Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Evidence of emotion dysregulation as a core symptom of adult ADHD: A systematic review

Abstract

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a developmental disorder, with an onset in childhood, that accompanies the person throughout their life, with prevalence between 3 and 5% in adults. Recent studies point towards a fourth core symptom of the disorder related to the emotional information processing that would explain the repercussions that ADHD has on the social, academic, and professional life of the people affected. This review aims to describe emotion dysregulation features as well as the brain activity associated in adults with ADHD. A search of the scientific literature was launched in specialized databases: PsycInfo, Medline, Eric, PsycArticle, Psicodoc and Scopus, following PRISMA guidelines. Twenty-two articles met the inclusion criteria: (a) an ADHD clinical diagnosis, (b) participants over 18 years old, (c) emotion regulation measurement, (d) empirical studies, and (c) in English. Due to the heterogeneity of the studies included, they were classified into three sections: measures and features of emotion regulation (ER) in people with ADHD, neurological and psychophysiological activity related to ER, and treatments. The studies found that meet the selection criteria are scarce and very heterogeneous both in aims and in sample features. Adults with ADHD show a more frequent use of non-adaptive emotion regulation strategies compared to people without ADHD symptoms. Moreover, emotion dysregulation was associated with symptom severity, executive functioning, psychiatric comorbidities, and even with criminal conviction. Different patterns of brain activity were observed when people with and without ADHD were compared. These results may suggest that psychopharmacological treatments as well as behavioral therapies could be useful tools for improving emotional difficulties in adult ADHD.

Introduction

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is one of the most prevalent neurodevelopmental disorders (between 5–7% among children and adolescents and between 3–5% among the adult population [1]) with onset in childhood that can persist into adulthood. It is characterized by a persistent pattern of three cardinal symptoms: inattention, impulsiveness and / or hyperactivity describing three specific subtypes: combined, predominantly inattentive and predominantly hyperactive-impulsive [2]. According to DSM-5 [2] criteria for diagnosis of ADHD, symptoms must appear before the age of 12, with a duration of at least 6 months in two or more contexts that negatively impact social and academic/occupational activities. ADHD is described as a disorder with very heterogeneous manifestations [3], of multifactorial etiology [4] and with different manifestations throughout lifespan [5]. The diagnosis of ADHD in adults is much more difficult because of the lack of specific criteria and the high comorbidity with other disorders [6].

The diagnosis of ADHD continues to be strictly clinical and based exclusively on the behavioral symptoms of inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity, despite the recent accumulation of evidence suggesting other affective difficulties frequently associated with the disorder beyond simple comorbidity [7]. For example, through a longitudinal network model, a recent study by Karalunas et al. [8] has associated temperamental irritability with ADHD symptomatology, supporting the hypothesis that this type of anger dysregulation might be a frequent feature of ADHD. Likewise, this study also confirmed that working memory (i.e., as an indicator of executive functioning) had independent direct influences on both ADHD and depression.

It is well established that ADHD is associated with executive function (EF) deficits [9]. According to Barkley [10], ADHD is both, a self-regulating deficit disorder and an executive function disorder, since affected people have greater difficulties using EF to self-regulate and achieve their goals. Other studies, however, have pointed out anatomical-functional alterations of the reward system with motivational deficits being a primary dysfunction of ADHD, as well as noting that the last deficits are independent of the executive ones [11]. In recent years, new evidence has emerged that suggests that emotion regulation deficits, or emotion dysregulation (ED), are a core symptom of ADHD [12, 13].

Neuroscience research has identified neural networks related to EF [14], reward system [15], and emotional information processing [1618]. Moreover, an overlap has been found between EF and emotional neural networks [1921]. Interestingly, neuroimaging studies have found that the brain structures involved in these processes are affected in ADHD, including the dorsal anterior cingulated cortex and posterior cingulated cortex [22], dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex, striatum, and cerebellum [23, 24], and the amygdale [25, 26]. Finally, other studies have focused on social impairments in ADHD that correlate with a reduced volume of subcortical areas: insula, amygdala and striatum [27].

Nigg and Casey [28] have proposed a model based on three neural networks that would be affected in ADHD and that would be at the basis of emotional, motivational, and cognitive deficit. The fronto-striatal network, which would account for the difficulties in working memory and response selection; the fronto-cerebellar network, involved in temporal organizing and timing of behavior; and finally, the fronto-limbic pathway that would explain the emotional regulation deficit. Specifically, the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) modulates the limbic system activation and integrates the emotional information that comes from this and other brain areas [19, 28]. Its abundant interconnection with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), results in the modulation and control of emotional responses, adapting them to personal goals and the social context [29, 30]. Thus, evidence may indicate that ADHD is associated with an inadequate top-down regulation of emotional reactivity by higher cortical regions [10, 12].

Emotion regulation in ADHD

In recent years, emotion regulation (ER) has become a focus of interest in the study of the emotional difficulties observed in ADHD [12, 31]. According to Gross [32], emotional self-regulation or ER can be defined as a complex process by which people modulate their emotions to direct their behavior towards goals using strategies (for example, cognitive change strategy of reappraisal and response modulation strategy of suppression, see [33, 34]), which start when there are overlearned responses that conflict with the desired goal [35]. Thus, executive functions and emotion regulation might be closely linked and interrelated [10]. As Bailey and Jones [108] claim, emotional regulation, along with cognitive and social regulation, would develop and become increasingly complex, based on the essential components of inhibition, attention, working memory and shifting. In fact, some studies suggest that appropriate EF skills are associated with the inhibition of negative affect expressions [36], as well as with a greater use of the reappraisal strategy [37]. On the contrary, the habitual use of the suppression strategy is related to difficulties in daily EF [37].

Emotional dysregulation appears very early in ADHD childhood compared to children with typical neurodevelopment [38]. Furthermore, there is abundant research on childhood ADHD that finds associations between emotion dysregulation and symptom severity (for reviews see [39, 40]), failures in emotion recognition [4143], working memory deficits [6, 12, 44], poor parental ER skills [4548], even with abnormal functioning of the autonomic nervous system [49, 50].

