Figures
Abstract
Solutions to environmental and social problems are often framed in dichotomous ways, which can be counterproductive. Instead, multiple solutions are often needed to fully address these problems. Here we examine how framing influences people’s preference for multiple solutions. In a pre-registered experiment, participants (N = 1,432) were randomly assigned to one of four framing conditions. In the first three conditions, participants were presented with a series of eight problems, each framed with multiple causes, multiple impacts, or multiple solutions to the problem. The control condition did not present any framing information. Participants indicated their preferred solution, perceived severity and urgency of the problem, and their dichotomous thinking tendency. Pre-registered analyses showed that none of the three frames had a significant impact on preference for multiple solutions, perceived severity, perceived urgency, or dichotomous thinking. However, exploratory analyses showed that perceived severity and urgency of the problem were positively correlated with people’s preference for multiple solutions, while dichotomous thinking was negatively correlated. These findings showed no demonstrable impact of framing on multi-solution preference. Future interventions should focus on addressing perceived severity and urgency, or decreasing dichotomous thinking to encourage people to adopt multiple solutions to address complex environmental and social problems.
Citation: Wu JS-T, Kremen C, Zhao J (2023) How does framing influence preference for multiple solutions to societal problems? PLoS ONE 18(5): e0285793. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0285793
Editor: Denis Alves Coelho, Jonkoping University, SWEDEN
Received: January 18, 2023; Accepted: May 2, 2023; Published: May 17, 2023
Copyright: © 2023 Wu et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: The dataset and the R code for this study are available on OSF: https://osf.io/k4ud5/.
Funding: The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Solutions to environmental and social problems (e.g., climate change, land management) are often framed in a dichotomous fashion [1–3]. For example, solutions to climate change are framed as either government action, such as enacting environmental regulations and implementing carbon taxes, or individual action, such as driving less, flying less, and eating less meat [4–6]. In reality, both government and individual actions are generally needed and neither should come to replace the other [7]. The dichotomous frame of solutions is partly driven by dichotomous thinking, that is, the tendency to categorize things, people, and events into two contrasting categories [8–10]. Dichotomous thinking is pervasive in all sectors of society: the two dominant political parties corresponding to left and right-wing political ideologies in many countries [11], the characterization of cultures as either Eastern or Western [12], and the scientific tendency to classify results as significant or not, based on an arbitrary threshold, the p-value [13]. Dichotomous thinking simplifies the complexities of reality (many shades of grey) into two discrete opposing categories (black versus white), which reduces cognitive load and allows humans to make decisions more efficiently [10].
However, in the context of solving important societal problems, dichotomous thinking has considerable downsides. Taking climate change mitigation as an example, its division into government and individual actions can be counterproductive, since effective climate action requires cooperation among all sectors of society [14]. Another example is the encroachment of agriculture on natural lands which subsequently leads to biodiversity loss. Two contrasting actions have been proposed: land-sparing, which is limiting highly intensive agriculture across a small area of land to spare land for nature, and land-sharing, which is practicing wildlife-friendly, less-intensive agriculture across a large area of land [15, 16]. However, a growing body of evidence suggests that both approaches are needed, and relying on just one or the other can lead to suboptimal outcomes for both agricultural productivity and biodiversity [2, 17, 18]. These examples not only highlight the flaws of dichotomous thinking, but also the importance of a multi-solution approach to address complex problems.
Thus, there is a need to develop framing interventions to reduce dichotomous thinking and promote a multi-solution approach. However, dichotomous thinking has been primarily studied as a personality trait predictive of mental and personality disorders [8, 19, 20], rather than in the context of environmental and social problems, or as an intervention point for behavioral change [21]. This said, previous research has examined framing as a behavioral intervention to promote the awareness of and action on important societal problems [22, 23]. Many past framing interventions aimed to reduce the psychological distance between the individual and the problem, with the idea being if a problem becomes more personally relatable, there is a higher chance that the individual will act to address it [22, 24]. For example, to elicit individual awareness and action on climate change, some studies use local frames which emphasize the impact of local extreme weather resulting from climate change instead of global increases in temperature and sea levels [25, 26]. Other studies frame climate change as a personal health problem, highlighting its relationship with worsening air quality and the spread of infectious diseases, instead of presenting it as an environmental problem [27, 28]. Aside from making a problem more relatable, these framings could also help people see a broader picture and understand different sides of a problem [29, 30], which is useful in reducing dichotomous thinking.
Knowing this, multi-cause, multi-impact, or multi-solution framing might be employed encourage a multi-solution approach. The multi-cause frame highlights that complex problems are the result of multiple causes, which promotes a broader understanding of a problem [31] and reveals the consequences of personal choices [32]. Understanding these subtleties can allow people to understand how each aspect of a problem needs to addressed using a unique solution [31, 33]. The multi-impact frame emphasizes how there are multiple impacts caused by a problem, which could increase the personal relatability of a problem, since at least one of the impacts might be relevant to a person’s daily life [34, 35]. Furthermore, being exposed to information on multiple impacts could increase people’s concern for a problem, which makes them more likely to support a wide range of measures to tackle a problem [35–37]. Lastly, the multi-solution frame explicitly points out multiple solutions to a complex problem. This frame could address people’s lack of knowledge on solutions, as well as outline the trade-offs between different solutions, underscoring how single solutions are inadequate [38].
