sampling, two online surveys of nationally representative samples of adults (aged 21–70 years) residing in Singapore were conducted, the first between October 2020 and February 2021 (N = 949), and the second between March and April 2022 (N = 1084). Results showed that 12.7% to 57.5% of the population were unable to identify various myths, such as COVID-19 was manmade, and that three of these myths persisted significantly over time (increases ranging from 3.9% to 9.8%). However, belief in myths varied across population demographics, with ethnic minorities (Indians and Malays), females, young adults and those with lower education levels being more susceptible to myths than their counterparts (p < 0.05). Our findings suggest that current debunking efforts are insufficient to effectively counter misinformation beliefs during health crises. Instead, a post-COVID-19 landscape will require targeted approaches aimed at vulnerable population sub-groups, that also focus on the erroneous beliefs with long staying power.

Introduction

Myths can be understood as widely circulated and believed ideas that are false and unsubstantiated by existing scientific facts [1]. They can range from statements that appear to sound logical or intuitive, to explanations based on flimsy or distorted evidence [1]. Health myths have existed for centuries. For instance, radioactive water was once a popular medicine touted to alleviate chronic pain in the 1900s [2], while claims that vaccines are harmful have been regularly promoted [3].

With global Internet penetration increasing to an all-time high [4], and the rising use of social media, there has been an alarming surge in the spread of health myths. Users can easily post falsehoods, either intentionally to cause harm, or out of misguided and uninformed concern, and these can be viewed by millions around the globe. In their systematic review, Wang and colleagues [5] found a sharp increase in the number of studies examining health misinformation from 2012 to 2018, particularly in the areas of infectious diseases and vaccines. Misinformation is expected to have increased exponentially since then with the COVID-19 pandemic [6].

While some misinformation beliefs might be innocuous, others can be injurious to health by causing harmful behaviours. For example, the myth in Northern Nigeria that people can develop cancer after exposure to mystical sources and that hospitals can exacerbate the symptoms may have prevented people from seeking appropriate and timely treatment [7]. Within certain communities in India, myths about menstruation being “dirty” have perpetuated the stigmatisation of menstruating individuals, leading to unhygienic practices that contribute to infections and mental health problems [8]. Over 20 years ago, Wakefield [9] published a later retracted and disproven paper claiming that childhood vaccinations lead to autism. This claim has driven parental hesitancy regarding child vaccinations and contributed to a resurgence in preventable childhood diseases [10]. A systematic review by Borges Do Nascimento and colleagues [11] found that misinformation can create more disharmony among people, lead to delays in obtaining healthcare, and contribute to misinterpretations of evidence. Health misinformation can also spur the use of potentially dangerous alternative medicines [12]. Left unchecked, the propagation of myths can lead to harmful behaviours that increase the health burden.

Research objectives

Although there have been various studies examining health misinformation, myths can vary considerably between different cultures. Hence, this study aims to understand beliefs within a multicultural Asian context by examining three research questions. Firstly, to investigate popular health myths in Singapore’s multicultural setting during a health crisis, specifically the COVID-19 pandemic, we pose the question “What and how strong are beliefs surrounding prevalent COVID-19 myths among the Singapore public?” (RQ1). Secondly, most studies examining myths are cross-sectional. To understand the typology of myths which have long staying power, we further examine the question “How have beliefs in these COVID-19 myths shifted over time?” (RQ2). Finally, to understand factors associated with susceptibility to health misinformation, we examine the question “Are there demographic differences (age, gender and ethnicity) regarding these COVID-19 myth beliefs?” (RQ3). A better understanding of common myths would enable the creation of more targeted health communication strategies to tackle misinformation during current and future health crises.

Background on COVID-19 myths

During times of crisis, such as public health emergencies, uncertainty and disruption to the normal routine can rapidly increase the spread of falsehoods [13, 14].

As a novel disease that first emerged in 2019, the COVID-19 pandemic rapidly evolved over time, with the resultant flows of emerging information causing great instability and anxiety among the general public. Enabled by a rise in Internet usage spurred by physical distancing regulations [15], and greater online health-seeking behaviours due to worries about in-person visits to doctors [16], myths about the disease have spread swiftly.

In early 2020, the World Health Organisation (WHO) classified the onslaught of COVID-19 misinformation as an “infodemic”, which they warned could lead to more uncertainty about appropriate behaviours and less trust in the authorities [17]. This infodemic has remained prevalent over the pandemic, with COVID-19 myths being spread through social media [18, 19]. For instance, there have been arson attacks on 5G phone masts in the United Kingdom after a conspiracy theory blamed 5G technology for spreading the virus [20], and poisoning incidents in Iran following the circulation of inaccurate statements that alcohol can treat COVID-19 [21]. The use of the anti-parasitic drug Ivermectin was promoted by a fringe group of doctors, contrary to official recommendations, and many who consumed it displayed adverse reactions [22]. Additionally, studies have found that those who believe in myths and conspiracy theories are less likely to comply with COVID-19 safety measures and less willing to get vaccinated [23, 24]. Hence, it is imperative to understand factors influencing beliefs in COVID-19 myths, to inform effective health communication strategies to stem the spread of false information.

The profile of COVID-19 myths may differ between various cultures, and what is prevalent in one country might not necessarily be generalisable to another. For instance, Alimardani and Elswah [25] highlighted how religious types of false information have become a particularly pronounced problem in the Middle East and North Africa region. In Ghana, the slow spread of the disease led many to initially believe that black genes conferred immunity to COVID-19 [26]. Elsewhere, in the United States, an early study showed that up to 43% of respondents believed that using antibiotics, hand dryers or mouthwash could prevent COVID-19 infection [27]. Therefore, it is useful to understand the specific myths present in different cultural contexts, which would allow governments to provide targeted health communication.

Singapore is a multicultural hub in Asia that draws cultural influences from China, Southeast Asia and India. The population consists of 74% Chinese, 14% Malays, 9% Indian, and 3% Other ethnic groups, with a median age of 41.5 years [28]. With English being the most spoken language for almost half of the population [29], the country also gets exposure to Western media. It has been ranked the number one smart city in the world [30], with an Internet and social media penetration rate of 92% and 89.5% respectively in 2022 [31].

As a small and densely populated nation, Singapore was one of the first countries outside of China to identify COVID-19 cases and impose movement restrictions, leading to an increase in social media use [32] and the spread of several myths. For instance, some Singaporeans purchased Ivermectin for self-medication against COVID-19 [33], causing the government to publish advisories against using it [34].

Various fact-checking and myth-debunking platforms have emerged in response to the rise of misinformation, including the WHO’s Mythbusters [35]. In Singapore, an anti-fake news law passed in 2019 (Protection from Online Falsehoods and Manipulation Act, 2019) has been invoked to correct COVID-19 falsehoods, such as claims that the Omicron variant is resistant to vaccines [36]. Therefore, it is important to examine the changes in myth beliefs over time, to understand whether such refutation efforts have been successful.

