Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Assessing the aesthetic attractivity of European butterflies: A web-based survey protocol

  • Elia van Tongeren ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Elia van Tongeren, Ginevra Sistri

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations NBFC, National Biodiversity Future Center, Palermo, Italy, Department of Biology, University of Florence, Florence, Italy

  • Ginevra Sistri ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Elia van Tongeren, Ginevra Sistri

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Biology, University of Florence, Florence, Italy

  • Vincenzo Zingaro,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Humanities and Philosophy, University of Florence, Florence, Italy

  • Alessandro Cini,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Biology, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy

  • Leonardo Dapporto,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations NBFC, National Biodiversity Future Center, Palermo, Italy, Department of Biology, University of Florence, Florence, Italy

  • Mariagrazia Portera

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    mariagrazia.portera@unifi.it

    Affiliation Department of Humanities and Philosophy, University of Florence, Florence, Italy

Abstract

Aesthetic attractivity stands as an underestimated yet fundamental feature of species in conservation biology, significantly driving disproportionate protection efforts towards charismatic species. Despite the evidence, few attempts sought to precisely quantify the impact of aesthetic attractivity in defining priority of species for conservation actions (e.g. inclusion in International Union for Conservation of Nature red lists and protection lists). This study protocol describes the setting of an online test (available from April 2022 to April 2023 at www.unveiling.eu) designed to i) quantify the aesthetic attractivity to humans of the 496 European butterfly species and ii) identify which features (both in the perceived animal and in the perceiver) influence the aesthetic attractivity of a given butterfly species. The test is divided in 5 sections (personal data, ranking, single morphological features, emotional engagement, dispositional variables) aimed at profiling the relation each participant has with the species examined. In the long-term, evaluating butterflies’ aesthetic attractivity could facilitate the critical assessment of current conservation strategies, such as the process of selection of flag and umbrella species by research institutions, environmental associations and Non Governative Organizations. This is expected to provide the much-needed evidence to set up unbiased biodiversity conservation strategies and counteract the selective anthropogenic pressure which favours the extinction of unattractive species, being no or less protected compared to charismatic species.

Introduction

Aesthetic values play a substantial role in almost every aspect of human everyday experience [1,2]. This is the case even with domains, apparently more objective and less obviously influenced by human aesthetic choices, such as scientific disciplines (mathematics, physics, biology etc.; see [35]). With specific reference to conservation biology, it has been argued that processes such as the choice of the subjects of interest by conservationists, the choice of representative animal species for raising-awareness projects by NGOs, and even the allocation of public funds for research initiatives are influenced by aesthetic values [615]. As a result, in the last few years the study of aesthetics has started to emerge as a key topic in conservation, as witnessed by a growing amount of research over very wide branches of the tree of life [1625].

It is indeed well known that some groups like birds [16,19,20], coral reef fishes [17] and big mammals [26] are generally recognized by humans as natural beauties. For this reason, they are considered ambassadors of biodiversity (flag species [27,28]) and harnessed by environmental organisations like WWF to gain public support for their campaigns and to motivate people to invest resources in conservation. The same scenario applies to butterflies which constitute a marked exception within insects. Indeed, while insects are generally not considered as popular and charismatic animals [29,30], an extraordinary aesthetic merit is attributed to butterflies (intended as superfamily Papilionoidea) [14,31,32]. This is likely due to their striking and charming colours and forms, their diurnal activity, making us more accustomed to encounters with them, to their “friendly” appearance (butterflies are generally perceived as harmless) and to their increasingly recognized role as pollinators.

Europe hosts 496 butterflies species, whose huge morphological variability in wing patterns, shapes and colours, due to mimetic, thermal and sexual strategies, provides a rich substrate for aesthetic attractivity. Indeed, in the history of Western aesthetics, features such as variation, novelty, extravagance have been traditionally understood as highly aesthetic [3335], as recognized by Charles Darwin himself in his attempt at making sense of the human and non-human aesthetic dimension within the evolutionary framework [3638].

In conservation biology, functional features such as those related to morphology (e.g. body size), feeding (e.g. ingestion rate), life history (e.g. reproduction mode), physiology (e.g. temperature tolerance), behaviour (e.g. dispersal mode) [39] represent the key-features determining a species’ fitness and survival in a given environment. Arguably, each species’ aesthetic attractivity to humans constitute a still underappreciated yet fundamental feature driving disproportionate conservation efforts towards charismatic species [14] and facilitating their persistence in highly human-impacted landscapes (“anthropogenic selection”, [40]). Butterflies seem to suit this framing [14].

Yet, only few studies [4143] have attempted to design protocols to rigorously quantify the aesthetic attractivity (less precisely, "beauty") of butterflies and its potential impact on conservation policies. For this reason, the “Unveiling” research project, led by the University of Florence (Italy), aims to test the hypothesis that aesthetic attractivity to humans increases the chances of survival of endangered butterfly species. In this paper, we describe the protocol of an online test (available at www.unveiling.eu) designed to i) quantify the aesthetic attractivity of European butterflies to humans; ii) identify which features (both in the perceived animal and in the perceiver) influence the aesthetic attractivity of a given butterfly species.

In the long-term, evaluating butterflies’ aesthetic attractivity will provide the much-needed evidence to set up unbiased biodiversity conservation strategies and counteract, therefore, the selective anthropogenic extinction of unattractive species [40,44].

Materials and methods

The first aim of our study is to assess aesthetic attractivity of 496 European butterfly species. Previous studies have mostly addressed the topic of aesthetic attractivity “per se” [1720], i.e. tracing it back to properties in the animal (as “causes” of the perceived attractivity). In our perspective, the aesthetic attractivity of a species should be considered as a relational functional feature, resulting from the interaction between the item/object and the human perceiver. What humans aesthetically like is indeed deeply influenced by what they feel, know, are interested in [45]. For this reason, we first evaluated i) the importance of some butterfly species’ morphological features in influencing the perceived attractivity, and then considered ii) the type and extent of emotional engagement elicited by butterflies’ images in the survey participants and iii) the participants’ dispositions and interests towards natural sciences and aesthetics and the arts.

The test, available for mobile and desktop devices at the link www.unveiling.eu, consists of 6 different parts which are thoroughly described below (to see the full list of questions see S1 Appendix) and summarised in Fig 1.

thumbnail
Fig 1. “Unveiling” test.

