Figures
Abstract
Injuries commonly occur on stairs, with high injury rates in young adults, especially young women. High injury rates could result from physiological and/or behavioral differences; this study focuses on behaviors. The purposes of this observational study were (1) to quantify young adult behaviors during stair descent and (2) to identify differences in stair descent behavior for young adult men versus women. Young adult pedestrians (N = 2,400, 1,470 men and 930 women) were videotaped during descent of two indoor campus staircases, a short staircase (2 steps) and a long staircase (17 steps). Behaviors during stair descent were coded by experimenters. Risky behaviors observed on the short staircase included: No one used the handrail, 16.1% used an electronic device, and 16.4% had in-person conversations. On the long staircase: 64.8% of pedestrians did not use the handrail, 11.9% used an electronic device, and 14.5% had in-person conversations. Risky behaviors observed more in women included: less likely to use the handrail (long staircase), more likely to carry an item in their hands (both staircases), more likely to engage in conversation (both staircases), and more likely to wear sandals or heels (both staircases) (p≤0.05). Protective behaviors observed more in women included: less likely to skip steps (both staircases), and more likely to look at treads during transition steps (long staircase) (p≤0.05). The number of co-occurring risky behaviors was higher in women: 1.9 vs 2.3, for men vs women, respectively (p<0.001). Five pedestrians lost balance but did not fall; four of these pedestrians lost balance on the top step and all five had their gaze diverted from the steps at the time balance was lost. The observed behaviors may be related to the high injury rate of stair-related falls in young adults, and young women specifically.
Citation: Cho H, Arnold AJ, Cui C, Yang Z, Becker T, Kulkarni A, et al. (2023) Risky behavior during stair descent for young adults: Differences in men versus women. PLoS ONE 18(7): e0288438. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0288438
Editor: In-Ju Kim, University of Sharjah, UNITED ARAB EMIRATES
Received: February 1, 2023; Accepted: June 15, 2023; Published: July 26, 2023
Copyright: © 2023 Cho et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: The dataset is publicly available at Purdue University Research Repository. https://purr.purdue.edu/publications/4218/1.
Funding: The authors received no specific funding for this work.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Falls often occur due to environmental hazards [1] and stairs are one of the most serious hazards encountered regularly in daily activities [2, 3]. Although less than 1% of the waking day is spent on stairs, 12% of falls in young adults occurred while navigating stairs [4, 5]. Falls on stairs are more likely to result in injuries [6–12], with 10% of fatal falls being associated with stair use [13].
Falls on stairs occur across the lifespan [14–16]; average injury rate of stair-related falls in the United States demonstrates a trimodal distribution with peaks at ≤ 3 years of age, young adults in their 20’s, and adults ≥ 85 years [16]. Higher injuries could result from physiological factors (e.g., decreased strength in older adults [17]), and/or behavioral factors (e.g., not using the handrail [18]). Since physiological parameters are presumably not compromised in young adults, it seems that quantifying behavior will be highly relevant to understand fall risk on stairs.
While both men and women demonstrated a peak in injury rate in their 20’s, there are two observations that emphasize that young women are particularly susceptible to injury on stairs. First, the injury rate for young women is approximately 80% higher than the rate for young men [16]. Second, the injury rate is highest for women in their 20’s relative to all other age decades (for both sexes) with the exception of women ≥ 81 years [16]. However, the reason women in their 20’s sustain more stair-related injuries is unknown. The focus of this study is to quantify behaviors observed in young adults that may contribute to fall risk on stairs, and to determine if the higher injury rate in young women versus men is related, at least partially, to behavioral differences.
Fall risk on stairs can be increased by a range of behaviors such as: not using the handrails, using the hands for other tasks such that the handrail cannot be grasped if a person stumbles, not visually attending the stairs, using electronic devices and other distracting activities, skipping steps, and wearing inappropriate footwear. Observational research has demonstrated that the majority of pedestrians do not use the handrails (57–94%) [18–22]. Gaze behavior is critical for safe stair traversal [23]. Video coding of a small group of pedestrians (from security video footage) indicated that not looking down at the step appeared to be associated with falls [24]. Furthermore, multitasking affects situational awareness and gait behavior [25], which may result in falls [5] and injuries [26]. Lab-based research has demonstrated that talking on a mobile phone impairs stair gait [27], and phone use strongly draws visual attention away from the stairs [28, 29]. Observational research indicates that those who use mobile devices on stairs were less likely to use the handrail and were more likely to drift across the stairway [22]. Identifying sex-related differences in these behaviors may reveal why women sustain more stair related injuries [16].
The purposes of this observational study were (1) to identify stair behaviors in young adults that may increase fall risk and (2) to determine if young women are more likely to demonstrate these risky behaviors. Since the majority of falls occur during descent [7, 9, 12, 13], we videotaped and coded stair descent of pedestrians. Indoor staircases were selected since the majority of stair-related falls in young adults occurred indoors (69%) [5]; staircases in university campus buildings were selected to ensure the majority of observed pedestrians were young adults. We videotaped both a short staircase (2 steps) and a long staircase (17 steps), as there is a high accident rate on staircases with five or fewer steps, but falls from higher heights result in more injuries [9, 30].
Methods
The study was approved by the University Institutional Review Board (IRB). The IRB determined that the study met the criteria for exemption since observations occurred in a public setting and we did not obtain any personal information from the pedestrians. No informed consent was required.
Staircases
The short staircase was located indoors and had two steps (Fig 1A and 1B). The lower level and step were carpeted with a low-pile gray carpet (multiple small loops that are not cut, sometimes called Berber carpet). The upper level was carpeted with a geometric patterned yellow and gray Berber carpet. The risers were black. The tread width was 173.0 cm. Both treads were 41.6 cm in depth and both risers were 10.2 cm in height. The treads had a square metal stair nosing that was 2 x 5 cm (height by depth); the nosing was almost flush with the tread and riser. Five black friction strips (0.7 cm wide) were embedded in the nosing. The first strip was placed 0.2 cm from the tread edge. No stair highlighters were present, although the metal nosing may act as a highlighter. The handrail was round with diameter 4.8 cm and located 93.2 cm above the stairs. No windows were located near the short staircase, so the lighting level was only affected minimally by outdoor light levels; the light level was 125 lux (daytime) and 112 lux (evening). Evening light levels were recorded because videotaping occurred in the evening.
The short staircase (two steps) (A, B) and long staircase (17 steps) (C, D). The long staircase (34 steps) included a landing, the video was coded from the landing to the bottom of the stairs (17 steps).
