Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Association between desire thinking and problematic social media use among a sample of Lebanese adults: The indirect effect of suppression and impulsivity

  • Emmanuelle Awad,

    Roles Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Social and Education Sciences Department, School of Arts and Sciences, Lebanese American University, Beirut, Lebanon

  • Myriam El Khoury-Malhame,

    Roles Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Social and Education Sciences Department, School of Arts and Sciences, Lebanese American University, Beirut, Lebanon

  • Ecem Yakin,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre d’Études et de Recherches en Psychopathologie et Psychologie de la Santé, Université de Toulouse, UT2J, Toulouse, France

  • Venise Hanna,

    Roles Data curation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Pharmacy, Lebanese International University, Beirut, Lebanon

  • Diana Malaeb,

    Roles Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations School of Pharmacy, Lebanese International University, Beirut, Lebanon, College of Pharmacy, Gulf Medical University, Ajman, United Arab Emirates

  • Souheil Hallit ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    souheilhallit@hotmail.com

    ‡ SH and SO are last coauthors.

    Affiliations School of Medicine and Medical Sciences, Holy Spirit University of Kaslik, Jounieh, Lebanon, Research Department, Psychiatric Hospital of the Cross, Jal Eddib, Lebanon, Applied Science Research Center, Applied Science Private University, Amman, Jordan

  • Sahar Obeid

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ SH and SO are last coauthors.

    Affiliation Social and Education Sciences Department, School of Arts and Sciences, Lebanese American University, Beirut, Lebanon

Abstract

Background

Desire thinking, impulsivity and suppression are psychological variables that are intricately related to behavioral addictions. Bearing in mind the scarcity of data on desire thinking, impulsivity, thought suppression and pathological social media use in developing countries such as Lebanon, with the existing literature suggesting a heightened mental health burden associated with this problematic social media use, it becomes all the more important to elucidate their relationship. Our study aims at investigating the association between desire thinking and problematic social media use specifically, and to further test the effect of impulsivity and thought suppression in mediating the relation between the two distinct facets of desire thinking and problematic social media use.

Methods

A cross-sectional study was carried out between November 2021 and March 2022 using a sample of 414 community-dwelling participants aged above 18 years from all Lebanese districts. The data was collected through an online questionnaire including a section about sociodemographic information, the Desire Thinking Questionnaire (DTQ), Impulsive Behavior Scale (S-UPPS-P), White Bear Suppression Inventory (WBSI) and Social Media Disorder Short Form (SMD). The tests used in the bivariate analysis to assess correlates of SMD were the Student t test to compare two means and the Pearson test to correlate two continuous scores. The PROCESS SPSS Macro version 3.4, model four was used to conduct the mediation analysis.

Results

Desire thinking was shown to correlate with increased social media use. Moreover, we found that suppression and lack of premeditation mediated the association between verbal perseveration and social media use disorder whereas suppression and urgency mediated the association between imaginal prefiguration and social media use disorder.

Conclusion

This study provides new insight on a topic of increasing public health concern. Although understudied to date, suppression and impulsivity differentially mediate the influence of both facets of desire thinking on problematic social media use disorder. The current findings point to the highly pervasive issue of social media use disorder and the need to investigate underlying psychological factors that aggravate it to better profile and support individuals struggling with it.

Introduction

Desire thinking is a cognitive process originally intended to control intrusive experiences related to cravings but ultimately escalates it by voluntarily elaborating a desired behavior on its two dimensions: imaginal prefiguration and verbal perseveration [1]. The first aspect consists of sustaining attention to the information related to desired behavior, generating a tendency to anticipate multi-sensory positive memories whereas the latter includes prolonged self-talks about positive reasons to engage in achievements related to desired behavior. The metacognitive theory [2] stipulates that this trait-like thought process is preserved by beliefs centered around elaborating the desired behavior yet not acting upon it, which increases the craving to go through with addictive behaviors [3].

Desire thinking was first shown to predict various substance addictions such as smoking [4], and alcohol consumption [5]. A recent meta-analysis further documented similar results in behavioral addiction including gambling and sex addiction [6]. Although initially intended to better control negative experiences associated with addictions such as cravings and negative affect, desire thinking may manifest as a maladaptive coping strategy eventually increasing vulnerability to addictions. This is particularly relevant in the case of Internet related maladaptive behaviors such as gaming, pornography and problematic social media use, with an exponential increase in reported numbers of such maladaptive behaviors in the recent years [7]. Desire thinking has been investigated as a predictor of Internet addiction [8] and maladaptive use of social media with platform such as Facebook, Twitter or Instagram [9]. Therefore, better understanding the relationship between desire thinking and such pathological behaviors could facilitate its subsequent exploration as a potential target for the treatment of pervasive behavioral addictions, notably the escalating pathological use of social media worldwide by billions of users.