In this figure are presented the 5 sections of the online “Unveiling” test. For each section a screenshot, a brief description and the summary of objectives are provided.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0283360.g001

Before starting the test, the participant is presented with an introductory page which provides some preliminary information such as: test duration (about 10 minutes); presence of a timer indicating that a limited amount of time is available for some answers; invitation to spontaneously answer the questions without thinking too much over them; declaration that no personal data are collected (in accordance to the European Regulation 2018/1725); the reward provided to the participant at the end of the test (butterfly pdf guides, reading suggestions); butterfly photo credits (available for download) based on iNaturalist dataset. After these introductory remarks, the participant starts the test, which is divided into five different sections.

The Ethics Committee for Research of the University of Florence with the opinion n. 168 of July 9, 2021 approved the general structure of the project "Identification, evaluation and application potential of the aesthetic dimension in conservation practices: butterflies as case study, between new theoretical ideas and practical applications" and the methods of execution of the same as illustrated by the scientific responsible Mariagrazia Portera and therefore expressed positive opinion on the project.

Furthermore, considering the extent of the project and the fact that no sensitive personal data are collected, the Ethics Committee deemed it appropriate not to require informed consent from participants, be they also under 18 years of age.

Section n. 1: Personal data

It has been shown that factors such as gender, level of education, cultural background, age etc. can substantially influence our aesthetic preferences, appreciation, liking and disliking [4649]. The first section comprises questions aimed at collecting general participant data anonymously: gender (male, female, non-binary), age, nationality, level of studies, employment sector, colour blindness. This section, together with the sections about interests and dispositions (see below), is expected to contribute to unveil the role of personal features in the aesthetic appreciation of endangered animals.

Section n. 2: Ranking

In this section participants are presented with a panel of 9 pictures of different butterfly species, when the user clicks on each picture to rank it, they see that picture enlarged (covering the other previews) with a clickable 0–10 meter to express their choice and they are asked to attribute a score to each of them in response to the question “How beautiful do they look to you?”. For each species we have collected up to 8 images of living individuals in their natural environment, representing all the combinations of: i) sex (male or female), ii) dorsal or ventral view, iii) flower or neutral backgrounds. For monomorphic species, whose sexual morphological differences are indistinguishable in pictures, only four pictures were collected. In some cases it has not been possible to obtain 8 pictures per species. For example, some species rarely visit flowers (e.g. Charaxes jasius) making it impossible to take pictures of them on flowers. Many lateral basking butterflies do not open their wings while resting, so photos of the species’ dorsal view were unavailable (e.g. Gonepteryx spp.). The poor contactability of a few rare species made it impossible to gather all 8 images. The list of butterfly species along with the collected pictures is provided in S1 Table.

To ensure a similar number of tests per species, the probability of a species to be selected is inversely proportional to the times it has been chosen in previous tests. This is done by attributing a random number between 0 and 1 to each species which was summed to the standardised number of times each species was chosen (times chosen divided by maximum number of times chosen in the dataset). For each test, each species potentially ranged from a minimum of 0 to a maximum of 2, and we selected the 9 species showing the lowest values. Once the species are selected, the choice between the pictures referring to that species is random.

No time limit applies to this section of the test; the participant can linger on each image as long as they want, also zooming on each photo to appreciate every detail before providing their score.

The pictures have been obtained by iNaturalist citizen science platform, selecting only the images with a Creative Commons (CC) licence, and the author photo credits list is available for download in the first introductory section of the test. Furthermore, for a little set of species pictures, not available as CC in iNaturalist platform, we directly asked the authors permission. The following websites also provided significant iconographic material: www.farfalleitalia.it; www.leps.it; www.pyrgus.de; www.europebutterflies.com; www.flickr.com; www.lepidoptera.eu; www.lepiforum.org; http://kajsnatur.dk.

Section n. 3: Single morphological features

In this section, 10 pairs of butterfly-drawings created ad hoc are shown to each participant in random positions (left-right). A 10 seconds’ countdown timer is displayed in order to invite the participant to express their preference for one of the two images quickly and instinctively. In each pair, one drawing faithfully represents the morphological-perceptual key-features of a particular species of butterfly, while the other represents the same key-features but altered. As for the features, on the basis of a substantial body of research both in empirical aesthetics and in conservation biology [45,5054], we selected: i) butterfly dimension, ii) colours of the wings contrast intensity, iii) grouping and order of the design patterns of the wings, iv) forewing/hindwing proportion, v) presence or absence of wing eyespots, vi) wing eyespots dimensions, vii) presence or absence of wing tails, viii) wing tail length, ix) smooth or jagged wing edges. In each pair of drawings, every alteration of the aforementioned traits comes from artificial interventions (through MS PowerPoint, Windows vers. 17) meant to highlight the aesthetically relevant features and to comparatively identify their influence on the votes given by the test-takers. The altered butterfly-drawings have no reference whatsoever to real phenotypes.

The species reproduced in the drawings (see Table 1) have been chosen because of the high perceptual perspicuity of the key-features. An example is shown in Fig 2, where two Aglais io drawings are compared, one of them presenting eyespots in the wings (natural aspect, Fig 2A), the other one lacking them (modified butterfly aspect, Fig 2B). All the couples of drawings with respective features analysis are available in S2 Appendix.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Two Aglais io drawings.

(A) One butterfly drawing is with eyespots (natural aspect) (B) and the other is without eyespots (modified aspect) on posterior wings.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0283360.g002

thumbnail
Table 1. Single morphological features analysed in the third section of the “Unveiling” test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0283360.t001

In this table are the single morphological features analysed in the third section of the “Unveiling” test, butterfly species associated with them and number of cases for each feature according to the classification shown in S2 Appendix.