The long staircase was located indoors and had 34 steps (Fig 1C and 1D). There was a landing (121.9 cm long) separating the top 17 steps from the bottom 17 steps. Because pedestrians were not sufficiently visible for coding on the top set of stairs, only the bottom 17 steps were coded. The flooring was the same on the landing, treads, and lower level: smooth finish, beige color with brown and black flecks. The risers were smoothed cement. The tread width was 182.0 cm. Tread depth ranged from 29.2 to 30.5 cm. Deeper treads were observed on the top to middle steps (steps 1–15 were 30.5 cm). Shallower treads were observed at the bottom steps (step 16 was 30.2 cm and step 17 was 29.2 cm). The risers ranged in height from 15.7 to 16.5 cm. The shortest risers were found at the top of the staircase (step 2 was 15.8 cm and step 3 was 15.7 cm), and taller risers occurred at the bottom (step 15 was 16.3 cm, step 16 was 16.2 cm, and step 17 was 16.5 cm). The remainder of the risers (step 1 and steps 4–14) were 16.0 cm. The stair nosing was made of the same material as the treads and was square in shape with a slope where the underside of the nosing joined the riser, termed bevel nosing. Nosing height was 3.2 cm, and depth was 1.2 cm (measured horizontally from front of nosing to riser). Three black friction strips (0.5 cm wide) were placed on each tread; the first strip was placed 1.3 cm from the tread edge. No stair highlighters were present, although the friction strips may act as a highlighter that was not flush with the front edge of the stair. Windows were located near the top of the long staircase (Fig 1), influencing the lighting level. Light levels were measured for two extremes: a sunny day and an overcast day (no evening videos were recorded on the long staircase). During the sunny day, the lighting level at the top of the stair was 278 lux and decreased monotonically to 120 lux at step 17. During the cloudy day, the lighting level at the top step was 122 lux and decreased monotonically to 68 lux at step 17.
Data collection
To record the videos, smartphone cameras were used (iPhone and Nokia). The cameras were placed unobtrusively at the bottom and to the side of the staircases. The camera position captured a frontal view of the pedestrian, such that the face and full body were visible for video coding. Due to the extensive use of electronic devices with videos, video recording in public settings is generally acceptable [21]. An experimenter was seated by the camera, was engaged in an activity (e.g., working on a laptop), and was not actively watching the pedestrians. Videos were recorded when experimenters had availability, between the hours of 8 am and 7 pm. The videos were recorded at different times of the day to reduce the likelihood that the same pedestrians were videotaped more than once. Since university classes occur on a Monday/Wednesday/Friday schedule, or a Tuesday/Thursday schedule, overlapping times were avoided across those days (i.e., if video was recorded at 8:00–10:00 am on Monday, that time interval was not recorded on Wednesday or Friday). Video recording continued until the semester ended.
Participants
A total of 2,671 pedestrians were captured descending the staircases. Since the study focused on young adults, 271 pedestrians were excluded (161 midlife adults, 43 older adults, and 67 non-determined age). Thus, 2,400 young adults were included in the analyses (1,470 men (61%)); 1,013 pedestrians on the short staircase (484 men (48%)), and 1,387 pedestrians on the long staircase (986 men (71%)).
Data coding
Trained coders analyzed the video recordings using PotPlayer (https://potplayer.daum.net/) with zoom features. The coders were graduate students in Health and Kinesiology; coders were taking a graduate level course that examined slips, trips, and falls across the adult lifespan. Coding criteria were developed for behaviors identified as risky on stairs [18, 21, 24, 27–29, 31]; coding criteria (outlined below) were developed using a set of pilot videos in multiple locations. These locations included other campus locations, shopping malls, and public transportation stations (results from these pilot videos not included here). For each coded variable, there were two coders: primary and secondary. The primary coder coded the assigned behavior for all pedestrians. The secondary coder coded approximately 25% of the same pedestrians. The results of the two coders were used to quantify coding reliability [32].
Pedestrian characteristics and footwear.
Coders quantified pedestrian characteristics including age group (young adults: 18–39 years, midlife adults: 40–60 years, or older adults: 61+ years), sex (man or woman), and footwear (flats, sandals, sliders, or high heels). Coding for age and sex was based on pedestrian appearance (e.g., facial features and anatomy) consistent with previous observational studies [18, 20–22, 33]. Footwear coding criteria were as follows: Flats had a flat footbed and the foot was fully enclosed by the shoe (e.g., running shoes). Sandals had a flat foot bed with straps at the toe and ankle. Sliders had a flat footbed with a toe strap but no ankle strap. Sliders are also known as flip flops, slides, or slip-ons. High heels included footwear with a raised heel.
Pedestrian behaviors.
Coders quantified hand behaviors, gaze behaviors, interpersonal behaviors, and gait deviations, as outlined below. All behaviors were coded as ‘yes’ if they occurred at least once during the descent. In addition, coders identified handrail use and tread gaze behaviors at the top four or bottom four steps, called the transition steps. Behavior during the transition steps was coded because fall risk is higher during transition to/from a staircase [34] and visual attention to the transition steps is apparent due to gaze shifts or overt visual attention [35]. Since the short staircase only contained two steps, both were transition steps, and this distinction was not relevant.
Hand behavior.
Hand behavior included four variables: handrail use, hands in pocket, carrying items, and electronic device use. Handrail use was identified if the pedestrian touched the handrail at least once while walking down the stairs (coded as yes/no). Handrail use on transition steps was identified separately if the pedestrian touched the handrail in the transition steps on the long staircase (yes/no). Hands in pocket was identified when at least one hand was in a pocket (yes/no). Carrying items was identified if carrying an item in at least one hand (yes/no). We coded when the person was using an electronic device such as a smartphone (yes/no).
Gaze behavior.
Gaze behavior was coded by reviewing head angle [21], as head angle provides a valid surrogate assessment of actual gaze direction [36]. Coders observed each pedestrian’s head angle to determine if their gaze would land on the stair tread. Note that the video software provided zoom features to facilitate viewing. Coders noted that pedestrians demonstrated multiple behaviors, including gaze on the treads, gaze on other pedestrians, gaze forward, etc., but coding was limited to pedestrians viewing the treads (except for the five pedestrians who lost balance, as described below). Tread gaze was coded if the gaze was located on the stair treads at least once during the descent (yes/no), and at least once on the transition steps (yes/no).
Interpersonal behavior.
Two variables for interpersonal behavior included: Pedestrians were coded as walking beside another pedestrian (i.e., walking abreast [33]) (yes/no) and if they were having an in-person conversation (yes/no). In-person conversations were coded as ‘yes’ based on both (1) proximity of pedestrians walking abreast and (2) mouth movement of at least one of the dyad, indicating speaking,
Gait deviations.