The current study can inform future health communication efforts to tailor correction strategies to specific population subsets and more effectively prevent misinformation spread. They can also allow policy makers to understand if refutational messaging is effectively correcting beliefs or whether new methods are necessary to mitigate their impact.

Method

Data collection

Data were collected from two online surveys of nationally representative and cognitively intact adults, defined by our university’s ethics board as aged between 21 and 70 years, residing in Singapore. Ethics approval for the surveys were obtained from the Nanyang Technological University Institutional Review Board (approval numbers IRB-2020-08-014 and IRB-2021-1034). A target sample size of approximately 1200 participants for each survey was decided based on similar survey studies [23, 24] and to account for dropouts and incomplete responses. Participants were recruited through an expert online panel provider using randomised sampling and were invited via email to participate in a 20-minute online survey, with the option to leave at any point. After reading a detailed information sheet about the study, participants had to provide their written consent to participate before being allowed to proceed with the survey. Recruitment and administration of the first survey was conducted from 19 October 2020 to 24 February 2021. To examine changes in the prevalence of myths over time, a follow-up survey was conducted. Using a similar sample profile from the same panel provider, a separate set of participants was recruited using the same methods for this follow-up survey from 15 March 2022 to 6 April 2022. Both surveys were conducted in English, the most widely spoken language in Singapore, so as to ensure that participants were able to answer the questions comfortably. Each of the participants received an honorarium as a token of appreciation.

Measures

Demographic information such as age, ethnicity, gender and education were collected in both surveys.

COVID-19 misinformation beliefs were selected through discussions with health communication experts and a review of the most widely reported myths in academic literature and Singapore’s mainstream news media (see Table 1). Across both surveys, seven highly popular false beliefs were examined. In the second survey administered in 2022, three additional myths were added based on the development of the pandemic. Participants responded to these myths with one of three options: “true”, “false” or “do not know”. As all the myths are incorrect, respondents were marked as correct only if they answered “false”, and incorrect if they selected “true” or “do not know”.

thumbnail
Table 1. Highly popular Singapore COVID-19 myths examined.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294471.t001

Data analysis

Data were analysed using IBM SPSS version 29 [61] and Stata version 15.1 [62]. Duplicate responses, those with missing data, and those who completed the survey in less than half of the median speed (suggesting they did not read the questions), were removed. Participants who exhibited extensive straight-lining behaviour (i.e., giving identical answers to multiple consecutive items, which suggests they were not providing genuine responses), and ineligible participants (i.e., those that were not residents of Singapore, outside of the age range or submitted responses from outside of Singapore) were also removed. Thus, a total of 1221 respondents participated in the first survey, of which 272 were removed. For the second survey in 2022, 1121 participants responded, of which 37 were removed.

To understand the changes in myth beliefs over time, the differences in the prevalence of the seven myths utilised in both the 2020 and 2022 survey questionnaires were assessed using crosstabulation and Pearson Chi-squared tests of independence. The relationship between demographic groups (gender, age groups, education and ethnicity) and all 10 myths were also examined using crosstabulation and Pearson Chi-squared tests of independence. For all analyses, a p-value of less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Overall, 2,033 participants were included in this study, whose characteristics are summarised in Table 2. The 2020 survey was completed by 949 participants (M = 45.2 years, SD = 13.5 years; 457 males), while 1084 participants completed the follow-up 2022 survey (M = 41.8 years, SD = 12.9 years; 523 males). Overall, the samples for both years were similar in their demographic profiles, although the follow-up 2022 sample was slightly younger than the 2020 sample (p < 0.001). Both samples were generally representative of the resident Singapore population (comprising Singapore Citizens and Permanent Residents), although there was a higher proportion of those who received a university education and lower proportion of individuals who attained below secondary education level [28].

thumbnail
Table 2. Demographic profile of the study participants (N = 2033).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294471.t002

Belief in myths (RQ1)

Overall, examining data from both years combined (N = 2033), the proportion of participants who were unable to identify the myths ranged from 12.7% to 57.5%.

For the myth that “COVID-19 can be treated with antibiotics”, 63.7% correctly answered that this was false. Almost half of the participants could not recognise that it is a myth that one must spend at least 10 minutes with an infected person to get COVID-19 (48.0%).

Most participants were correctly aware that being able to hold their breath for at least 10 seconds was not an indicator of being uninfected (73.6%). The majority of people correctly believed that wearing a mask could not guarantee prevention from COVID-19 (78.4%). In contrast, over half the participants incorrectly believed or were unsure whether COVID-19 was manmade (57.5%). The majority of participants correctly answered that eating garlic and exposure to sunlight cannot prevent COVID-19.

For the three myths examined only in 2022, almost half of the participants were unsure or incorrectly believed that Ivermectin can treat COVID-19 (48.0%). Over half of the participants could not identify the myth that child immune systems cannot handle so many vaccines (51.5%), while 76.2% of participants correctly answered that 5G does not cause COVID-19.

Changes in myth prevalence over time (RQ2)

Examining changes in myth prevalence over time, belief in several myths significantly changed from 2020 to 2022 (Fig 1). The results of Chi-squared tests evaluating how beliefs in the 7 myths (examined in both surveys) varied by year are summarised in Table 3. The proportion of people correctly believing that wearing a mask could not guarantee prevention from COVID-19 increased significantly (χ2 = 18.65, p < 0.001) by 7.9% between 2020 and 2022, from 74.2% to 82.1%. In contrast, the proportion of people incorrectly believing or not knowing whether COVID-19 was manmade increased significantly by 9.8% from 2020 to 2022 (χ2 = 20.19, p < 0.001). In 2022, a significantly higher proportion of participants (χ2 = 24.40, p < 0.001) either answered incorrectly or were unsure about whether eating garlic can prevent COVID-19 (31.2%) as compared to 2020 (21.5%). Similarly, the percentage of people that either answered incorrectly or were unsure about the myth that exposure to sunlight can prevent COVID-19 significantly increased from 25.3% in 2020 to 29.2% in 2022 (χ2 = 3.98, p = 0.046).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Percentage of incorrectly answered myths in 2020 versus 2022.

Asterisk (*) indicates that there was a significant difference in the percentage that did not correctly answer this myth between the 2020 and 2022 survey. Statements 8 to 10 were only included in 2022. Answers were marked as incorrect if participants selected “true” or “do not know”.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294471.g001

thumbnail
Table 3. Results of Chi-squared tests to examine differences in 7 myth beliefs between 2020 and 2022.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294471.t003

There were no significant differences between 2020 and 2022 for the myths that COVID-19 can be treated with antibiotics, one must spend at least 10 minutes with an infected person to get COVID-19, or that being able to hold one’s breath for at least 10 seconds was an indicator of being uninfected.

