Figures
Abstract
The effects of postural training on postural stability and vestibulospinal reflexes (VSRs) were investigated in normal subjects. A period (23 minutes) of repeated episodes (n = 10, 50 seconds) of unipedal stance elicited a progressive reduction of the area covered by centre of pressure (CoP) displacement, of average CoP displacement along the X and Y axes and of CoP velocity observed in this challenging postural task. All these changes were correlated to each other with the only exception of those in X and Y CoP displacement. Moreover, they were larger in the subjects showing higher initial instability in unipedal stance, suggesting that they were triggered by the modulation of sensory afferents signalling body sway. No changes in bipedal stance occurred soon and 1 hour after this period of postural training, while a reduction of CoP displacement was apparent after 24 hours, possibly due to a beneficial effect of overnight sleep on postural learning. The same period of postural training also reduced the CoP displacement elicited by electrical vestibular stimulation (EVS) along the X axis up to 24 hours following the training end. No significant changes in postural parameters of bipedal stance and VSRs could be observed in control experiments where subjects were tested at identical time points without performing the postural training. Therefore, postural training led to a stricter control of CoP displacement, possibly acting through the cerebellum by enhancing feedforward mechanisms of postural stability and by depressing the VSR, the most important reflex mechanism involved in balance maintenance under challenging conditions.
Citation: Grasso C, Barresi M, Tramonti Fantozzi MP, Lazzerini F, Bruschini L, Berrettini S, et al. (2023) Effects of a short period of postural training on postural stability and vestibulospinal reflexes. PLoS ONE 18(6): e0287123. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0287123
Editor: Manabu Sakakibara, Tokai University, JAPAN
Received: November 15, 2022; Accepted: May 31, 2023; Published: June 12, 2023
Copyright: © 2023 Grasso et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: Data are available at https://osf.io/umg84/.
Funding: This study was supported by grants of the University of Pisa (Ateneo 2022 to LB and SB). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Similarly to voluntary movements, postural control may undergo learning processes, increasing the ability of maintaining balance [1]. Specific exercises challenging balance and sport activities requiring balance abilities [1–4] are most powerful in improving postural control, but also non-specific exercises can be effective [1].
So far, the effects of postural training have been mostly investigated over rather long time lapses (from 5 weeks to 3 months). Experiments have been conducted in people of different gender, age and ability, resorting to a wide range of experimental paradigms, rather heterogeneous in terms of duration of training periods, frequency/duration of individual training sessions, as well as total training time per week [5, 6].
Postural training increased the ability to maintain balance under challenging conditions, such as tandem [7] and unipedal stance [7–11], leading to a reduction of trunk acceleration [12] and of the area covered by the Centre of Pressure (CoP) [1, 13, 14].
There is evidence that postural improvement elicited by balance training is specific. For instance, expert gymnasts, who usually practise with their eyes open, show a superior balance than sedentary people only when vision is allowed under challenging postures [15]. Apparently, in this population, the acquired postural ability does not transfer from a posture to another [16]. Similarly, stability in normal, bipedal stance is not improved when a subject acquires the skill to maintain balance on a slackline [1]. Other studies, however, report that postural training performed by standing in unipedal stance or on unstable surfaces may reduce the area of CoP motion observed in the normal bipedal stance [13, 14].
Only few studies have so far addressed the short-term effects of balance training on postural stability: there is evidence that practising on an unstable board for 6–16 minutes increases the time a subject can stand without falling in this unstable condition, from about 1.5 to 4 seconds [17]. Moreover, the ability of balancing on unstable support in unipedal stance is increased following 10–30 minutes of training [18]. None of these studies, however, have addressed the progressive evolution of postural stability by analysing the characteristics of CoP motion during short-term training in a challenging posture such as unipedal stance.
We may expect that an improvement in postural performance is associated with modifications in the visual, vestibular, proprioceptive and somatosensory reflexes [19, 20] which contribute to postural stability. Somatosensory [21, 22] and vestibular [23] reflexes involve both short and long latency pathways and can be modulated by the cerebral cortex, the cerebellum and the basal ganglia [21].
There is evidence that long periods of postural training may decrease the amplitude of the soleus stretch [24] and of the H-reflex [25–28]. The depression of the H-reflex could be specific for the training posture/task [26, 28] or could otherwise generalise to normal bipedal stance on firm support [25]. In other long-term studies, however, postural training did not elicit H-reflex depression at all, either in the untrained or in the trained condition [29–31]. Finally, an increase in H-reflex amplitude has been reported in subjects tested following 6 weeks of combined postural and strength training [32], as well as following a 12-week period of Tai Chi Yang training [33, 34], implying wide stance width and slow speed of movement during practice, leading to an improvement of lower limbs muscle strength by imposing constant knee flexion.
Only few studies addressed the effects of short periods (10–20 minutes) of postural training, showing a decrease in H-reflex amplitude evaluated while sitting, standing in unipedal stance [17] or standing on an unstable surface [35].
As to the effect of postural training on the short and long latency electromyographic (EMG) responses evoked by the translation of the support surface, no modifications have been shown following four weeks of balance training [36, 37]. At variance, responses to rotation of the support surface in the sagittal plane, which further destabilise the subjects [38] are decreased following three days of training [39]. Finally, 13 days of balance training reduce the latency and increase the amplitude of the late component of the EMG responses to gait perturbation [40].
Long-term postural training has been shown to also modify the cortical control on postural reflexes, as indicated by post-training reduction of the cortical elicited facilitation of the H-reflex is with respect to control [30, 36]; interestingly, this phenomenon is associated to an enhancement of intracortical inhibition [41].
No study has so far addressed the modifications elicited by balance training on postural changes evoked by stimulation of the labyrinth, which is the sensory system in charge of maintaining balance under challenging conditions, such as when standing on unstable surfaces or when assuming tandem and unipedal stance [42–44].
The aims of the present investigation were:
- to study the progressive evolution of postural stability in unipedal stance by analysing the characteristics of CoP motion during and after a short-term training in this posture,
- to verify whether this training also modifies the reflex responses arising from labyrinthine stimulation.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Twenty-one healthy volunteers (9 women and 12 men, 31.6±7.7, mean age ± SD) free from cardiovascular, neurological and orthopaedic problems were enrolled in the experiment. The state of health of participants was assessed by a preliminary talk with the researchers of our team who asked whether they were suffering of cardiovascular, respiratory, metabolic, neurological, vestibular, psychiatric and orthopaedic symptoms. Only subjects who did report absence of these medical problems were enrolled in the study. Eighteen of them (9 women and 9 men) were right handed, while 3 left handed. The study was approved by the Committee on Bioethics of the University of Pisa (Protocol Number: 28/2022) and the subjects signed a written informed consent.
Experimental design
As shown in Table 1, the experimental design included four different sessions (2 training and 2 control sessions).
Subjects were invited to avoid any excess in alcohol, coffee and smoking on the experimental day and the preceding one. Only six of the subjects successfully completed all four sessions, occurring at least three months apart. The training-stance sessions (Table 1A) began with the measurement of the Centre of Pressure (CoP) by a stabilometric platform, during barefoot bipedal stance, each lasting 50 seconds. Recordings were taken with eyes open (OE) and closed (CE), on hard (HS) and soft support (SS), according to the following sequence: OE-HS, CE-HS, OE-SS, CE-SS. Subjects were allowed to sit for 60–120 seconds between HS and SS recordings. During OE recordings, subjects were asked to fixate a black spot (2 cm of diameter), at a distance of 90 cm, at the eyes level, while during CE recordings they were invited to keep the same gaze position. Following these initial bipedal stance recordings, after 60–120 seconds of rest, subjects underwent a postural training period, lasting about 23 minutes. The training protocol consisted of 10 successive periods of unipedal stance, in OE-HS conditions, performed at regular intervals (90 seconds). Subjects were allowed to sit between a training period and the following one. During each training period the CoP displacement was recorded. Subjects were invited to choose their dominant leg for standing. 90 seconds after the training (Time 0h), 1 hour (Time 1h) and 24 hours (Time 24h) later, the subjects repeated the whole sequence of CoP displacement recordings in bipedal stance.