Gait deviations were defined as a change in the expected gait descent behavior. These deviations included skipping steps (yes/no) (pedestrian intentionally skipped alternate steps), stopping (yes/no), and loss of balance (yes/no). Anomalies (yes/no) were a change in the expected behavior that resulted from another pedestrian, such as changing direction to avoid another pedestrian that had slowed down.
Coding reliability
As noted above, a primary coder quantified stair-descent behavior for all captured pedestrians and a secondary coder coded approximately 25% of the same pedestrians. To quantify reliability between the primary and secondary coders, Cohen’s kappa statistics were calculated and interpreted using Landis and Koch [32]: almost perfect (κ range: 0.81–1.00), substantial (0.61–0.80), moderate (0.41–0.60), fair (0.21–0.40), slight (0.00–0.20) and poor agreement (<0.00). We excluded any variables that had less than moderate agreement. On the short staircase, almost perfect agreement was observed for all variables except for skipping steps (κ = 0.50, moderate) and gaze behavior (κ = 0.03, slight); gaze behavior on the short staircase was excluded due to slight agreement [Table 1]. On the long staircase, almost perfect and substantial agreement were observed in all variables except for skipping steps, which had moderate agreement.
Co-occurrence of risky behaviors
To quantify the number of co-occurring risky behaviors, we computed the total number of risky behaviors for each pedestrian. Eight behaviors were considered risky behavior for this analysis: no handrail use, no tread gaze, footwear (sandals, sliders, or high heels), having an in-person or smartphone conversation, using an electronic device, having hands in pocket(s), using hands, and skipping steps. The behaviors on the transition steps were excluded here (handrail use and tread gaze), because they are a subset of the behaviors on the staircase. Thus, each pedestrian had a value ranging from 0 to 8 for co-occurrence of risky behaviors.
Details regarding pedestrians who lost balance
The details regarding the circumstances and behavior of the pedestrians who lost balance were summarized in table format. These details included the eight coded risky behaviors (as in the preceding section) as well as: step number where balance was lost, recovery of balance, number of steps to recover balance, grabbed handrail, handrail within reaching distance, hand(s) available to grab handrail, gaze on staircase at time of balance loss, and gaze location at time of balance loss. To judge proximity of the handrail, anything ≥1.5 arm’s length was considered unreachable. The handrail proximity and the number of steps to recover balance was coded by two coders for the pedestrians who lost balance; they had 100% agreement.
Statistics
Descriptive statistics were reported as (1) number of pedestrians demonstrating the behavior and (2) percent of pedestrians demonstrating the behavior. Behaviors were also reported for men and women separately, also as number and percent. Statistical analysis was conducted using SAS 9.3 (Cary, NC, USA). For each coded behavior, odds ratio (OR) was calculated to determine differences between men and women during stair descent (men/women). For number of co-occurring risky behaviors, a two-sample t-test was used to analyze differences across men and women. With large sample sizes, parametric tests become robust to deviations in most of their key assumptions [37]. Significance level was p ≤ .05.
Results
Results are presented first for all pedestrians, followed by results for men versus women.
Footwear
On the short staircase, 92% of pedestrians (n = 931) wore flats, 5% (n = 52) wore sandals, 2% (n = 23) wore sliders, and 1% (n = 7) wore high heels [Table 2]. On the long staircase, 94% (n = 1,306) wore flats, 4% (n = 56) wore sandals, 1% (n = 15) wore sliders, and 1% (n = 10) wore high heels [Table 2]. On both staircases, a higher percentage of men wore flat shoes (short staircase: 95% and 89%, men and women, respectively; OR (95% CI): 2.22 (1.36–3.6l); long staircase: 97% and 87%; OR (95% CI): 5.21 (3.24–8.38)), and a lower percentage of men wore sandals (short staircase: 2% and 8%; OR (95% CI): 0.21 (0.10–0.45); long staircase: 2% and 10%; OR (95% CI): 0.16 (0.09–0.29)) [Table 2].
Odds ratio (OR) and 95% confidence interval (CI) for men versus women.
Hand behavior: Handrail use
None of the pedestrians used the handrail of the short staircase. Conversely, while descending the long staircase, 35% (n = 488) of pedestrians used the handrail at least once; men used the handrail more than women (37% and 31%, men and women, respectively; OR (95% CI): 1.33 (1.04–1.70); p = .03). In the transition steps of the long staircase, 33% (n = 463) of pedestrians used the handrail; men used the handrail more than women (35% and 29%, men and women, respectively; OR (95% CI): 1.36 (1.05–1.75); p = .02).
Hand behavior: Hands in pocket and hand use
Men put their hands in their pocket more than women on both the short staircase (15% and 10%, men and women, respectively; OR (95% CI): 1.69 (1.16–2.48); p < .01) and the long staircase (21% and 9%, men and women, respectively; OR (95% CI): 2.71 (1.86–3.95); p < .01). Women used their hands to hold items (i.e., coffee cup or clothing) more than men on both the short staircase (63% and 78%, men and women, respectively; OR (95% CI): 0.48 (0.37–0.64); p < .01) and the long staircase (38% and 60%, men and women, respectively; OR (95% CI): 0.41 (0.32–0.52); p < .01). One couple on each staircase held hands during descent. On the short staircase, 16% of pedestrians (17% and 15%, men and women, respectively; OR (95% CI): 1.20 (0.86–1.67); p = .30) used electronic devices. On the long staircase, 12% of pedestrians used electronic devices while descending the stairs (12% and 12%, men and women, respectively; OR (95% CI): 0.96 (0.67–1.37); p = .81) [Table 3].
All variables were coded yes/no, and percentage indicates numbers of participants with the behavior. Odds ratio (OR) and 95% confidence interval (CI) for men versus women.
Gaze behavior
On the long staircase, 91% of pedestrians gazed in the direction of the stair tread at least once while descending; male gaze behavior was not different from female gaze behavior (90% and 93%, men and women, respectively; p = .07). During the transition steps, 91% of pedestrians gazed at the tread at the beginning and/or at the end of staircase; more women gazed at the transition steps than men (90% and 93%, men and women, respectively; OR (95% CI): 0.64 (0.41–1.03); p = .05) [Table 3].
Interpersonal behavior
In-person conversations were significantly more frequent in women compared to men on both the short staircase (14% and 19%, men and women, respectively; OR (95% CI): 0.68 (0.48–0.95); p = .02) and the long staircase (13% and 18%; OR (95% CI): 0.72 (0.53–1.00); p = .05) [Table 3]. Since in-person conversations can only occur when walking with a colleague, and the number of women walking with others was significantly higher than men pedestrians in both staircases (p ≤ .05), the data was filtered to include only pedestrians walking with others (16% of participants, n = 380 for both staircases). In the filtered analysis, differences between men and women having a personal conversation were no longer significant for either staircase (p > .16).