The goal of the current study is to examine how framing influences people’s preference for multiple solutions to societal problems and whether it changes people’s tendencies towards dichotomous thinking. Specifically, we conducted a pre-registered experiment where we designed and tested three frames against a control condition: a multi-solution frame that explicitly calls for the need to use multiple solutions for a problem, a multi-cause frame that presents the multiple causes of a problem, and a multi-impact frame that presents the multiple negative impacts of a problem. In the experiment, participants were randomly assigned to one of the four conditions (control, multi-cause, multi-impact or multi-solution frame) and read descriptions of eight environmental and social problems presented in a random order (see S1 File). The eight problems included climate change [4], crop yield and biodiversity [2], food waste [39], plastic pollution [40], homelessness [41], police reform [42], public education [43], and early pandemic response [44]. The problems were chosen to represent a diverse and timely assortment of societal problems. Due to the severity, pressing urgency, and complexity of these problems, a multi-solution approach is necessary in each case. After reading each problem, participants rated the perceived severity and perceived urgency of the problem, and indicated their preferred solution to address the problem among four options. The four options included two distinct solutions that were usually portrayed in dichotomous ways, a multi-solution option, and neither of the first two solutions. Participants’ choice of the multi-solution option was the primary measure of interest. At the end of the experiment, participants completed the Dichotomous Thinking Inventory which was a validated questionnaire to measure their level of dichotomous thinking [45].
Materials and methods
Pre-registration and ethics approval
The experiment was pre-registered on the Open Science Framework (OSF): https://osf.io/gx64r. The study was approved by the University of British Columbia’s Behavioral Research Ethics Board (ID: H22-01832). All participants provided written informed consent prior to their participation in the study. All statistical analyses were conducted in R Version 4.2.1. The dataset and the R code are available on OSF: https://osf.io/k4ud5/.
Participants
Data collection for this study occurred in August 2022. A total of 1,630 participants completed the study without failing the attention check. After applying our pre-registered exclusion criteria, a final sample of 1,432 participants were included in the analysis. This number slightly exceeded the required sample size of 1,424 from an a priori power analysis (conducted in G*Power Version 3.1.9.6) for a one-way ANOVA, where we assumed a minimum effect size of d = 0.1, alpha = 0.05, and power = 0.9. This study was conducted on Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk), an online platform where participants received payment for completing surveys and other tasks. In our study, we required MTurk participants to be based in the U.S. and have an approval rate greater than or equal to 95%. A majority of our participants were female (54.49%), white (81.18%), and highly educated (73.74% had a bachelor’s degree or above). Participants were on average middle-age (M = 38.53, SD = 12.21), liberal leaning (45.57% liberal, 35.81% conservative), and had a median annual personal income of US$31,754. There were no significant differences in any of the demographic variables between conditions.
Procedures
We used a randomized between-subjects design with four conditions (control, multi-cause, multi-impact, and multi-solution) to examine the effect of the framing on preference for the multi-solution option, perceived severity, perceived urgency, and level of dichotomous thinking. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the four conditions. In all four conditions, participants were presented with information on eight societal problems (climate change, crop yield and biodiversity, food waste, plastic pollution, homelessness, police reform, public education, early pandemic response) in a random order (see S1 File). Because this study involved U.S. participants, these problems were presented and tailored to a U.S. context. In the control condition (N = 356), participants were presented with a short paragraph presenting an overview of a given problem in neutral language with several bullet points presenting multiple facts about the problem. In the multi-cause condition (N = 358), participants were presented with the same paragraph with bullet points describing the multiple causes of the problem. In the multi-impact condition (N = 361), participants were presented with the same paragraph with bullet points describing the multiple impacts of the problem. In the multi-solution condition (N = 357), participants were presented with the same paragraph with bullet points describing the multiple solutions to the problem. For each problem, the description and bullet points contained the same number of words across the four conditions. To make sure participants read each problem, we added a manipulation check where participants had to arrange the bullet points in the order they were presented. Participants could only proceed when they had correctly ordered the bullet points. After reading the problem, participants indicated the perceived severity and perceived urgency of the problem, and chose their preferred solution among four options (see S1 File). The four options included two distinct solutions that were framed in dichotomous ways, a multi-solution option, and neither of the first two solutions. At the end of the experiment, participants completed the Dichotomous Thinking Inventory which was a validated questionnaire to measure their level of dichotomous thinking [42], and answered some demographic questions.
Measures
Perceived severity.