Demographic factors and myth beliefs (RQ3)

In Chi-squared tests, combined survey data (N = 2033) was used to examine myths by age groups, gender, ethnicity and education level for the first seven myths, while 2022 data (N = 1084) was used for the last three myths that were only included in the second survey (see Fig 2 and Table 4). Results showed that a significantly lower proportion of females could correctly answer that “COVID-19 can be treated with antibiotics” is false (59.7%), compared to males (68.1%) (χ2 = 15.61, p < 0.001). There were significant differences between ethnicities for this myth belief (χ2 = 92.77, p < 0.001), with Chinese participants being the most likely to answer correctly that this myth is false (68.6%). Across all age groups, the majority of respondents were able to answer correctly, with 29.4% of 51–70-year-olds, 35.2% of 36–50-year-olds and 43.9% of 21–35-year-olds either answering incorrectly or being unsure about this myth (χ2 = 31.48, p < 0.001). Those who received a higher level of education were better at recognising this myth (χ2 = 28.55, p < 0.001).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Percentage of participants within demographic groups who could not identify each myth as false.

Asterisk (*) indicates that there were significant associations between the demographic variable and the myth.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294471.g002

thumbnail
Table 4. Results of Chi-squared tests to examine differences in proportion of participants who could not identify each myth as false, among demographic groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294471.t004

Significant differences in ethnicity were also found for the myth that one needs to be with an infected person for 10 minutes to contract the virus (χ2 = 39.70, p < 0.001), with 44.7% of Chinese and 47.9% of Other ethnicities answering incorrectly or being unsure about this myth, as compared to 57% of Indians and 65.1% of Malays. There were also significant differences between genders (χ2 = 4.26, p = 0.039), with 50.2% of females more likely to incorrectly answer or select the “do not know” option, as compared to 45.7% of males. There were significant differences by age (χ2 = 7.78, p = 0.020), with 48.2% of 21–35-year-olds, 44.2% of 36–50-year-olds, and 51.8% of 51–70-year-olds answering incorrectly. Participants who were more highly educated were better able to identify this myth (χ2 = 44.17, p < 0.001).

22.6% of Chinese and 31% of Other ethnicities answered incorrectly or were unsure about the myth that being able to hold one’s breath for 10 seconds or more is an indicator that a person does not have COVID-19, in comparison to 41.4% of Malays and 40.1% of Indians (χ2 = 53.85, p < 0.001). Although the majority of participants in all three age groups answered correctly, those aged 21 to 35 (32.5%) and 36 to 50 (23.6%) were significantly more likely to answer incorrectly or be unsure about this myth, compared to those aged 51 to 70 (22.9%) (χ2 = 20.20, p < 0.001). There were no significant differences in gender or education level.

For the least prevalent myth that wearing a mask ensures one will not get COVID-19, Malays (40.6%) and Indians (33.3%) were more likely to answer incorrectly or be unsure about this myth, in comparison to those of Chinese (17.6%) or Other (22.5%) ethnicities (χ2 = 76.95, p < 0.001). No significant differences were found for gender, age groups and education, with the majority of participants across these demographic groups answering correctly.

Examining the most pervasive myth that COVID-19 is manmade and was released deliberately, 51.8% of older participants incorrectly responded or were unsure whether this was true, in comparison to 59.3% within the middle-aged group and 61.3% in the younger group (χ2 = 13.79, p = 0.001). Malays (71.8%) and Indians (69.0%) were significantly more likely to incorrectly believe or be unsure about whether COVID-19 was manmade than Chinese (54.3%) and Other ethnicities (57.7%) (χ2 = 34.25, p < 0.001). Those with lower education levels were less able to identify this myth as false (χ2 = 17.80, p < 0.001). There were no significant differences between genders.

For the myth that eating garlic can prevent COVID-19, significant differences were found within all four demographic variables. 28.9% of females were unable to identify this myth, in comparison to 24.3% of males (χ2 = 5.39, p = 0.020). Malays were the only ethnic group where a greater proportion (52.9%) answered incorrectly or were unsure (χ2 = 127.32, p < 0.001). While most participants in all age groups correctly identified that the statement is a myth, 51 to 70-year-olds were the most likely to answer correctly (77.7%) (χ2 = 15.93, p < 0.001). Those with bachelor’s degrees or higher qualifications were the least likely (23%) to answer incorrectly or be unsure about this myth (χ2 = 18.12, p < 0.001).

The majority of males and females answered correctly that exposing oneself to sunlight above 25 degrees cannot prevent COVID-19. However, females were significantly less likely to answer correctly (68.9%) as compared to males (76.6%) (χ2 = 15.45, p < 0.001). Most respondents in all ethnic groups could correctly identify this statement as a myth, but Malays (55.2%) and Indians (54.9%) were significantly less able to identify this myth compared to Chinese (76.7%) and Other ethnicities (74.6%) (χ2 = 72.21, p < 0.001). While most participants in all age groups did not believe this myth, those 36 to 50 years old were the least likely to incorrectly believe or be unsure about this myth (24.3%) (χ2 = 12.37, p = 0.002). Those with no formal schooling were the most likely to answer incorrectly or be unsure about this myth compared to those with higher education levels (χ2 = 19.80, p < 0.001).

Examining ethnicity, 42.7% of Chinese and 47.4% of Other ethnicities answered incorrectly or were unsure about the myth that Ivermectin is safe and effective in treating COVID-19, and this proportion was significantly higher in Indians (59.2%) and highest in Malays (76.6%) (χ2 = 54.78, p < 0.001). There were significant differences in gender (χ2 = 5.85, p = 0.016), with 55.8% of males correctly identifying this myth as false, in comparison to 48.5% of females. In terms of age, younger participants were more likely to answer incorrectly or respond with “do not know” for this myth (52.5%) than those aged 36 to 50 (50.8%) and 51 to 70 (38.3%) (χ2 = 15.67, p < 0.001). Significant differences among those of different education levels were also observed, where bachelor’s and postgraduate degree holders combined were the only group where a majority of respondents could identify this myth (χ2 = 20.81, p < 0.001).

Similarly, Chinese and Other ethnicities were significantly less likely to be incorrect or unsure about the myth that child immune systems cannot handle so many vaccines (48.2% and 47.4%, respectively), in comparison to Malays (73.4%) and Indians (53.5%) (χ2 = 28.80, p < 0.001). The pattern of differences in education levels was also similar to the previous myths discussed, with only 20% of those with no formal schooling able to identify this myth, in comparison to 56.9% of those with at least a bachelor’s degree (χ2 = 33.50, p < 0.001). There were no significant differences between genders and age groups.