In the control-stance sessions (Table 1B), participants were submitted to all the sequence of bipedal stance CoP measurements performed during the training session, but the 23 minutes postural training period was substituted by a resting period of the same duration.
In the training-EVS session (Table 1C) participants underwent, on hard support and closed eyes, electrical bipolar stimulation of the labyrinth (Electrical Vestibular Stimulation, EVS), which gave rise to stimulus-locked CoP displacements and leg postural muscles EMG responses (Vestibulo Spinal Reflexes, VSRs). During EVS the feet were always touching at the heel and positioned at an angle of 35°, bisected by the medial sagittal plane. EVS was delivered while participants were asked to keep their head in three different positions: forward (HF), leftward (HL), rightward (HR). In each position the stimulation lasted for about two minutes. Subjects were asked to maintain HL and HR orientations avoiding any effort. The angle of head-to-body rotation was evaluated before and after each recording with the help of a 180° circular sector with a 30 cm radius placed on the subject’s shoulders. Moreover, the stability of head position was visually controlled by an experimenter during the entire recording session. Trunk displacements associated with head rotation were corrected according to the instruction of the experimenters. Subjects were allowed to sit with open eyes for about 60 seconds between consecutive trials. Then, each subject underwent the same postural training (with simultaneous recording of CoP displacement) as the one performed in the training-stance session.
The whole sequence of EVS elicited in HF, HR and HL was repeated soon after, 1 hour and 24 hours following the postural training.
In the control-EVS session (Table 1D), participants were submitted to EVS at the same times as in the training/EVS session, but instead of performing the postural training they were invited to rest for a corresponding time span.
Labyrinthine stimulation
In the present study EVS was performed by using trains of pulses (1 msec pulse duration, train duration 300 msec), applied through surface electrodes placed over the mastoid bones (cathode always placed on the right side). The interpulse interval was fixed at 40 μsec, which produced a quasi-constant current [45]. These stimuli were delivered by an isolated, constant-current stimulator (Digitimer model DS7A) driven by a PC. The intensity of the current was checked at the end of the stimulation by measuring the voltage drop induced by the current across a 1 KΩ resistor. The stimulus intensity was 1.2 times the lower stimulus intensity (threshold) inducing a perception of body sway without autonomic symptoms and/or visual flash hallucinations. The applied current intensity values ranged from 0.4 to 2.0 mA. To elicit unpredictable stimuli, the PC generated a sequence of train pulses with a randomly varying interval at the average frequency of 0.35 Hz. Typically, 30–45 CoP and EMG responses to vestibular stimulation were recorded for each of the three head positions.
CoP and EMG recordings
The position of the CoP was recorded through a stabilometric platform (Dune 2000; Dune S.A.R.L., Negenheim, France), endowed with three force sensors disposed at the apices of an equilateral triangle having one of the sides parallel to the platform posterior edge. CoP coordinates were centred within the centre of the triangle, about 30–40 cm in front of the habitual CoP position. Average CoP X and Y coordinates, Standard Deviation (SD) of CoP position on both X and Y axes (corresponding to the average CoP oscillation with respect to the mean position), average CoP velocity, 95% ellipse area (the area of the ellipse containing 95% of the CoP positions) and LFS (cumulative length of CoP displacement/95% ellipse), evaluated over the recording time (50 seconds) were automatically calculated by the platform software.
To evaluate the parameters of the stimulus-locked CoP motion, the original signals from the force sensors were separately acquired and analysed through a Lab-view software prepared ad hoc (sampling rate 2KHz), which calculated the time course of CoP’s X and Y coordinates.
EMG recordings were taken from the medial (GM) and lateral (GL) gastrocnemius of both sides. EMG signals were amplified and filtered through a Lace amplifier (amplification 10000, high-pass frequency 0.5 Hz, low-pass frequency 5000 Hz). A further step of filtering was performed off-line on the acquired digital data by a custom-made Labview software: the low frequency cut-off was set to 5–10 Hz and the high cut-off frequency to 800 Hz when high frequency artefacts due to the stimulation train occurrence. Finally, EMG traces were full-wave rectified before evaluation of time-locked responses to EVS.
Time-locked CoP and EMG responses to EVS
A Lab-view, custom-made software was used to obtain stimulus-locked CoP and EMG responses. For this purpose, CoP coordinates were referred to the average of the X and Y CoP values relative to the pre-stimulus period. 30–45 successive sweeps were averaged over epochs of 3 seconds (pre-stimulus: 1 second; post-stimulus: 2 seconds). Rarely (overall x out of y trials), for unintentional technical reasons, the recorded data covered only a 1.5 seconds time span (pre-stimulus: 0.5 second; post-stimulus: 1 second). The obtained CoP traces were submitted to a smoothing procedure in which a given point (at the Nth position in the trace) was substituted by the average of all the points (101) included between N+50 and N-50. This procedure obliged to discard the first 50 and the last 50 points of each trace but did not modify the response time course. Examples of EVS-elicited responses of CoP position along X and Y axes are shown in Fig 1A/1B, respectively. In Fig 1, positive and negative values of the X component (Fig 1A) represent right and left displacements, respectively, while positive and negative values of the Y component (Fig 1B) correspond to forward and backward displacements. CoP responses were biphasic. In this representative case the initial displacement took place forward and rightward and the second one was in the opposite directions. Fig 1C and 1D show the instantaneous rate of change (CoP Velocity), digitally computed and smoothed (101 points), as the first derivative of the data depicted in Fig 1A and 11B, respectively.
Grand average of CoP position (A, B) and velocity (C, D) changes elicited by EVS delivered in HL position. Modifications elicited along the X and Y axes are displayed in A/C and B/D, respectively. All the subjects analysed over a 3 seconds time frame have been included (n = 11). In each panel the grey area encompassing the average trace represents SE, while the vertical grey bar depicts the stimulus onset.
Yet, positive and negative values of the X velocity component (Fig 1C) represent right/left directions of movement, while positive and negative values of the Y component (Fig 1D) refer to forward and backward directions of movement. Both components showed biphasic changes in opposite directions (rightward/leftward and forward/backward), the corresponding peaks being indicated as V1 and V2. The CoP is continuously modified by the changes in feet push onto the platform and in spatial position of the centre of mass, both of them being affected by VSR. On account of this, both components are reliable indicators of the VSR strength. Although the initial displacement of CoP velocity trace is related to feet push, the exact contribution of the two components to CoP velocity trajectory cannot be disentangled. For this reason, in addition to the V1 and V2 components, the peak to peak excursion (V = V2-V1) of CoP velocity traces was evaluated as a measure of the amplitude of CoP velocity response to vestibular stimulation.
The V values obtained for the X components were always negative (final CoP velocity to the left), regardless of head position. As to the Y components, V values were positive in HR (final CoP velocity directed forward) and negative (final CoP velocity directed backward) in HL, while nihil in HF.
The rectified EMG traces were submitted to 201 points smoothing to decrease the high frequency noise component and normalised as percentage of the pre-stimulus baseline values. Post-stimulus increases in EMG activity with respect to baseline corresponded to muscle contraction, while reduced values indicated muscle relaxation. As shown in Fig 2, EMG responses were in general biphasic, with an early peak consisting either in contraction (Fig 2A) or relaxation (Fig 2B) followed by a second component of opposite sign. As a first step, we evaluated, by visual inspection, the points where the EMG traces raised above (response latency) and went back to the baseline values (response end) following the stimulation. As shown in Fig 2, the early responses were quantified, whatever their nature (contraction or relaxation), as the area under the first component response curve (response area).
Grand average of the EMG response of the right GM to EVS in HL (A) and HR (B) position. All the subjects analysed over a 3 seconds time frame are included (HL: n = 10, HR: n = 11). The surface of the grey area underlying the peak activity (response area) was computed as a measure of the EMG evoked-response strength. In each panel the dotted line represents SE, while the vertical grey bar is the stimulus onset. Data are expressed as a percentage of the average value evaluated over the pre-stimulus period.