In contrast to the vast evidence on ED in children with ADHD, little is known about ED in adults with ADHD. The differences in aspects related to ER in people with and without ADHD are not conclusive. For example, while some studies find that adults with ADHD use the maladaptive ER strategy of suppressing emotions more frequently than controls [51, 52], others do not find this [53].

Contradictions are also observed depending on the emotional assessment instrument used in the different studies (for instance, [54, 55]). One variable that could explain the contradictory results is the high comorbidity present in adults with ADHD, which makes it difficult to assess emotional aspects independently [6, 56]. More studies are needed to clarify what emotional aspects are impaired in adult ADHD, what factors are involved, as well as what tools are the most appropriate to assess them. Meanwhile, this review aims to locate and unify the most relevant results in ED obtained so far in adults diagnosed with ADHD.

Aims

The main goal of this work was to conduct a systematic review (SR) of the literature on emotional dysregulation in adults with ADHD.

Moreover, three specific goals are proposed: (1) to analyze emotion dysregulation in adults with ADHD; (2) to examine ADHD symptoms and their relationship with ED; (3) to identify ADHD neuro-functional abnormalities related to ED. In line with these specific goals, we hypothesize that adults with ADHD will score lower on ER measurements, associated with greater impairment (H1); will use maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (H2), and will have a different ER-related pattern of brain activity (H3) than controls.

Method

A systematic literature review (SLR) was carried out, following the principles and phases of the PRISMA model [57]. Although a meta-analytical study would be the most desirable, the systematic review methodology seems to be the most appropriate in this case due to the small number and the heterogeneity of the selected articles.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

The studies had to meet the following criteria to be included: (a) participants diagnosed with ADHD following DSM-IV or later criteria, (b) over 18 years old, (c) to have an ER measure, (d) empirical studies, (e) published in English.

The exclusion criteria were: (a) the lack of a clinical diagnosis of ADHD (e.g., symptoms or probable ADHD) or not being ADHD the main disorder of the study, (b) including people under 18 years of age in the study without offering analysis of the data by age range, (c) not providing ADHD participants ER measures (e.g., parents), (d) reviews, qualitative studies or meta-analyses.

Procedure

A search was launched through EBSCOhost in PsycInfo, Medline, Eric, PsycArticle and Psicodoc, and in Scopus databases from February 1 to 25, 2022. The following search criteria were entered into the databases anywhere to the title, keywords, or abstract fields, filtered by academic publications in English: [ADHD OR "Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder"] AND ["Emotion regulation" OR "Emotion Dysregulation"] AND [adult*]. An informal search was also carried out on Google Scholar throughout the first fortnight of March 2022. The process of eliminating non-relevant papers following PRISMA guidelines [57] can be seen in the flowchart (Fig 1). When the criteria were not clear, the inclusion or exclusion of the article was decided among all three authors. S1 Table shows the bibliometric properties of the included articles [13, 5154, 5874], which are marked with * in the references section.

Results

Twenty-two studies met all the inclusion criteria. However, given the great heterogeneity of the studies selected, they were classified into three sections. The first section included studies on measures and features of ER in people with ADHD. The second section comprised studies that analyze the neurological and psychophysiological activity during the performance on ER tasks in ADHD. Finally, the third section included interventions that provided ER outcomes in adults with ADHD.

S2 Table shows the demographic variables of the studies selected showing a wide heterogeneity, especially in terms of sample size, gender, ADHD subtypes, comorbidity, and medication status. However, most of the ADHD sample comes from clinical contexts (see S2 Table).

Regarding the methodological variables, Table 1 summarizes the measurement tools used in each study (see also Table 2), as well as the main results obtained related to emotional regulation, which are described below. Results were presented in three different sections. Finally, Table 2 lists the assessment tools used [53, 7597].

thumbnail
Table 1. Methodological information and main results of the reviewed articles.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0280131.t001

Emotion regulation features in adult ADHD

Ten studies compared ER scores in adults with ADHD and controls, finding that ADHD group had consistently lower ER scores than control groups, ranging from medium to very large effect sizes [d’ = 0.31–2.27] [5154, 5863] (see Table 1). In a series of two studies, Hirsch et al. [13, 60] investigated the role of ER in ADHD. In a first study, they carried out confirmatory factor analysis [13]. They found a good model fit to the ADHD four-factor model, with values below 0.10 in RMSEA [0.07; 90% confidence interval: 0.068–0.071] and around 2 in χ2/df ratio [2.03], suggesting that ED is a core symptom of ADHD. In a second study [60], they carried out a person-centered approach, through cluster analysis, to distinguish ADHD subtypes based on the presence or absence of ED. They found a two-cluster solution [F = 87.21; η2 = 0.185] which, once the variables with small effect size were eliminated, explained 25% of the variance between clusters. The differences in the classification variables were significant and large in size [specifically in the ERQS: d = 1, p <0.0001]. They found that Cluster 2 was loaded with higher proportion of combined ADHD presentation, women, and ED. Moreover, other studies also showed that women with ADHD presented greater emotion dysregulation [64] and emotionality [65].

Some studies found that emotion dysregulation was significantly associated with impulsivity [13], executive function deficits and functional impairment [66]; as well as total Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale Symptom Checklist (ASRSv1.1) score with non-adaptative cognitive strategies [63]. High scores of emotion dysregulation were also associated with substance use disorder [64], criminal conviction [67], BPD comorbidity [62, 63] and sluggish cognitive tempo comorbidity [66], as well as recall of parental ADHD symptoms [65]. However, no significant differences were found between ADHD emotional dysregulation scores and other disorders such as autism spectrum disorder [59]. Results regarding BPD were inconclusive. Cavelti et al. [58] found no significant difference between ADHD and BPD, but when they compared them together with controls, they found significant differences [F4, 179 = 0.64, p < 0.001]. "Confronting emotions with self-encouragement" was the factor from ERQS that best discriminated between the clinical group and the control group [F2, 180 = 5.29, p = 0.006]. Moukhtarian et al. [62] found no significant differences between ADHD and BPD in emotion dysregulation measured through retrospective reports, although they found that the ADHD+BPD group reported elevated ratings compared to the only ADHD group. Nevertheless, Rüfenacht et al. [63] found that the ADHD group had higher emotion dysregulation scores compared to other clinical and community samples [t = 4.79; p < 0.001] but less than BPD and ADHD + BPD [p < 0.001]. As for the comparison in ED between ADHD and cyclothymia, Brancati et al. [68] only found significant differences in negative emotionality and in negative/ positive emotionality ratio measured with the Reactivity, Intensity, Polarity, and Stability questionnaire (RIPoSt-40), in which adults with cyclothymia scored higher than ADHD.