Perceived severity, a measure of how severe participants think a problem is, was measured on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = not severe at all, 2 = slightly severe, 3 = somewhat severe, 4 = moderately severe, 5 = considerably severe, 6 = very severe, 7 = extremely severe). Average perceived severity was obtained by averaging the perceived severity of the eight problems for each participant.
Perceived urgency.
Perceived urgency, a measure of how urgent participants think a problem is, was measured on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = not urgent at all, 2 = slightly urgent, 3 = somewhat urgent, 4 = moderately urgent, 5 = considerably urgent, 6 = very urgent, 7 = extremely urgent). Average perceived urgency was obtained by averaging the perceived urgency of the eight problems for each participant.
Preferred solution.
Preferred solution is the primary pre-registered measure and is participant’s choice of solution to a given problem out of the four options. Option 1 and Option 2 were binary opposing solutions (e.g., government action and individual action for climate change). Option 3 is the multi-solution option, worded as “Multiple solutions are needed in addition to 1 and 2”. Option 4 is neither, which is worded as “Neither 1 nor 2”. For each participant, we calculated the total number of multi-solution options chosen out of the eight problems. The total number of multi-solution options chosen is referred to as preference for multiple solutions in the analysis.
Level of dichotomous thinking.
The level of dichotomous thinking was measured using the Dichotomous Thinking Inventory [42], which is a questionnaire consisting of 15 statements (see S2 File). Participants rated how much they agreed with each item on a 6-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 6 = strongly agree). The level of dichotomous thinking was obtained by averaging the scores of the 15 ratings. The Dichotomous Thinking Inventory was completed after participants have responded to the eight problems, and is therefore a measure of a person’s tendency towards dichotomous thinking, instead of serving as the baseline level of dichotomous thinking.
Demographic covariates.
We collected demographic information on gender, age, ethnicity, political orientation, education level, annual household income, and household size as demographic variables (see S3 File). Annual personal income was calculated by dividing annual household income by the square root of household size.
Results
Pre-registered analyses
To examine the first pre-registered hypothesis (participants who are presented with the multi-cause, multi-impact, and multi-solution frame will be more likely to choose the multi-solution option compared to the control condition), we conducted a Kruskal-Wallis test to compare the number of multi-solution choices between the four conditions (Fig 1a). This is because our data violated assumptions of normality (Shapiro-Wilk: W = 0.90, p < .001), so a Kruskal-Wallis test was used instead of a one-way ANOVA. The Kruskal-Wallis test showed no significant difference between conditions (χ2(3) = 6.21, p = .10), which means that the hypothesis was not supported. Per our pre-registration, we also conducted a one-way ANCOVA to examine the effect of demographic covariates (gender, age, ethnicity, political orientation, education level, and annual personal income); in line with the Kruskal-Wallis test, there was no significant main effect of condition (F(3,1428) = 1.71, p = .16), while all six demographic covariates were significant (see S1 Table in S4 File). Fig 1a shows the number for multiple solution choices in each condition (control: M = 4.60, SE = .15; multi-cause: M = 4.20, SE = .15; multi-impact: M = 4.27, SE = .15; multi-solution: M = 4.58, SE = .15).
(a) Preference for multi-solutions, which is defined as the number of multi-solution option chosen out of the eight problems in each condition. (b) Average perceived severity in each condition. (c) Average perceived urgency in each condition. (d) Dichotomous thinking in each condition. Error bars represent ±1 standard error.
To examine the second pre-registered hypothesis (perceived severity and urgency will be higher for participants who are presented with the multi-impact frame, followed by the multi-cause and multi-solution frame, and finally the control condition), we conducted a Kruskal-Wallis test to compare the perceived severity between the four conditions (Fig 1b), due to violation of assumptions of normality (Shapiro-Wilk: W = 0.98, p < .001). The Kruskal-Wallis test showed no significant difference between conditions (χ2(3) = 1.18, p = .76), which means that the hypothesis was not supported. Per our pre-registration, we also conducted a one-way ANCOVA to examine the effect of demographic covariates; there was no significant main effect of condition (F(3,1428) = 0.42, p = .74), while gender, ethnicity, and political orientation were significant covariates (see S2 Table in S4 File). Fig 1b shows the average perceived severity of the eight problems in each condition (control: M = 5.00, SE = .06; multi-cause: M = 4.91, SE = .07; multi-impact: M = 4.97, SE = .06; multi-solution: M = 4.99, SE = .06).
We also conducted a Kruskal-Wallis test to compare the perceived urgency between the four conditions (Fig 1c), due to violation of assumptions of normality (Shapiro-Wilk: W = 0.98, p < .001). The Kruskal-Wallis test showed no significant difference between conditions (χ2(3) = 2.83, p = .42), which means that the hypothesis was not supported. Per our pre-registration, we also conducted a one-way ANCOVA to examine the effect of demographic covariates; there was no significant main effect of condition (F(3,1428) = 0.91, p = .44), while gender, ethnicity, and political orientation were significant covariates (see S3 Table in S4 File). Fig 1c shows the average perceived urgency of the eight problems in each condition (control: M = 5.05, SE = .06; multi-cause: M = 4.89, SE = .06; multi-impact: M = 4.98, SE = .06; multi-solution: M = 4.98, SE = .06).