Statistical analysis
In the Training-stance and in the Training-EVS sessions, stabilometric unipedal stance parameters sampled 10 times every 2.3 minutes, were averaged in pairs of two, so to obtain 5 time points, each encompassing a time window of 4.6 minutes. For each session and variable, changes induced by postural training were analysed by a 5 Time repeated measures ANOVA, with LSD as post-hoc test.
Moreover, all data were expressed as a percentage of the value obtained by averaging the 5 time points of each individual subject. Data from different subjects were pooled together and correlated with time. The latter analysis was performed by using linear, logarithmic, inverse and exponential models.
In all the four sessions, possible modifications observed in bipedal stance parameters soon after postural training/rest periods, at 1 and 24 hours with respect to baseline (BSL) were analysed by a 4 Time (BSL, 0h, 1h, 24h) x 2 Eye (OE, CE) x 2 Support (HS, SS) repeated measures ANOVA. As to stimulus-locked CoP velocity responses to EVS, a 4 Time (BSL, 0h, 1h, 24h) x 3 Head position (HF, HR, HL) repeated measures ANOVA was applied to V values. In this instance, zero amplitude values indicated lack of CoP responses to labyrinthine stimulation. In this analysis, X and Y components of responses were analysed separately. Since V had opposite sign in HR (positive) with respect to HL (negative), ANOVA was applied to the corresponding absolute values. The amplitude of stimulus-locked EMG responses were analysed by a 4 Time (BSL, 0 h, 1h, 24h) repeated measures ANOVA, separately applied to the different head positions and muscles. All the statistical computations were performed by a SPSS software package. When data distribution did not respect the sphericity assumption, p values were corrected as appropriate.
Results
Training-induced changes in unipedal stance: Training-stance session
Analysis of the data collected during unipedal stance in the training-stance session revealed significant time effects for 95% ellipse (F(4,40) = 6.143, p = 0.001), CoP velocity (F(4,40) = 7.39, p = 0.006), X SD (F(4,40) = 5.853, p = 0.006) and Y SD (F(4,40) = 3.969, p = 0.007). Post-hoc analysis indicated that, in general, 95% ellipse, CoP velocity, X SD and Y SD showed significant decreases across training with respect to the first time interval, encompassing the initial 4.6 minutes. These data are summarized in Fig 3, where the initial and final values obtained in bipedal stance on hard support and open eyes were also reported. No significant Time effect could be observed for LFS.
Mean±SD values of 95% ellipse (A), CoP velocity (B), X SD (C) and Y SD (D) evaluated in unipedal stance at different time points during postural training. Data plotted in light grey, before and after the training interval, represent average values obtained in bipedal stance on hard support, eyes open. Asterisks indicate significant differences with respect to the first training time interval (0–4.6 minutes). *: p<0.05; **: p<0.02; ***: p<0.01. Single subject data relative to the training period can be identified on the basis of a colour code.
Another evaluation of the training effect was performed by correlation analysis. For this purpose, in each subject, all parameters were normalised (as %) with respect to the average value obtained during the training period. Data from different subjects were pooled together. Except for LFS, all parameters appeared to be negatively correlated with time. All the tested models were well fitting the experimental data: average values of correlation coefficients evaluated across parameters (except LFS) corresponded to 0.534, 0.575, 0.574 and 0.538 for the linear, logarithmic, inverse and exponential model, respectively. In particular, the correlation coefficients (R) of the logarithmic models corresponded to 0.610 (95% ellipse, Y = -18.060lnX+139.790, p<0.0005), 0.589 (X SD, Y = -9.002lnX+119.833, p<0.0005), 0.450 (Y SD, Y = -8.534lnX+118.801, p = 0.001) and 0.652 (CoP velocity, Y = -9.735lnX+121.448, p<0.0005).
Training-induced changes in the different variables were correlated to each other. This analysis was based on the last two points of the training session and the degree of such changes was evaluated as the difference relative to the initial time point (2.3 minutes, see Fig 3). Changes in 95% ellipse strongly covaried with those in X SD (R = 0.76, p<0.0005, Y = 138.72X-44.33), Y SD (R = 0.79, p<0.0005, Y = 129.72X-65.82) and CoP velocity changes (R = 0.77, p<0.0005, Y = 39.27X-33.93). CoP velocity changes were significantly correlated with both X SD (R = 0.835, Y = 3.014X-0.648, p<0.0005) and Y SD (R = 0.474, Y = 1.569X–1.978, p<0.0005) changes, while the correlation between X and Y SD changes did not reach significance level (R = 0.26, p = 0.084).
It was observed that the training-induced changes in the recorded variables, evaluated as difference with respect to the initial training interval value (2.3 minutes, see Fig 3), were significantly correlated with their initial amplitude: larger initial values corresponded to larger training-induced drops. This held true for all the variables at the last two (16.1 and 20.7 minutes, see Fig 3) time points and for 95% ellipse, X SD and CoP velocity also at the second and third time points (6.9 and 11.9 minutes, see Fig 3). These data are summarized in Fig 4, where changes obtained at 6.9, 11.9, 16.1 and 20.7 minutes for each subject have been cumulatively plotted with respect to the initial values (95% ellipse: R = 0.805, Y = -0.653X+244.00, p<0.0005; X SD: R = 0.850, Y = -0.678X+3.378, p<0.0005; Y SD: R = 0.547, Y = -0.499X+2.51, p<0.0005; CoP velocity: R = 0.809, Y = -0.641X+11.978, p<0.0005).
Data relative to the second (6.9 minutes), third (11.5 minutes), fourth (16.1 minutes) and fifth (20.7 minutes) time points of the training interval have been reported. A: 95% ellipse. B: CoP velocity. C: X SD. D: Y SD. In all the scatterplots data in ordinate correspond to the difference between the variable value at a given time point and the initial time point value (2.3 minutes). The lines are regression lines evaluated for all the plotted points.
Changes in bipedal stance induced by unipedal postural training
Stabilometric data recorded in bipedal stance in the training-stance session were recorded at four time points in two different conditions of Support (soft, hard) and Eye (open, closed), so that they were initially analysed by a three-way ANOVA model (4 Time (BSL, 0h, 1h, 24h) x 2 Support (HS, SS) x 2 Eye (OE, CE) repeated measures ANOVA). A Time effect was observed for 95% ellipse (F(3,30) = 3.13, p = 0.040) (Table 2A) due to a 24h value significantly or quasi significantly lower than all the preceding data points (BSL: p = 0.026, Time 0h: p = 0.051, Time 1h: p = 0.056). In the control-stance session, no significant Time effect or interactions were observed.
Since interactions between factors showed eta2 values lower than 0.2, we also run two-way ANOVA models. Indeed, a 2 Support (HS, SS) x 4 Time (BSL, 0h, 1h, 24h) repeated measures ANOVA and a 2 Eye (OE, CE) x 4 Time (BSL, 0h, 1h, 24h) repeated measures ANOVA were run separately for the OE and CE conditions and for the HS and SS conditions, respectively. These analyses revealed a significant Time effect for 95% ellipse only under CE (F(3,30) = 3.458, p = 0.029) and SS conditions (F(3,30) = 2.948, p = 0.049). This effect is decomposed in Table 2B. The 24h value of 95% ellipse was significantly lower than both baseline (p = 0.01) and Time 0h (p = 0.048) under the CE condition. With SS, only the difference between baseline and Time 24h reached the significance level (p = 0.034). Moreover a significant Time effect could be also observed for Y SD (F(3,30) = 5.364, p = 0.004) with CE, being the 24h value (5.23±1.23, mm) significantly lower than both baseline (6.46±1.65, mm, p = 0.002) and 0h (5.82±1.59, p = 0.046) values. No significant effects and interactions could be found when these ANOVA models were applied within the control-stance session.
Postural training and vestibular stimulation
With respect to the training-stance session, following EVS, significant/quasi-significant Time effects could be observed only for 95% ellipse (F(4,48) = 2.974, p = 0.028) and CoP velocity (F(4,48) = 3.889, p = 0.06), when recorded in unipedal stance. In the training-EVS session, however, a significant Time effect could be observed also for LFS (F(4,48) = 2.784, p = 0.037). All variables decreased during training with respect to the initial training interval values: average and single subjected data can be found in Fig 5 for 95% ellipse, X SD, CoP velocity and LFS.