Concerning the myth that 5G causes COVID-19, Malays (57.8%) and Indians (64.8%) were less able to correctly identify this myth, compared to Chinese (80%) and Other (73.7%) ethnicities (χ2 = 36.01, p < 0.001). While the majority of participants across age groups answered correctly, those aged 51 to 70 showed the best performance (82%) (χ2 = 15.75, p < 0.001). There were no significant differences between genders. Finally, while a higher proportion (85.7%) of those who received primary education correctly identified this myth in comparison to other education levels, the overall pattern of results showed that those with higher education levels generally performed better (χ2 = 10.64, p = 0.014).

Discussion

In times of crisis, falsehoods are quick to spread, causing erroneous beliefs and unwanted behaviours that can be detrimental to society. This is evinced in the case of the COVID-19 pandemic, which has spurred a surge of misinformation worldwide. The current study examined the level of belief in ten popular myths within Singapore over two timepoints during the pandemic and how these beliefs varied by demographic profiles. Identifying the most prevalent myths and the types of people most vulnerable to them can aid health policymakers in designing targeted health communication strategies to dispel not only COVID-19 myths, but also counter other myths during infectious diseases outbreaks and future health crises.

Beliefs in individual myths

The first objective of this study was to understand the strength of belief in prevalent COVID-19 myths in a multicultural Asian setting. Our findings showed that the number of people who could not identify the myths as false varied. For the most widely held myth that COVID-19 was manmade and released deliberately, over half of the participants were unable to identify this myth as false at both timepoints. This could be due to the ongoing debate about the virus’ origins during the periods where the surveys were administered, which has been reinvigorated several times with new findings reported by respectable scientists and news outlets [63]. With novel and contradicting ideas about COVID-19’s origins spreading, laypersons may have thought that there was no consensus on this matter. The results mirror a study in Jordan where close to 60% of respondents believed COVID-19 is a man-made disease [64].

The second most prevalent myth was that child immune systems cannot handle so many vaccines (i.e. COVID-19 vaccine and other routine childhood vaccinations). This is concerning given the considerable parental hesitancy around child COVID-19 vaccination in Singapore (15.9% compared to 9.9% hesitancy for adult own vaccination) [65] and lowered uptake of vaccines for measles [66]. Parents are more risk-averse when vaccinating younger children [67] and may be especially attuned to concerns about safety and long-term effects. This follows other prominent falsehoods about children’s vaccines in recent years, leading to a decline in childhood vaccinations [68]. These findings underscore the need to bolster existing efforts to address parental hesitancy towards child vaccination, in preparation for future health crises.

The high level of beliefs in myths can be concerning, particularly if the myths lead to harmful behaviours. Over a third of participants incorrectly believed or were unsure whether antibiotics can be used to treat COVID-19, despite local awareness campaigns to deter antibiotic misuse [69]. Overuse of antibiotics is problematic as it can lead to the spread of antimicrobial resistance [70]. Similarly, almost half of the participants believed or were not sure if one needed to be in contact with an infected person for 10 minutes to get COVID-19, even though this has been debunked [39]. Therefore, health communicators need to find novel approaches to promote the correct preventative behaviours during infectious disease outbreaks.

Some myths, such as 5G causes COVID-19 and people wearing masks cannot get COVID-19 had low levels of incorrect beliefs. This contrasts with Western countries, such as the United States, where mask-wearing has been a controversial issue [71], or the United Kingdom, where 5G towers have been attacked after the spread of 5G myths [20]. This could be because mask-wearing was mandatory and highly regulated in Singapore from April 2020 to August 2022 [72, 73], driving adherence to this behaviour [74]. Despite this, Singapore still experienced multiple waves of COVID-19 cases, highlighting that mask-wearing alone cannot eliminate infection. In contrast, in countries like the United States, mask wearing requirements and adherence varied across different states at various times [75]. Additionally, Singapore is ranked as one of the top smart cities in the world [30] and is highly digitally connected, hence, many residents are less wary of technology in their lives, which makes it less likely that myths about 5G can propagate.

Changes in myth beliefs over time

The second objective of this study was to examine how beliefs in COVID-19 myths have changed over time. We found that many myths remained prevalent over the pandemic, either remaining at similar levels or increasing, despite public debunking from the government and national newspapers. This could be due to the uncertainty surrounding the disease, which can result in stress, fear and a loss of control over one’s health [76]. Such feelings can deepen conspiracy beliefs [77, 78]. Furthermore, anger became a prominent emotion as the pandemic progressed [79], which can fuel the spread of misinformation [80]. In tandem with increased information-seeking behaviours to make sense of the crisis [81], this could have led to the rapid circulation of misinformation and greater exposure to myths, which in turn is associated with higher belief in misinformation [18]. Future research should continue to examine the factors that allow myth beliefs to persist. Effective communication strategies should also account for the uncertainties and emotional responses that people are experiencing in times of crisis, to help to foster a sense of control.

The fact that these myths remained prevalent is of particular interest as research has found a strong link between higher trust in government and lower belief in false information [82]. Several studies have found that Singaporeans have a high level of trust in authorities regarding COVID-19 information [83], which should have led to lower myth beliefs among the population. Hence, trust should be studied in relation to myths in the future to better understand how they are linked in the Singapore population.

Demographic factors and myth beliefs

Finally, the last aim of this study was to examine the demographic factors associated with myth beliefs. The results indicate that inequities in misinformation beliefs and knowledge regarding COVID-19 exist, even in a highly educated country with a high Internet penetration rate [84]. Across the 10 myths examined, females, ethnic minorities (Malays and Indians) and those from younger age groups were more vulnerable to misinformation.

Examining myths by gender, males are better able to correctly identify the myths as incorrect compared to females. This is consistent with existing research demonstrating that women are more likely to believe COVID-19 misinformation [82] and more likely to accept general health misinformation [85]. However, other studies have shown that males demonstrate higher beliefs in COVID-19 misinformation [86]. These contradictory findings should be further investigated to identify why gender differences exist.

More attention needs to be paid to debunking myths for those in the younger age groups of 21 to 35 in the future, as they were more likely to believe in or be unsure about the myths compared to other age groups. Similar findings were also reported in multi-country studies on coronavirus misinformation [24, 86] and health misinformation [85]. Additionally, younger adults in Singapore are more likely to use social media [87]. Frequent social media use was associated with greater beliefs in particular types of COVID-19 misinformation in the United States [88]. Indeed, De Coninck and colleagues’ [89] study found that lower misinformation and conspiracy beliefs were observed among those who frequently used traditional media sources. In contrast, greater information-seeking from online platforms was associated with greater misinformation beliefs.