Additionally, an ADHD trend to use maladaptive emotional regulation strategies and to rate low their own regulatory skills measured by Selbsteinschätzung emotionaler Kompetenzen (SEK-27) was observed. In this regard, Materna et al. [51] found that the ADHD group scored lower on reappraisal but higher on suppression than the control group [t = 2.40, p = 0.020; t = −3.28, p = 0.002; respectively] and valued their own regulation strategies more negatively that controls did [t = –8.06, p < 0.001]. Similar results were obtained by Shushakova et al. [52] [t = 2.60, p = 0.01, for reappraisal; t = 2.96, p = 0.008, for suppression; t = 7.59, p <0.001, for SEK-27] and by Rüfenacht et al. [63] [t = 9.12–14.19, p < 0.001, for non-adaptative Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) scales].

Mitchell et al. [61] analyzed the effect on ER of abstinence in smokers with and without ADHD. They found significant differences between the ADHD group and the control group [F = 36.17, p <0.001, η2 = 0.49]. These differences were maintained throughout the two experimental conditions (abstinence—non-abstinence), both in the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) [p ≤ 0.001, η2 ≥ 0.47], and in the laboratory tasks [p <0.001, η2 ≥ 0.31]. Increases in emotion dysregulation in the abstinence condition were due to significant increases in Impulse Control Difficulties [p = 0.005] and Lack of Emotional Clarity [p = 0.014] subscales.

Neurological and psychophysiological activity related to emotion dysregulation in adult ADHD

Matthies et al. [69] measured psychophysiological variables in a group of adults with ADHD during a sadness induction experimental task. The participants were divided into two groups, each of which received specific instructions to carry out the task based on emotion regulation strategies: acceptance of emotion (ACC) and suppression of emotion (SUP). As a dependent variable, they measured on three occasions throughout the experiment the level of sadness that the subject was experiencing (T1 = before the video, T2 = immediately after the video, T3 = after viewing landscapes). The results indicated that the participants in both conditions did not differ significantly in the level of feeling overwhelmed by emotion in T1 and T2, but in T3 [SUP: 26.61 / 26.97, ACC: 8.11 / 10.71, t1,34 = 2.71, p <0.050], pointing out a slower recovery of emotion in the suppression condition. Additionally, they found a paradoxical decrease in the activation of the sympathetic autonomic nervous system.

Regarding brain activity, four studies that were conducted with adults diagnosed with ADHD were reviewed. In the first study, Shushakova et al. [52] analyzed emotion regulation strategies in adults with and without ADHD using Event Related Potentials (ERP). They observed a greater amplitude of the Late Positive Potential (LPP) in the ADHD group than in the control group [F1,69 = 3.908, p = 0.052, d = 0.47]. Likewise, the group effect was significant, showing that the frontal amplitudes of the LPPs were higher in ADHD adults without medication compared to the control group [F1,63 = 7.083, p = 0.010, d = 0.69]. In ADHD cases, the habitual use of reappraisal was associated with smaller LPP amplitudes in the SUP condition [r = −0.44, p = 0.005]. In a second study also using ERPs, Shushakova et al. [70] investigated attentional bias towards emotional stimuli in adults with ADHD using the dote-probe paradigm. They focused on the N2pc component that has classically been related to emotion dysregulation in healthy people [70]. The ANOVA of omission rates revealed a significant group x emotion interaction [F1, 78 = 7.102, p = 0.009]. Post hoc t Tests indicated that ADHD patients made significantly fewer omissions in the trials with positive stimuli than in the negative trials [t38 = 2.104, p = 0.042, d = 0.322], while the controls showed a similar pattern of omissions. The ANOVA of accuracy revealed a significant group x congruence interaction [F1,78 = 6.358, p = 0.014], ADHD being less accurate than the controls in the congruent condition [t78 = 2.748, p = 0.007, d = 0.613]. Different t tests showed that in the ADHD group the N2pc component was significant for both conditions, positive [t38 = 2.211, p = 0.033] and negative [t38 = 2.048, p = 0.048], while for the controls it was only significant in the positive [t40 = 2.475, p = 0.01] condition. Overall, the amplitude of the P1 component was smaller in the ADHD group compared to the controls [F1,78 = 9.488, p = 0.003]. Regarding the clinical correlates, the authors observed a positive correlation between the SEK-27 scores and the N2pc amplitude [r = 0.35, p = 0.033], which differed significantly from the control group [r = -0.106, p = .511; z = -1,999, p = 0.023].

Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), Materna et al. [51] monitored brain activity while the participants (adults with ADHD and healthy controls) performed an emotional regulation task, consisting of "viewing" neutral or negative images or "reappraisal" of negative images. In the reappraisal condition, participants were instructed to reduce their negative emotional response by reinterpreting the image in a less negative way. They found a significant interaction effect when comparing neutral-negative conditions in ADHD with controls, resulting in three differential activity clusters in the dorsal and ventral anterior cingulated cortex, even after being corrected for multiple comparisons [p <0.05].

Finally, Li et al. [54] analyzed the relationship between resting-state EEG and daily executive functioning in adults with ADHD with high intelligence quotient (IQ). These authors found an association between relative beta power in central region and emotional control scores for ADHD [r = 0.464, p = 0.027], but did not in the control group [r = 0.129, p = 0.385].

