Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Association of urinary sex hormones with mood and behavior changes in a community adolescent cohort

  • Philip Hazell ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Philip Hazell, Ben W. R. Balzer

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney School of Medicine, Camperdown, NSW, Australia

  • Ben W. R. Balzer ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Philip Hazell, Ben W. R. Balzer

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ben.balzer@health.nsw.gov.au

    Affiliations Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney School of Medicine, Camperdown, NSW, Australia, The University of New South Wales Faculty of Medicine, School of Women’s and Children’s Health, Randwick, NSW, Australia, Sydney Children’s Hospital, Randwick, NSW, Australia

  • Frances Garden,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Resources, Software, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Ingham Institute for Medical Research, The University of New South Wales, Liverpool, NSW, Australia

  • David J. Handelsman,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney School of Medicine, Camperdown, NSW, Australia

  • Karen Paxton,

    Roles Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney School of Medicine, Camperdown, NSW, Australia

  • Catherine Hawke,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney School of Medicine, Camperdown, NSW, Australia

  • Rebecca Ivers,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation The University of New South Wales Faculty of Medicine, School of Public Health and Community Medicine, Kensington, NSW, Australia

  • S. Rachel Skinner,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney School of Medicine, Camperdown, NSW, Australia, Specialty of Child and Adolescent Health, The Children’s Hospital at Westmead Clinical School, Sydney, NSW, Australia

  • Georgina Luscombe,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney School of Medicine, Camperdown, NSW, Australia

  • Katharine S. Steinbeck

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney School of Medicine, Camperdown, NSW, Australia, Specialty of Child and Adolescent Health, The Children’s Hospital at Westmead Clinical School, Sydney, NSW, Australia

Abstract

Objective

To examine the contribution of variation in sex hormone excretion to mood and behavioral changes in adolescent females and males.

Design

Prospective, longitudinal observational cohort study.

Methods

Participants were 342 volunteers aged 10–12 years living in rural Australia. Urinary estradiol and testosterone levels measured by liquid chromatography-mass spectrometry were obtained at three-month intervals for three years. Integrated measures (area-under-curve) of urinary steroid excretion summarised as absolute and variability during each 12-month period of the study. Psychosocial data were gathered annually with the primary outcome of depressive symptomatology. Secondary outcomes were the other subscales of the Youth Self-Report, impulsive-aggression, sleep habits, and self-harm.

Results

277 (158 male) participants contributed data over the full duration of the study and could be included in the analyses. In females, analyses of absolute urine hormone levels found no relationship between estradiol and any outcome, but higher testosterone was significantly associated with depression and poorer sleep. Greater variability of both urine estradiol and testosterone was associated with lower total psychopathology, anxious/depressed and social problems scores. Greater variability in urine estradiol was associated with lower attention problems and impulsive aggression in females. In males, higher testosterone and estradiol levels were associated with rule-breaking, and poorer sleep, and no associations were found for gonadal hormone variability for males.

Conclusions

Longitudinal measurement of both iso-sexual and contra-sexual gonadal hormones contributes to a more nuanced view of the impact of sex steroids on mood and behavior in adolescents. These findings may enlighten the understanding of the impact of sex steroids during normal male and female puberty with implications for hormone replacement therapies as well as management of common mood and behavioral problems.

Introduction

The impact of the major puberty hormones on adolescent mood and behavior based on empirical evidence is not well understood and remains controversial. Older studies have relied on cross-sectional data and less accurate steroid hormone immunoassays [1,2]. More recent longitudinal cohort studies focus more on the impact of early ontogeny or metabolic effects of puberty and usually lack the intensity of biological sampling required to decompose the contribution of normal puberty hormone changes on mood and behavior [37].

While it is certainly plausible that the dramatic changes in gonadal sex hormones during puberty may mediate dynamic changes in mood and behavior across adolescence, the relationship is likely to be complex [8,9]. The motivation of this study was to evaluate the association of pubertal hormonal changes with mood and behavior patterns. Despite a lack of confirmatory evidence, popular science writers, the lay community and some clinicians continue to express the stereotypical view that normal changes in testosterone during puberty produce aggression and out-of-control behaviour in males, and that estradiol produces depression and low mood in females–conceptualised as the ‘storm and stress’ hypothesis [1013].