Mean±SD values of 95% ellipse (A), CoP velocity (B), X SD (C) and LFS (D) evaluated in unipedal stance at different time points during postural training performed following EVS. Data plotted in light grey, before and after the training interval, represent average values obtained in bipedal stance on hard support, eyes open. Asterisks indicate significant differences with respect to the first training time interval (0–4.6 minutes). *: p<0.05; **: p<0.02; ***: p<0.01. Single subject data relative to the training period can be identified on the basis of a colour code.
The regression analysis of postural variables with respect to time revealed, however, a significant decrease in all the parameters (LFS included), whatever the model applied. Average R values evaluated across parameters corresponded to 0.353, 0.409, 0.428 and 0.361 for the linear, logarithmic, inverse and exponential models, respectively. In particular, R values relative to the logarithmic model corresponded to 0.428 (95% ellipse, Y = -13.802lnX+130.408, p<0.0005), 0.436 (X SD, Y = -10.022lnX+122.080, p<0.0005), 0.318 (Y SD, Y = -10.420lnX+122.957, p = 0.010), 0.493 (CoP velocity, Y = -11.475lnX+125.280, p<0.0005) and 0.372 (LFS, Y = -4.187lnX+109.223, p = 0.002).
Consistently with the training-stance session, the changes in the different parameters were always negatively correlated with the initial values (95% ellipse: R = 0.818, Y = -0.833X +397.00, p<0.0005; CoP velocity: R = -0.933, Y = -0.957X+20.11, p<0.0005; X SD: R = 0.946, Y = -0.964X+5.292, p<0.0005, Y SD: R = 0.859, Y = -0.815X+5.259, p<0.0005; LFS: R = 0.507, Y = -0.405X+0.678, p<0.0005). As observed in the training-stance session (without EVS), the changes in the different parameters were correlated to each other (Table 3). In the training-EVS session, however, unlike the training-group, a correlation was found also between changes in Y SD and X SD.
Training-induced changes in VSRs: Kinematics
The changes in CoP velocity typically induced by EVS along X and Y axes were biphasic, with the two components (V1 and V2) occurring in opposite directions. This can be appreciated in Fig 6, where baseline data have been shown. The peak to peak changes along the X axis were larger in the HF position with respect to both HR (p = 0.002) and HL (p<0.0005), while the opposite held true for Y displacements, which were generally absent in HF. Along the X axis, V1 was always oriented to the cathode side (right side, positive values in Fig 6), while V2 to the anode side (left side, negative values). Along the Y axis V1 was directed backward (negative values) and forward (positive values) when the head was in HR and in HL, respectively. The opposite held true for V2.
Grand average of baseline CoP velocity changes elicited by EVS delivered in HF (A, B, n = 10), HR (C, D, n = 12) and HL (E, F, n = 11) position. Panels A, C, E and B, D, F, represent modifications elicited along the X and Y axes, respectively. All the subjects analysed over a 3 seconds time frame have been included. In each panel the grey area encompassing the average trace represents SE, while the vertical grey bar is the stimulus onset.
The peak to peak change along the Y axis was larger than that along the X axis in both HR (p<0.0005) and HL (p = 0.002) position. The opposite held true in HF (p<0.0005).
Data along both axes were separately analysed to verify possible training-induced changes in VSRs. A 4 Time (BSL, 0h, 1h, 24h) x 3 Head (HF, HR, HL) and 4 Time (BSL, 0h, 1h, 24h) x 2 Head (HR, HL) repeated measures ANOVA were used for analysing the X and Y components of CoP velocity (V), respectively. In the latter analysis, both HR and HL data were taken with their absolute values.
It must be pointed out that the angle of head-to-body rotation observed in both HR and HL was remarkably stable across the whole session. HR and HL values obtained at the different time points were correlated with each other, with high coefficients of correlation (0.974–0.993) and slopes values close to 1 (0.937–1.024). In HR, the average angles observed at the beginning of the baseline and 24h time points were 45.6±8.9, SD, ° and 43.7±8.9, SD, °, respectively. In HL, baseline and 24h values corresponded to 133.4±6.6, SD, ° and 135.1±9.4, SD, °.
Within the training-EVS session a significant Time effect could be found for V (F(3,33) = 6.85, p<0.001) along the X axis. This effect was related to a decrease in V amplitude following postural training, illustrated in Fig 7 for a representative case.
EVS-elicited changes in the X component of CoP velocity observed in a representative subject (HF position) at different time points (baselinecontrol, 0h, 1h, 24h) with respect to postural training.
As shown in Fig 8A, the V amplitudes (average of the values obtained for the different head positions) were significantly or quasi significantly reduced soon after (p = 0.064), at 1 hour (p = 0.007) and 24 hours (p = 0.01) following the adaptation period. As to the Y component of V, depicted in Fig 8B, a 4 Time x 2 Head position ANOVA revealed a significant Time effects for V (F(3,33) = 5.387, p = 0.028), with a significant reduction of the peak to peak excursion of CoP velocity soon after (p = 0.047), 1 hour (p = 0.029) and 24 hours (p = 0.023) following the adaptation period. No significant differences could be found for baseline values of both X and Y components between the control-EVS (X: 18.54±3.34, SD, mm/s; Y: 32.23±9.40, SD, mm/s) and the training-EVS (X: 23.56±8.54, SD, mm/s; Y: 39.28±13.55, SD, mm/s) sessions.
Mean±ES values of X (A) and Y (B) components of CoP velocity (V) changes evaluated at different time points. The training-EVS and the control-EVS sessions are represented by the black and white bars, respectively. Measurements were taken before the unipedal training/rest period (baseline), soon after (0h), as well as 1 hour (1h) and 24 hours (24h) later. Asterisks refer to significant differences with respect to baseline. *: p<0.05; **: p<0.02; ***: p<0.01.
Qualitatively similar effects of training could be observed when the V1 and V2 components of the changes in CoP velocity were analysed separately: in this instance significant/quasi-significant Time effects could be observed along both the X (V1: F(3,33) = 3.014, p = 0.068, V2: F(3,33) = 8.211, p<0.0005) and Y (V1: F(3,33) = 3.741, p = 0.055; V2: F(3,33) = 4.991, p = 0.017) axes.
As to the Control-EVS session, no Time or Head x Time effect could be observed for V, V1 and V2 amplitudes along both axes. However, the Time effect for the Y component of V (F(3,27) = 2.694, p = 0.066) was not very far from the significance level. Moreover, post-hoc analysis indicated quasi-significant and significant differences of 0h (p = 0.058) and 1h (p = 0.024) time points with respect to the initial (baseline) value. These results are illustrated in Fig 8A/8B for X and Y velocity components, respectively. It must be pointed out that, when V amplitudes along the Y axis were expressed as differences versus the initial (baseline) values, no significant differences were found between the EVS-training and control session, whatever time point was considered. At variance, when the V amplitudes along the X axis were submitted to the same procedure, a significant difference was observed at 1h (p = 0.025) between the training-EVS and control-EVS session.
Training-induced changes in postural stability and VSR
A regression analysis was performed between the changes elicited in unipedal stance parameters in the last 4.6 minutes (corresponding to the ninth and tenth time points) of postural training and the corresponding changes in CoP responses to EVS. For this analysis V values were taken as positive so that a negative difference in ordinate indicated a decrease relative to the baseline time point. Significant correlations were found only in the HL position. Fig 9 shows that, although postural training reduced VSRs and increased postural stability, subjects showing the largest drops in VSRs (negative values on ordinate) were those increasing less postural stability, expressed as a reduction in 95% ellipse, Y SD or CoP velocity (negative values on abscissas). Indeed, EVS-driven changes in CoP velocity along the X axis were negatively correlated with those in 95% ellipse (Fig 9A, R = 0.682, p<0.0005, Y = -0.028X–5.322), Y SD (Fig 9B, R = 0.729, p<0.0005, Y = -3.945X–4.509) and CoP velocity (not shown, R = 0.472, Y = -1.560X-7.818, p = 0.023).