Overall, the pre-registered analyses show that there was no significant difference in the preference for multiple solutions, perceived severity, and perceived urgency between conditions. This suggests that the framing of multiple causes, impacts, or solutions had little influence on the multi-solution preference.
Exploratory analyses
As exploratory analyses, we examined whether framing had any impact on dichotomous thinking (Fig 1d). A Kruskal-Wallis test revealed no significant difference between conditions (χ2(3) = 0.47, p = .93). Fig 1d shows the average level of dichotomous thinking in each condition (control: M = 4.23, SE = .05; multi-cause: M = 4.19, SE = .05; multi-impact: M = 4.20, SE = .05; multi-solution: M = 4.18, SE = .05).
We further examined the proportion of participants who chose each option for each of the eight problems (see S1 Fig in S4 File). For almost every problem, a majority of participants (between 50% and 60%) chose the multi-solution option, while the rest were split between one of the two binary options. These patterns were consistent across all four conditions, and showed that the participants generally preferred multiple solutions. We conducted chi-squared tests for the eight problems to see whether the percentage of participants that chose the multi-solution option differed between the four conditions. The results showed that there was no significant difference between conditions for any problem (see S4 Table in S4 File). In addition, to see whether the participants became more or less likely to choose the multi-solution option as they proceeded through the study, we examined the percentage of participants that choose each option in temporal sequence (i.e., item number 1 represents the first problem a participant is presented). We found that the participants did not seem to gravitate toward certain options as time went on (see S2 Fig in S4 File).
To examine what predicts the preference for multiple solutions, we ran a series of correlations between perceived severity, perceived urgency, dichotomous thinking and preference for multiple solutions. The correlations (see S5 Table in S4 File) showed that preference for multiple solutions was positively correlated with perceived severity (control: r = .25, p < .001; multi-cause: r = .21, p < .001; multi-impact: r = .16, p < .001; multi-solution: r = .29, p < .001), positively correlated with perceived urgency (control: r = .28, p < .001; multi-cause: r = .24, p < .001; multi-impact: r = .18, p < .001; multi-solution: r = .29, p < .001), but negatively correlated with dichotomous thinking (control: r = −.10, p = .001; multi-cause: r = −.12, p = .002; multi-impact: r = −.13, p < .001; multi-solution: r = −.12, p = .002).
To understand how demographic variables were associated with the preference for multiple solutions, we ran a series of correlations between demographic variables and preference for multiple solutions. The correlations (see S5 Table in S4 File; see also S3 Fig in S4 File) showed that there was a negative correlation between political orientation and preference for multiple solutions in most conditions (control, r = −.16, p < .001; multi-impact, r = −.15, p < .001; multi-solution, r = −.1, p = .014), meaning that the more conservative a participant was, the less likely they were to choose multi-solution options. There was also a negative correlation between education level and preference for multiple solutions in most conditions (multi-cause condition, r = −.11, p = .01; multi-impact condition, r = −.17, p < .001; multi-solution condition, r = −.14, p < .001), meaning that the more highly educated a participant was, the less likely they were to choose multi-solution options.
The final exploratory analysis was a multiple regression (GLM) with binomial distribution. In this GLM, the eight problems were computed as eight trials, where choosing a multi-solution option counts as a success, while all other choices (binary options, neither) count as a failure. The purpose of this GLM is to run a multiple regression with all variables measured in this study, as well as providing a more robust alternative to ANCOVA. The results (see S6 Table in S4 File) are generally consistent with the aforementioned analyses.
Discussion
The current study examined the influence of framing on people’s preference for a multi-solution approach to societal problems. We found framing the multiple causes, impacts, or solutions of the problem had little impact on the preference for multiple solutions, perceived severity, or perceived urgency of the problem. This null result is largely driven by the high baseline of preference for multiple solutions, as can be seen in the majority of participants that preferred the multi-solution option in the control condition (Fig 1). There are several possible reasons behind this high baseline. First, participants might have thought that employing more solutions is more effective when addressing a complex problem [46, 47]. This “more is better” mindset can be further compounded by a person’s concern for a problem (i.e., more solutions feel more assuring), and the lack of resource constraints (i.e., without financial constraints it feels intuitive to employ all possible solutions), which can further encourage people to choose the multi-solution option by default [48, 49]. Second, many participants might have preferred not to take a side in these problems, a lot of which are contentious and potentially damaging to interpersonal relationships [50, 51]. Under such a mindset, choosing the multi-solution option might have allowed them to feel less divisive. Third, many participants might have not known enough about the problems or solutions, thus defaulting to the middle option out of uncertainty [52]. Lastly, high preference for multiple solutions might be partially attributed to survey design. Specifically, choosing the multi-solution option might have required less cognitive effort due to the way the options are worded. Because the multi-solution option is shorter than the two binary solutions (see S1 File), it is possible that after a quick reading, participants who considered the two binary solutions somewhat plausible would be inclined to choose the multi-solution option, which is shorter, instead of carefully considering the two binary solutions, which requires more effort. Furthermore, the text for the multi-solution option was identical throughout the eight problems in the survey (see S1 File), which could mean it required less effort to choose the multi-solution option by default, instead of considering the set binary solutions which are unique to each problem. In addition, on a more fundamental level, the null results in the preference for multiple solutions, perceived severity, or perceived urgency of the problem could also be attributed to the small effect size of framing interventions aimed at reducing psychological distance. In fact, according to one recent study, there is weak empirical support for the effectiveness of such interventions [53].