Section n. 4: Emotional engagement

In this section the participant’s emotional engagement is investigated. Emotions significantly affect our aesthetic experiences [5557]. In order to assess the role of emotions in butterfly aesthetic attractivity, we present the participant with a selection of 5 images of butterflies and we ask them to identify the emotion that comes closest to what they feel, also quantifying the emotional intensity in a scale range from 1 to 10. The species selected for this section of the test are representative both in terms of morphological characteristics and phylogenetic diversity, in order to ensure a heterogeneous selection of butterflies that can elicit a wide range of emotions in the participants, based on both peculiar traits and outward appearance of species belonging to different phyla. Relying on previous studies in the field of empirical aesthetics [45,5860], we selected the following emotions: awe, confusion, joy, disgust, fear, cuteness, feeling of challenge or none of these [6163]. This section is time-constrained: the participant is invited to make their choices in 20 seconds, 10 to express their emotion and 10 to quantify it. As for the species represented in each selection of five pictures, we have selected 26 species representative of 5 families of European butterflies: 5 Papilionidae (Iphiclides podalirius, Papilio machaon, Parnassius apollo, Archon apollinus, Zerynthia cassandra), 5 Pieridae (Gonepteryx cleopatra, Colias hyale, Aporia crataegi, Anthocharis cardamines, Leptidea sinapis), 5 Lycaenidae (Lycaena dispar, Callophrys rubi, Satyrium w-album, Agriades orbitulus, Polyommatus icarus), 6 Nymphalidae (Issoria lathonia, Argynnis paphia, Aglais io, Charaxes jasius, Polygonia c-album, Coenonympha oedippus), 5 Hesperiidae (Heteropterus morpheus, Carterocephalus silvicola, Ochlodes sylvanus, Spialia therapne, Pyrgus sidae).

The species listed above cover only the first 2,500 tests that will be carried out. After the first 2,500 answers, the 26 pictures will be substituted by another cluster of 25 pictures portraying the following species (representative of all the 6 families of European butterflies):

2 Papilionidae (Parnassius mnemosyne, Iphiclides feisthamelii), 4 Pieridae (Pieris rapae, Colias alfacariensis, Zegris pyrothoe, Leptidea reali), 4 Lycaenidae (Phengaris arion, Cupido minimus, Lysandra coridon, Polyommatus thersites), 1 Riodinidae (Hamearis lucina), 11 Nymphalidae (Libythea celtis, Limenitis reducta, Boloria euphrosyne, Euphydryas cynthia, Apatura ilia, Danaus chrysippus, Melanargia galathea, Hipparchia semele, Erebia cassioides, Maniola jurtina, Erebia tyndarus), 3 Hesperiidae (Gegenes pumilo, Gegenes nostrodamus, Carcharodus alceae). This change is meant to grant a higher degree of representativity of the butterfly species diversity (as to subfamilies and tribes).

Section n. 5: Dispositional variables. Interests and inclinations

Being interested into and favourably predisposed towards the natural sciences, on the one hand, and the arts and aesthetic experiences on the other hand are among the most crucial individual differences which influence people’s response to nature and the environment, and to beauty and the aesthetic [19,20,6469]. In this section, we collect data about the participants’ dispositions towards natural science and the arts in general. We present the participant with a questionnaire which is a modified version of a model (about subjective dispositions towards the arts) administered and validated by Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham [70]. To reduce the fatigue effect [71,72], we shortened the original questionnaire from 36 to 18 questions.

The participant is offered a 5-point Likert scale (strongly agree; agree; neither agree nor disagree; disagree; strongly disagree) from which to select a response. In compliance with the Likert scale standard protocol, the 18 questions are asked half in a negative form, half in a positive one; the order of positive and negative questions randomly occurs, so as to avoid possible acquiescence effects [73,74].

Our test is freely available in both Italian and English language on our dedicated website www.unveiling.eu. The test has been online since April 2022 and can be accessed without limitations of age, nationality, education and socio-cultural background. So far, the “Unveiling” test has been advertised by means of the “snowballing” technique [75], i.e. through national magazines in Italy, direct mailing to researchers’ contacts as well as social media, asking the participants to forward the test to their own families and contacts. Moreover, it has been promoted through a wide array of public engagement events in Italy (museums, libraries, university events, edutainment and citizen science events, during “bioblitz” in Italian National Parks etc.). The main target of the test are European citizens, in line with the biodiversity conservation strategies our project is focused on. To obtain a sample size able to support reliable analyses, we plan to involve at least 5,000 participants, corresponding approximately to 100 scores for each species in the ranking section. The test will presumably be online until April 2023.

What outcomes will be measured, when and how

Section n. 2: Ranking.

Single species mean scores obtained in the “ranking” section of the test will provide a first index of the aesthetic attractivity. Such an index is expected to be strongly variable among pictures and less accurate for those species with high sexual dimorphism and strong differences between dorsal/ventral sides. For these reasons we will compute a second index, only considering the mean scores of the sex/side of each species reaching the highest scores in the test. A source of possible uncertainty in the results is the disposition of participants to provide higher or lower scores. For this reason we will calculate a third index as the mean scores scaled and centred among those of the same participant. These three different indexes (species mean scores, mean scores of the best aspect [sex and dorsal/ventral wing view], mean scaled scores) will be submitted to a PCA (Principal Component Analysis) in order to extract an expected single factor, accounting for the combination of these three indexes. This factor can be considered as the aesthetic attractivity value, which will be tested for accuracy by using two intrinsic features of butterflies:

  1. Since highly phylogenetically related butterfly species are more similar to each other, we expect to find a strongly significant phylogenetic signal in the aesthetic attractivity value. This will be tested using the recently published time-calibrated phylogenetic tree of European butterflies [76] and a typical test for phylogenetic signal, the phylosig function of the phytools R package [77].
  2. European butterfly cryptic taxa, objectively identified by Voda et al. [78], should obtain more similar scores to each other than random pairs.

It is reasonable to suppose that picture quality, in particular different backgrounds, may affect the score assigned by people to butterflies. To evaluate the magnitude of this effect we calculated average scores for each species for pictures with and without flower on the background separately. Then we applied a mixed generalised linear model (using glmmTMB R package) comparing the average scores for pictures with and without a flower on the background using species as a random factor. The effect of the background has been evaluated in two ways: 1) by obtaining estimated marginal means for the background factor by using the emmeans R package 2) by obtaining using the r.squaredGLMM function of the ‘MuMIn’ package the proportion of variance explained by different backgrounds (marginal R2) and that explained by the full model including the species random factor (conditional R2).

Section n. 3: Single morphological features.