Chinese respondents, who are the majority ethnic group within Singapore, were the least likely to believe in the myths out of all the ethnic groups. This echoes recent research where those who identified as a minority group member were more susceptible to COVID-19 misinformation [24]. Respondents who belong to the ethnic group categorised as ‘Other’ were the second least likely to believe in myths after the Chinese respondents. This pattern of results could be because both groups largely consist of highly educated people [28], and higher education levels are associated with lower sharing and believing of COVID-19 rumours [90].

The Indian population within Singapore were the most likely to believe in or be unsure about the myths, followed by the Malay ethnic group. This could be due to both ethnic groups having higher proportions of people who ascribe to a particular faith, in comparison to the Chinese and Other populations [28], as research has found that higher religiosity is associated with greater belief in conspiracy theories [91]. In addition, Al-Zaman [92] found that out of 138 countries, India was the most affected by misinformation and produced the most misinformation. Many Singaporean residents of Indian ethnicity have extended family residing in India and thus may have been more exposed to myths than the other ethnicities. Effective health communication strategies in future health crises will need to focus on these vulnerable population sub-groups.

Finally, those with lower education levels were less likely to correctly identify the myths. Similar disparities in myth beliefs have been found across multiple existing studies, which indicate that higher education levels are negatively associated with belief in Covid-19 misinformation and conspiracy theories [82, 85, 86, 89, 90]. This highlights a possible gap in public health messaging for those with lower educational attainment.

Limitations and future directions

This study has a few limitations. Firstly, although this was one of the first studies in Southeast Asia to examine myths over time, different cohorts (with similar demographic profiles) were used between the first and second surveys. Hence, we could not compare direct changes in the myth beliefs of the same cohort over time. Secondly, the surveys were conducted online, which might have skewed the data towards more proficient Internet users. Additionally, the surveys were conducted in English, which might exclude non-English-speaking respondents from participation. However, English is the most widely used language in Singapore [29], and the samples for both surveys were representative of the national adult population.

Future studies should examine the attitudes and media use behaviours of the different demographic groups, to better understand the pattern of associations between media utilisation and myth beliefs. Further, comparisons with myth beliefs in other countries can be investigated to see if there are cross-cultural and regional patterns of relationships. Finally, to obtain a more comprehensive understanding of trends in myth beliefs over time in health crises, surveys can be conducted at timepoints where significant events occurred, such as the peak of cases or introduction of key measures.

Implications and conclusion

To our knowledge, this is one of the first studies examining the levels of belief in a range of COVID-19 myths over time in Southeast Asia. The findings have practical implications for health communication efforts to reduce misinformation beliefs during crisis situations. The data indicate that, despite debunking efforts, the prevalence of myths remained high at two time points in the pandemic. This could lead to detrimental consequences, such as the perpetuation of dangerous health behaviours and the further spread of falsehoods. Beyond misinformation correction, other health messaging strategies are needed to manage long-standing myths in future health crises. The major dissimilarities observed across population sub-groups also suggest that efforts focussed on vulnerable population segments will be necessary to counter these erroneous beliefs. Policymakers should pursue a more targeted health communication approach that engages younger populations, females, ethnic minorities and those with lower education levels to debunk and reduce the spread of health myths. Our findings also point to the need for myths to be addressed longitudinally during crises, instead of through one-time mass campaign approaches.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Results of binary logistic regression analyses to examine differences in proportion of participants who could not identify each myth as false, among demographic groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294471.s001

(DOCX)

S1 Dataset. Minimal anonymized dataset containing survey data for 2020 and 2022.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294471.s002

(SAV)