Emotion regulation outcomes in adult ADHD treatment

As far as we know just four studies have measured ER as outcome in interventions to date. Mitchell et al. [71] observed a group (ADHD-waiting list) x time (before-after the intervention) interaction, with an improvement after a randomized-controlled intervention based in Mindfulness training in the inattention symptoms [p ≤ 0.002, d ≥ 1.09], in hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms [p ≤ 0.008, d ≥ 0.75], in functional impairment [p ≤ 0.003, d ≥ 1.52], in executive functions [p ≤ 0.005, d ≥ 1.45] and in emotion dysregulation [p ≤ 0.002, d ≥ 1.27]. Significant differences before-after treatment compared to the control group were found on the DERS Impulse Control Difficulties and Limited Access to Emotion Regulation Strategies subscales, both experiencing a significant reduction [p = 0.002, d = 1.60, and p = 0.007, d = 1.38, respectively]. However, Badoud et al. [72] did not obtain significant changes in any measure after a mentalization-based intervention without a control group, observing lower scores on the ASRSv1.1 and Emotion Reactivity Scale (ERS) subscales, which would indicate a reduction in symptoms, with small effect sizes.

Lastly, Reimherr et al. [73, 74] measured the effect on ED of atomoxetine and osmotic release oral system (OROS) methylphenidate in two double-blinded randomized-controlled trials in adults with ADHD. They found that both atomoxetine and OROS methylphenidate medications improved ED measured through the Wender–Reimherr Adult ADHD–Emotion Dysregulation Scale (WRAADDS-EDS) compared to a placebo [F = 15.10, p = 0.001, d’ = 0.66; F = 10.476, p = 0.002, d’ = 0.70; respectively].

Discussion

The aim of this review was to unify available data related to ED in adults with ADHD. To date, few studies have been conducted with adult population diagnosed with ADHD on emotion dysregulation, which is altered in 34–70% of affected adults, according to the review carried out by Shaw et al. [31]. However, some preliminary outcomes suggests that not only inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsiveness, but also emotional dysregulation are core components of ADHD [13] (see also [12, 98, 99] for similar conclusions). Despite this, no significant differences in ED were found between people with ADHD and people with other disorders, such as borderline personality disorder (BPD) [58, 62], being a controversial point.

According to our first objective of analyzing ER features of people with ADHD, the studies reviewed suggest that when adults with or without ADHD are compared, in line with our first hypothesis (H1), the ADHD group have consistently lower ER scores than controls using different measures (k = 10) [5154, 5863]. Although a differential response pattern between ADHD and other disorders (examples) could not be identified, evidence may indicate that high emotional dysregulation scores are associated with greater socio-functional impairment as H1 proposed (k = 3) [13, 63, 66]. Such emotional impairment could be the reason why health problems are usually observed in adults with ADHD, for instance those related to accidents and substance misuse, psychiatric comorbidity, poor academic and occupational performance [6, 100, 101], as well as in their personal relationships, self-esteem, and daily activities [100].

In terms of the second hypothesis (H2), another consistent finding is the habitual use of non-adaptive ER strategies in adults with ADHD (k = 4). For example, Rüfenacht et al. [63] observed an ADHD trend to blame themselves, catastrophize, blame others, and ruminate. Studies using ERQ consistently report a greater use of the suppression strategy and a lower use of reappraisal in the ADHD group compared to controls [51, 52, 70]. However, Matthies et al. [69] found that when using the suppression of emotion, a slower return to the baseline occurs in ADHD adults, not being so when using acceptance. This could indicate that despite being the most widely used strategy in this population, its implementation causes greater emotional distress by prolonging the time in which negative emotions are experienced, maintaining the use of other non-adaptive strategies (rumination, catastrophizing, etc.) and, consequently, of emotion dysregulation, in a kind of vicious circle. Nevertheless, it could also indicate the possibility of improving the emotional response by training on strategies used to regulate emotions, as it has been observed that when ADHD symptoms improve, so does emotional dysregulation [31, 71]. Likewise, the application of specific training in ER in children with ADHD may improve their ER skills [102]. It also seems that the usual pharmacological treatments for ADHD improve the symptoms of emotional dysregulation [73, 74], reducing their emotional discomfort.

Regarding our second objective of analyzing ADHD symptoms and its relationship with ED, we conclude that it is not possible to establish a clear distinction between ADHD subtypes based on performance on ER measurement instruments with the available evidence. However, it has been observed that the combined subtype tends to load more ED [60] and that the greater the severity of ADHD symptoms, the greater the emotion dysregulation [63]. These data are in line with the results obtained in other studies carried out with children and adolescents with ADHD [103]. In contrast, Hirsch et al. [13] considered the presence of high negative affect and failure to implement adaptive ER strategies as a distinctive feature of adult ADHD. Similarly, Edel et al. [65] identified having parents with ADHD symptoms as a risk factor of ED, with maternal symptoms having the greatest impact. These last results are supported by a subsequent study carried out by Mazursky-Horowitz et al. [104], in which they found that the ED in mothers mediated the relationship between maternal ADHD symptoms and severe parenting responses to the expression of negative emotions by adolescents.

Concerning intervention programs to improve ED in people with ADHD, preliminary data points to the effectiveness of Mindfulness training as an effective intervention for this purpose [71]. However, the results of the mentalization-based intervention are not significant, despite appreciating a trend towards improvement in symptoms [72]. More studies of treatments that consider emotional variables are needed. Additionally, it is interesting to highlight the results obtained by Mitchell et al. [71] regarding the improvement in EF measured with questionnaires, but not with lab task. According to Barkley [10], daily EF and ER correlate when measured with rating scales, but this is not what the lab tasks showed, as he has challenged. Silverstein et al. [66] obtained evidence that supports these last results.

Finally, our third objective focused on identifying characteristics of brain functioning in adults with ADHD. Using fMR, Materna et al. [51] did not find differences in the brain activation of people with and without ADHD in the condition of explicit emotional regulation when participants were instructed to reappraise the image in a less negative way. However, given the condition of view, where they had to attend to the image without performing avoidance behaviors, authors found significant differences in the cortical activation of both groups, with the dorsal and ventral anterior cingulated cortex being highly activated in the ADHD group compared to control group. Authors suggested that these differences are due to the implementation of implicit regulatory strategies. However, as we have pointed out previously, people with ADHD often use emotional suppression strategies rather than reappraisal ones. In these cases, therapeutic training in the use of more adaptive regulatory strategies, such as reappraisal, could improve their emotional competence, as noted above.