The preference for either the natural or the altered version of different butterfly features will be assessed by using a Chi-squared test. We will also evaluate the possible interaction between the participant’s preference for a specific version of a given feature and their dispositional variables. For example, participants self-reporting higher-than-average knowledge in butterflies and strong interest in natural sciences might prefer the drawing portraying the non-altered version of the species.

Then, it will be possible to score the presence and/or the magnitude of these features in the European butterflies (e.g. with presence-absence variables or with more complex morphological analyses) and to verify which of them most explain the aesthetic attractivity value (section “ranking”) by a phylogenetic regression [79] using aesthetic attractivity value as a response variable, the scores for morphological features of each species as predictors while correcting for phylogenetic autocorrelation using the phylogenetic tree of European butterflies [76].

Section n. 4: Emotional engagement.

In this section we will provide the analysis of the data concerning the emotional engagement of the participants. Since representatives for all butterfly subfamilies will be included in this section, we will perform a chi-squared test to assess whether images of phylogenetically distant species of butterflies with different morphological features are associated with different elicited emotions, while phylogenetically close species should arouse similar emotional responses. For example, we would expect paler coloured and smaller butterflies, such as the Lycaenidae family, to be associated with cuteness and lower emotional intensity, whereas brighter coloured and bigger ones (e.g. Papilionidae) should arouse awe [8082] and in general a higher intensity. Moreover, we could expect to find an interaction between the self-reported level of knowledge and interest and the kind of emotions elicited: notably, experts should experience fear and/or disgust less frequently [45,8385] while reporting more frequently emotions with positive valence towards rare or endangered species.

Section n. 1 and section n. 5: Personal data and dispositional variables.

We will use a PCA (Principal Component Analysis) algorithm (like dudi.mix of the ade4 R package) on personal data and dispositional variables datasets in order to extract the main factors (PCs) associated with specific characteristics of the user. Such principal components will then be used to detect the possible influence of experiences and dispositions of each participant in the answers given to the questions concerning the first three sections of the test (ranking, emotional engagement and single morphological features). As a general rule we expect that dispositional factors show significant interactions in the relationships between species attributes and participant responses in all the sections of the test.

Preliminary assessment of test appreciation

The first period of beta testing (April 1 2022 to May 22 2022) provided the results for the first 500 participants. Among them, 453 entered the “ranking” section of the test and 94.5% of them ranked all the 9 proposed species; 428 participants entered the “single morphological features” section and 418 of them expressed their preferences to all the 10 pairs of butterfly drawings; 412 participants entered he “emotional engagement” section and 402 of them evaluated all the five proposed species; 376 participants entered the “dispositional variables” section; 361 out of 376 answered all the questions. Overall, about three quarters of the participants went through all the main sections of the test and more than 90% answered all the questions in each section.

The evaluation for the effect of different backgrounds on votes revealed a significant effect with pictures representing a flower in the background receiving higher votes (estimate, 0.165; standard error, 0.0417; z-value, 3.950; p-value <0.001). However, the difference in estimated marginal means in quite low (mean vote without flower 6.42 +- 0.0441 standard error, with flower 6.59 +- 0.0453) and the variance explained by the full model (species+background, conditional R2 = 0.585) is much higher than the variance explained by background alone (residual R2 = 0.007). Nevertheless, due to the significant effect for the presence of a flower in the background we decided to replace all the images at the midpoint of the assessment (more or less 2500 test) so that the effect(s) of any uncontrolled feature(s) of individual pictures is halved. This is one of the means by which we expect to evaluate, in the analyses, the effect of confounding factors (i.e. geographical distribution and the interaction with the living environment of the test-respondents).

Another small yet not negligible impact registered throughout the beta-testing period’s results, concerns reported experiences of discomforts caused by the very view of butterfly pictures. Out of 500 finished preliminary tests, in fact, we collected 1823 feedbacks about the emotions experienced by the participants with 160 claims of negative ones, 45 of which reporting “disgust”, 37 reporting “fear” and 78 experiencing “confusion” for a total of 8,77% of the whole.

The status and timeline of the study

We started collecting data on May 1st, 2022. The test will be online until April 2023. April 2021/May 2022

  1. Development and beta testing.
  2. May 2022
  3. Launch of the website.
  4. May 2022/April 2023
  5. Advertisement of the test and dissemination activities.
  6. April 2023
  7. End of data collection and statistical processing of collected data.
  8. September 2023
  9. Publication of results.

Discussion

In our view, aesthetic attractiveness of different butterfly species to humans represents a feature influencing their possibility to survive in the Anthropocene. A choice of which species to protect influenced by their aesthetic value may not be an optimal strategy to maintain ecosystem functionality. Indeed, charismatic species selected on aesthetic grounds belong to a few phylogenetic clades, thus encompassing a disproportionately low fraction of evolutionary and functional diversity [14,17]. Being such an aesthetic attractivity driven protection undesirable, we need to determine and quantify the aesthetic attractivity of target species in order to evaluate the occurrence of such bias in conservation policies and activities (red lists, funds for establishment of conservation actions).

This is not to overlook that, in other cases, aesthetic attractivity can indeed work as a fly-wheel and a booster, rather than a brake, for biodiversity protection. Flag and umbrella species are often chosen among the most attractive taxa and their importance in advertising diversity loss is widely known [86].

The protocol described here is designed to determine and quantify the aesthetic index and to analyse its interconnection with different kinds of emotions and with a set of dispositional variables in the human perceiver. The long-term goal of our project is to set up targeted educational strategies to re-modulate people’s current (non-functional) aesthetic experience regarding the different species of butterflies, so as to make it more effective for conservation purposes. Our aesthetic experience of the same objects and phenomena can indeed change over time, and aesthetic standards and principles are not set in stone. For instance, we tend to be more attracted by the things we know more, i.e. to get attached to the things we are most frequently exposed to [8789], although boredom is notoriously a limiting condition of the exposure effect [90]. On the other hand, it is not always true that the better informed our aesthetic judgements are, the stronger or more pleasurable is the aesthetic experience we get [91], since a pivotal role in aesthetic experience is also played by personal engagement, openness and other dispositional variables [92]. In this sense, by providing people with more information about butterflies, by offering them more valuable occasions to interact with the natural world and with insects in particular, and by fostering their interests and openness to nature by means of targeted educational activities [93], it could be possible to re-tune the public aesthetic experience of butterflies towards a more functional set of target species and to take advantage more effectively of the fascination that butterflies exert. Instead of being attracted exclusively by beautiful butterflies, for instance, people could start to care also for unusual, original, surprising, “diverse” forms and colours [94].