Our research aimed to determine the attributable impact of puberty hormone changes on adolescent mood and behavior in both sexes, based on intensively sampled sex hormone excretion during puberty with measures of mood and behavior in a community-based sample of adolescent males and females followed for three years. Our original hypothesis was the rate of change (speed/tempo) and/or the degree of change in gonadal hormones during puberty may explain the mood and behaviour changes observed during adolescence among individuals progressing through normal puberty [14].

Materials and methods

The Adolescent Rural Cohort study of Hormones, health, Education, environments, and Relationships (ARCHER) was a five-year, prospective, multi-disciplinary longitudinal study with rolling recruitment. Its focus was on health and wellbeing in rural adolescents, centred on the regional cities of Orange and Dubbo in central western New South Wales, Australia. The detailed protocol is published [14]. Briefly, young adolescents aged between 10- and 12-years attending school in the region of interest and their parents were recruited via advertisement. This was not a study primarily about the timing of puberty, rather the age range selected was to ensure that most participants reached mid puberty where there is a marked rise in sex hormone production in both sexes. Participants were recruited and data collected between 2011 and 2016.

During this period individual participants provided, for three years, three-monthly first morning fasted urine specimens collected at home, yielding 13 data collection points in total. Post-menarcheal females provided samples in the mid-follicular phase (menstrual cycle days 7–10). Participants attended annual research office visits for anthropometry assessment, blood collection (fasting) and detailed survey completion. The approach afforded the rare opportunity to examine the cumulative influence of hormone change and variability on mood and behavior over time during puberty. However, as a community rather than clinical sample, ethical approval was denied to undertake pubertal staging by direct observation. As a result, we employed self-report against computerised Tanner line drawings as part of the annual survey. We have reported on relationships between Tanner staging and hormone results in this study which show longitudinal changes in self-rated Tanner stage were positively associated with changes in serum testosterone and estradiol in males and females, respectively (2), as well as wide variation in sex hormone excretion for any single Tanner stage.

Hormone analyses

There was high adherence to providing urine samples so that only urine data are reported whereas adherence to blood sampling reduced significantly over time during the study. Congruence between urine and serum samples has been previously reported [15]. Hormone analysis was conducted on biological samples using a liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS) method using methods as reported [9,15]. Within-day and between-day reproducibility at three levels of quality control for each analyte ranged from 2.6–9.7%, respectively. LC-MS/MS is the gold standard for steroid hormone analysis, with higher sensitivity and specificity than steroid immunoassays [16] and is capable of detecting the low levels of circulating sex steroids typical of early puberty [15,17].

The limit of detection and quantitation for testosterone was 5 pg/mL and 25 pg/mL and for estradiol 25 pg/mL and 50 pg/ml, respectively (9). In a preliminary analysis in our cohort, 14 of 484 (3%) of urine sample were undetectable for estradiol and nine of 484 (2%) of urine testosterone measures were undetectable (10). Urine hormones were corrected for specific gravity (measured by an accredited pathology service on the day of initial urine collection) although first morning urine specimens appear unaffected by hydration status [18].

Mood, behavior and sleep measures

At recruitment and at each annual clinic visit, adolescents, and a parent/carer each completed a survey encompassing a wide range of outcomes pertinent to the broader aims of ARCHER. Relevant to the present work, adolescents completed the Youth Self-Report (YSR), a 112-item validated self-report instrument which includes social competence items and eight sub-scales measuring internalizing and externalizing behaviors [19]. An impulsive-aggression measure was derived from YSR items 16, 20, 21, 37, 41, 57, 87, 95, 97, and 104, as per the method reported by Van Meter et al. [20] Adolescents completed the Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ), a uni-dimensional measure of symptom severity of childhood depression [6]. Adolescents were also asked to self-report estimated sleep duration on weekdays and at weekends, as well as report on self-harm thoughts and behaviors. For this analysis the SMFQ and YSR anxious/depressed and withdrawn/depressed subscales were the primary outcome measures. Collection of mood, behavior and sleep measures was timed to occur within seven days of a urine sample.