For their part, Shushakova et al. [52, 70] focused on evoked potentials, finding that the amplitude of the LPP in the central parietal and frontal areas, as well as the amplitude of the N2pc (related to attentional bias) were associated with the severity of the ADHD symptoms. The N2pc component confirms the attention bias that people with ADHD manifest towards emotional stimuli. The greater amplitude of the frontal LPP suggests a greater cognitive effort to regulate their emotions and would support the hypothesis of emotional hyper-reactivity in ADHD when they process aversive stimuli. This type of activity would fit with the difficulties of top-down regulation, that is, the modulating effect that EF would have on the emotional reaction at the base through frontal networks. According to Barkley [12], this fact would reflect that dysregulation in ADHD is a top-down problem rather than a bottom-up problem, and would be independent of the IQ, since adults with ADHD with high IQ also showed impairments in daily EF along with aberrant resting-state EEG patterns relative to controls [54].

Overall, anomalous activation patterns are observed in adults with ADHD compared to adults without ADHD. It is well-known that the anterior cingulate cortex integrates attentional, mnemonic, and emotional information and activates the dlPFC, which promotes decision-making based on emotional and objective information [19]. Abnormal activation patterns seen in ADHD may interfere with emotional processing from the beginning, biasing attention toward emotional salient stimuli [52, 70, 105, 106]. This would limit ER and executive processes carried out by the dlPFC, due to the lack of relevant emotional and contextual information. In this line, one possible strategy of interventions that could be used to improve these impaired patterns in ADHD patients may focus on attention processes such as Mindfulness, given certain preliminary outcomes [71, 107]. This idea finds support in Bailey and Jones’ integrated model of regulation [108]. According to this model, the development of ER would rely on the establishment of essential components, such as attention and inhibition mechanisms that are affected in ADHD from early childhood.

Some limitations need to be highlighted. The studies found that meet the selection criteria are scarce and very heterogeneous both in aims and in sample features, mainly in variables as relevant as sex, medication status and comorbidity. As noted above, not taking gender into account, as well as the presence of comorbidities such as BPD, could overestimate ED scores differences between ADHD and controls. In contrast, ADHD drugs could mask them, as they tend to reduce ED severity symptoms [73, 74]. The lack of unifying criteria for the assessment of both ADHD and ED makes it difficult to compare studies, as well as to draw generalizable conclusions. Furthermore, recent research suggests the need to add performance tests to ADHD diagnosis to avoid malingering [109]. Symptom-based tests have been shown to be less effective in detecting it, but they are the main source of diagnosis [109]. This fact of high heterogeneity reduces the potential for future meta-analyses on this topic unless they are limited to a small subset of comparable studies.

To conclude, our findings highlight the emotional side of ADHD in adults and the convenience of new research on the topic. Although emotional dysregulation is present in ADHD, its presence in other disorders does not justify that ED is a core symptom of ADHD. The establishment of flexible, widely agreed guidelines for diagnosis, including performance and symptom-based tests, and psycho-educational interventions focused on improving the impact of emotional dysregulation is needed. Future studies are necessary to jointly analyze the results obtained with different measures of ER, including ER strategies. Other aspects such as emotional recognition and processing, which are a prerequisite for optimal ER [42], need more research in adults with ADHD as well. In addition, the impact of emotional dysregulation on adult ADHD needs to be analyzed in terms of sex differences, comorbidity, and medication treatments.