Gait deviations
Observations of gait deviations from each staircase were too low to calculate odd ratios for each staircase separately. Therefore, odd ratios were calculated on the data combined across the two staircases [Table 4]. Anomalies and stopping were not significantly different between men and women (anomalies: 0.4% and 0.4% for men and women, respectively; p = 0.94; stopping: 1.0% and 0.6% for men and women, respectively; p = .42). However, men were significantly more likely to skip steps than women (3.7% and 0.8%, men and women, respectively; OR (95% CI): 5.03 (2.28–11.10); p < .01).
All variables were coded yes/no, and percentage indicates numbers of participants with the behavior. Since low numbers of incidents were observed at each individual staircase (less than 5), odds ratio (OR) and 95% confidence interval (CI) for men versus women were calculated for both staircases.
Loss of balance was infrequent for both men and women (0.3% and 0.1% for men and women, respectively) and therefore, odd ratios were not calculated. Five pedestrians (four men, one woman) lost their balance, one woman on the short staircase and four men on the long staircase; all five recovered their balance and no falls were observed. None of the five pedestrians were using the handrail or looking at the stairs at the time of balance loss. Four of the five were completing another task at the time: one was texting with both hands, two were looking at the phone (perhaps texting with one hand), and one was putting an item in a bag. After losing balance, only one pedestrian grabbed the handrail (and then continued texting). All five looked at the stairs after losing balance.
Co-occurring risky behaviors
The number of co-occurring risky behaviors in the pedestrians ranged from 0 to 6 (maximum possible was 8) (Fig 2A). The majority of pedestrians (69.2%) had two or more risky behaviors, and few pedestrians had zero risky behaviors (9.6%). The majority of men (62.2%) had two or more risky behaviors, and 13% had zero risky behaviors. The vast majority of women (80.3%) had two or more risky behaviors, and 4.3% had zero risky behaviors (Fig 2B). Men had fewer co-occurring risky behaviors than women (1.9 vs 2.3, men vs women, respectively; t(2162.2) = 8.33, p<0.001).
Histogram of the number of risk behaviors for all pedestrians combined (A) and for men and women separately (B).
Pedestrians who lost balance
Five pedestrians lost their balance, and all five recovered their balance (Table 5). Four of the five pedestrians were men, all the men lost balance on the long staircase, the woman lost balance on the short staircase. Four of the pedestrians lost balance at the top step, and one lost balance midway on the long staircase. Mean number of co-occurring risky behaviors in the pedestrians was 2.6, and mean number of steps to recover balance was 1.4 steps. Only one pedestrian grabbed the handrail during balance recovery. This pedestrian was able to move quickly enough to stop texting and grab the handrail. At the time of balance loss, all five pedestrians were looking at something other than the stairs.
Discussion
Previous research has demonstrated that average injury rate of stair-related falls has a trimodal distribution with a peak for young adults in their 20’s, with women in their 20’s sustaining about 80% more injuries than men of the same age [16]. The purpose of this study was to quantify young adult behaviors during staircase descent, with the goal of identifying behaviors that increase fall risk on stairs, and to determine if young women are more likely to demonstrate these risky behaviors. Young adults engaged in behaviors that may increase fall risk, including not using the handrail, carrying items, using electronic devices, holding a conversation, and wearing footwear that increases fall risk. Women demonstrated several behaviors that increase their fall risk relative to men: less likely to use the handrail, more likely to be holding something in their hands, more likely to be engaged in conversation, more likely to wear sandals and heels, and they demonstrated a higher number of co-occurring risky behaviors. However, women also engaged in behaviors that may be protective of fall risk: less likely to skip steps and more likely to look at the stair tread during transition steps. Overall, the observed sex-related differences are strengthened by the inclusion of both short and long staircases since similar sex-related effects were observed on both staircases.
If a person loses their balance, a handrail can aid in balance recovery when standing on the ground [38] and when standing on stairs [39]. However, in the present study, no one used the handrail on the short staircase, consistent with observations on a three-step staircase in laboratory research (participants included older adults and young adults with a simulated vision disability) [40, 41]. Not using the handrail on short staircases is especially relevant because there is a high occurrence of falls on stairs with less than five steps [9, 30]. On the long staircase, there is an increase in injury severity if a fall occurs [9, 30], but the majority of young adults (65%) did not use the handrail. This finding is consistent with previous observational research (range: 57–76%) [18–21], but less than the 94% observed in one study [22]. The higher rate in the latter study is most likely due to data collection during the height of the COVID-19 pandemic [22], while the current dataset and the other cited studies were collected before the pandemic. Cultural norms and physical proximity were altered during the pandemic, which likely reduced handrail use in [22]. Overall, the majority of young adults did not use handrails proactively (i.e., holding the handrail while descending, before a loss of balance occurs). However, laboratory research demonstrates that a reactive response occurs when balance is lost on the stairs: the hand rapidly moves toward an external support and sufficient force is generated to prevent a fall on stairs [39]. It is important to note that in the Maki, Perry [39] study, participants expected their balance to be disturbed, but the timing and magnitude of the perturbation were unpredictable. When a fall on stairs in the lab was unexpected due to deception, not all young adult participants tried to grasp the handrail, and grasp errors were observed in participants who tried [42]. In the current study, we also observed that four of five young adults who lost their balance did not grasp the handrail. Thus, while young adults have the ability to reactively grasp and use the handrail to recover their balance when they are expecting a loss of balance, they are less successful when the loss to balance is unexpected, such as would occur outside the lab setting. Therefore, not using the handrail in a proactive manner is risky–and women were less likely to use the handrail than men. Lower proactive handrail use in women may explain, at least in part, the high stair-related injury rate in young adult women [16], especially since women have slower and more variable reaction times [43] and are slower to grasp a handrail in a reactive task when walking in a lab setting [44].