Statistical analysis

We estimated that 160 boys and 160 girls at Tanner Stage 3 would be required to detect a one standard deviation difference in testosterone or estradiol levels between those with and without depression, based on a prevalence of 5% and allowing for a 30% attrition rate. The total number analysed was 277 with that shortfall offset by a higher than expected number of participants exceeding the clinical cut-point for depression on the SMFQ (17% at year 3).(24) In order to describe changes in urine hormones over time, the area under the curve (AUC) with respect to the annual increase was calculated for each twelve-month time period using the trapezoid formula [21]. AUC calculation allows integration of urine hormone measures to bring four measures of urine hormones into one meaningful annualised measure. Two formulae were used to generate separate AUC data: AUC relative to ground (AUCG) and AUC relative to the increase (AUCI). AUCG reflects total hormone output from baseline and thus the overall intensity of change. It is calculated using the sum of trapezoids calculated by the area under the curve formed by hormone levels at each time point. AUCI which indicative of the change or variability in hormone levels over time as it ignores the distance from zero (as included in the AUCG). It is calculated by subtracting the area between zero and the first measure for the year of collection from the AUCG [21]. As the AUCI measures variability, and allows for negative values which would indicate a decrease in hormone concentrations [15].

Mixed-model linear regression analysis was used to account for repeated measurements to estimate a longitudinal relationship between the outcomes and exposure. Separate analyses were conducted for males and females, with testosterone and estradiol in both sexes. Mixed-models linear regression was also used to assess the relationship between the gender, time, and the outcome variables. Random intercepts for each subject were used in all mixed models to account for repeated measures in each adolescent. Analyses were conducted using Stata 16.1 (StataCorp, College Station, USA). With three primary outcomes (SMFQ score, YSR anxious/depressed, and YSR withdrawn/depressed subscales) that were interrelated, a modified Bonferroni correction was applied to the pre-specified significance of p = 0.05, halfway between no and full adjustment and set at p = 0.033.

Ethics and consent

The ARCHER study has ethics approval from the Human Research Ethics Committee, University of Sydney (HREC 2010/13094, 2012/2425 and 2015/199) within the framework of the National Health and Medical Research Council Guidelines for Human Experimentation and consistent with the Declaration of Helsinki. A parent/guardian provided full written informed consent and adolescents provided assent prior to commencement in the study. The authors involved in data analysis did not have access to information that would directly identify study participants.

Results

Cohort characteristics

Of 342 adolescents recruited to the ARCHER study, 277 adolescents (158 males, 119 females) with complete or near-complete urine data were included in the mixed-models regression analyses. The mean (standard deviation (SD)) age at baseline was 11.8 (1.02) years for males and 11.6 (0.96) years for females (p = 0.23). The intended duration of follow-up for each participant in this study was three years and at the end 86% of females had reached menarche (2). There is no comparable measure in males however 134 of 158 (85%) males reached Tanner stage 4 or 5 at the end of the study.

The cohort demographics were similar to national data for age and sex, except for a higher percentage of Indigenous adolescents (11% v. 6%), and a slightly higher level of maternal education. The parent questionnaire included questions on chronic conditions, with only asthma (inhaler therapy only) and ADHD (treated and untreated) recorded. One endocrinologist (KS) reviewed all final growth curves. Height growth patterns (and self-reported Tanner stages) consistent with earlier, average, and later normal puberty were present. None were consistent with an endocrine abnormality.

Longitudinal change in urine testosterone and estradiol hormone measures

All testosterone and estradiol results are centred on Tanner stage rather than against chronological age, given the wide variation in in Tanner stage for any given chronological age during puberty. The urine values are all higher than would be found in serum samples.

Fig 1 displays the mean urine hormone concentrations for each three-month collection. There were significant increases in both urine testosterone and estradiol over the collection period in both males and females (p<0.001 for all). Urine testosterone and estradiol were different between sexes across the course of the study (p<0.001 for all). Urine testosterone and estradiol were positively correlated for males (r = 0.53, p<0.001), and in females (r = 0.70, p<0.001).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Mean three-month urine hormone levels for males and females.