A. Relation between the changes observed during the last two time points of postural training in 95% ellipse (A) and Y SD (B), and those in the EVS-evoked CoP responses recorded along the X axis (HL position). In both A and B, negative values along the ordinate axis indicated largest drops of the peak-to-peak CoP velocity.
As to the EVS-driven changes in Y CoP velocity, they were negatively correlated only with training-induced changes in X SD (R = 0.503, Y = -5.3774X–6.583, p = 0.014). These correlations indicated that subjects who decreased less their VSR were those decreasing more their postural oscillations.
Training-induced changes in VSRs: EMG activity
EMG responses of plantar flexors locked to EVS are shown in Fig 10 for the left (A, C, E) and right (B, D, F) mean (GL and GM) activity. As expected, they depended upon the relative body-to-head position. In general, when the head was kept forward (HF), the EMG activity of both GL and GM was initially decreased on the left side (Fig 10A), while enhanced on the right side (Fig 10B). When the head was turned to the right side (HR), EMG responses consisted mostly in an initial bilateral depression of GL and GM (Fig 10C/10D) activity, larger on the left side. In the HL position, the EVS elicited on average an initial increase in the GM (Fig 10E/10F) and GL muscle activity, stronger on the right side.
Grand average of the EMG response recorded from the left (A, C, E) and right (B, D, F) to EVS in HF (A, B), HR (C, D) and HL (E, F) position, at the beginning of the training session (baseline). All the subjects analysed over a 3 seconds time frame have been included (HF: n = 9, HL: n = 10, HR: n = 11). In each panel the grey area encompassing the average black line represents SE, while the vertical grey line is the stimulus onset. Data are expressed as a percentage of the average value evaluated over the pre-stimulus period.
As shown in Fig 10 GM and GL data of the same side were averaged. Moreover, the response areas were all taken as positive and averaged across head positions. A 4 Time (baseline, 0h, 1h, 24h) repeated measure ANOVA applied to the averaged responses observed on the right side revealed a significant Time effect (F(3,30) = 4.885, p = 0.007), illustrated in Fig 11A. At variance, no significant time-related modifications could be observed on the left side responses (Fig 11B). Finally, these modifications were not dependent upon parallel changes in pre-stimulus EMG activity. Indeed, a 4 Time repeated measure ANOVA did not show significant Time effects for both left (F(3,24) = 1.454, p = 0.252) and right (F(3,30) = 0.910, p = 0.448) muscles (average of GL and GM across head positions), in spite of the significant changes observed in the EMG and CoP responses. Moreover, no correlation was found—at the level of individual muscles—between modifications in response area and in pre-stimulus activity (evaluated as a difference with respect to the baseline).
Mean±SE of response area absolute values evaluated across different head positions (HF, HR and HL) and muscles (GM and GL) for both right (A) and left (B) sides. Measurements were taken before (baseline), soon after (0h), as well as 1 hour (1h) and 24 hours (24h) after the training/rest period. Asterisks refer to significant differences with respect to control. *: p<0.05; **: p<0.02; ***: p<0.01.
Discussion
A short-term form of postural learning improving unipedal stance
The present study, investigating four indicators of body sway (95% ellipse, CoP velocity and CoP oscillation along both X and Y axes), indicated that postural stability increased progressively in unipedal stance when this position was maintained 10 times for periods of 50 seconds spaced by sitting intervals of 90 seconds. Significant changes could be observed for all the parameters analysed, despite the limited sample size. The decreases in CoP velocity and in 95% ellipse were of similar amplitude so that the LFS values remained constant. These data show a more efficient control of body sway, which could be achieved through a change in the feedback mechanisms stabilizing posture [46–48]. Moreover, a modification could occur in the forward, centrally generated motor commands which are delivered on the basis of a body neural representation (forward model) [49]. There is indeed evidence that both feedback and forward mechanisms participate in the control of normal bipedal and unipedal stance [50, 51]. The large body oscillations occurring in unipedal stance are likely to increase the modulation of sensory signals converging at the level of central neural structures controlling posture [52–60]. Somatosensory and vestibular signals are particularly important in the challenging, unstable unipedal stance, since visual information are not appropriate for evoking fast, postural reaction, although it may enhance the response to the other inputs [61]. It is likely that the central convergence of posture-related signals (particularly of vestibular and somatosensory nature) may modify the excitability (and gain) of individual reflex pathways [62, 63] as well as the internal models [64] which sustain forward control mechanisms, leading to an improvement of body stability. Training-induced changes in postural variables were correlated to each other, except for CoP oscillation along X and Y axes: this finding is consistent with evidence indicating rather independent mechanisms [65] of vestibular control of postural stability along the two axes. The effects of training were stronger in those subjects showing the highest initial instability. This finding suggests that the learning process becomes less efficient when the postural stability is initially high, likely due to a decreased modulation of sensory afferents signalling body sway and to a reduced efficacy in changing the excitability of central pathways.
The effectiveness of postural training on postural stability was reduced by pre-training pseudo-random EVS, which continuously elicited VSRs. This procedure injected into the brain a labyrinthine input in the absence of a corresponding head movement, inducing a mismatch between somatosensory and vestibular signals and altering the central integration of vestibular and proprioceptive signals during unipedal stance. Such a mismatch may lead to a down regulation of the central processing of vestibular input which respect to the other sensory channels [66]. It is possible that this down regulation may last for few minutes, covering, at least in part, the following period of unipedal stance training and attenuating the changes in networks excitability which enhance stability during postural training.
EVS performed in HR and HL positions elicits simultaneous activation of balance reaction in the frontal and sagittal plane and this may increase the coupling between the corresponding neural control systems. This may explain why the changes elicited in postural sway along the X (X SD) and Y (Y SD) axes, although weaker with respect to those elicited in the training-stance session, are strongly correlated to each other.
From unipedal to bipedal stance
No appreciable changes in postural parameters recorded in bipedal stance could be observed soon and 1 hour after the postural training performed in the more challenging unipedal stance. It has to be acknowledged, however, that the small sampling size might have hampered the detection of the less sizeable changes in postural parameters. At 24 hours from the training session a decrease in 95% ellipse and Y SD could be observed on soft support or with the eye closed. This observation is in agreement with studies showing that long-term postural learning may also modify normal stance condition in young athletes [13]. In the present report generalization of learning to bipedal stance took 24 hours to be observed. This time window encompassed a night of sleep. It is therefore possible that the beneficial effect of sleep documented for motor learning processes affecting voluntary movements [67, 68] may also extend to postural control mechanisms. A specifically designed sleep versus control study could test this hypothesis [69].
Persistence of postural learning and translational applications
A limit of the present study was that the ability to maintain a challenging posture such as the unipedal stance was investigated only soon after the training session. It has been shown that the effects of a single session in postural training may persist the whole day, so that they may sum to those of another training session performed the day after [70]. This is probably the reason why repeated session of postural training regularly spaced within the week may lead in the long term to substantial improvements of postural stability [1, 5–14]. Such an improvement is not only exploited for improving postural control in sports performance, but also for rehabilitative purposes. Indeed, Vestibular rehabilitation programs (VRPs) use the plasticity of the balance system to speed up the natural compensation process that normally occurs following vestibular pathologies and are effective in enhancing gaze and postural stability, reducing vertigo, and improving daily living activities [71]. The training in unipedal stance that we have described could be adapted for VRP, which usually includes at least two daily sessions of increasing duration [72].
Networks involved in the plasticity of postural functions
The training-induced changes in postural control may depend on plasticity mechanisms affecting several brain motor structures, which underlines general motor learning [73–75]. In this respect, it has been shown that postural training may lead to rapid and specific cortical grey matter changes [76] and modulates cortical excitability [30, 36, 41]. Another good candidate for contributing to postural learning is the cerebellum, which plays an important role in motor learning phenomena, such as visuomotor adaptation [77–79]. It has been shown that Purkinje cells undergo to significant changes in the density of their dendritic spine following an acrobatic training [80]. Indeed, the medial cerebellar vermal area controls stance [81–86] and promotes learning processes that modulates postural reflexes [63, 87–90], enhancing postural stability. Plastic changes of postural reflexes may be also elicited by manipulating the noradrenergic input impinging on the cerebellar vermis [91, 92], which arises from the Locus Coeruleus [93, 94].