Other psychological constructs that are closely linked to behavioral and Internet addictions include cognitive and emotional variables such as thought suppression [10] and impulsivity [11]. Higher impulsivity and an inability to regulate one’s behaviors is indeed linked to higher levels of addiction [12]. Similarly, attempting to inhibit or suppress one’s thought increases risk of addiction [13]. This could be the case as thought suppression could be viewed an attempt to ignore unwanted thoughts, which backfires [14]. On the other side, impulsivity is the tendency to react in a fast and incautious manner regardless of consequent repercussions to the self and others [15], increasing addictive tendencies. Impulsivity was further shown to include multiple facets [16], such as negative urgency, lack of perseverance, lack of premeditation, sensation seeking and positive urgency [17], which could reinforce addictions differently. Negative urgency consists of the inclination to behave in an impulsive way when experiencing negative affect [18]. Lack of perseverance is failing to maintain attention and complete challenging tasks [18]. Lack of premeditation is the tendency to act without assessing the consequences of the behavior [18]. Sensation seeking is pursuing certain actions that are pleasurable and enticing despite it being threatening [18].

Impulsivity and thought suppression were consistently studied in correlation to desire thinking [19]. It is important to note that those factors were identified in many different pathways related to cravings and behavioral addictions. For instance, some studies showed that those who engage in attempts to control and suppress addiction-related thoughts had higher desire thinking scores and ended up having increased addictive behaviors [20]. This was found to be significant in relation to problematic use of social media, where desired thinking was defined as a predictor in one study [21] and a mediator in the relation between craving and problematic social media use in another [22]. One recent study showed that impulsivity was a direct predictor of addiction to social media in a sample of adults [23]. Alongside thought suppression, impulsivity was also shown to have an indirect effect on the problematic use of social media [22]. In yet another contemporary research, suppression and impulsivity both mediated the association between thought processes and multiple behavioral addictions, including problematic use of social media [24].

The latest COVID pandemic has particularly inflated the already problematic use of social media platforms [25], as reports of hours spent on social media skyrocketed. Social media addiction has been reported as a pervasive problem in modern societies and raises significant concerns related to mental and physical health [26]. In this context, the Middle East is known to have a high level of Internet accessibility and subsequent smartphone and social media use. Lebanon is one of the Middle Eastern countries with the fastest increase in internet use. In fact in 2017, 80% of Lebanese adults had a smartphone with internet access, in comparison with only 57% in 2015 [27]. Additionally, 72% of Lebanese adults in 2017 reported using social media in comparison to only 53% of French adults and 20% of Indian adults [28]. As such, we decided to investigate the association between problematic social media use and desire thinking in Lebanon. Knowing that desire thinking, impulsivity and thought suppression might intervene at various steps in the etiology of this behavioral addictions [21], we further examined the effect of different facets of impulsivity and thought suppression in mediating the relation between the two distinct dimensions of desire thinking and problematic social media use among a sample of Lebanese adults.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first time such factorial triads are investigated. Bearing in mind the scarcity of data on desire thinking, impulsivity, thought suppression and pathological social media use in developing countries such as Lebanon, with the existing literature suggesting a heightened mental health burden associated with this problematic social media use [29], it becomes all the more important to investigate the relationships at hand. As such, we hypothesized that higher desire thinking would be correlated with higher pathological social media use, mediated by higher suppression and impulsivity. In addition, the mediation role of thought suppression between desire thinking and problematic social media use was assessed.

Methods

Study design and participants

A cross-sectional study, enrolling 414 participants, was carried out between November 2021 and March 2022, using a sample of community-dwelling Lebanese residents aged above 18 years. Due to the social restrictions caused by the COVID-19 pandemic, we used an anonymous, self-administered questionnaire created on Google forms. The link was shared among the participants and sent to all districts/governorates of Lebanon (Beirut, Mount Lebanon, North Lebanon, South Lebanon, and Bekaa) through social networks, using the snowball technique.

Minimal sample size calculation

A minimal sample of 410 was deemed necessary using the formula suggested by Fritz and MacKinnon [30] to estimate the sample size: , where f = 0.14 for small effect size, L = 7.85 for an α error of 5% and power β = 80%, and k = 8 variables to be entered in the model.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Psychiatric Hospital of the Cross Ethics and Research Committee approved this study protocol (HPC-041-2021). A written informed consent was considered obtained from each participant when submitting the online form. All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.

Questionnaire and variables

A self-administered, anonymous questionnaire with closed-ended questions was circulated to participants. It required approximately 25–30 minutes to be completed.