Urine hormone levels are corrected for specific gravity. * Note different y-axes for each sex/hormone combination. Urine hormones are measured via LC-MS/MS and reported in nmol/L. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293040.g001

Annual AUCG (total) and AUCI (variability) are provided in Table 1 and demonstrate some sex differences. There was a significant increase, as expected, in the AUCG for testosterone and estradiol in both males and females (p<0.001 for all). In males, the AUCG for testosterone and estradiol were positively correlated (r = 0.64, p<0.001), but not in females (r = 0.04, p = 0.52).

There was a significant decrease in variability—AUCI—for estradiol for both females and males (p<0.001 for both) over time. There was also a significant decrease in AUCI for testosterone in females (p<0.001) but no significant change in males (p = 0.15). In both males and females AUCI for testosterone and estradiol were both positively correlated (M: r = 0.33, p<0.001; F: r = 0.50, p<0.001).

Longitudinal change in mood, behavior and sleep measures

The median values (interquartile range) for SMFQ, YSR and sleep variables are shown in Table 2. Except for sleep, higher values are less favourable. With respect to mood there were significant changes in the SMFQ and YSR anxious/depressed over the study, with values improving for males and deteriorating for females. The YSR withdrawn/depressed subscale followed a similar trend, but the sex divergence was not statistically significant. Females had significantly higher SMFQ scores compared to males across the study duration.

thumbnail
Table 2. Median values (and interquartile range) for mood and behaviour survey results.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293040.t002

With respect to other behaviors the YSR total pathology scale improved in males and deteriorated in females. Males had significant changes in their somatic complaints, social problems, thought problems, and rule-breaking behavior subscales, with median scores improving across the study duration. Females had significant deterioration in their rule-breaking behavior subscale only. Both sexes had significant decreases in the amount of self-reported sleep during weekdays over the study. Females progressively slept less on weekend nights. These symptom scores are mostly subthreshold for clinical diagnosis, and reflect the multiple epidemiological studies that show an upswing of psychopathology in females but not in males during adolescence [1618], and indicate that our sample has the trends observed in the wider community.

Relationships between mood and behavior measures and urine hormones

Total testosterone and estradiol changes.

In males, a significant positive association was observed for urine testosterone and estradiol AUCG with the YSR rule-breaking behavior subscale. Negative associations were observed for both urine testosterone and estradiol AUCG and sleep on weekdays.

In females, no significant associations were observed for AUCG for urine estradiol and any mood or behavioral outcome. Females had significant positive associations between total change in urine testosterone (as measured by AUCG) and YSR anxious/depressed and withdrawn/depressed subscales. Significant negative associations were observed for AUCG for testosterone and self-reported weekday and weekend sleep for females. These relationships are shown in Table 3.

thumbnail
Table 3. Relationships between survey outcomes and hormone Area Under the Curve Relative to Ground (AUCG).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293040.t003

Annual hormone variability in testosterone and estradiol.

In females there were significant inverse relationships between urine hormone variability (measured by AUCI) and mood and behavioral problems. Greater variability of both urine estradiol and testosterone was associated with lower YSR total psychopathology, anxious/depressed, and social problems scores. Greater variability in urine estradiol only was associated with both lower YSR attention problems and impulsive-aggressive behaviors in females. Testosterone variability in females was negatively associated with self-harm behaviors. In males, the only significant finding in hormone variability was a negative association between estradiol and sleep at weekends. These relationships are shown in Table 4.

thumbnail
Table 4. Relationships between survey outcomes and annual hormone Area Under the Curve Relative to Increase (AUCI).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293040.t004

Discussion

This study suggests that the associations of these sex hormones, iso-sex and cross-sex hormones, with adolescent mood and behaviour are more complex when multiple outcomes and patterns of change are considered. Some significant associations described are not always in agreement with previous studies on this topic. This may be due to the cross-sectional nature of many previous studies assessing only a single time point during puberty as well as less accurate laboratory measures for sex steroids, and without consideration of cross-sex hormones.