Postural learning and VSRs
A reduction in the velocity of CoP displacement elicited by EVS along the X and Y axes was clearly observed following postural training. A similar, less significant trend was observed for the Y component which, however, underwent some minor time-dependent attenuation also when the subject was not involved in the training sequence. This finding could be attributed to adaptation processes [66, 95], not observed for the X component of the vestibular response. This observation further emphasises the differences between neural mechanisms controlling postural stability on the sagittal and the frontal plane. The training-induced changes in CoP velocity could be attributed to the decrease of the EVS-evoked EMG responses observed at the level of the right GM and GL. In the present experiment, the stimulation was applied to the right side. Since the lateral vestibulospinal tract, which controls lumbar motoneurons, projects to the ipsilateral side of the spinal cord, this could explain the lack of changes on the contralateral muscle activation. However, we cannot exclude the hypothesis that contralateral changes have been undetected, due to the limited sample size. In addition, VSRs also involve neck, trunk and feet muscles [96–98] that were not investigated in the present study and could have contributed to the kinematic changes.
The depression of VSRs observed in the present investigation reminds the down regulation of H-reflex most often elicited by balance training [40, 99], via enhanced presynaptic inhibitory mechanisms [24, 26, 100, 101]. The latter phenomenon would be appropriate for increasing postural stability, which requires complex actions at multiple body levels [102], while the myotatic reflex has a more local extent of action. Moreover, delayed feedback responses associated with the myotatic reflex may lead to subsequent joint oscillations [25]. However, the vestibular system exerts a diffuse influence on body muscles [96–98] and the responses observed in the present study act in a feedforward manner [103], being developed before the sensory-based correction. In addition, although in the present experiment the VSR was depressed by postural training, which increased postural stability, unexpectedly subjects who decreased less the VSR were those decreasing more their postural oscillations (Fig 9). We may propose that postural training in unipedal stance acts by reducing the central, feedforward component of the postural oscillations, leading to a general weakening of postural reflexes, which are of reduced amplitude because of a stricter central control of CoP displacement. It is likely that the cerebellar structures involved in postural control play a key role in this process.
References
- 1. Paillard T. Plasticity of the postural function to sport and/or motor experience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;72: 129–152. pmid:27894829
- 2. Kiers H, van Dieën J, Dekkers H, Wittink H, Vanhees L. A systematic review of the relationship between physical activities in sports or daily life and postural sway in upright stance. Sports Med. 2013;43: 1171–1189. pmid:23955562
- 3. Low DC, Walsh GS, Arkesteijn M. Effectiveness of Exercise Interventions to Improve Postural Control in Older Adults: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analyses of Centre of Pressure Measurements. Sports Med. 2017;47: 101–112. pmid:27245061
- 4. Huang Y, Liu X. Improvement of balance control ability and flexibility in the elderly Tai Chi Chuan (TCC) practitioners: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2015;60: 233–238. pmid:25497683
- 5. DiStefano LJ, Clark MA, Padua DA. Evidence supporting balance training in healthy individuals: a systemic review. J Strength Cond Res. 2009;23: 2718–2731. pmid:19910803
- 6. Lesinski M, Hortobágyi T, Muehlbauer T, Gollhofer A, Granacher U. Dose-response relationships of balance training in healthy young adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2015;45: 557–576. pmid:25430598
- 7. Iwamoto J, Suzuki H, Tanaka K, Kumakubo T, Hirabayashi H, Miyazaki Y, et al. Preventative effect of exercise against falls in the elderly: a randomized controlled trial. Osteoporos Int. 2009;20: 1233–1240. pmid:19011727
- 8. Toulotte C, Thevenon A, Fabre C. Effects of training and detraining on the static and dynamic balance in elderly fallers and non-fallers: a pilot study. Disabil Rehabil. 2006;28: 125–133. pmid:16393843
- 9. Chulvi-Medrano I, Colado JC, Pablos C, Naclerio F, García-Massó X. A lower-limb training program to improve balance in healthy elderly women using the T-bow device. Phys Sportsmed. 2009;37: 127–135. pmid:20048519
- 10. Battaglia G, Bellafiore M, Bianco A, Paoli A, Palma A. Effects of a dynamic balance training protocol on podalic support in older women. Pilot Study. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2010;22: 406–411. pmid:20009497
- 11. Bernard PL, Blain H, Gerazime A, Maurelli O, Bousquet J, Ninot G. Relationship between a three-month physical conditioning “posture-balance-motricity and health education” (PBM-HE) program on postural and balance capacities of sedentary older adults: influence of initial motor profile. Eur Rev Aging Phys Act. 2018;15: 14. pmid:30479673
- 12. Cruz-Montecinos C, Cuesta-Vargas A, Muñoz C, Flores D, Ellsworth J, D la Fuente C, et al. Impact of Visual Biofeedback of Trunk Sway Smoothness on Motor Learning during Unipedal Stance. Sensors (Basel). 2020;20: E2585. pmid:32370050
- 13. Pau M, Loi A, Pezzotta MC. Does sensorimotor training improve the static balance of young volleyball players? Sports Biomech. 2012;11: 97–107. pmid:22518948
- 14. Cè E, Longo S, Paleari E, Riboli A, Limonta E, Rampichini S, et al. Evidence of balance training-induced improvement in soccer-specific skills in U11 soccer players. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2018;28: 2443–2456. pmid:29885054
- 15. Asseman FB, Caron O, Crémieux J. Are there specific conditions for which expertise in gymnastics could have an effect on postural control and performance? Gait Posture. 2008;27: 76–81. pmid:17337190
- 16. Asseman F, Caron O, Crémieux J. Is there a transfer of postural ability from specific to unspecific postures in elite gymnasts? Neurosci Lett. 2004;358: 83–86. pmid:15026154
- 17. Giboin L-S, Tokuno C, Kramer A, Henry M, Gruber M. Motor learning induces time-dependent plasticity that is observable at the spinal cord level. J Physiol. 2020;598: 1943–1963. pmid:32115702
- 18. Marcori AJ, Teixeira LA, Mathias KR, Dascal JB, Okazaki VHA. Asymmetric interlateral transfer of motor learning in unipedal dynamic balance. Exp Brain Res. 2020;238: 2745–2751. pmid:32979050
- 19. Massion J. Postural control system. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1994;4: 877–887. pmid:7888772
- 20. Paillard T, Noé F. Techniques and Methods for Testing the Postural Function in Healthy and Pathological Subjects. Biomed Res Int. 2015;2015: 891390. pmid:26640800
- 21. Jacobs JV, Horak FB. Cortical control of postural responses. J Neural Transm (Vienna). 2007;114: 1339–1348. pmid:17393068
- 22. Kurtzer IL. Long-latency reflexes account for limb biomechanics through several supraspinal pathways. Front Integr Neurosci. 2014;8: 99. pmid:25688187
- 23. Britton TC, Day BL, Brown P, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, Marsden CD. Postural electromyographic responses in the arm and leg following galvanic vestibular stimulation in man. Exp Brain Res. 1993;94: 143–151. pmid:8335069
- 24. Gruber M, Taube W, Gollhofer A, Beck S, Amtage F, Schubert M. Training-specific adaptations of H- and stretch reflexes in human soleus muscle. J Mot Behav. 2007;39: 68–78. pmid:17251172