An exploratory finding was that higher perceived severity and urgency was correlated with a stronger preference for multiple solutions, and higher dichotomous thinking was correlated with a weaker preference for multiple solutions. These patterns were consistent across all conditions. This suggests that an individual’s level of concern and perception of the problem may be a better predictor for multi-solution preference than how the problem was framed. Future research should focus on interventions that appeal to people’s sense of severity and urgency towards certain problems, as well as how addressing this sense of severity and urgency benefits their well-being. Relevant studies have been done on risk perception. For example, one study found that a framing intervention highlighting specific threats to online privacy increased perceived severity and caused participants to take a more proactive role in guarding their personal information [54]. In other studies, severity framing highlighting known risks to patient and public health increased patient adherence to taking prescription drugs and following public health protocols [55, 56]. Future research on preference for multiple solutions can use similar interventions to highlight perceived severity and urgency to encourage action. Moreover, since risk perception is often associated with anxiety, further studies can examine how interventions on anxiety drive behavior change. For example, in some studies, participants with higher self-reported climate anxiety are more likely to engage in pro-environmental behavior [57].
In addition to perceived severity and urgency, our results suggest that reducing dichotomous thinking, or in other words, broadening thinking patterns, could assist in promoting preference for multiple solutions that may be more likely to succeed in addressing complex societal and environmental issues. However, a research gap exists on behavioral or educational interventions to encouraging broader thinking and reducing dichotomous thinking; previous studies focused on dichotomous thinking as a personality trait that is a predictor for borderline personality disorder [19, 58], eating disorders [59], and depression [19, 60]. Improving emotional well-being could be one way of reducing dichotomous thinking. For example, politically polarized individuals who often exhibit dichotomous tendencies, tend to report lower levels of emotional well-being [61]. Some interventions, such as reducing the time spent on social media and news can help reduce polarization, and therefore dichotomous thinking, as well as improving emotional well-being [62]. However, changing dichotomous thinking may ultimately require more than behavioral interventions, starting with changes in the education system to include a more multifaceted and diverse curriculum in schools [63, 64], improving science communication regarding complex and often counterintuitive issues [65], systemic changes to improve equality and inclusivity [12], as well as reducing political polarization [66]. These larger system-level changes could contribute to a less dichotomous sociopolitical environment where people are more open to different perspectives, emotionally balanced, and have less motivation to think dichotomously [67]. These changes are especially important in light the fact that some solutions (e.g., cash transfers to people experiencing homelessness) have been experimentally verified to be effective, yet are shunned by the public due to misconceptions regarding its efficacy or political affiliation that excludes certain solutions regardless of their efficacy.
A surprising exploratory finding in the current study was that the more conservative or more highly educated a participant was, the less likely they were to choose multi-solution options. The fact that conservatives were less receptive to the multi-solution approach may be attributed to the fact that some of these societal problems can be seen as liberal problems. There is generally less support from conservative individuals on climate change or police reform, and conservatives are less likely to think that these problems require urgent action [68]. Previous studies showed that highly educated people tend to show a lower level of dichotomous thinking [10], which is the opposite of what we found. This result may be because the multi-solution option contained a smaller amount of information compared to the two binary options, which listed specific examples. This lack of information could be interpreted as poor information quality, making the multi-solution option less appealing to highly educated participants [69]. Second, while highly educated individuals are generally more comfortable with non-dichotomous thought [70, 71], they are also more likely to engage in more complex thinking that might not have been captured by the limited set of solutions in the current study. For example, a person can fully recognize the importance of both government and individual climate action, but still choose the former over the latter because they believe that government policies make it easier for individuals to engage in climate action, or vice versa [14]. Furthermore, we also did not explicitly ask the participants to consider the information presented in the frames when choosing their preferred solution. This could have caused them to rely more on their existing beliefs and preferences, which could explain why some demographic variables were good predictors of preferred solution.