Potential reasons for not using the handrail include: (1) using the hands for another purpose, (2) having hands in pockets, (3) walking with another person (when the other person is beside the handrail), (4) being too far from the handrail, and (5) concerns about cleanliness of the handrails. Hand use prevents proactive use of the handrail as observed, for example, in four of the five pedestrians who lost balance (pedestrians A, B, D, E; Table 5). Three of these four were using an electronic device, consistent with previous stair observation research that handrail use was reduced when using a mobile device [22]. Further, holding an item also inhibits the ability to reactively grasp the handrail [38]. The tendency to keep hold of an item despite a loss of balance has even been observed in infants, demonstrating the pervasiveness of maintaining hold on an item [45]. Four of the five pedestrians who lost balance had items in their hands, and three of them did not grab the handrail. The fourth pedestrian was texting with both hands, he stopped texting and grabbed the handrail with one hand while maintaining hold of the phone in his other hand (pedestrian D). In both staircases, the majority of women used their hands to hold items (78.1% and 59.9% of women on the short and long staircases, respectively). Conversely, while men were more likely than women to have their hands in their pockets, this behavior was observed in the minority of participants (15.3% and 21.1% of men on the short and long staircases, respectively). When walking with someone, one pedestrian would be between the other pedestrian and the handrail, preventing grasping the handrail. Women were more likely to be walking with someone, and thus would be more likely to be prevented from grasping the handrail; however, walking with someone was observed in the minority of participants (22.9% and 15.5% of women on the short and long staircases, respectively). Finally, we consider that people may avoid using handrails because they are frequently touched by others; high-touch surfaces may be contaminated by microorganisms, leading to transmission of infections [46]. Since women appear to be more concerned about personal hygiene than men (women wash their hands more frequently, are more likely to use soap, and wash their hands for a longer duration [47, 48]), the higher percentage of women not using the handrail may also be associated with gender-related differences in personal hygiene.
Falls on stairs can occur when a person is distracted [9]; the young adults here were distracted by using electronic devices (13.7% of all pedestrians) and by having conversations (15.3% of all pedestrians). Stair descent requires visual monitoring of the environment and control of trunk and limb movements, with appropriate foot trajectories that clear each stair edge and land safely on each stair tread [49–51]. Using an electronic device reduces attention, alters gaze behavior, reduces the visual field, and impairs gait on stairs [28, 29, 52, 53]. Similarly, when gaze is diverted by a cognitive task on stairs, gait is altered [22, 35, 54]. These impairments result in the increasing pedestrian injuries due to mobile phone use [26]. Previous research demonstrated that half of the falls on stairs in young adults were related to texting [5]. Thus, high injury rates of stair-related falls in young adults [16] may be related to the use of electronic devices. However, since men and women were not different in their use of electronic devices when descending stairs, this behavior does not explain the higher number of stair-related injuries in women unless women are more impaired/distracted by the use of electronic devices than men. The effect of sex during locomotor multitasks in young adults has not been extensively studied; two papers indicate that young men and women were not impaired differently during the following locomotor multitasks: cognitive task during timed-up-and-go task [55], and cognitive task when using a treadmill desk [56]. Thus, there is no evidence that young adult women are more impaired than men by locomotor multitasks, but future studies should continue to examine sex-related differences among young adults in a wider range of locomotor multitasks.
Young adult women were more likely to engage in conversations during stair descent, which may be related to the higher injury rate of stair-related falls for women. During conversations, the pedestrian is either listening or speaking; both tasks are continuous and cognitively demanding. Listening involves cognitive, affective, and behavioral processes; the listener attends to, understands, and interprets verbal and non-verbal cues [57]. Speech requires memory, selection of appropriate words and grammar, and the coordination of breathing and speech patterns [58, 59]. During challenging multitasks, young healthy adults flexibly allocate resources between the tasks [59, 60], and tasks with higher values are prioritized [61, 62]. Young adults may prioritize the conversation task, as people with impaired speech are perceived as incompetent [63, 64]. This possibility is supported by lab-based research that observed gait impairment during stair negotiation when young adults have a phone conversation [27]. Similarly, in fall survey research in young adults, the most common secondary task at the time of a fall was talking to a friend [4, 5], and women were more likely than men to report talking to a friend at the time of a fall [5]. A recent observational study on stair negotiation also found that women were more likely to be distracted (distraction included one or more of the following: Mobile device (looking at, talking on, or holding), using earbuds or headphones, talking with a peer [22].
While women were more likely to engage in conversation than men, it may be related to the observation that women were more likely to walk with a colleague. Being more likely to walk with a colleague is consistent with the previous research that women often interact at a closer intimate distance with colleagues than men [65, 66], and female dyads walk abreast more often than male dyads [33]. Since an in-person conversation requires a colleague, we completed a follow-up analysis where people who were not walking with a colleague were excluded. After this exclusion, there was no difference in talking with a colleague between men and women. Thus, while women were more likely to have an in-person conversation, this was driven by the greater likelihood that the women walked with a colleague.
The vast majority of pedestrians (91%) looked at the steps of the long staircase at least once, likely to gather visual information regarding stair features (i.e., position of stair edge, dimensions of the risers and treads) and drive motor behavior [23]. In the long staircase, women were more likely to gaze at the steps, which may be protective for falls. Women were also more likely to look at the tread in a recent observational study on stairs in a public setting [21]. For loss of balance, we note that none of the five pedestrians who lost balance were looking at the stairs when their balance was lost, and all of them looked at the stairs during the recovery. The pedestrians were looking at the following items rather than the stairs: smartphone (two pedestrians), the surrounding environment (two pedestrians), and looking at clothing item being put in bag (one pedestrian). Four of the five pedestrians were at the top/transition step when they lost balance, consistent with previous research [34]. On the long staircase, lighting was brightest at the transition step so loss of balance in these cases does not appear to be related to lighting levels. The apparent role of gaze diversion in the loss of balance is consistent with two observational studies that demonstrated that not looking down at the step appeared to be associated with falls [24] and more stair incidents were observed with infrequent tread gaze [21]. Therefore, gaze behavior and its association with falls on stairs should be examined further.
Seven percent of young adults wore shoes that are known to increase fall risk and injuries in midlife and older adults: sandals, sliders, and high heels. In older adults, footwear is a risk factor for falls [67, 68]. In adults aged 45 years and over, sandals and sliders increased the risk of fall-related foot fractures and medium- to high-heel shoes increased the risk of fractures at multiple sites [69]. In young adults, lab-based research indicates impaired balance when young adults wear sliders or heels. Sliders resulted in longer heel slip distance and velocity during slips relative to industry standard slip resistant shoes [70]. High heels compromised balance and induced changes to the neuromechanics of gait that were widespread and mostly disadvantageous [71, 72]. Regarding sex differences, young women may sustain more injuries on stairs due to their footwear: Only women wore high heels, and women were more than four times as likely to wear sandals. In addition, many students wear comfortable shoes due to the size of the campus; in settings such as a professional workplace or social gatherings, women may be even more likely to wear heels.
Skipping steps was observed more frequently in men; skipping steps is a deliberate, intentional act and implies confidence during stair descent. However, stumbling and/or falling may result if they misjudge the step position. Skipping steps was mostly observed at the bottom of the staircase, consistent with previous reports [9, 73, 74]. Therefore, although the risk of falling may be higher with this behavior, the impact forces will be reduced when falling from the bottom versus top of the stairs [9, 30]. We note that the fall risk associated with skipping steps may be compounded by the lower number of men that gazed at the tread in the transition steps; future studies should identify if those skipping steps are more or less likely to gaze at the transition steps.