References

  1. 1. Kessler SH, Bachmann E. Debunking health myths on the internet: the persuasive effect of (visual) online communication. J Public Health (Berl). 2022 Aug 1;30:1823–35. pmid:35070640
  2. 2. Jorgensen TJ. The Conversation. 2016 [cited 2022 Sep 2]. When “energy” drinks actually contained radioactive energy. Available from: https://theconversation.com/when-energy-drinks-actually-contained-radioactive-energy-67976.
  3. 3. Quick J, Larson H. Time. 2018 [cited 2022 Oct 6]. The vaccine-autism myth started 20 years ago. Here’s why it endures today. Available from: https://time.com/5175704/andrew-wakefield-vaccine-autism/
  4. 4. Petrosyan A. Statista. 2022 [cited 2022 Oct 30]. Internet and social media users in the world 2022. Available from: https://www.statista.com/statistics/617136/digital-population-worldwide/
  5. 5. Wang Y, McKee M, Torbica A, Stuckler D. Systematic literature review on the spread of health-related misinformation on social media. Soc Sci Med. 2019 Nov;240:112552. pmid:31561111
  6. 6. Balakrishnan V, Ng WZ, Soo MC, Han GJ, Lee CJ. Infodemic and fake news–A comprehensive overview of its global magnitude during the COVID-19 pandemic in 2021: A scoping review. Int J Disaster Risk Reduct. 2022 Aug;78:103144. pmid:35791376
  7. 7. Sarki AM, Roni BL. This disease is “not for hospital”: myths and misconceptions about cancers in Northern Nigeria. J Glob Health Rep. 2019 Sep 1;3:e2019070.
  8. 8. Maniar A, Mehta S. Menstrual hygiene practices: Myths and taboo. Int J Res Soc Sci. 2017;7(12): 93–101.
  9. 9. Wakefield AJ, Murch SH, Anthony A, Linnell J, Casson DM, Malik M, et al. RETRACTED: Ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific colitis, and pervasive developmental disorder in children. Lancet. 1998 Feb 28;351(9103):637–641. [retracted] pmid:9500320
  10. 10. Vojtek I, Larson H, Plotkin S, Van Damme P. Evolving measles status and immunization policy development in six European countries. Hum Vaccin Immunother. 2022 Dec 31;18(1):2031776. pmid:35180372
  11. 11. Borges do Nascimento IJ, Pizarro AB, Almeida JM, Azzopardi-Muscat N, Gonçalves MA, Björklund M, et al. Infodemics and health misinformation: a systematic review of reviews. Bull World Health Organ. 2022 Sep 1;100(9):544–561. pmid:36062247
  12. 12. Johnson SB, Park HS, Gross CP, Yu JB. Use of alternative medicine for cancer and its impact on survival. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2018 Jan;110(1):121–124. pmid:28922780
  13. 13. Bordia P, DiFonzo N. When social psychology became less social: Prasad and the history of rumor research. Asian J Soc Psychol. 2002;5(1):49–61.
  14. 14. Chew C, Eysenbach G. Pandemics in the age of Twitter: Content analysis of tweets during the 2009 H1N1 outbreak. PLoS One. 2010 Nov 29;5(11):e14118. pmid:21124761
  15. 15. Branscombe M. The network impact of the global COVID-19 pandemic [Internet]. The New Stack. 2020 [cited 2022 Sep 7]. Available from: https://thenewstack.io/the-network-impact-of-the-global-covid-19-pandemic/
  16. 16. Bright HR, Chandy SJ, Rajkumar P, Philip DM. Online health information seeking behaviour due to COVID-19 pandemic-induced health related anxiety among the general population in India. J Assoc Physicians India. 2022 Jan;70(1):11–12.
  17. 17. World Health Organization. Novel coronavirus (2019-nCoV): Situation report 13 [Internet]. World Health Organization; 2020 [cited 2022 Oct 19]. Report No.: 13. Available from: https://www.who.int/docs/default-source/coronaviruse/situation-reports/20200202-sitrep-13-ncov-v3.pdf
  18. 18. Lee JJ, Kang KA, Wang MP, Zhao SZ, Wong JYH, O’Connor S, et al. Associations between COVID-19 misinformation exposure and belief with COVID-19 knowledge and preventive behaviors: Cross-sectional online study. J Med Internet Res. 2020 Nov 13;22(11):e22205. pmid:33048825
  19. 19. Naeem SB, Bhatti R, Khan A. An exploration of how fake news is taking over social media and putting public health at risk. Health Info Libr J. 2021 Jun;38(2):143–149. pmid:32657000
  20. 20. Satariano A, Alba D. Burning cell towers, out of baseless fear they spread the virus. The New York Times [Internet]. 2020 Apr 10 [cited 2022 Sep 21]; Available from: https://www.nytimes.com/2020/04/10/technology/coronavirus-5g-uk.html
  21. 21. Shokoohi M, Nasiri N, Sharifi H, Baral S, Stranges S. A syndemic of COVID-19 and methanol poisoning in Iran: Time for Iran to consider alcohol use as a public health challenge? Alcohol. 2020 Sep;87:25–27. pmid:32505493
  22. 22. Romo V. Poison control centers are fielding a surge of Ivermectin overdose calls. NPR [Internet]. 2021 Sep 4 [cited 2022 Sep 17]; Available from: https://www.npr.org/sections/coronavirus-live-updates/2021/09/04/1034217306/ivermectin-overdose-exposure-cases-poison-control-centers
  23. 23. Romer D, Jamieson KH. Conspiracy theories as barriers to controlling the spread of COVID-19 in the U.S. Soc Sci Med. 2020 Oct;263:113356. pmid:32967786
  24. 24. Roozenbeek J, Schneider CR, Dryhurst S, Kerr J, Freeman ALJ, Recchia G, et al. Susceptibility to misinformation about COVID-19 around the world. R Soc Open Sci. 2020 Oct 14;7(10):201199. pmid:33204475
  25. 25. Alimardani M, Elswah M. Online temptations: COVID-19 and religious misinformation in the MENA Region. Soc Media Soc. 2020 Jul 30;6(3). pmid:34192037
  26. 26. Adom D. The COVID-19 global pandemic: Socio-cultural, economic and educational implications from Ghana. Int Multidiscip J Soc Sci. 2020 Nov 30;9(3):202–229.
  27. 27. Geldsetzer P. Knowledge and perceptions of COVID-19 among the general public in the United States and the United Kingdom: A cross-sectional online survey. Ann Intern Med. 2020 Mar 20;173(2):157–160. pmid:32196071
  28. 28. Census of Population 2020 Statistical Release 1: Demographic Characteristics, Education, Language and Religion [Internet]. Department of Statistics, Ministry of Trade and Industry, Republic of Singapore; 2021 [cited 2022 Sep 12]. Available from: https://www.singstat.gov.sg/-/media/files/publications/cop2020/sr1/cop2020sr1.ashx
  29. 29. Ong J. English most spoken at home for nearly half of S’pore residents: Population census. The Straits Times [Internet]. 2021 Jun 16 [cited 2022 Oct 11]; Available from: https://www.straitstimes.com/singapore/english-most-spoken-at-home-for-nearly-half-of-spore-residents-population-census
  30. 30. Low D. Singapore is world’s smartest city for the third year: IMD Smart City Index. The Straits Times [Internet]. 2021 Nov 2 [cited 2022 Sep 14]; Available from: https://www.straitstimes.com/tech/tech-news/singapore-is-worlds-smartest-city-for-the-third-year-imd-smart-city-index
  31. 31. Kemp S. DataReportal. 2022 [cited 2022 Sep 8]. Digital 2022: Singapore. Available from: https://datareportal.com/reports/digital-2022-singapore
  32. 32. Tandoc EC, Goh ZH. New year, new normal [Internet]. Centre for Information Integrity and the Internet, Nanyang Technological University; 2021 [cited 2022 Oct 20]. Available from: https://www.ntu.edu.sg/docs/librariesprovider127/default-document-library/new-normal-factbookff9614de-c3b7-4c6d-8ecd-56e83958114b.pdf?sfvrsn=51071326_6