In conclusion, the current study showed no demonstrable impact of framing on people’s preference for multiple solutions to address societal problems. Future interventions should instead focus on helping people deal with the sense of severity and urgency they feel towards certain problems, or decreasing dichotomous thinking to encourage people to adopt multiple solutions to address complex environmental and social problems.
Supporting information
S1 File. Eight environmental and social problems presented in the four conditions.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0285793.s001
(DOCX)
References
- 1. Bouman T, Verschoor M, Albers CJ, Böhm G, Fisher SD, Poortinga W, et al. When worry about climate change leads to climate action: How values, worry and personal responsibility relate to various climate actions. Global Environmental Change. 2020 May 1;62: 102061.
- 2. Kremen C. Reframing the land-sparing/land-sharing debate for biodiversity conservation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2015 Oct;1355(1): 52–76.
- 3. Simonson J. Police reform through a power lens. The Yale Law Journal. 2020;130: 778.
- 4. Mees HL, Uittenbroek CJ, Hegger DL, Driessen PP. From citizen participation to government participation: An exploration of the roles of local governments in community initiatives for climate change adaptation in the Netherlands. Environmental Policy and Governance. 2019 May;29(3): 198–208.
- 5. Pedersen RL, Lam DP. Second comment on ‘The climate mitigation gap: Education and government recommendations miss the most effective individual actions’. Environmental Research Letters. 2017 Jul 12;12(7): 074024.
- 6. Wynes S, Zhao J, Donner SD. How well do people understand the climate impact of individual actions? Climatic Change. 2020 Oct;162: 1521–1534.
- 7. Chater N, Loewenstein G. The i-frame and the s-frame: How focusing on individual-level solutions has led behavioral public policy astray. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2022 Sep 5: 1–60. pmid:36059098
- 8. Jonason PK, Oshio A, Shimotsukasa T, Mieda T, Csathó Á, Sitnikova M. Seeing the world in black or white: The Dark Triad traits and dichotomous thinking. Personality and Individual Differences. 2018 Jan 1;120: 102–106.
- 9. Klapper A, Dotsch R, van Rooij I, Wigboldus D. Four meanings of “categorization”: A conceptual analysis of research on person perception. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2017 Aug;11(8): e12336.
- 10. Mieda T, Taku K, Oshio A. Dichotomous thinking and cognitive ability. Personality and Individual Differences. 2021 Feb 1;169: 110008.
- 11. Rhodes-Purdy M, Navarre R, Utych SM. Populist psychology: economics, culture, and emotions. The Journal of Politics. 2021 Oct 1;83(4): 1559–1572.
- 12. le Ha P, Mohamad A. The making and transforming of a transnational in dialog: Confronting dichotomous thinking in knowledge production, identity formation, and pedagogy. Research in Comparative and International Education. 2020 Sep;15(3): 197–216.
- 13. Amrhein V, Greenland S, McShane B. Scientists rise up against statistical significance. Nature. 2019 Mar 2. https://www.nature.com/articles/d41586-019-00857-9 pmid:30894741
- 14. Cole DH. Advantages of a polycentric approach to climate change policy. Nature Climate Change. 2015 Feb;5(2): 114–118.
- 15. Egan JF, Mortensen DA. A comparison of land-sharing and land-sparing strategies for plant richness conservation in agricultural landscapes. Ecological applications. 2012 Mar;22(2): 459–471. pmid:22611847
- 16. Phalan B, Onial M, Balmford A, Green RE. Reconciling food production and biodiversity conservation: land sharing and land sparing compared. Science. 2011;333: 1289–1291. pmid:21885781
- 17. Crespin SJ, Simonetti JA. Reconciling farming and wild nature: Integrating human–wildlife coexistence into the land-sharing and land-sparing framework. Ambio. 2019;48: 131–138. pmid:29752683
- 18. Grass I, Batáry P, Tscharntke T. Combining land-sparing and land-sharing in European landscapes. Advances in Ecological Research. 2021;64: 251–303.
- 19. Bonfá-Araujo B, Oshio A, Hauck-Filho N. Seeing Things in Black-and-White: A Scoping Review on Dichotomous Thinking Style. Japanese Psychological Research. 2022 Oct;64(4): 461–472.
- 20. Mieda T, Oshio A. Dichotomous Thinking and Personality Traits: From the Viewpoints of the Big Five and HEXACO. Japanese Psychological Research. 2021 Jul;63(3): 219–226.
- 21. Lu FC, Sinha J. Dichotomous thinking and ethical consumerism: Overcoming the negative effects of self-interest bias through third-person narrative persuasion. Journal of Advertising. 2019 May 27;48(3):271–283.
- 22. Brügger A. Understanding the psychological distance of climate change: The limitations of construal level theory and suggestions for alternative theoretical perspectives. Global Environmental Change. 2020 Jan 1;60: 102023.
- 23. Harackiewicz JM, Priniski SJ. Improving student outcomes in higher education: The science of targeted intervention. Annual Review of Psychology. 2018;69: 409–435. pmid:28934586
- 24. Loy LS, Spence A. Reducing, and bridging, the psychological distance of climate change. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2020 Feb 1;67: 101388.