Of clinical importance is the association of higher urinary testosterone and lower estradiol variability in females with anxiety, depression, and withdrawn mood. In support of our finding is a re-analysis of data from the Great Smoky Mountain Study (GMS), a longitudinal psychiatric epidemiology study. Estradiol levels from dried blood spots, based on random sampling for time of day or menstrual cycle, and less frequently than in the present study, were positively associated with depression in females. A subsequent re-analysis of these dried blood spots for testosterone found a stronger relationship for testosterone with depression in females [22]. Our study demonstrated less variability in estradiol levels and higher absolute testosterone levels have negative associations with affect. This combination is frequently observed together with affect disturbance in hyperandrogenic states in females such as insulin resistance and the polycystic ovary syndrome [23,24]. Thus, these conditions may benefit by reduction in testosterone exposure and a more cyclical estradiol production, either by pharmacotherapy or by restoration of ovarian cyclicity with lifestyle intervention and weight reduction. Clinical trial data are needed to support this proposition.

Our study was unable to demonstrate associations of testosterone with aggressive behaviors with the normal increases of this hormone during puberty. The two significant associations with total testosterone in males were a positive one with YSR- rule breaking scale and a negative one with sleep duration. Goddings et al recently reported on a cross-sectional study measuring salivary testosterone, self-reported tendencies on risk taking in males and fMRI neural activation that provides support our longitudinal observations if risk taking is viewed as one aspect of rule breaking. It is conceivable that our data may be useful in the interpretation of the numerous studies now considering the impact of puberty on neurocognition [1012,25].

The study data may also be useful clinically for in three other ways. First, results provide reassurance for parents that induction of puberty in hypogonadal males should not increase aggressive behaviours [26,27]. In males the absolute increase in urine testosterone was associated with increased rule breaking, but not with measures of aggression or mood disturbance. This finding was a conclusion hinted at in a previous systematic review [1]. It is likely that the adverse effects of testosterone, which were not apparent in community adolescents in this study, have been extrapolated from androgen abuse in adult males with often massive supra-physiological dosing and where anger, anxiety and low mood are features [28,29]. Secondly, cross-sex hormone data combined with same sex hormone data may also be useful for understanding the effects of gender affirming hormone therapy in adolescents with gender dysphoria [30]. Thirdly, these data may direct clinicians and parents to consider alternative explanations for concerning adolescent behaviours. The hormones remain at the same level after puberty, but behaviours generally ameliorate, an observation that may fit better with neurocognitive maturation extending into the early 20s. As an extension, alternative management approaches should be considered. These might include interventions around improving self-regulation for emotions. Alternatively, exogenous causes for anger and /or aggression because of depression or distress caused by others may need to be addressed. This would include assessment of less than ideal sleep patterns that in both sexes show some negative associations with puberty hormones and may amplify the classic sleep architecture changes during adolescence [31].

The strengths of this study are its advantages over other recent adolescent cohort studies [3,4,32] with an intensive biological sampling protocol with precise timing of sample collection and hormone measurements with the current gold standard in order to describe the progression of puberty. That we achieved superior, near complete longitudinal biological data collection using urine specimens [14] highlights the need for ‘adolescent friendly’ research protocols to answer important research questions in this age group. The limitations were first the relatively short period of observation, three years for an individual meant not all participants reached pubertal maturity and some were already well advanced in puberty at recruitment. The lower age limit for recruitment was to ensure the participants were able to provide their own mood and behavior assessments. It is also acknowledged that there are external influences on mental health and wellbeing, but much larger numbers would have been required and the resources, especially for hormone measures, beyond the limit of most granting bodies.

Conclusion

This study provides a more nuanced view of the progression of puberty hormone change and its impacts on mood and behaviour during puberty. Our original hypothesis was part proven and variability in change had a minor role, not rate of change per se. Without any implications for any hormonal interventions during normal puberty, the present study provides clinically relevant data that supporting an interpretation of impact of hyperandrogenemia in female affective disorders., There are possible implications for the use of gonadal hormone replacement in hypogonadism and in affirmative management of gender dysphoria but this would require specific studies designed to test any hypotheses. There is a final important health message in our findings. Puberty is only one part of the complex developmental stage of adolescence that includes psychosocial transitions and neurocognitive maturation. If mood and behavioural changes are attributed solely to hormones in the second decade many young people will be deprived of health care interventions that would go far towards improving their health and wellbeing.