Fitness and expected impact

In conservation biology, many traits are available for European butterflies and they typically describe intrinsic functional traits (such as size, host plants, phenology and behaviour) and variables referring to habitat preferences (temperature, precipitation, altitude and vegetational units) [39]. These traits have been largely used to predict the decline of species under current environmental changes [9598].

Adding a new relational aesthetic dimension to biological conservation can facilitate the critical assessment of current conservation strategies, such as the process of selection of certain species (over others) as flag species and umbrella species by research institutions, environmental associations and NGOs and the selection of the species of butterflies to be included into national, international and European conservation actions (e.g. CEE habitat directive, LIFE projects, IUCN red lists, local red list, National and regional protection lists).

Limitations and improvements of the study design

Our test has so far been disseminated mainly through the snowballing technique, initially including our research group closest contacts (friends, relatives and colleagues) and subsequently expanding the diffusion range to “contacts of contacts". This allows an effective spreading of the test, but at the same time it compromises the representativeness of the sample. Indeed, people involved will be in the majority direct or indirect contact of the researchers and/or more involved in the field of conservation biology and of natural sciences than the general public. Moreover, our study sample includes a large number of participants (at least 5000 planned), but it is still a limited sample in relation to the European population whose representativeness is sought. In fact, it is very difficult to obtain an effective representativeness of all the various segments of the European population (age, geography, sociocultural aspects etc.). That said, however, it should be pointed out that in the case of this study the test will not investigate in detail the population’s sociocultural aspects and how these affect the aesthetic appreciation, although the same test could be reproposed and distributed in a more oriented way to study the various sociocultural segments in future research. Another possible limitation of our study is the technological mediation, that is the device through which the participants carry out their aesthetic experience to the vision of the butterflies’ images. In fact, the butterfly images presented on the PC, tablet or mobile phone may not accurately reflect the real appreciation of butterflies in nature. Static images (digital reproductions) of dynamic living beings are evaluated [99], while the appreciation in nature would include aspects related to the dynamism of the organisms (e.g. butterfly flight diversity in the four dimensions of space) that are impossible to include in the test. Surely, these are interesting aspects to be considered in future studies, in which we could test the aesthetic appreciation of people towards certain animal species directly in nature or more easily using new technologies such as VR (virtual reality), in order to ensure dynamism and immersivity during the experience [100,101].

Measures to maximise impact: Dissemination, exploitation and communication of results

At the academic level, the release of scientific papers in high-ranking journals is expected; in line with the Open Access policy, every research product will be freely available online. As the project targets specific grounds for action, public and private institutions and organisations managing protected areas are relevant addressees of the study. Stakeholders as the six national parks of the Central-Northern Italian Apennines and institutions such as the Italian Ministry of Ecological Transition, Butterfly Conservation Europe, W.W.F., Legambiente, ALI (Associazione Lepidotterologica Italiana) will be involved in the design of specifically thought-of events at both specialist and non-specialist level (i.e. bioblitz, guided nature walks, field trips, talks and workshops). These events are expected to take place starting from September 2022 with the discussion of the preliminary results and, from September 2023, with the disclosure of the final results of the study. Schools and students represent a further target of the dissemination activities; we have planned a series of seminars aimed at engaging students from secondary schools to universities, mainly in the city of Florence (IT) and neighbouring areas. The coverage of the project results will also involve public media such as newspapers, television and social media; regular updates on the main social networks (i.e. Twitter, Facebook and Instagram), along with online videos and podcasts, are also planned.

Supporting information

S1 Table. The list of butterfly species pictures.

The list of butterfly species along with the collected pictures used in section n.1 of “Unveiling” test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0283360.s001

(DOCX)

S1 Appendix. Test question list.

All the questions proposed to the participants during the test are collected together with their answer options in this file.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0283360.s002

(DOCX)

S2 Appendix. Single morphological features drawings.

All the couples of drawings with respective features analysis are available in this file.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0283360.s003

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Roger Vila, CSIC Research Scientist at the Pompeu Fabra University, Institute of Evolutionary Biology, Barcelona, Spain; Alice Chirico, Research Fellow at the Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Department of Psychology, Milan, Italy; the colleagues of the University of Florence, Department of Literature and Philosophy, Florence, Italy.