The majority of young adult pedestrians demonstrated co-occurring risky behaviors during stair descent, with young women demonstrating a higher mean number of risky behaviors than men (1.9 vs 2.3, men vs women, respectively; Fig 2). Therefore, although women demonstrated both risky behavior and protective behavior, the overall result was a higher number of risky behaviors for women than men. As noted in earlier text, the higher number of co-occurring behaviors in women appear to result from behaviors associated with gender. These gender-specific behaviors include avoiding high-touch surfaces (i.e., handrails), footwear choices, and being more likely to walk with a colleague and thus more likely to be engaged in in-person conversation [33, 47, 48]. Stair safety strategies should target the risky behaviors most commonly observed: not using the handrail and hands being unavailable to grasp the handrail if needed. While people should be encouraged to use the handrail proactively and not to carry items in their hands, the prevalence of electronic devices and to-go coffees will likely impair compliance. Therefore, we propose task-specific training where participants practice reactive handrail grasping following a perturbation while safely harnessed (i.e., the stair-perturbation protocol from Maki, Perry [39]). This training is similar to the promising approach to fall prevention where slips and/or trips are applied during walking to effectively train neuromuscular balance responses [75, 76]. Slip and trip training is effective after a single training session, so it is possible that short training sessions will promote successful handrail grasping. Handrail grasping training should include the participants holding an item, since grasping the handrail is inhibited when holding an item [38]. This training may improve movement time and success in grasping the handrail. Further, environmental modifications would be based on identifying handrail characteristics that improve balance recovery such as handrail height [77] and handrail size/shape [78, 79]. Similarly, increasing handrail visibility may be effective, since hand muscle activity increased when participants simply viewed a safety handle [80]. Increasing the visibility of the stairs or making the stairs appear larger through illusions improves stair gait behavior [81]; these modifications may also help mitigate the effects of distracted attention due to electronic devices and in-person conversations.
There are several limitations that warrant consideration. First, the variables age and sex were coded based on participant appearance. While coders demonstrated good agreement (Table 1), this may result from both coders being incorrect since both coders used the same appearance to make judgements. However, judgement based on appearance is the only method available to identify age and sex in observational/field research and has been used previously [18, 20–22, 33]. Second, pedestrians walking behind others were not coded due to visual obstruction. People who walk close behind others may demonstrate risky behaviors that were not captured in this study. Third, staircase observations were limited to a university campus setting. Typical university behaviors (e.g., walking to classes at frequent intervals, higher density of people on stairs at certain times of day) may not be similar in other settings/populations, and the results may not translate to young adults in settings outside of campus. Fourth, most stair-related injuries occur at home [16], but it was not possible to observe people in their homes. Fifth, no women in the study were visibly pregnant, and pregnant women are at a high risk of falls [82]. Sixth, the risk factors were summed where each risk factor contributed equally, which assumes that each factor contributed equally to fall risk. However, it is likely that some factors connote greater risk than others. but we did not have a basis for determining weighting of each factor, so we opted for the simple summation. Finally, the illumination on the stairs was affected by the weather and time of day. The variability in factors such as lighting is a common issue for observational studies, and will increase variability. We did not collect lighting level at the same time as the pedestrians were videotaped, but this should be included in future observational studies to identify the effect of lighting.
In conclusion, young adults demonstrated multiple behaviors that are likely to increase fall risk during stair descent. These behaviors include: not using the handrail, carrying items, using electronic devices, holding a conversation, and wearing footwear that increases fall risk. Risky behaviors observed more frequently in women include: less likely to use the handrail, more likely to be holding something in their hands, more likely to be engaged in conversation, more likely to wear sandals and heels, and demonstrating a higher number of co-occurring risky behaviors. Risky behaviors observed more frequently in men include: more likely to skip steps and less likely to look at the stair tread during transition steps. These behaviors may be related to the high injury rate of stair-related falls in young adults, and the higher injury rate observed in young women versus men [16]. The observations on the pedestrians who lost their balance indicate that the top step appears to be particularly risky, as well as gaze diverted away from the stairs.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Marissa Munoz-Ruiz and Lucas J. Rooney for participating in helpful discussions when the project was conceived.
References
- 1. Rubenstein LZ. Falls in older people: epidemiology, risk factors and strategies for prevention. Age and ageing. 2006;35(suppl_2):ii37–ii41. pmid:16926202
- 2. Nemire K, Johnson DA, Vidal K. The science behind codes and standards for safe walkways: changes in level, stairways, stair handrails and slip resistance. Applied ergonomics. 2016;52:309–16. pmid:26360223
- 3. Templer J, Archea J, Chen HH. Study of factors associated with risk of work-related stairway falls. Journal of Safety Research. 1985;16(4):183–96.
- 4. Heijnen MJH, Rietdyk S. Falls in young adults: Perceived causes and environmental factors assessed with a daily online survey. Human movement science. 2016;46:86–95. pmid:26741254
- 5. Cho H, Heijnen MJ, Craig BA, Rietdyk S. Falls in young adults: The effect of sex, physical activity, and prescription medications. PloS one. 2021;16(4):e0250360. pmid:33886646
- 6. Bosse I, Oberländer KD, Savelberg HH, Meijer K, Brüggemann G-P, Karamanidis K. Dynamic stability control in younger and older adults during stair descent. Human movement science. 2012;31(6):1560–70. pmid:22853941
- 7. Cohen HH, Templer J, Archea J. An analysis of occupational stair accident patterns. Journal of safety research. 1985;16(4):171–81.
- 8. Roys MS. Serious stair injuries can be prevented by improved stair design. Applied ergonomics. 2001;32(2):135–9. pmid:11277505
- 9.
Templer J. The staircase: studies of hazards, falls, and safer design: MIT press; 1995.
- 10. Startzell JK, Owens DA, Mulfinger LM, Cavanagh PR. Stair negotiation in older people: a review. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2000;48(5):567–80. pmid:10811553
- 11. Verghese J, Wang C, Xue X, Holtzer R. Self-reported difficulty in climbing up or down stairs in nondisabled elderly. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation. 2008;89(1):100–4. pmid:18164338
- 12. Svanström L. Falls on stairs: an epidemiological accident study. Scandinavian journal of social medicine. 1974;2(3):113–20. pmid:4432054
- 13. Masud T, Morris RO. Epidemiology of falls. Age and ageing. 2001;30(suppl_4):3–7. pmid:11769786
- 14. Jacobs JV. A review of stairway falls and stair negotiation: Lessons learned and future needs to reduce injury. Gait & posture. 2016;49:159–67. pmid:27427833
- 15. Timsina LR, Willetts JL, Brennan MJ, Marucci-Wellman H, Lombardi DA, Courtney TK, et al. Circumstances of fall-related injuries by age and gender among community-dwelling adults in the United States. PloS one. 2017;12(5):e0176561. pmid:28472065
- 16. Blazewick DH, Chounthirath T, Hodges NL, Collins CL, Smith GA. Stair-related injuries treated in United States emergency departments. The American journal of emergency medicine. 2018;36(4):608–14. pmid:28947224
- 17. Pavol MJ, Owings TM, Foley KT, Grabiner MD. Influence of lower extremity strength of healthy older adults on the outcome of an induced trip. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2002;50(2):256–62. pmid:12028206
- 18.