References

  1. 1. Roberts W, Milich R, Barkley RA. Primary symptoms, diagnostic criteria, subtyping, and prevalence of ADHD. In: Barkley RA, editor. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, fourth edition. New York: Guilford Publication; 2015. p. 51–80.
  2. 2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edition. Arlington: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
  3. 3. Luo Y, Weibman D, Halperin J, Li X. A Review of heterogeneity in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2019;13(42). pmid:30804772
  4. 4. Thapar A, Harold G, Rice F, Langley K, O’Donovan M. The contribution of gene-environment interaction to psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19(4):989–1004. pmid:17931430
  5. 5. Franke B, Michelini G, Asherson P, Banaschewski T, Bilbow A, Buitelaar JK, et al. Live fast, die young? A review on the developmental trajectories of ADHD across the lifespan. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;28(10):1059–1088. pmid:30195575
  6. 6. Barkley RA. Comorbid psychiatric disorders and psychological maladjustment in adults with ADHD. In: Barkley RA, editor. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, fourth edition. New York: Guilford Publication; 2015. p. 343–355.
  7. 7. Leffa DT, Caye A, Rohde LA. ADHD in children and adults: diagnosis and prognosis. In: Current topics in behavioral neurosciences. Berlin: Springer; 2022. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2022_329
  8. 8. Karalunas S, Antovich D, Goh P, Martel M, Tipsord J, Nousen E, et al. Longitudinal network model of the co-development of temperament, executive functioning, and psychopathology symptoms in youth with and without ADHD. Development and Psychopathology. 2021;33(5):1803–1820. pmid:35210712
  9. 9. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin. 1997;121(1): 65–94. pmid:9000892
  10. 10. Barkley RA. Executive Functioning and Self-Regulation viewed as an extended phenotype implications of the theory for ADHD and its treatment. In: Barkley RA, editor. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, fourth edition. New York: Guilford Publication; 2015. p. 405–434.
  11. 11. Stark R, Bauer E, Merz CJ, Zimmermann M, Reuter M, Plichta MM, et al. ADHD related behaviors are associated with brain activation in the reward system. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(3):426–434. pmid:21163276
  12. 12. Barkley RA. Emotional dysregulation is a core component of ADHD. In: Barkley RA, editor. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, fourth edition. New York: Guilford Publication; 2015. p. 81–115.
  13. 13. Hirsch O, Chavanon ML, Reichmann E, Christiansen H. Emotional dysregulation is a primary symptom in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Affective Disorders. 2018;232:41–47. pmid:29477097
  14. 14. Otero TM, Barker LA. The frontal lobe and executive functioning. In: Goldstein S, Naglieri J, editors. Handbook of executive functioning. New York: Springer; 2014. p. 29–44. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-8106-5
  15. 15. Sesack SR, Grace AA. Cortico-basal ganglia reward network: microcircuitry. Neuropsychopharmacology.2010;35(1):27–47. pmid:19675534
  16. 16. Catani M, Dell’Acqua F, De Schotten MT. A revised limbic system model for memory, emotion and behaviour. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013;37(8):1724–1737. pmid:23850593
  17. 17. LeDoux J. Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron. 2012;73(4):653–676. pmid:22365542
  18. 18. Panksepp J, Clarici A, Vandekerckhove M, Yovell Y. Neuro-evolutionary foundations of infant minds: from psychoanalytic visions of how primal emotions guide constructions of human minds toward affective neuroscientific understanding of emotions and their disorders. Psychoanalytic Inquiry. 2019;39(1):36–51.
  19. 19. Damasio AR. Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: G.P. Putnam. 1994.
  20. 20. Gyurak A, Goodkind MS, Kramer JH, Miller BL. Executive functions and the down-regulation and up-regulation of emotion. Cognition and Emotion. 2012;26(1):103–118. pmid:21432634
  21. 21. Pessoa L. The cognitive-emotional brain: From interactions to integration. MIT press. 2013.
  22. 22. Sato JR, Hoexter MQ, Castellanos XF, Rohde LA. Abnormal brain connectivity patterns in adults with ADHD: a coherence study. PloS ONE. 2012;7(9):e45671. pmid:23049834
  23. 23. Bush G. Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Attention Networks. Neuropsychopharmacology Reviews. 2010;35:278–300. pmid:19759528
  24. 24. Friedman LA, Rapoport JL. Brain development in ADHD. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2015;30:106–111. pmid:25500059
  25. 25. Frodl T, Stauber J, Schaaff N, Koutsouleris N, Scheuerecker J, Ewers M, et al. Amygdala reduction in patients with ADHD compared with major depression and healthy volunteers. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2010;121(2):111–118. pmid:19878138
  26. 26. Nickel K, van Elst LT, Perlov E, Jitten-Schachenmeier R, Beier D, Endres D, et al. Manual morphometry of hippocampus and amygdala in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 2017;267:32–35. pmid:28734177
  27. 27. Baribeau DA, Dupuis A, Paton TA, Hammill C, Scherer SW, Schachar RJ, et al. Structural neuroimaging correlates of social deficits are similar in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: analysis from the POND Network Translational Psychiatry. 2019;9:72. pmid:30718456
  28. 28. Nigg JT, Casey BJ. An integrative theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder based on the cognitive and affective neurosciences. Development and Psychopathology. 2015;17:785–806. pmid:16262992
  29. 29. Jonker FA, Jonker C, Scheltens P, Scherder EJA. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in cognition and behavior. Reviews in the Neurosciences. 2015;26(1):1–11. pmid:25252749
  30. 30. Kringelbach M, Rolls E. The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: Evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Progress in Neurobiology. 2004;72(5):341–372. pmid:15157726
  31. 31. Shaw P, Stringaris A, Nigg J, Leibenluft E. Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The American journal of psychiatry. 2014;171(3):276–293. pmid:24480998
  32. 32. Gross J J. The emerging field of Emotion Regulation: an integrative review. Review of General Psychology. 1998;2(3):271–299.
  33. 33. Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology. 2002;39:281–291. pmid:12212647
  34. 34. Gross JJ. Emotion Regulation: current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry. 2015;26(1):1–26.
  35. 35. Gallo IS, Keil A, McCulloch KC, Rockstroh B, Gollwitzer PM. Strategic automation of emotion regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2009;96(1):11–31. pmid:19210061
  36. 36. Bridgett DJ, Oddi KB, Laake LM, Murdock KW, Bachmann MN. Integrating and differentiating aspects of self-regulation: effortful control, executive functioning, and links to negative affectivity. Emotion. 2013;12(1):47–63. pmid:22906086
  37. 37. Lantrip C, Isquith PK, Koven NS, Welsh K, Roth RM. Executive function and emotion regulation strategy use in adolescents. Applied Neuropsychology: Child. 2015;0:1–6. pmid:25650638
  38. 38. Sjöwall D, Thorell LB. A critical appraisal of the role of neuropsychological deficits in preschool ADHD. Child Neuropsychology. 2019;25(1):60–80. pmid:29536801
  39. 39. Bunford N, Evans SW, Wymbs F. ADHD and Emotion Dysregulation among children and adolescents. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2015;18:185–217. pmid:26243645