- 25. Degeling D, Koolen R. Communicating Climate Change to a Local but Diverse Audience: On the Positive Impact of Locality Framing. Environmental Communication. 2022 Feb 17;16(2): 243–261.
- 26. Spence A, Pidgeon N. Framing and communicating climate change: The effects of distance and outcome frame manipulations. Global Environmental Change. 2010 Oct 1;20(4): 656–67.
- 27. Rossa-Roccor V, Giang A, Kershaw P. Framing climate change as a human health issue: enough to tip the scale in climate policy? The Lancet Planetary Health. 2021 Aug 1;5(8): e553–9. pmid:34390673
- 28. Stevenson KT, King TL, Selm KR, Peterson MN, Monroe MC. Framing climate change communication to prompt individual and collective action among adolescents from agricultural communities. Environmental Education Research. 2018 Mar 4;24(3): 365–377.
- 29. Badullovich N, Grant WJ, Colvin RM. Framing climate change for effective communication: a systematic map. Environmental Research Letters. 2020 Dec 1;15(12): 123002.
- 30. Luo Y, Zhao J. Attentional and perceptual biases of climate change. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2021 Dec 1;42: 22–26.
- 31. Soucy A, De Urioste-Stone S, Rahimzadeh-Bajgiran P, Weiskittel A. Drivers of Climate Change Risk Perceptions among Diverse Forest Stakeholders in Maine, USA. Society & Natural Resources. 2022 May 4;35(5): 467–486.
- 32. Brownstein M, Kelly D, Madva A. (2022). Individualism, Structuralism, and Climate Change. Environmental Communication. 2022 Feb 17;16(2): 269–288.
- 33. Lee TM, Markowitz EM, Howe PD, Ko CY, Leiserowitz AA. Predictors of public climate change awareness and risk perception around the world. Nature Climate Change. 2015 Nov;5(11): 1014–1020.
- 34. Grant S, Tamason CC, Jensen PKM. Climatization: A critical perspective of framing disasters as climate change events. Climate Risk Management. 2015 Jan 1;10: 27–34.
- 35. Zhu W, Yao N, Guo Q, Wang F. Public risk perception and willingness to mitigate climate change: city smog as an example. Environmental Geochemistry and Health. 2020 Mar;42: 881–893. pmid:31227948
- 36. Ferguson MA, Branscombe NR. Collective guilt mediates the effect of beliefs about global warming on willingness to engage in mitigation behavior. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2010 Jun 1;30(2): 135–142.
- 37. Lemée C, Fleury-Bahi G, Navarro O. Impact of place identity, self-efficacy and anxiety state on the relationship between coastal flooding risk perception and the willingness to cope. Frontiers in Psychology. 2019 Mar 11;10: 499. pmid:30915001
- 38. Bateman T S, O’Connor K. Felt responsibility and climate engagement: Distinguishing adaptation from mitigation. Global Environmental Change. 2016 Nov 1;41: 206–215.
- 39. Chalak A, Abou-Daher C, Chaaban J, Abiad MG. The global economic and regulatory determinants of household food waste generation: A cross-country analysis. Waste Management. 2016;48: 418–422. pmid:26680687
- 40. MacLeod M, Arp HP, Tekman MB, Jahnke A. The global threat from plastic pollution. Science. 2021;373: 61–65. pmid:34210878
- 41.
Dwyer R, Palepu A, Williams C, Zhao J. Unconditional cash transfers reduce homelessness. PsyArXiv:ukngr [Preprint]. 2021 [cited 2022 Dec 20]. https://psyarxiv.com/ukngr/
- 42. Akbar AA. An abolitionist horizon for (police) reform. California Law Review. 2020;108: 1781.
- 43. Larson S, Chapman S, Spetz J, Brindis CD. Chronic childhood trauma, mental health, academic achievement, and school-based health center mental health services. The Journal of School Health. 2017;87: 675–686. pmid:28766317
- 44. Biggerstaff M, Slayton RB, Johansson MA, Butler JC. Improving pandemic response: Employing mathematical modeling to confront coronavirus disease. Clinical Infectious Diseases. 2022 Mar 1;74(5): 913–917. pmid:34343282
- 45. Oshio A. Development and validation of the dichotomous thinking inventory. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal. 2009;37: 729–741.
- 46. Beattie G, McGuire L. Personality and climate change mitigation: A psychological and semiotic exploration of the sustainable choices of optimists. Semiotica. 2021 Jul 1;2021(241): 237–273.
- 47.
Riehl S. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Environmental Science. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2019.
- 48. Böhm R, Gürerk Ö, Lauer T. Nudging climate change mitigation: a laboratory experiment with inter-generational public goods. Games. 2020;11: 42.