- 25. Keller M, Pfusterschmied J, Buchecker M, Müller E, Taube W. Improved postural control after slackline training is accompanied by reduced H-reflexes. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2012;22: 471–477. pmid:21385217
- 26. Freyler K, Weltin E, Gollhofer A, Ritzmann R. Improved postural control in response to a 4-week balance training with partially unloaded bodyweight. Gait Posture. 2014;40: 291–296. pmid:24836698
- 27. Behrens M, Mau-Moeller A, Wassermann F, Bader R, Bruhn S. Effect of balance training on neuromuscular function at rest and during isometric maximum voluntary contraction. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2015;115: 1075–1085. pmid:25557387
- 28. Giboin L-S, Loewe K, Hassa T, Kramer A, Dettmers C, Spiteri S, et al. Cortical, subcortical and spinal neural correlates of slackline training-induced balance performance improvements. Neuroimage. 2019;202: 116061. pmid:31374329
- 29. Beck S, Taube W, Gruber M, Amtage F, Gollhofer A, Schubert M. Task-specific changes in motor evoked potentials of lower limb muscles after different training interventions. Brain Res. 2007;1179: 51–60. pmid:17889840
- 30. Schubert M, Beck S, Taube W, Amtage F, Faist M, Gruber M. Balance training and ballistic strength training are associated with task-specific corticospinal adaptations. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;27: 2007–2018. pmid:18412622
- 31. Ruffieux J, Mouthon A, Keller M, Wälchli M, Taube W. Behavioral and neural adaptations in response to five weeks of balance training in older adults: a randomized controlled trial. J Negat Results Biomed. 2017;16: 11. pmid:28610582
- 32. Penzer F, Duchateau J, Baudry S. Effects of short-term training combining strength and balance exercises on maximal strength and upright standing steadiness in elderly adults. Exp Gerontol. 2015;61: 38–46. pmid:25449860
- 33. Chen Y-S, Zhou S, Cartwright C. Effect of 12 weeks of Tai Chi training on soleus Hoffmann reflex and control of static posture in older adults. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2011;92: 886–891. pmid:21621664
- 34. Chen Y-S, Crowley Z, Zhou S, Cartwright C. Effects of 12-week Tai Chi training on soleus H-reflex and muscle strength in older adults: a pilot study. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2012;112: 2363–2368. pmid:21947456
- 35. Kitano K, Tsuruike M, Robertson CT, Kocejal DM. Effects of a complex balance task on soleus H-reflex and presynaptic inhibition in humans. Electromyogr Clin Neurophysiol. 2009;49: 235–243. pmid:19694211
- 36. Taube W, Gruber M, Beck S, Faist M, Gollhofer A, Schubert M. Cortical and spinal adaptations induced by balance training: correlation between stance stability and corticospinal activation. Acta Physiol (Oxf). 2007;189: 347–358. pmid:17263693
- 37. Taube W, Kullmann N, Leukel C, Kurz O, Amtage F, Gollhofer A. Differential reflex adaptations following sensorimotor and strength training in young elite athletes. Int J Sports Med. 2007;28: 999–1005. pmid:17497570
- 38. Nashner LM. Adapting reflexes controlling the human posture. Exp Brain Res. 1976;26: 59–72. pmid:964327
- 39. Sayenko DG, Masani K, Vette AH, Alekhina MI, Popovic MR, Nakazawa K. Effects of balance training with visual feedback during mechanically unperturbed standing on postural corrective responses. Gait Posture. 2012;35: 339–344. pmid:22118729
- 40. Granacher U, Gollhofer A, Strass D. Training induced adaptations in characteristics of postural reflexes in elderly men. Gait Posture. 2006;24: 459–466. pmid:16472525
- 41. Mouthon A, Taube W. Intracortical Inhibition Increases during Postural Task Execution in Response to Balance Training. Neuroscience. 2019;401: 35–42. pmid:30660672
- 42. Cenciarini M, Peterka RJ. Stimulus-dependent changes in the vestibular contribution to human postural control. J Neurophysiol. 2006;95: 2733–2750. pmid:16467429
- 43. Hlavacka F, Horak FB. Somatosensory influence on postural response to galvanic vestibular stimulation. Physiol Res. 2006;55 Suppl 1: S121–S127. pmid:17177620
- 44. Sprenger A, Wojak JF, Jandl NM, Helmchen C. Postural Control in Bilateral Vestibular Failure: Its Relation to Visual, Proprioceptive, Vestibular, and Cognitive Input. Front Neurol. 2017;8: 444. pmid:28919878
- 45. Grasso C, Orsini P, Bruschini L, Manzoni D, Barresi M. A new technique to investigate vestibulo-spinal reflexes. Arch Ital Biol. 2013;151: 54–66. pmid:24442983
- 46. Johansson R, Magnusson M. Human postural dynamics. Crit Rev Biomed Eng. 1991;18: 413–437. pmid:1855384
- 47. Peterka RJ. Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88: 1097–1118. pmid:12205132
- 48. Horak FB. Postural orientation and equilibrium: what do we need to know about neural control of balance to prevent falls? Age Ageing. 2006;35 Suppl 2: ii7–ii11. pmid:16926210
- 49. Loram ID, Maganaris CN, Lakie M. Human postural sway results from frequent, ballistic bias impulses by soleus and gastrocnemius. J Physiol. 2005;564: 295–311. pmid:15661824
- 50. Collins JJ, De Luca CJ. Open-loop and closed-loop control of posture: a random-walk analysis of center-of-pressure trajectories. Exp Brain Res. 1993;95: 308–318. pmid:8224055
- 51. Collins JJ, De Luca CJ. The effects of visual input on open-loop and closed-loop postural control mechanisms. Exp Brain Res. 1995;103: 151–163. pmid:7615030
- 52. Boyle R, Pompeiano O. Convergence and interaction of neck and macular vestibular inputs on vestibulospinal neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1981;45: 852–868. pmid:7241173
- 53. Kubin L, Manzoni D, Pompeiano O. Responses of lateral reticular neurons to convergent neck and macular vestibular inputs. J Neurophysiol. 1981;46: 48–64. pmid:6114992
- 54. Pompeiano O, Manzoni D, Srivastava UC, Stampacchia G. Convergence and interaction of neck and macular vestibular inputs on reticulospinal neurons. Neuroscience. 1984;12: 111–128. pmid:6611516
- 55. Xerri C, Barthelemy J, Borel L, Lacour M. Neuronal coding of linear motion in the vestibular nuclei of the alert cat. III. Dynamic characteristics of visual-otolith interactions. Exp Brain Res. 1988;70: 299–309. pmid:3384033
- 56. Manzoni D, Pompeiano O, Barnes CD, Stampacchia G, d’Ascanio P. Responses of locus coeruleus neurons to convergent neck and vestibular inputs. Acta Otolaryngol Suppl. 1989;468: 129–135. pmid:2635490
- 57. Fukushima K. Corticovestibular interactions: anatomy, electrophysiology, and functional considerations. Exp Brain Res. 1997;117: 1–16. pmid:9386000
- 58. Bruschini L, Andre P, Pompeiano O, Manzoni D. Responses of Purkinje-cells of the cerebellar anterior vermis to stimulation of vestibular and somatosensory receptors. Neuroscience. 2006;142: 235–245. pmid:16843608
- 59. Brooks JX, Cullen KE. Multimodal integration in rostral fastigial nucleus provides an estimate of body movement. J Neurosci. 2009;29: 10499–10511. pmid:19710303
- 60. McCall AA, Miller DM, Balaban CD. Integration of vestibular and hindlimb inputs by vestibular nucleus neurons: multisensory influences on postural control. J Neurophysiol. 2021;125: 1095–1110. pmid:33534649
- 61. Nashner L, Berthoz A. Visual contribution to rapid motor responses during postural control. Brain Res. 1978;150: 403–407. pmid:678978
- 62. Pompeiano O, Andre P, D’Ascanio P, Manzoni D. Role of the spinocerebellum in adaptive gain control of cat’s vestibulospinal reflex. Acta Otolaryngol Suppl. 1995;520 Pt 1: 82–86. pmid:8749087
- 63. Andre P, Pompeiano O, Manzoni D. Adaptive modification of the cats vestibulospinal reflex during sustained and combined roll tilt of the whole animal and forepaw rotation: cerebellar mechanisms. Neuroscience. 2005;132: 811–822. pmid:15837141