  40. 40. Christiansen H, Hirsch O, Albrecht B, Chavanon ML. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and Emotion Regulation over the life span. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2019;21(17). pmid:30826879
  41. 41. Ludlow AK, Garrood A, Lawrence K, Gutiérrez R. Emotion recognition from dynamic emotional displays in children with ADHD. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2014;33(5):413–427.
  42. 42. Rodrigo-Ruiz D, Pérez-González JC, Cejudo J. Emotional facial recognition difficulties as primary deficit in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review. Revista de Neurologia. 2017;65(4):145–152.
  43. 43. Tehrani-Doost M, Noorazar G, Shahrivar Z, Banaraki AK, Beigi PF, Noorian N. Is emotion recognition related to core symptoms of childhood ADHD? J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;26(1):31–38. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5349280/
  44. 44. Groves NB, Kofler MJ, Wells EL, Day TN, Chan ESM. An examination of relations among Working Memory, ADHD symptoms, and Emotion Regulation. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2020;48:525–537. pmid:31900835
  45. 45. Breaux RP, McQuade JD, Harvey EA, Zakarian RJ. Longitudinal associations of parental emotion socialization and children’s Emotion Regulation: The moderating role of ADHD symptomatology. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2018;46:671–683. pmid:28710531
  46. 46. Margherio SM, Brickner MA, Evans SW, Sarno Owens J, DuPaul GJ, Allan NP. The role of emotion regulation in alcohol use among adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2020;34(7):772–782. pmid:32271054
  47. 47. Sheenar-Golan V, Wald N, Yatzkar U. Patterns of emotion regulation emotion-related behaviors among parents of children with and without ADHD. Psychiatry Research. 2017;258:494–500. pmid:28890229
  48. 48. Vertsberger D, Roskam I, Talmon A, van Bakel H, Hall R, Mikolajczak M, et al. Emotion regulation during the COVID-19 pandemic: risk and resilience factors for parental burnout (IIPB). Cognition and Emotion. 2022;36(1):100–105. pmid:34821543
  49. 49. Morris SSJ, Musser ED, Tenembaum RB, Ward AR, Martínez J, Raiker JS, et al. Emotion Regulation via the Autonomic Nervous System in Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Replication and Extension. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2020;48:361–373. pmid:31808007
  50. 50. Tenembaum RB, Musser ED, Morris S, Ward AR, Raiker JS, Coles EK, et al. Response Inhibition, Response Execution, and Emotion Regulation among Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2019;47:589–203. pmid:30112596
  51. 51. Materna L, Wiesner CD, Shushakova A, Trieloff J, Weber N, Engell A, et al. Adult patients with ADHD differ from healthy controls in implicit, but not explicit, emotion regulation. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2019;44(5):340–349. pmid:31025560
  52. 52. Shushakova A, Ohrmann P, Pedersen A. Exploring deficient emotion regulation in adult ADHD: electrophysiological evidence. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2018;268:359–371. pmid:28770370
  53. 53. Thorell LB, Tilling H, Sjöwall D. Emotion dysregulation in adult ADHD: Introducing the Comprehensive Emotion Regulation Inventory (CERI). Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2020;42(7):747–758. pmid:32787552
  54. 54. Li H, Zhao Q, Huang F, Cao Q, Qian Q, Johnstone SJ, et al. Increased beta activity links to impaired emotional control in ADHD adults with high IQ. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2019;23(7):754–764. pmid:29110563
  55. 55. Friedman SR, Rapport LJ, Lumley M, Tzelepis A, VanVoorhis A, Stettner L, et al. Aspects of social and emotional competence in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology. 2003;17(1):50. pmid:12597073
  56. 56. Berking M, Wupperman P. Emotion regulation and mental health: recent findings, current challenges, and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2012;25(2):128–134. pmid:22262030
  57. 57. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6:e1000097. pmid:19621072
  58. 58. Cavelti M, Corbisiero S, Bitto H, Moerstedt B, Newark P, Faschina S, et al. A Comparison of self-reported emotional regulation skills in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and borderline personality disorder. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2019;23(12):1396–1406. pmid:28367706
  59. 59. Helfer B, Boxhoorn S, Songa J, Steel C, Maltezos S, Asherson P. Emotion recognition and mind wandering in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder or autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2021;134:89–96. pmid:33373778
  60. 60. Hirsch O, Chavanon ML, Christiansen H. Emotional dysregulation subgroups in patients with adult Attention- Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): a cluster analytic approach. Scientific Reports. 2019;9:5639. pmid:30948735
  61. 61. Mitchell JT, McClernon FJ, Beckham JC, Brown RA, Lejuez CW, Kollins SH. Smoking abstinence effects on emotion dysregulation in adult cigarette smokers with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2019;205:107594. pmid:31639512
  62. 62. Moukhtarian TR, Reinhard I, Moran P, Ryckaert C, Skirrow C, Ebner-Priemer U, et al. Comparable emotional dynamics in women with ADHD and borderline personality disorder. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation. 2021;8(6). pmid:33579385
  63. 63. Rüfenacht E, Euler S, Prada P, Nicastro R, Dieben K, Hasler R, et al. Emotion dysregulation in adults suffering from attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), a comparison with borderline personality disorder (BPD). Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation. 2019;6(11). pmid:31360522
  64. 64. Anker E, Haavik J, Heir T. Alcohol and drug use disorders in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: prevalence and associations with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom severity and emotional dysregulation. World Journal of Psychiatry. 2020;10(9):202. pmid:33014721
  65. 65. Edel MA, Juckel G, Brüne M. Interaction of recalled parental ADHD symptoms and rearing behavior with current attachment and emotional dysfunction in adult offspring with ADHD. Psychiatry Research. 2010;178:137–141. pmid:20452044
  66. 66. Silverstein M, Leon T, Krone B, Faraone S, Newcorn J, Adler L. The characteristics and unique impairments of comorbid adult ADHD and sluggish cognitive tempo: An interim analysis. Psychiatric Annals. 2019;49(10):457–465.
  67. 67. Anker E, Ginsberg Y, Heir T. Prevalence of criminal convictions in Norwegian adult ADHD outpatients and associations with ADHD symptom severity and emotional dysregulation. BMC psychiatry. 2021;21(1):1–8. pmid:33941124
  68. 68. Brancati GE, Barbuti M, Schiavi E, Colombini P, Moriconi M, Pallucchini A, et al. Comparison of emotional dysregulation features in cyclothymia and adult ADHD. Medicina. 2021;57(5):489. pmid:34066126
  69. 69. Matthies S, Philipsen A, Lackner HK, Sadohara C, Svaldi J. Regulation of sadness via acceptance or suppression in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Psychiatry Research. 2014;220:461–467. pmid:25085791
  70. 70. Shushakova A, Weisner CD, Ohrmann P, Pedersen A. Electrophysiological evidence of an attentional bias towards appetitive and aversive words in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2018;129:1937–1946. pmid:30007893
  71. 71. Mitchell JT, McIntyre EM, English JS, Dennis MF, Beckham JC, Kollins SH. A pilot trial of Mindfulness meditation training for ADHD in adulthood: impact on core symptoms, executive functioning, and emotion dysregulation. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2017;21(3):1105–1120. pmid:24305060