- 49. Gampfer R. Do individuals care about fairness in burden sharing for climate change mitigation? Evidence from a lab experiment. Climatic Change. 2014 May;124(1–2): 65–77.
- 50. Silver I, Shaw A. When and why “staying out of it” backfires in moral and political disagreements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2022;151(10): 2542–2561. pmid:35446106
- 51. Yang H, van de Vliert E, Jehn K. A psychological approach to third-party side-taking in interpersonal conflicts. Organizational Psychology Review. 2018 May;8(2–3): 174–191.
- 52. Sovacool BK, Demski C, Noel L. Beyond climate, culture and comfort in European preferences for low-carbon heat. Global Environmental Change. 2021 Jan 1;66: 102200.
- 53.
Maier M, Bartoš F, Oh M, Wagenmakers, EJ, Shanks D, Harris A. Adjusting for Publication Bias Reveals That Evidence for and Size of Construal Level Theory Effects is Substantially Overestimated. PsyArXiv:r8nyu [Preprint]. 2022 [cited 2023 Mar 24]. https://psyarxiv.com/r8nyu/
- 54. Gerber N, Reinheimer B, Volkamer M. Investigating People’s Privacy Risk Perception. Proceedings on Privacy Enhancing Technologies. 2019;2019(3): 267–288.
- 55. Nie R, Han Y, Xu J, Huang Q, Mao J. Illness perception, risk perception and health promotion self-care behaviors among Chinese patient with type 2 diabetes: A cross-sectional survey. Applied Nursing Research. 2018 Feb 1;39: 89–96. pmid:29422183
- 56. Webster RK, Brooks SK, Smith LE, Woodland L, Wessely S, Rubin GJ. How to improve adherence with quarantine: rapid review of the evidence. Public Health. 2020;182: 163–169. pmid:32334182
- 57. Clayton S. Climate anxiety: Psychological responses to climate change. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2020 Aug 1;74: 102263. pmid:32623280
- 58. Puri P, Kumar D, Muralidharan K, Kishore MT. Individuals with Borderline Personality Disorder manifest cognitive biases implicated in psychosis. Psychiatry Research. 2018;267: 414–419. pmid:29960939
- 59. Linardon J, Messer M. My fitness pal usage in men: Associations with eating disorder symptoms and psychosocial impairment. Eating Behaviors. 2019;33: 13–17. pmid:30772765
- 60. Kawabata T, Abe N, Wakai T. The Effects of Dichotomous Thinking on Depression in Japanese College Students. Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology. 2021;11(1): 1–28.
- 61. Grossmann I, Weststrate NM, Ardelt M, Brienza JP, Dong M, Ferrari M, et al. The science of wisdom in a polarized world: Knowns and unknowns. Psychological Inquiry. 2020 Apr 2;31(2): 103–133.
- 62. Asimovic N, Nagler J, Bonneau R, Tucker JA. Testing the effects of Facebook usage in an ethnically polarized setting. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2021 Jun 22;118(25): e2022819118. pmid:34131075
- 63. Duncan C, Sankey D. Two conflicting visions of education and their consilience. Educational Philosophy and Theory. 2019 Dec 6;51(14): 1454–1464.
- 64. Parsons ER. Why not an integrative and inclusive approach—hands on and “minds on?” A lesson for mentoring 21st century science education researchers. Science Education. 2019 Sep;103(5): 1284–1288.
- 65. Seethaler S, Evans JH, Gere C, Rajagopalan RM. Science, values, and science communication: competencies for pushing beyond the deficit model. Science Communication. 2019 Jun;41(3): 378–388.
- 66. Kubin E, von Sikorski C. The role of (social) media in political polarization: a systematic review. Annals of the International Communication Association. 2021 Jul 3;45(3): 188–206.
- 67. Wu JS, Hauert C, Kremen C, Zhao J. A Framework on Polarization, Cognitive Inflexibility, and Rigid Cognitive Specialization Frontiers in Psychology. 2022;13: 1386. pmid:35401343
- 68.
Pew Research Center. As Economic Concerns Recede, Environmental Protection Rises on the Public’s Policy Agenda. Pew Research Center. 2020, Feburary 13 [cited 2022 Dec 20]. https://www.pewresearch.org/politics/2020/02/13/as-economic-concerns-recede-environmental-protection-rises-on-the-publics-policy-agenda/
- 69. Almahamid S, McAdams AC, AL Kalaldeh T, Al-Sa’eed M. The relationship between perceived usefulness, perceived ease of use, perceived information quality, and intention to use E-government. Journal of Theoretical & Applied Information Technology. 2010 Jan 1; 11.
- 70. Chew S H, Yi J, Zhang J, Zhong S. Education and anomalies in decision making: Experimental evidence from Chinese adult twins. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. 2016 Dec;53: 163–200.
- 71. Toh CA, Miller SR. Choosing creativity: the role of individual risk and ambiguity aversion on creative concept selection in engineering design. Research in Engineering Design. 2016 Jul;27: 195–219.