- 64. Bakkum A, Donelan JM, Marigold DS. Challenging balance during sensorimotor adaptation increases generalization. J Neurophysiol. 2020;123: 1342–1354. pmid:32130079
- 65. Carpenter MG, Allum JH, Honegger F. Vestibular influences on human postural control in combinations of pitch and roll planes reveal differences in spatiotemporal processing. Exp Brain Res. 2001;140: 95–111. pmid:11500802
- 66. Dilda V, Morris TR, Yungher DA, MacDougall HG, Moore ST. Central adaptation to repeated galvanic vestibular stimulation: implications for pre-flight astronaut training. PLoS One. 2014;9: e112131. pmid:25409443
- 67. Pan SC, Rickard TC. Sleep and motor learning: Is there room for consolidation? Psychol Bull. 2015;141: 812–834. pmid:25822130
- 68. King BR, Hoedlmoser K, Hirschauer F, Dolfen N, Albouy G. Sleeping on the motor engram: The multifaceted nature of sleep-related motor memory consolidation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;80: 1–22. pmid:28465166
- 69. Fischer S, Hallschmid M, Elsner AL, Born J. Sleep forms memory for finger skills. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99: 11987–11991. pmid:12193650
- 70. Schedler S, Leifeld P, Seidel T, Brueckner D, Muehlbauer T. Effects of task difficulty during practice on learning a dynamic balance task in healthy young adults: An intervention study. BMC Res Notes. 2021;14: 235. pmid:34134764
- 71. Shepard NT, Telian SA. Programmatic vestibular rehabilitation. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 1995;112: 173–182. pmid:7816453
- 72. Han BI, Song HS, Kim JS. Vestibular rehabilitation therapy: review of indications, mechanisms, and key exercises. J Clin Neurol. 2011;7: 184–196. pmid:22259614
- 73. Suppa A, Asci F, Guerra A. Transcranial magnetic stimulation as a tool to induce and explore plasticity in humans. Handb Clin Neurol. 2022;184: 73–89. pmid:35034759
- 74. Macedo-Lima M, Remage-Healey L. Dopamine Modulation of Motor and Sensory Cortical Plasticity among Vertebrates. Integr Comp Biol. 2021;61: 316–336. pmid:33822047
- 75. Hull C, Regehr WG. The Cerebellar Cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2022;45: 151–175. pmid:35803588
- 76. Taubert M, Mehnert J, Pleger B, Villringer A. Rapid and specific gray matter changes in M1 induced by balance training. Neuroimage. 2016;133: 399–407. pmid:26994831
- 77. Menzocchi M, Mecacci G, Zeppi A, Carli G, Santarcangelo EL. Hypnotizability and Performance on a Prism Adaptation Test. Cerebellum. 2015;14: 699–706. pmid:25913127
- 78. Martin TA, Keating JG, Goodkin HP, Bastian AJ, Thach WT. Throwing while looking through prisms. I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. Brain. 1996;119 (Pt 4): 1183–1198. pmid:8813282
- 79. Martin TA, Keating JG, Goodkin HP, Bastian AJ, Thach WT. Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. Brain. 1996;119 (Pt 4): 1199–1211. pmid:8813283
- 80. González-Tapia D, Velázquez-Zamora DA, Olvera-Cortés ME, González-Burgos I. The motor learning induces plastic changes in dendritic spines of Purkinje cells from the neocerebellar cortex of the rat. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2015;33: 639–645. pmid:25720541
- 81.
Brooks VB, Thach WT. Cerebellar Control of Posture and Movement. Comprehensive Physiology. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2011. pp. 877–946.
- 82. Andre P, d’Ascanio P, Manzoni D, Pompeiano O. Depression of the vestibulospinal reflex by intravermal microinjection of GABAA and GABAB agonists in the decerebrate cat. J Vestib Res. 1994;4: 251–268. pmid:7921344
- 83. Mauritz KH, Dichgans J, Hufschmidt A. Quantitative analysis of stance in late cortical cerebellar atrophy of the anterior lobe and other forms of cerebellar ataxia. Brain. 1979;102: 461–482. pmid:315255
- 84. Colnaghi S, Honeine J-L, Sozzi S, Schieppati M. Body Sway Increases After Functional Inactivation of the Cerebellar Vermis by cTBS. Cerebellum. 2017;16: 1–14. pmid:26780373
- 85. Foerster Á, Melo L, Mello M, Castro R, Shirahige L, Rocha S, et al. Cerebellar Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (ctDCS) Impairs Balance Control in Healthy Individuals. Cerebellum. 2017;16: 872–875. pmid:28456902
- 86. Emadi Andani M, Villa-Sánchez B, Raneri F, Dametto S, Tinazzi M, Fiorio M. Cathodal Cerebellar tDCS Combined with Visual Feedback Improves Balance Control. Cerebellum. 2020;19: 812–823. pmid:32734378
- 87. Manzoni D, Andre P, d’Ascanio P, Pompeiano O. Depression of the vestibulospinal reflex adaptation by intravermal microinjection of GABA-A and GABA-B agonists in the cat. Arch Ital Biol. 1994;132: 243–269. pmid:7893198
- 88. Ehsani F, Ahmadi M, Masoudian N, Jaberzadeh S. Priming of postural training with cerebellar anodal transcranial direct current stimulation for its effects on postural balance and fear of falling in patients with multiple sclerosis: A randomized, double-blind, sham-controlled study. J Clin Neurosci. 2022;99: 294–301. pmid:35325728
- 89. Yosephi MH, Ehsani F, Zoghi M, Jaberzadeh S. Multi-session anodal tDCS enhances the effects of postural training on balance and postural stability in older adults with high fall risk: Primary motor cortex versus cerebellar stimulation. Brain Stimul. 2018;11: 1239–1250. pmid:30017699
- 90. Kolb FP, Lachauer S, Maschke M, Timmann D. Classically conditioned postural reflex in cerebellar patients. Exp Brain Res. 2004;158: 163–179. pmid:15170524
- 91. Pompeiano O, Manzoni D, d’Ascanio P, Andre P. Injections of beta-noradrenergic substances in the cerebellar anterior vermis of cats affect adaptation of the vestibulospinal reflex gain. Arch Ital Biol. 1994;132: 117–145. pmid:7979861
- 92. Pompeiano O, Manzoni D, D’Ascanio P, Andre P. Noradrenergic agents in the cerebellar vermis affect adaptation of the vestibulospinal reflex gain. Brain Res Bull. 1994;35: 433–444. pmid:7859100
- 93. Olson L, Fuxe K. On the projections from the locus coeruleus noradrealine neurons: the cerebellar innervation. Brain Res. 1971;28: 165–171. pmid:4104275
- 94. Foote SL, Bloom FE, Aston-Jones G. Nucleus locus ceruleus: new evidence of anatomical and physiological specificity. Physiol Rev. 1983;63: 844–914. pmid:6308694
- 95. Guyot J-P, Sigrist A, Pelizzone M, Kos MI. Adaptation to steady-state electrical stimulation of the vestibular system in humans. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol. 2011;120: 143–149. pmid:21510138
- 96. Watson SR, Colebatch JG. Vestibulocollic reflexes evoked by short-duration galvanic stimulation in man. J Physiol. 1998;513 (Pt 2): 587–597. pmid:9807006
- 97. Fitzpatrick RC, Day BL. Probing the human vestibular system with galvanic stimulation. J Appl Physiol (1985). 2004;96: 2301–2316. pmid:15133017
- 98. Wallace JW, Rasman BG, Dalton BH. Vestibular-Evoked Responses Indicate a Functional Role for Intrinsic Foot Muscles During Standing Balance. Neuroscience. 2018;377: 150–160. pmid:29524635
- 99. Chen Y-S, Zhou S. Soleus H-reflex and its relation to static postural control. Gait Posture. 2011;33: 169–178. pmid:21211976
- 100. Chalmers GR, Knutzen KM. Soleus H-reflex gain in healthy elderly and young adults when lying, standing, and balancing. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2002;57: B321–329. pmid:12145358
- 101. Mynark RG, Koceja DM. Down training of the elderly soleus H reflex with the use of a spinally induced balance perturbation. J Appl Physiol (1985). 2002;93: 127–133. pmid:12070195
- 102.
Horak FB, Macpherson JM. Postural Orientation and Equilibrium. Comprehensive Physiology. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2011. pp. 255–292.
- 103. Day BL, Guerraz M. Feedforward versus feedback modulation of human vestibular-evoked balance responses by visual self-motion information. J Physiol. 2007;582: 153–161. pmid:17446222