  72. 72. Badoud D, Rüfenacht E, Debbané M, Perroud N. Mentalization-based treatment for adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a pilot study. Research in Psychotherapy: Psychopathology, Process and Outcome. 2018;21(3):149–154. pmid:32913767
  73. 73. Reimherr FW, Marchant BK, Strong RE, Hedges DW, Adler L, Spencer TJ, et al. (2005). Emotional dysregulation in adult ADHD and response to atomoxetine. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;58(2):125–131. pmid:16038683
  74. 74. Reimherr FW, Williams ED, Strong RE, Mestas R, Soni P, Marchant BK. A double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover study of osmotic release oral system methylphenidate in adults with ADHD with assessment of oppositional and emotional dimensions of the disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2007:68(1):93–101. pmid:17284136
  75. 75. Epstein JN, Johnson DE, Conners CK. Conners Adult ADHD Diagnostic Interview for DSM-IV. New York: Multi-Health Systems. 1999.
  76. 76. Kooij JJS, Francken MH. Diagnostisch Interview Voor ADHD bij volwassenen (DIVA 2.0). DIVA Fundation. 2010.
  77. 77. DuPaul G, Power T, Anastopoulos A, Reid R. ADHD Rating Scales, IV: Checklist Norms and Clinical Interpretation. New York: Guilford Press. 1998.
  78. 78. Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, Demler O, Faraone S, Hiripi E, et al. The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med. 2005;35:245–56. pmid:15841682
  79. 79. Barkley RA. Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV (BAARS-IV). New York: Guilford Press. 2011.
  80. 80. Conners C, Erhardt D, Sparrow N. Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS). New York: Multi-Health Systems. 1999.
  81. 81. Barkley RA, Murphy KR. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A clinical workbook. 3rd ed. New York: Guilford Press. 2006.
  82. 82. Wender PH. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. Oxford University Press. 1995.
  83. 83. Ward MF, Wender PH, Reimherr FW. The Wender Utah Rating Scale: an aid in the retrospective diagnosis of childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150:885–890. pmid:8494063
  84. 84. Rösler M, Retz W, Thome J, Schneider M, Stieglitz RD. Psychopathological rating scales for diagnostic use in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). European archives of psychiatry and clinical neuroscience. 2006;256(1):i3–i11. pmid:16977549
  85. 85. Adler L, Cohen J. Diagnosis and evaluation of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2004;27(2):187–201. pmid:15063992
  86. 86. Adler L, Spencer T. The Adult ADHD Clinical Diagnostic Scale (ACDS), version 1.2. New York University School of Medicine. 2004.
  87. 87. Silverstein MJ, Faraone SV, Alperin S, Leon TL, Biederman J, Spencer TJ, et al. Validation of the expanded versions of the adult ADHD self-report scale v1.1 symptom checklist and the adult ADHD investigator symptom rating scale. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2019;23(10):1101–1110. pmid:29419345
  88. 88. Gratz K, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2004;26:41–54.
  89. 89. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;85:348–362. pmid:12916575
  90. 90. Garnefsky N, Kraaij V, Spinhoven P. Manual for the use of Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. Leiderdorp: DATEC. 2002.
  91. 91. Nock MK, Wedig MW, Holmberg EB, Hooley JM. The Emotion Reactivity Scale: Development, evaluation, and relation to self-injurious thoughts and behaviours. Behavior Therapy. 2008;39(2):107–116. pmid:18502244
  92. 92. Berking M, Znoj H. Entwicklung und Validierung eines Fragebogens zur standardisierten Selbsteinschätzung emotionaler Kompetenzen (SEK-27). Zeitschrift für Psychiatrie, Psychologie und Psychotherapie. 2008;56(2):141–53.
  93. 93. Grant M, Salsman NL, Berking M. The assessment of successful emotion regulation skills use: Development and validation of an English version of the Emotion Regulation Skills Questionnaire. PloS ONE. 2018;13(10):e0205095. pmid:30281666
  94. 94. Behr M, Becker M. Skalen zum Erleben von Emotionen (SEE). Göttingen: Hogrefe. 2004.
  95. 95. Barkley RA. ADHD in adults: comorbidity and adaptive impairments. NIMH Grant number 1R01MH054509-01A2. 1997.
  96. 96. Brancati GE, Barbuti M, Pallucchini A, Cotugno B, Schiavi E, Hantouche EG, et al. Reactivity, Intensity, Polarity and Stability questionnaire (RIPoSt-40) assessing emotional dysregulation: Development, reliability and validity. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2019;257:187–194. pmid:31301622
  97. 97. Roth RM, Isquith PK, Gioia GA. BRIEF-A: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function–Adult Version: Professional manual. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources. 2005.
  98. 98. Barkley RA. Deficient Emotional Self-Regulation: A Core Component of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of ADHD and Related Disorders. 2010;1:5–37.
  99. 99. Graziano P, Garcia A. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and children’s emotion dysregulation: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2016;46:106–123. pmid:27180913
  100. 100. Kirino E, Imagawa H, Goto T, Montgomery W. Sociodemographics, comorbidities, healthcare utilization and work productivity in Japanese patients with adult ADHD. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(7):e0132233. pmid:26147097
  101. 101. Newlove-Delgado T, Stein K. Adult Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): public health implications. Perspectives in Public Health. 2012;132(5):209–210. pmid:22991365
  102. 102. Sánchez M, Lavigne R, Romero JF, Elósegui E. Emotion regulation in participants diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, before and after an emotion regulation intervention. Frontiers in Psychology. 2019;10:1092. pmid:31178779
  103. 103. Baykal S, Nalbantoğlu A. An Examination of Emotion Regulation and Associated Factors in Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder. Konuralp Tıp Dergisi. 2019; 269–273.
  104. 104. Mazursky-Horowitz H., Felton JW, MacPherson L, Ehrlich KB, Cassidy J, Lejuez CW, et al. Maternal emotion regulation mediates the association between adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and parenting. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 2015;43(1):121–131. pmid:24928735
  105. 105. Marx I, Domes G, Havenstein C, Berger C, Schulze L, Herpertz SC. Enhanced emotional interference on working memory performance in adults with ADHD. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry. 2011;12(sup1):70–75. pmid:21905999
  106. 106. Marx I, Krause J, Berger C, Häßler F. Dissociable patterns in the control of emotional interference in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and in adults with alcohol dependence. PloS ONE. 2014;9(9):e107750. pmid:25265290
  107. 107. Huguet A, Eguren JI, Miguel-Ruiz D, Vallés XV, Alda JA. Deficient emotional self-regulation in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: mindfulness as a useful treatment modality. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2019;40(6):425–431. pmid:31135603
  108. 108. Bailey R, Jones SM. An integrated model of regulation for applied settings. Clinical child and family psychology review. 2019;22(1): 2–23. pmid:30783912
  109. 109. Wallace ER, Garcia-Willingham NE, Walls BD, Bosch CM, Balthrop KC, Berry DT. A meta-analysis of malingering detection measures for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Assessment, 2019;31(2): 265. pmid:30359048