References

  1. 1. Saito Y. Everyday Aesthetics [Internet]. 2007 [cited 2022 Aug 1]. Available from: https://academic.oup.com/book/7389.
  2. 2. Saito Y. Everyday Aesthetics. Philosophy and Literature. 2001;25(1):87–95.
  3. 3. Breitenbach A. V—Aesthetics in Science: A Kantian Proposal. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society (Hardback). 2013;113(1pt1):83–100.
  4. 4. Breitenbach A. The Beauty of Science without the Science of Beauty: Kant and the Rationalists on the Aesthetics of Cognition. Journal of the History of Philosophy. 2018;56(2):281–304.
  5. 5. Hossenfelder S. Lost in math: How beauty leads physics astray. Hachette UK; 2018.
  6. 6. Knight AJ. “Bats, snakes and spiders, Oh my!” How aesthetic and negativistic attitudes, and other concepts predict support for species protection. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2008 Mar 1;28(1):94–103.
  7. 7. Brambilla M, Gustin M, Celada C. Species appeal predicts conservation status. Biological Conservation. 2013 Apr 1;160:209–13.
  8. 8. Colléony A, Clayton S, Couvet D, Saint Jalme M, Prévot AC. Human preferences for species conservation: Animal charisma trumps endangered status. Biological Conservation. 2017 Feb 1;206:263–9.
  9. 9. Garnett ST, Ainsworth GB, Zander KK. Are we choosing the right flagships? The bird species and traits Australians find most attractive. PLOS ONE. 2018 Jun 26;13(6):e0199253. pmid:29944681
  10. 10. Veríssimo D, Campbell HA, Tollington S, MacMillan DC, Smith RJ. Why do people donate to conservation? Insights from a ‘real world’ campaign. PLOS ONE. 2018 Jan 25;13(1):e0191888. pmid:29370291
  11. 11. Lundberg P, Vainio A, MacMillan DC, Smith RJ, Veríssimo D, Arponen A. The effect of knowledge, species aesthetic appeal, familiarity and conservation need on willingness to donate. Animal Conservation. 2019;22(5):432–43.
  12. 12. Lundberg P, Veríssimo D, Vainio A, Arponen A. Preferences for different flagship types in fundraising for nature conservation. Biological Conservation. 2020 Oct 1;250:108738.
  13. 13. Curtin P, Papworth S. Coloring and size influence preferences for imaginary animals, and can predict actual donations to species-specific conservation charities. Conservation Letters. 2020;13(4):e12723.
  14. 14. Habel JC, Gossner MM, Schmitt T. Just beautiful?! What determines butterfly species for nature conservation. Biodivers Conserv. 2021 Jul 1;30(8):2481–93.
  15. 15. Adamo M, Chialva M, Calevo J, Bertoni F, Dixon K, Mammola S. Plant scientists’ research attention is skewed towards colourful, conspicuous and broadly distributed flowers. Nat Plants. 2021 May;7(5):574–8. pmid:33972712
  16. 16. Senior RA, Oliveira BF, Dale J, Scheffers BR. Wildlife trade targets colorful birds and threatens the aesthetic value of nature. Current Biology [Internet]. 2022 Sep 15 [cited 2022 Sep 19];0(0). Available from: pmid:36113469
  17. 17. Langlois J, Guilhaumon F, Baletaud F, Casajus N, Braga CDA, Fleuré V, et al. The aesthetic value of reef fishes is globally mismatched to their conservation priorities. PLOS Biology. 2022 Jun 7;20(6):e3001640. pmid:35671265
  18. 18. Barbato D, Benocci A, Guasconi M, Manganelli G. Light and shade of citizen science for less charismatic invertebrate groups: quality assessment of iNaturalist nonmarine mollusc observations in central Italy. Journal of Molluscan Studies. 2021 Dec 1;87(4):eyab033.
  19. 19. Randler C. Users of a citizen science platform for bird data collection differ from other birdwatchers in knowledge and degree of specialization. Global Ecology and Conservation. 2021 Jun 1;27:e01580.
  20. 20. Callaghan CT, Poore AGB, Hofmann M, Roberts CJ, Pereira HM. Large-bodied birds are over-represented in unstructured citizen science data. Sci Rep. 2021 Sep 24;11(1):19073. pmid:34561517
  21. 21. Frynta D, Lišková S, Bültmann S, Burda H. Being Attractive Brings Advantages: The Case of Parrot Species in Captivity. PLOS ONE. 2010 Sep 7;5(9):e12568. pmid:20830206
  22. 22. Stokes DL. Things We Like: Human Preferences among Similar Organisms and Implications for Conservation. Hum Ecol. 2007 Jun 1;35(3):361–9.
  23. 23. Lišková S, Frynta D. What Determines Bird Beauty in Human Eyes? Anthrozoös. 2013 Mar 1;26(1):27–41.
  24. 24. Landová E, Marešová J, Šimková O, Cikánová V, Frynta D. Human responses to live snakes and their photographs: Evaluation of beauty and fear of the king snakes. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2012 Mar 1;32(1):69–77.
  25. 25. Gunnthorsdottir A. Physical Attractiveness of an Animal Species as a Decision Factor for its Preservation. Anthrozoös. 2001 Dec 1;14(4):204–15.
  26. 26. Landová E, Poláková P, Rádlová S, Janovcová M, Bobek M, Frynta D. Beauty ranking of mammalian species kept in the Prague Zoo: does beauty of animals increase the respondents’ willingness to protect them? Sci Nat. 2018 Nov 28;105(11):69.
  27. 27. Verissimo D, MacMillan DC, Smith RJ. Toward a systematic approach for identifying conservation flagships. Conservation Letters. 2011;4(1):1–8.
  28. 28. Roberge JM, Angelstam P. Usefulness of the Umbrella Species Concept as a Conservation Tool. Conservation Biology. 2004;18(1):76–85.
  29. 29. Montgomery GA, Dunn RR, Fox R, Jongejans E, Leather SR, Saunders ME, et al. Is the insect apocalypse upon us? How to find out. Biological Conservation. 2020 Jan 1;241:108327.
  30. 30. Lobo JM. The use of occurrence data to predict the effects of climate change on insects. Current Opinion in Insect Science. 2016 Oct 1;17:62–8. pmid:27720075
  31. 31. Preston SD, Liao JD, Toombs TP, Romero-Canyas R, Speiser J, Seifert CM. A case study of a conservation flagship species: the monarch butterfly. Biodivers Conserv. 2021 Jun 1;30(7):2057–77.
  32. 32. Sumner S, Law G, Cini A. Why we love bees and hate wasps. Ecological Entomology. 2018;43(6):836–45.
  33. 33. Hume D. Four Dissertations. New York: St. Augustine’s Press; 1757.
  34. 34. Burke E. A Philosophical Enquiry Into the Origin of Our Ideas of the Sublime and Beautiful, 1759. Menston, Scolar P.; 1759.
  35. 35. Berlyne DE. Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value. Perception & Psychophysics. 1970 Sep 1;8(5):279–86.