References

  1. 1. Duke SA, Balzer BW, Steinbeck KS. Testosterone and its effects on human male adolescent mood and behavior: a systematic review. J Adolesc Health. 2014;55(3):315–22. Epub 2014/08/26. pmid:25151053
  2. 2. Balzer BW, Duke SA, Hawke CI, Steinbeck KS. The effects of estradiol on mood and behavior in human female adolescents: A systematic review. Eur J Pediatr. 2015;174(3):289–98. pmid:25567794
  3. 3. Boyd A, Thomas R, Hansell AL, Gulliver J, Hicks LM, Griggs R, et al. Data Resource Profile: The ALSPAC birth cohort as a platform to study the relationship of environment and health and social factors. Int J Epidemiol. 2019;48(4):1038–9k. pmid:31006025
  4. 4. Mostazir M, Jeffery A, Hosking J, Metcalf B, Voss L, Wilkin T. Evidence for energy conservation during pubertal growth. A 10-year longitudinal study (EarlyBird 71). Int J Obes (Lond). 2016;40(11):1619–26. Epub 20160908. pmid:27604470
  5. 5. Joinson C, Heron J, Araya R, Paus T, Croudace T, Rubin C, et al. Association between pubertal development and depressive symptoms in girls from a UK cohort. Psychol Med. 2012;42(12):2579–89. Epub 20120412. pmid:22717026
  6. 6. Angold A, Costello EJ, Messer SC, Pickles A. Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 1995;5(4):237–49.
  7. 7. Kolby N, Busch AS, Aksglaede L, Sorensen K, Petersen JH, Andersson AM, et al. Nocturnal Urinary Excretion of FSH and LH in Children and Adolescents With Normal and Early Puberty. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 2017;102(10):3830–8. pmid:28938419
  8. 8. Naninck EF, Lucassen PJ, Bakker J. Sex differences in adolescent depression: do sex hormones determine vulnerability? Journal of neuroendocrinology. 2011;23(5):383–92. Epub 2011/03/23. pmid:21418338
  9. 9. Steinbeck KS, Garden FL, Cheng HL, Luscombe GM, Handelsman DJ. Bumpy and Smoother Pathways of Puberty Hormone Change: A Novel Way to Define Gonadal Hormone Trajectories in Adolescents. J Endocr Soc. 2020;4(2):bvz014. Epub 2020/02/06. pmid:32016164
  10. 10. Holm MC, Leonardsen EH, Beck D, Dahl A, Kjelkenes R, de Lange AG, et al. Linking brain maturation and puberty during early adolescence using longitudinal brain age prediction in the ABCD cohort. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2023;60:101220. Epub pmid:36841180.
  11. 11. Goddings AL, Dumontheil I, Viner RM, Blakemore SJ. Puberty and risky decision-making in male adolescents. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2023;60:101230. Epub pmid:36965437.
  12. 12. Piekarski DJ, Colich NL, Ho TC. The effects of puberty and sex on adolescent white matter development: A systematic review. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2023;60:101214. Epub pmid:36913887.
  13. 13. Hall GS. Adolescence: its psychology and its relation to physiology, anthropology, sociology, sex, crime, religion and education. New York,: D. Appleton and company; 1904.
  14. 14. Steinbeck K, Hazell P, Cumming RG, Skinner SR, Ivers R, Booy R, et al. The study design and methodology for the ARCHER study—adolescent rural cohort study of hormones, health, education, environments and relationships. BMC Pediatr. 2012;12:143. Epub 2012/09/07. pmid:22950846
  15. 15. Singh GK, Balzer BW, Kelly PJ, Paxton K, Hawke CI, Handelsman DJ, et al. Urinary sex steroids and anthropometric markers of puberty—a novel approach to characterising within-person changes of puberty hormones. PLoS One. 2015;10(11):e0143555. pmid:26599397
  16. 16. Handelsman DJ, Wartofsky L. Requirement for mass spectrometry sex steroid assays in the Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 2013;98(10):3971–3. Epub 2013/10/08. pmid:24098015