Templer JA. Stair shape and human movement: Columbia University; 1975.
- 19. Pauls J. Are functional handrails within our grasp? Building Standards. 1991;60(1):6–12.
- 20.
Cohen J, Cohen HH, editors. Hold on! An observational study of staircase handrail use. Proceedings of the human factors and ergonomics society annual meeting; 2001: SAGE Publications Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA.
- 21. Kim K, Steinfeld E. The effects of glass stairways on stair users: an observational study of stairway safety. Safety science. 2019;113:30–6.
- 22. Harper SA, Corbridge S, Long C, Barrett TS, Braeger A, Zollinger BJ, et al. Safe stairway negotiation: role of distractions and handrail use. Journal of safety research. 2022;82:314–22. pmid:36031259
- 23. Hollands M, Hollands K, Rietdyk S. Visual control of adaptive locomotion and changes due to natural ageing. Locomotion and Posture in Older Adults. 2017:55–72.
- 24. Archea J, Collins BL, Stahl FI. Guidelines for stair safety: The Bureau; 1979.
- 25. Lim J, Amado A, Sheehan L, Van Emmerik RE. Dual task interference during walking: The effects of texting on situational awareness and gait stability. Gait & posture. 2015;42(4):466–71. pmid:26298159
- 26. Nasar JL, Troyer D. Pedestrian injuries due to mobile phone use in public places. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 2013;57:91–5. pmid:23644536
- 27. Di Giulio I, McFadyen BJ, Blanchet S, Reeves ND, Baltzopoulos V, Maganaris CN. Mobile phone use impairs stair gait: A pilot study on young adults. Applied Ergonomics. 2020;84. WOS:000512220600002. pmid:31987506
- 28. Miyasike-daSilva V, McIlroy WE. Does it really matter where you look when walking on stairs? Insights from a dual-task study. 2012.
- 29. Ioannidou F, Hermens F, Hodgson TL. Mind your step: the effects of mobile phone use on gaze behavior in stair climbing. Journal of technology in behavioral science. 2017;2(3):109–20. pmid:29387779
- 30.
Templer JA, Mullet GM, Archea J, Margulis S. An analysis of the behavior of stair users: US Government Printing Office Washington, DC; 1978.
- 31.
Kim K. Contemporary Practices in Stairway Design: Behavior of Stair Users in Public Buildings: State University of New York at Buffalo; 2013.
- 32. Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. biometrics. 1977:159–74. pmid:843571
- 33. Costa M. Interpersonal distances in group walking. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 2010;34(1):15–26.
- 34. Jackson PL, Cohen HH. An in-depth investigation of 40 stairway accidents and the stair safety literature. Journal of Safety Research. 1995;26(3):151–9.
- 35. Miyasike-daSilva V, McIlroy WE. Gaze shifts during dual-tasking stair descent. Experimental brain research. 2016;234(11):3233–43. pmid:27401474
- 36.
Barañano AR, Barrett B, Buckley J, editors. Assessing gaze angles during walking without an eye tracker: validation and limitations. ISPGR; 2022; MONTREAL, CANADA.
- 37. Rietdyk S, Ambike S, Amireault S, Haddad JM, Lin G, Newton D, et al. Co-occurrences of fall-related factors in adults aged 60 to 85 years in the United States National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. PloS one. 2022;17(11):e0277406. pmid:36346815
- 38. Bateni H, Zecevic A, McIlroy WE, Maki BE. Resolving conflicts in task demands during balance recovery: does holding an object inhibit compensatory grasping? Experimental brain research. 2004;157(1):49–58. pmid:14758453
- 39. Maki B, Perry SD, Mcllroy WE. Efficacy of handrails in preventing stairway falls: a new experimental approach. Safety Science. 1998;28(3):189–206.
- 40. Foster RJ, Hotchkiss J, Buckley JG, Elliott DB. Safety on stairs: Influence of a tread edge highlighter and its position. Experimental gerontology. 2014;55:152–8. pmid:24768822
- 41. Foster RJ, Whitaker D, Scally AJ, Buckley JG, Elliott DB. What you see is what you step: the horizontal–vertical illusion increases toe clearance in older adults during stair ascent. Investigative ophthalmology & visual science. 2015;56(5):2950–7. pmid:26024081
- 42. Gosine P, Komisar V, Novak AC. Characterizing the demands of backward balance loss and fall recovery during stair descent to prevent injury. Applied ergonomics. 2019;81:102900. pmid:31422249
- 43. Der G, Deary IJ. Age and sex differences in reaction time in adulthood: results from the United Kingdom Health and Lifestyle Survey. Psychology and aging. 2006;21(1):62. pmid:16594792
- 44. McIntosh EI, Vallis LA. Performance during reactive handrail grasping during forward walking by young and older adults with and without prior knowledge of the direction of movement is correlated with grip strength. Experimental Gerontology. 2021;151:111386. pmid:33957264
- 45. Arnold AJ, Claxton LJ. To drop or not to drop: newly standing infants maintain hold of objects when experiencing a loss of balance. Journal of Motor Learning and Development. 2017;5(2):181–92.
- 46. Cobrado L, Silva-Dias A, Azevedo M, Rodrigues A. High-touch surfaces: microbial neighbours at hand. European Journal of Clinical Microbiology & Infectious Diseases. 2017;36(11):2053–62. pmid:28647859
- 47. Borchgrevink CP, Cha J, Kim S. Hand washing practices in a college town environment. Journal of environmental health. 2013;75(8):18–25. pmid:23621052
- 48. Anderson JL, Warren CA, Perez E, Louis RI, Phillips S, Wheeler J, et al. Gender and ethnic differences in hand hygiene practices among college students. American journal of infection control. 2008;36(5):361–8. pmid:18538703
- 49. Simoneau GG, Cavanagh PR, Ulbrecht JS, Leibowitz HW, Tyrrell RA. The influence of visual factors on fall-related kinematic variables during stair descent by older women. Journal of gerontology. 1991;46(6):M188–M95. pmid:1940077
- 50. Zietz D, Johannsen L, Hollands M. Stepping characteristics and Centre of Mass control during stair descent: Effects of age, fall risk and visual factors. Gait & posture. 2011;(No.2):279–84. pmid:21680187
- 51. Shin SS, An DH, Yoo WG. Effects of differences in visual acuity on gait time and trunk acceleration when older women negotiate stairs. European Geriatric Medicine. 2016;7(6):538–42. WOS:000393044000014.