  36. 36. Darwin C. The descent of man. New York: D Appleton. 1871;
  37. 37. Menninghaus W, Skinner A. Biology à la mode: Charles Darwin’s Aesthetics of “Ornament.” History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences. 2009;31(2):263–78.
  38. 38. Menninghaus W. Aesthetics after Darwin: The multiple origins and functions of the arts. Academic Studies Press; 2019.
  39. 39. Moretti M, Dias ATC, de Bello F, Altermatt F, Chown SL, Azcárate FM, et al. Handbook of protocols for standardized measurement of terrestrial invertebrate functional traits. Functional Ecology. 2017;31(3):558–67.
  40. 40. Otto SP. Adaptation, speciation and extinction in the Anthropocene. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2018 Nov 21;285(1891):20182047. pmid:30429309
  41. 41. Haas AF, Guibert M, Foerschner A, Co T, Calhoun S, George E, et al. Can we measure beauty? Computational evaluation of coral reef aesthetics. PeerJ. 2015 Nov 10;3:e1390. pmid:26587350
  42. 42. Pinho JR de, Grilo C, Boone RB, Galvin KA, Snodgrass JG. Influence of Aesthetic Appreciation of Wildlife Species on Attitudes towards Their Conservation in Kenyan Agropastoralist Communities. PLOS ONE. 2014 Feb 14;9(2):e88842. pmid:24551176
  43. 43. Casalegno S, Inger R, DeSilvey C, Gaston KJ. Spatial Covariance between Aesthetic Value & Other Ecosystem Services. PLOS ONE. 2013 Jun 28;8(6):e68437.
  44. 44. Sullivan AP, Bird DW, Perry GH. Human behaviour as a long-term ecological driver of non-human evolution. Nat Ecol Evol. 2017 Feb 21;1(3):1–11. pmid:28812734
  45. 45. Leder H, Nadal M. Ten years of a model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments: The aesthetic episode–Developments and challenges in empirical aesthetics. British Journal of Psychology. 2014;105(4):443–64. pmid:25280118
  46. 46. Jacobsen T. Beauty and the brain: culture, history and individual differences in aesthetic appreciation. Journal of anatomy. 2010;216(2):184–91. pmid:19929909
  47. 47. Jacobsen T, Beudt S. Domain generality and domain specificity in aesthetic appreciation. New Ideas in Psychology. 2017;47:97–102.
  48. 48. Schabmann A, Gerger G, Schmidt BM, Wögerer E, Osipov I, Leder H. Where Does It Come From? Developmental Aspects of Art Appreciation. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2016 Jul 1;40(4):313–23.
  49. 49. Pugach C, Leder H, Graham DJ. How Stable Are Human Aesthetic Preferences Across the Lifespan? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience [Internet]. 2017 [cited 2022 Sep 3];11. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00289 pmid:28620290
  50. 50. Berlyne DE. Aesthetics and psychobiology. East Norwalk, CT, US: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1971. xiv, 336 p. (Aesthetics and psychobiology).
  51. 51. Martindale C, Moore K. Priming, prototypicality, and preference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1988;14(4):661.
  52. 52. Nijhout H. Elements of Butterfly Wing Patterns. The Journal of experimental zoology. 2001 Oct 15;291:213–25. pmid:11598911
  53. 53. Monteiro A, Podlaha O. Wings, Horns, and Butterfly Eyespots: How Do Complex Traits Evolve? PLOS Biology. 2009 Feb 24;7(2):e1000037. pmid:19243218
  54. 54. Manesi Z, Lange PAMV, Pollet TV. Butterfly Eyespots: Their Potential Influence on Aesthetic Preferences and Conservation Attitudes. PLOS ONE. 2015 Nov 6;10(11):e0141433. pmid:26544692
  55. 55. Droit-Volet S, Meck WH. How emotions colour our perception of time. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007 Dec 1;11(12):504–13. pmid:18023604
  56. 56. Zadra JR, Clore GL. Emotion and perception: the role of affective information. WIREs Cognitive Science. 2011;2(6):676–85. pmid:22039565
  57. 57. Prinz J. Penetrating Beauty: Knowledge, Culture and Context in Aesthetic Perception. In: Perception, Cognition and Aesthetics. Routledge; 2019.
  58. 58. Cupchik GC. Emotion in Aesthetics and the Aesthetics of Emotion. In: New directions in aesthetics, creativity and the arts. Amityville, NY, US: Baywood Publishing Co; 2006. p. 209–24. (Foundations and Frontiers in Aesthetics).
  59. 59. Silvia PJ, Brown EM. Anger, disgust, and the negative aesthetic emotions: Expanding an appraisal model of aesthetic experience. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 2007;1(2):100–6.
  60. 60. Marković S. Components of Aesthetic Experience: Aesthetic Fascination, Aesthetic Appraisal, and Aesthetic Emotion. i-Perception. 2012 Jan 1;3(1):1–17. pmid:23145263
  61. 61. Menninghaus W, Wagner V, Wassiliwizky E, Schindler I, Hanich J, Jacobsen T, et al. What are aesthetic emotions? Psychol Rev. 2019 Mar;126(2):171–95. pmid:30802122
  62. 62. Schindler I, Hosoya G, Menninghaus W, Beermann U, Wagner V, Eid M, et al. Measuring aesthetic emotions: A review of the literature and a new assessment tool. PLOS ONE. 2017 Jun 5;12(6):e0178899. pmid:28582467
  63. 63. Lundy DE, Schenkel MB, Akrie TN, Walker AM. How Important is Beauty to You? The Development of the Desire for Aesthetics Scale. Empirical Studies of the Arts. 2010 Jan 1;28(1):73–92.
  64. 64. Lizardo O, Skiles S. Reconceptualizing and Theorizing “Omnivorousness”: Genetic and Relational Mechanisms. Sociological Theory. 2012 Dec 1;30(4):263–82.
  65. 65. Specker E, Forster M, Brinkmann H, Boddy J, Pelowski M, Rosenberg R, et al. The Vienna Art Interest and Art Knowledge Questionnaire (VAIAK): A unified and validated measure of art interest and art knowledge. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 2020;14(2):172–85.
  66. 66. Cotter KN, Chen DF, Christensen AP, Kim KY, Silvia PJ. Measuring art knowledge: Item response theory and differential item functioning analysis of the Aesthetic Fluency Scale. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 2021;No Pagination Specified-No Pagination Specified.
  67. 67. Tulloch AIT, Szabo JK. A behavioural ecology approach to understand volunteer surveying for citizen science datasets. Emu—Austral Ornithology. 2012 Dec;112(4):313–25.
  68. 68. Troudet J, Grandcolas P, Blin A, Vignes-Lebbe R, Legendre F. Taxonomic bias in biodiversity data and societal preferences. Sci Rep. 2017 Aug 22;7(1):9132. pmid:28831097