  33. 33. Meah N. Supporters who believe ivermectin can treat Covid-19 unfazed by ill effects; HSA warns self-medicating with drug can be dangerous. Today Online [Internet]. 2021 Oct 6 [cited 2022 Oct 2]; Available from: https://www.todayonline.com/singapore/supporters-who-believe-ivermectin-can-treat-covid-19-unfazed-ill-effects-hsa-warns-drug-dangerous
  34. 34. GovSG. Factually. 2021 [cited 2022 Sep 1]. Misleading claims that Ivermectin is effective against COVID-19. Available from: https://www.gov.sg/article/misleading-claims-that-ivermectin-is-effective-against-COVID-19
  35. 35. World Health Organisation [Internet]. 2022 [cited 2022 Oct 1]. Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) advice for the public: Mythbusters. Available from: https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019/advice-for-public/myth-busters
  36. 36. GovSG. Factually. 2021 [cited 2022 Sep 3]. Corrections and clarifications on falsehoods regarding content about COVID-19 and the Omicron variant in Facebook posts by Goh Meng Seng. Available from: https://www.gov.sg/article/factually031221
  37. 37. Arnold FW, Fuqua JL. Viral respiratory infections: A cause of community-acquired pneumonia or a predisposing factor? Curr Opin Pulm Med. 2020 May;26(3):208–214. pmid:32068577
  38. 38. Wu Z, McGoogan JM. Characteristics of and important lessons from the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) outbreak in China: Summary of a report of 72 314 cases from the Chinese Center for Disease Control and Prevention. J Am Med Assoc. 2020 Feb 24;323(13):1239–1242. pmid:32091533
  39. 39. Godoy M. Coronavirus FAQ: What’s the deal with the “15 minute rule”? NPR [Internet]. 2020 Oct 9 [cited 2022 Sep 12]; Available from: https://www.npr.org/sections/goatsandsoda/2020/10/09/922385856/coronavirus-faq-whats-the-deal-with-the-15-minute-rule
  40. 40. Mack CD, Wasserman EB, Perrine CG, MacNeil A, Anderson DJ, Myers E, et al. Implementation and evolution of mitigation measures, testing, and contact tracing in the National Football League, August 9–November 21, 2020. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2021 Jan 29;70(4):130–135. pmid:33507889
  41. 41. Elassar A. CNN. 2020 [cited 2022 Sep 21]. One dangerous coronavirus “self-check test” is circulating on social media. Here’s why you should avoid it. Available from: https://www.cnn.com/2020/03/16/us/coronavirus-myths-debunking-holding-breath-10-seconds-trnd/index.html
  42. 42. Cher A. CNBC. 2020 [cited 2022 Sep 28]. Don’t hold your breath. Experts debunk dangerous myths about the coronavirus. Available from: https://www.cnbc.com/2020/05/01/experts-explain-why-coronavirus-myths-misinformation-can-be-dangerous.html
  43. 43. Singh M, Davidson H, Borger J. Trump claims to have evidence coronavirus started in Chinese lab but offers no details. The Guardian [Internet]. 2020 May 1 [cited 2022 Sep 25]; Available from: https://www.theguardian.com/us-news/2020/apr/30/donald-trump-coronavirus-chinese-lab-claim
  44. 44. Maxmen A. Wuhan market was epicentre of pandemic’s start, studies suggest. Nature. 2022 Feb 27;603:15–16. pmid:35228730
  45. 45. Choo S, Chin VK, Wong EH, Madhavan P, Tay ST, Yong PVC, et al. Review: Antimicrobial properties of allicin used alone or in combination with other medications. Folia Microbiol. 2020 Mar 23;65:451–465. pmid:32207097
  46. 46. Rosenbloom C. Garlic and bleach won’t cure coronavirus. How such myths originated—and why they’re wrong. The Washington Post [Internet]. 2020 Mar 10 [cited 2022 Sep 20]; Available from: https://www.washingtonpost.com/lifestyle/wellness/cures-coronavirus-myths-garlic-silver-bleach/2020/03/09/04ec9fa6-6234-11ea-845d-e35b0234b136_story.html
  47. 47. Lin K, Fong DYT, Zhu B, Karlberg J. Environmental factors on the SARS epidemic: air temperature, passage of time and multiplicative effect of hospital infection. Epidemiol Infect. 2006;134(2):223–230. pmid:16490124
  48. 48. Martineau AR, Cantorna MT. Vitamin D for COVID-19: where are we now? Nat Rev Immunol. 2022 Jul 22;22:529–530. pmid:35869321
  49. 49. Funke D. PolitiFact. 2020 [cited 2022 Sep 7]. Sun exposure does not kill the coronavirus. Available from: https://www.politifact.com/factchecks/2020/mar/11/facebook-posts/sun-exposure-does-not-kill-coronavirus/
  50. 50. Caly L, Druce JD, Catton MG, Jans DA, Wagstaff KM. The FDA-approved drug ivermectin inhibits the replication of SARS-CoV-2 in vitro. Antiviral Res. 2020 Jun;178:104787. pmid:32251768
  51. 51. Mega ER. Latin America’s embrace of an unproven COVID treatment is hindering drug trials. Nature. 2020 Oct 20;586:481–482. pmid:33077974
  52. 52. Chong C, Lim MZ. “Trending” Covid-19 claims: Fact or fiction? The Straits Times [Internet]. 2021 Jun 17 [cited 2022 Oct 11]; Available from: https://www.straitstimes.com/singapore/trending-covid-19-claims-fact-or-fiction
  53. 53. Chia O. HSA reiterates advice against use of ivermectin to treat Covid-19. The Straits Times [Internet]. 2021 Oct 5 [cited 2022 Sep 28]; Available from: https://www.straitstimes.com/singapore/health/hsa-reiterates-advice-against-use-of-ivermectin-to-treat-covid-19
  54. 54. Schraer R, Goodman J. Ivermectin: How false science created a Covid “miracle” drug. BBC News [Internet]. 2021 Oct 6 [cited 2022 Sep 24]; Available from: https://www.bbc.com/news/health-58170809
  55. 55. World Health Organisation [Internet]. 2019 [cited 2022 Oct 18]. Ten threats to global health in 2019. Available from: https://www.who.int/news-room/spotlight/ten-threats-to-global-health-in-2019
  56. 56. Leo L. Video containing “misinformation” about COVID-19 vaccination exercise “a great disservice to our children”: MOH. Channel News Asia [Internet]. 2021 Dec 31 [cited 2022 Sep 11]; Available from: https://www.channelnewsasia.com/singapore/moh-video-misinformation-children-covid-19-vaccination-exercise-social-media-2409181
  57. 57. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [Internet]. 2020 [cited 2022 Oct 10]. Multiple vaccinations at once. Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/vaccinesafety/concerns/multiple-vaccines-immunity.html
  58. 58. Vox Heilweil R. 2020 [cited 2022 Oct 14]. How the 5G coronavirus conspiracy theory went from fringe to mainstream. Available from: https://www.vox.com/recode/2020/4/24/21231085/coronavirus-5g-conspiracy-theory-covid-facebook-youtube
  59. 59. Ahmed W, Downing J, Tuters M, Knight P. The Conversation. 2020 [cited 2022 Oct 2]. Four experts investigate how the 5G coronavirus conspiracy theory began. Available from: http://theconversation.com/four-experts-investigate-how-the-5g-coronavirus-conspiracy-theory-began-139137
  60. 60. Yasmin S. Debunking global COVID-19 myths [Internet]. Digital Medic. 2020 [cited 2022 Oct 2]. Available from: https://digitalmedic.stanford.edu/our-work/debunking-myths/
  61. 61. IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh (Version 29.0). IBM Corp; 2022.