- 52. Schabrun SM, van den Hoorn W, Moorcroft A, Greenland C, Hodges PW. Texting and walking: strategies for postural control and implications for safety. PloS one. 2014;9(1):e84312. pmid:24465402
- 53. Hashish R, Toney-Bolger ME, Sharpe SS, Lester BD, Mulliken A. Texting during stair negotiation and implications for fall risk. Gait & posture. 2017;58:409–14. pmid:28898800
- 54. Telonio A, Blanchet S, Maganaris CN, Baltzopoulos V, Villeneuve S, McFadyen BJ. The division of visual attention affects the transition point from level walking to stair descent in healthy, active older adults. Experimental gerontology. 2014;50:26–33. pmid:24291246
- 55. Almajid R, Keshner E. Role of gender in dual-tasking timed up and go tests: a cross-sectional study. Journal of motor behavior. 2019;51(6):681–9. pmid:30676272
- 56. Marshall MR, Duckworth SC, Currie MR, Schmid D, Rogers RR. Role of age and sex on dual tasking using a treadmill desk while performing cognitive tests. Gait & posture. 2021;90:148–53. pmid:34481265
- 57. Halone KK, Cunconan TM, Coakley CG, Wolvin AD. Toward the establishment of general dimensions underlying the listening process. International Journal of Listening. 1998;12(1):12–28.
- 58. Mitchell HL, Hoit JD, Watson PJ. Cognitive-linguistic demands and speech breathing. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1996;39(1):93–104. pmid:8820701
- 59. Raffegeau TE, Haddad JM, Huber JE, Rietdyk S. Walking while talking: young adults flexibly allocate resources between speech and gait. Gait & posture. 2018;64:59–62. pmid:29859413
- 60. Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. Do we always prioritize balance when walking? Towards an integrated model of task prioritization. Movement disorders: official journal of the Movement Disorder Society. 2012;27(6):765–70. pmid:22419512.
- 61. Rapp MA, Krampe RT, Baltes PB. Adaptive task prioritization in aging: selective resource allocation to postural control is preserved in Alzheimer disease. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2006;14(1):52–61. pmid:16407582
- 62. Doumas M, Smolders C, Krampe RT. Task prioritization in aging: effects of sensory information on concurrent posture and memory performance. Experimental Brain Research. 2008;187(2):275–81. pmid:18273609
- 63. Blood GW, Mahan BW, Hyman M. Judging personality and appearance from voice disorders. Journal of communication disorders. 1979;12(1):63–7. pmid:422748
- 64. Ryan EB, Capadano HL III. Age perceptions and evaluative reactions toward adult speakers. Journal of Gerontology. 1978;33(1):98–102. pmid:618973
- 65.
Aiello J, Stokols D, Altman I. Handbook of environmental psychology. Human Spatial Behavior; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA. 1987:389–504.
- 66.
Hall JA. Nonverbal gender differences: Accuracy of communication and expressive style. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press; 1984.
- 67. Connell BR, Wolf SL, Group AF. Environmental and behavioral circumstances associated with falls at home among healthy elderly individuals. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation. 1997;78(2):179–86.
- 68. Menant JC, Steele JR, Menz HB, Munro BJ, Lord SR. Optimizing footwear for older people at risk of falls. 2008.
- 69. Keegan TH, Kelsey JL, King AC, Quesenberry CP Jr, Sidney S. Characteristics of fallers who fracture at the foot, distal forearm, proximal humerus, pelvis, and shaft of the tibia/fibula compared with fallers who do not fracture. American journal of epidemiology. 2004;159(2):192–203. pmid:14718222
- 70. Chander H, Garner JC, Wade C. Heel contact dynamics in alternative footwear during slip events. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics. 2015;48:158–66.
- 71. Wiedemeijer M, Otten E. Effects of high heeled shoes on gait. A review. Gait & posture. 2018;61:423–30. pmid:29475153
- 72. Cronin NJ. The effects of high heeled shoes on female gait: a review. Journal of electromyography and kinesiology. 2014;24(2):258–63. pmid:24508305
- 73. Sheldon JH. On the natural history of falls in old age. British Medical Journal. 1960;2(5214):1685. pmid:20789006
- 74. Wild D, Nayak U, Isaacs B. Description, classification and prevention of falls in old people at home. Rheumatology. 1981;20(3):153–9. pmid:7280490
- 75. Pai Y-C, Bhatt TS. Repeated-slip training: an emerging paradigm for prevention of slip-related falls among older adults. Physical therapy. 2007;87(11):1478–91. pmid:17712033
- 76. Allin LJ, Brolinson PG, Beach BM, Kim S, Nussbaum MA, Roberto KA, et al. Perturbation-based balance training targeting both slip-and trip-induced falls among older adults: a randomized controlled trial. BMC geriatrics. 2020;20(1):1–13. pmid:32532221
- 77. Komisar V, Novak AC. Effect of handrail height and age on trunk and shoulder kinematics following perturbation-evoked grasping reactions during gait. Human factors. 2021:00187208211013631. pmid:33945338
- 78. Gosine P, Komisar V, Novak AC. The effect of handrail cross-sectional design and age on applied handrail forces during reach-to-grasp balance reactions. Journal of biomechanics. 2021;129:110788. pmid:34666247
- 79. Gosine P, Komisar V, Novak AC. The effect of handrail cross-sectional design and age on the speed and quality of reach-to-grasp reactions to recover balance. Human factors. 2022:00187208221082439. pmid:35465756
- 80. Bolton DA, Cole DM, Butler B, Mansour M, Rydalch G, McDannald DW, et al. Motor preparation for compensatory reach-to-grasp responses when viewing a wall-mounted safety handle. cortex. 2019;117:135–46. pmid:30974321
- 81. Elliott DB, Foster RJ, Whitaker D, Scally AJ, Buckley JG. Analysis of gait kinematics to determine the effect of manipulating the appearance of stairs to improve safety: a linked series of laboratory-based, repeated measures studies. Public Health Research. 2015;3.
- 82. Dunning K, LeMasters G, Bhattacharya A. A major public health issue: the high incidence of falls during pregnancy. Maternal and child health journal. 2010;14(5):720–5. pmid:19672702