  17. 17. Harwood DT, Handelsman DJ. Development and validation of a sensitive liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry assay to simultaneously measure androgens and estrogens in serum without derivatization. Clin Chim Acta. 2009;409(1–2):78–84. pmid:19747904
  18. 18. Singh GK, Balzer BW, Desai R, Jimenez M, Steinbeck KS, Handelsman DJ. Requirement for specific gravity and creatinine adjustments for urinary steroids and luteinizing hormone concentrations in adolescents. Ann Clin Biochem. 2015;52(6):665–71. Epub 2015/03/18. pmid:25780247
  19. 19. Ivanova MY, Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA, Dumenci L, Almqvist F, Bilenberg N, et al. The generalizability of the Youth Self-Report syndrome structure in 23 societies. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2007;75(5):729–38. Epub 2007/10/03. pmid:17907855
  20. 20. Van Meter A, Youngstrom E, Freeman A, Feeny N, Youngstrom JK, Findling RL. Impact of Irritability and Impulsive Aggressive Behavior on Impairment and Social Functioning in Youth with Cyclothymic Disorder. Journal of child and adolescent psychopharmacology. 2016;26(1):26–37. Epub 2016/02/03. pmid:26835744
  21. 21. Pruessner JC, Kirschbaum C, Meinlschmid G, Hellhammer DH. Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2003;28(7):916–31. Epub 2003/08/02. pmid:12892658
  22. 22. Copeland WE, Worthman C, Shanahan L, Costello EJ, Angold A. Early Pubertal Timing and Testosterone Associated With Higher Levels of Adolescent Depression in Girls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019;58(12):1197–206. Epub 2019/02/16. pmid:30768421
  23. 23. Cooney LG, Dokras A. Depression and Anxiety in Polycystic Ovary Syndrome: Etiology and Treatment. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2017;19(11):83. Epub pmid:28929349.
  24. 24. Zehravi M, Maqbool M, Ara I. Depression and anxiety in women with polycystic ovarian syndrome: a literature survey. Int J Adolesc Med Health. 2021;33(6):367–73. Epub pmid:34420269.
  25. 25. Larsen B, Luna B. Adolescence as a neurobiological critical period for the development of higher-order cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;94:179–95. Epub pmid:30201220.
  26. 26. Santhakumar A, Miller M, Quinton R. Pubertal induction in adult males with isolated hypogonadotropic hypogonadism using long-acting intramuscular testosterone undecanoate 1-g depot (Nebido). Clin Endocrinol (Oxf). 2014;80(1):155–7. Epub pmid:23383861.
  27. 27. Wang C, Swerdloff RS, Iranmanesh A, Dobs A, Snyder PJ, Cunningham G, et al. Transdermal testosterone gel improves sexual function, mood, muscle strength, and body composition parameters in hypogonadal men. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 2000;85(8):2839–53. pmid:10946892
  28. 28. Maravelias C, Dona A, Stefanidou M, Spiliopoulou C. Adverse effects of anabolic steroids in athletes. A constant threat. Toxicol Lett. 2005;158(3):167–75. pmid:16005168
  29. 29. Handelsman DJ. Androgen Misuse and Abuse. Endocr Rev. 2021;42(4):457–501. pmid:33484556
  30. 30. Coleman E, Radix AE, Bouman WP, Brown GR, de Vries ALC, Deutsch MB, et al. Standards of Care for the Health of Transgender and Gender Diverse People, Version 8. International Journal of Transgender Health. 2022;23(sup1):S1–S259. pmid:36238954
  31. 31. Colrain IM, Baker FC. Changes in sleep as a function of adolescent development. Neuropsychol Rev. 2011;21(1):5–21. Epub 2011/01/13. pmid:21225346
  32. 32. Sorensen K, Aksglaede L, Petersen JH, Juul A. Recent changes in pubertal timing in healthy Danish boys: associations with body mass index. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 2010;95(1):263–70. Epub 2009/11/21. pmid:19926714