  69. 69. Stoudt S, Goldstein BR, Valpine PD. Identifying Charismatic Bird Species and Traits with Community Science Observations [Internet]. bioRxiv; 2021 [cited 2022 Feb 2]. p. 2021.06.05.446577. Available from: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2021.06.05.446577v1.
  70. 70. Chamorro-Premuzic T, Furnham A. A possible model for understanding the personality-intelligence interface. British Journal of Psychology. 2004;95(2):249–64. pmid:15142305
  71. 71. Chyung SY Yonnie , Roberts K, Swanson I, Hankinson A. Evidence-Based Survey Design: The Use of a Midpoint on the Likert Scale. Performance Improvement. 2017;56(10):15–23.
  72. 72. Chyung SY Yonnie Barkin JR, Shamsy JA. Evidence-Based Survey Design: The Use of Negatively Worded Items in Surveys. Performance Improvement. 2018;57(3):16–25.
  73. 73. Likert R. A technique for the measurement of attitudes. Archives of Psychology. 1932;22(140):55–55.
  74. 74. Cronbach LJ. Studies of acquiescence as a factor in the true-false test. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1942;33(6):401–15.
  75. 75. Streeton R, Cooke M, Campbell J. Researching the researchers: using a snowballing technique. Nurse Researcher. 2004 Sep 1;12(1):35–47. pmid:15493213
  76. 76. Wiemers M, Chazot N, Wheat CW, Schweiger O, Wahlberg N. A complete time-calibrated multi-gene phylogeny of the European butterflies. Zookeys. 2020 Jun 4;938:97–124. pmid:32550787
  77. 77. Revell LJ. phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods in ecology and evolution. 2012;(2):217–23.
  78. 78. Vodă R, Dapporto L, Dincă V, Vila R. Cryptic matters: overlooked species generate most butterfly beta-diversity. Ecography. 2015;38(4):405–9.
  79. 79. Grafen A, Hamilton WD. The phylogenetic regression. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, Biological Sciences. 1989 Dec 21;326(1233):119–57. pmid:2575770
  80. 80. Cowen AS, Keltner D. Self-report captures 27 distinct categories of emotion bridged by continuous gradients. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2017 Sep 19;114(38):E7900–9. pmid:28874542
  81. 81. Kakehashi E, Muramatsu K, Hibino H. Perceptual holistic color combination analysis of Papilionidae butterflies as aesthetic objects. PLOS ONE. 2020 Oct 28;15(10):e0240356. pmid:33112869
  82. 82. Kakehashi E, Muramatsu K, Hibino H. Computational color combination analysis of Papilionidae butterflies as aesthetic objects. Color Research & Application. 2020;45(1):65–84.
  83. 83. Leder H, Belke B, Oeberst A, Augustin D. A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments. British Journal of Psychology. 2004 Nov;95(4):489–508.
  84. 84. Grando G, Bramuzzo S, Callegaro E, Guidolin L, Irato P, Santovito G. Who is afraid of insects? A didactic research in the biological field in kindergarten. EDULEARN18 Proceedings. 2018;
  85. 85. Papworth S, Curtin P. Information about conservation status is more important than species appearance in the species preferences of potential conservation donors. Environmental Conservation. 2022 Sep;49(3):146–54.
  86. 86. Ducarme F, Luque G, Courchamp F. What are “charismatic species” for conservation biologists? BioSciences Master Reviews. 2013 Jul 1;1:1–8.
  87. 87. Bornstein RF. Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. Psychological bulletin. 1989;106(2):265.
  88. 88. Reber R, Winkielman P, Schwarz N. Effects of perceptual fluency on affective judgments. Psychological science. 1998;9(1):45–8.
  89. 89. Winkielman P, Cacioppo JT. Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits positive affect. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2001;81(6):989. pmid:11761320
  90. 90. Bornstein RF, Kale AR, Cornell KR. Boredom as a limiting condition on the mere exposure effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;58:791–800.
  91. 91. Nanay B. Aesthetics Oxford University Press, USA; 2019.
  92. 92. Fayn K, MacCann C, Tiliopoulos N, Silvia PJ. Aesthetic emotions and aesthetic people: Openness predicts sensitivity to novelty in the experiences of interest and pleasure. Frontiers in psychology. 2015;6:1877. pmid:26696940
  93. 93. Barthel S, Belton S, Raymond CM, Giusti M. Fostering Children’s Connection to Nature Through Authentic Situations: The Case of Saving Salamanders at School. Frontiers in Psychology [Internet]. 2018 [cited 2022 Sep 9];9. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00928. pmid:29937747
  94. 94. Wilson EO. Biophilia. In: Biophilia. Harvard university press; 1984.
  95. 95. Vandewalle M, de Bello F, Berg MP, Bolger T, Doledec S, Dubs F, et al. Functional traits as indicators of biodiversity response to land use changes across ecosystems and organisms. Biodiversity and Conservation. 2010;19:2921–47.
  96. 96. Eskildsen A, Carvalheiro LG, Kissling WD, Biesmeijer JC, Schweiger O, Høye TT. Ecological specialization matters: long-term trends in butterfly species richness and assemblage composition depend on multiple functional traits. Mac Nally R, editor. Diversity Distrib. 2015 Jul;21(7):792–802.
  97. 97. Melero Y, Stefanescu C, Pino J. General declines in Mediterranean butterflies over the last two decades are modulated by species traits. Biological Conservation. 2016 Sep 1;201:336–42.
  98. 98. Dapporto L, Cini A, Menchetti M, Vodă R, Bonelli S, Casacci LP, et al. Rise and fall of island butterfly diversity: Understanding genetic differentiation and extinction in a highly diverse archipelago. Diversity and Distributions. 2017;23(10):1169–81.
  99. 99. Leder H, Hakala J, Peltoketo VT, Valuch C, Pelowski M. Swipes and Saves: A Taxonomy of Factors Influencing Aesthetic Assessments and Perceived Beauty of Mobile Phone Photographs. Front Psychol. 2022;13:786977. pmid:35295400
  100. 100. Ahn SJ Grace , Bostick J, Ogle E, Nowak KL, McGillicuddy KT, Bailenson JN. Experiencing Nature: Embodying Animals in Immersive Virtual Environments Increases Inclusion of Nature in Self and Involvement with Nature. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 2016 Nov 1;21(6):399–419.
  101. 101. Breves P, Heber V. Into the Wild: The Effects of 360° Immersive Nature Videos on Feelings of Commitment to the Environment. Environmental Communication. 2020 Apr 2;14(3):332–46.