  62. 62. Stata Statistical Software (Version 15). StataCorp; 2017.
  63. 63. Engber D, Federman A. The Atlantic. 2021 [cited 2022 Sep 3]. The lab-leak debate just got even messier. Available from: https://www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2021/09/lab-leak-pandemic-origins-even-messier/620209/
  64. 64. Sallam M, Dababseh D, Eid H, Al-Mahzoum K, Al-Haidar A, Taim D, et al. High rates of COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and its association with conspiracy beliefs: A study in Jordan and Kuwait among other Arab countries. Vaccines. 2021 Jan 12;9(1):42. pmid:33445581
  65. 65. Griva K, Tan KYK, Chan FHF, Periakaruppan R, Ong BWL, Soh ASE, et al. Evaluating rates and determinants of COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy for adults and children in the Singapore population: Strengthening our community’s resilience against threats from emerging infections (SOCRATEs) cohort. Vaccines. 2021 Nov 30;9(12):1415. pmid:34960161
  66. 66. Zhong Y, Clapham HE, Aishworiya R, Chua YX, Mathews J, Ong M, et al. Childhood vaccinations: Hidden impact of COVID-19 on children in Singapore. Vaccine. 2021 Jan 29;39(5):780–785. pmid:33414050
  67. 67. Damnjanović K, Graeber J, Ilić S, Lam WY, Lep Ž, Morales S, et al. Parental decision-making on childhood vaccination. Front Psychol. 2018 Jun 13;9:735. pmid:29951010
  68. 68. Guglielmi G. Pandemic drives largest drop in childhood vaccinations in 30 years. Nature. 2022 Jul 26;608:253. pmid:35883006
  69. 69. Health Promotion Board [Internet]. 2021 [cited 2022 Oct 5]. Antimicrobial resistance. Available from: https://hpb.gov.sg/healthy-living/preventive-health/antimicrobial-resistance
  70. 70. Ventola CL. The antibiotic resistance crisis. P T. 2015 Apr;40(4):277–283.
  71. 71. Aratani L. How did face masks become a political issue in America? The Guardian [Internet]. 2020 Jun 29 [cited 2022 Sep 2]; Available from: https://www.theguardian.com/world/2020/jun/29/face-masks-us-politics-coronavirus
  72. 72. Tay TF. Coronavirus: Mandatory for all in Singapore to wear mask when out, except for kids under 2 and those doing strenuous exercise. The Straits Times [Internet]. 2020 Apr 14 [cited 2022 Sep 15]; Available from: https://www.straitstimes.com/singapore/coronavirus-mandatory-for-all-to-wear-a-mask-when-out-with-exceptions-for-kids-under-2-and
  73. 73. Goh YH. From Monday, masks not needed except on public transport and in hospitals, clinics. The Straits Times [Internet]. 2022 Aug 24 [cited 2022 Sep 14]; Available from: https://www.straitstimes.com/singapore/from-aug-29-masks-not-needed-in-singapore-except-on-public-transport-and-in-hospitals-clinics
  74. 74. Robinson F. YouGov. 2022 [cited 2022 Oct 8]. Nine in ten S’pore adults still wear a mask outdoors, despite no longer being mandated to. Available from: https://business.yougov.com/content/42641-nine-ten-spore-adults-still-wear-mask-outdoors-des
  75. 75. Fischer CB, Adrien N, Silguero JJ, Hopper JJ, Chowdhury AI, Werler MM. Mask adherence and rate of COVID-19 across the United States. PLoS One. 2021 Apr 14;16(4):e0249891. pmid:33852626
  76. 76. Shorey S, Ang E, Yamina A, Tam C. Perceptions of public on the COVID-19 outbreak in Singapore: a qualitative content analysis. J Public Health (Oxf). 2020 Nov 23;42(4):665–671. pmid:32648577
  77. 77. van Prooijen JW, Acker M. The influence of control on belief in conspiracy theories: conceptual and applied extensions. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2015 Aug 10;29(5):753–761.
  78. 78. van Prooijen JW. Sometimes inclusion breeds suspicion: Self-uncertainty and belongingness predict belief in conspiracy theories. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2016;46(3):267–279.
  79. 79. Lwin MO, Lu J, Sheldenkar A, Schulz PJ, Shin W, Gupta R, et al. Global sentiments surrounding the COVID-19 pandemic on Twitter: analysis of Twitter trends. JMIR Public Health Surveill. 2020 May 22;6(2):e19447. pmid:32412418
  80. 80. Han J, Cha M, Lee W. Anger contributes to the spread of COVID-19 misinformation. Harvard Kennedy School Misinformation Review [Internet]. 2020 Sep 17 [cited 2022 Sep 17];1(3). Available from: https://misinforeview.hks.harvard.edu/article/anger-contributes-to-the-spread-of-covid-19-misinformation/
  81. 81. Salvi C, Iannello P, Cancer A, McClay M, Rago S, Dunsmoor JE, et al. Going viral: How fear, socio-cognitive polarization and problem-solving influence fake news detection and proliferation during COVID-19 pandemic. Front Commun (Lausanne). 2021;5:562588. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2020.562588
  82. 82. Melki J, Tamim H, Hadid D, Makki M, Amine JE, Hitti E. Mitigating infodemics: The relationship between news exposure and trust and belief in COVID-19 fake news and social media spreading. PLoS One. 2021 Jun 4;16(6):e0252830. pmid:34086813
  83. 83. Lim VW, Lim RL, Tan YR, Soh AS, Tan MX, Othman NB, et al. Government trust, perceptions of COVID-19 and behaviour change: cohort surveys, Singapore. Bull World Health Organ. 2021 Feb 1;99(2):92–101. pmid:33551503
  84. 84. Statista Research Department. Statista. 2023 [cited 2022 Sep 18]. Internet usage in Singapore—statistics & facts. Available from: https://www.statista.com/topics/5852/internet-usage-in-singapore/?#topicOverview
  85. 85. Pan W, Liu D, Fang J. An examination of factors contributing to the acceptance of online health misinformation. Front Psychol. 2021 Mar 1;12:630268. pmid:33732192
  86. 86. Pickles K, Cvejic E, Nickel B, Copp T, Bonner C, Leask J, et al. COVID-19 misinformation trends in Australia: prospective longitudinal national survey. J Med Internet Res. 2021 Jan 7;23(1):e23805. pmid:33302250
  87. 87. Statista Research Department. Statista. 2022 [cited 2022 Oct 30]. Share of Instagram users in Singapore as of June 2022, by age and gender. Available from: https://www.statista.com/statistics/952815/instagram-users-singapore-age-gender/
  88. 88. Enders AM, Uscinski JE, Seelig MI, Klofstad CA, Wuchty S, Funchion JR, et al. The relationship between social media use and beliefs in conspiracy theories and misinformation. Polit Behav. 2021 Jul 7;45:781–804. pmid:34248238
  89. 89. De Coninck D, Frissen T, Matthijs K, d’Haenens L, Lits G, Champagne-Poirier O, et al. Beliefs in conspiracy theories and misinformation about COVID-19: Comparative perspectives on the role of anxiety, depression and exposure to and trust in information sources. Front Psychol. 2021 Apr 16;12:646394. pmid:33935904
  90. 90. Long VJ, Koh WS, Saw YE, Liu JC. Vulnerability to rumours during the COVID-19 pandemic in Singapore. Ann Acad Med Singap. 2021 Mar;50(3):232–240. pmid:33855319
  91. 91. Jasinskaja-Lahti I, Jetten J. Unpacking the relationship between religiosity and conspiracy beliefs in Australia. Br J Soc Psychol. 2019 Oct;58(4):938–954. pmid:30706498
  92. 92. Al-Zaman MS. Prevalence and source analysis of COVID-19 misinformation in 138 countries. IFLA J. 2022 Mar;48(1):189–204.