Sociodemographic characteristics

The first part included socio-demographic characteristics: age, gender, educational level, and household crowding index (the latter was calculated by dividing the number of persons living in the house by the number of rooms, excluding the kitchen and the bathroom [31]). The physical activity index was calculated by multiplying the intensity by the frequency by the time of physical activity [32]. Body Mass Index (BMI) was calculated from self-reported height and weight. Participants were asked to rate their financial burden using one question on a scale from 1 to 10, with 10 referring to overwhelming pressure.

Desire Thinking Questionnaire (DTQ)

It is a self-reported measure of desire thinking that includes 5 items evaluating imaginal prefiguration (e.g. “I imagine myself doing the desired activity”) and 5 others evaluating verbal perseveration (e.g. “If I did not practice the desired activity for a long time, I would think about it continuously”) [33]. All 10 items are rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (always). Higher scores on the scale indicate higher tendency for desire thinking. The Cronbach alpha values in this study were 0.97 for verbal perseveration and 0.99 for imaginal prefiguration.

White Bear Suppression Inventory (WBSI)

It is a self-reported measure of thought suppression including 15 items such as “There are things I prefer not to think about” [34]. All items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 5 (Strongly agree). Scores range between 15 and 75 with higher scores on the scale indicate higher tendency for suppressing one’s thoughts. The Cronbach’s alpha in this study was 0.94.

Social Media Disorder Short Form (SMD)

Validated in Lebanon [35], it is a self-reported measure of problematic social media use including 9 items such as “During the past year have you often felt bad when you could not use social media?” [36]. All items have dichotomous (yes/no) answers. Higher scores on the scale indicate higher tendency for problematic social media use. The Cronbach’s alpha in this study was 0.85.

I-8 scale

Impulsivity was measured using the I-8 scale, which yields four dimensions: urgency, lack of premeditation, lack of perseverance and sensation seeking on a 6-point scale from 0 (doesn’t apply at all) to 5 (applies completely). Higher scores indicate higher impulsivity level in each of the four subscales: urgency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.86), lack of premeditation (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.93), lack of perseverance (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.72) and sensation seeking (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.97). The factor structure of the Arabic version of the scale has been previously verified [37].

Translation procedure

A bilingual translator was responsible for the translation from English to Arabic. A backward translation was then performed by a native English-speaking translator, fluent in Arabic and unfamiliar with the concepts of the scales. Discrepancies were resolved by consensus between translators and researchers.

Statistical analysis

To check the psychometric properties of the DTQ and WBSI scales, we used an EFA-to-CFA strategy [38]. To ensure adequate sample sizes for both EFA and CFA, we split the main sample using the SPSS computer-generated random technique; there were no significant differences between the two subsamples in terms of mean age, t(412) = .286, p = .775, BMI, t(412) = 1.059, p = .290, and household crowding index, t(412) = .175, p = .861, as well as the distribution of women and men, χ2(1) = .137, p = .711, single and married participants χ2(1) = .590, p = .443 and secondary or less and university level of education χ2(1) = .023, p = .881.

Exploratory factor analysis

To explore the factor structure of both scales, we computed a principal-axis EFA with the first split-half subsample using the SPSS software [39]. We verified all requirements related to item-communality, average item correlations, and item-total correlations [40]. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy (which should ideally be ≥ .80) and Bartlett’s test of sphericity, which should be significant, ensured the adequacy of our sample [41]. The procedure followed for determining the number of dimensions was the Parallel Analysis (PA) [42], using the Pearson correlation matrix. Weighted Root Mean Square Residual (WRMR) were also calculated to assess the model fit (values < 1 have been recommended to represent good fit) [43]. Item retention was based on the recommendation that items with “fair” loadings and above (i.e., ≥ .40) and with low inter-item correlations (suggestive of low item redundancy) as indicated by the anti-image correlation matrix should be retained [44].

Confirmatory factor analysis

We used data from the second split-half to conduct a CFA using the SPSS AMOS v.26 software. A previous study suggested that the minimum sample size to conduct a confirmatory factor analysis ranges from 3 to 20 times the number of the scale’s variables [45]. Therefore, we assumed a minimum sample of 150 participants needed to have enough statistical power based on a ratio of 10 participants per one item of the scale, which was exceeded in the second subsample. Parameter estimates were obtained using the maximum likelihood method and fit indices. Evidence of convergent validity was assessed in this subsample using the Fornell-Larcker criterion u8, with average variance extracted (AVE) values of ≥ .50 considered adequate [46]. The normed model chi-square (χ2/df), the Steiger-Lind root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) and the comparative fit index (CFI). Values ≤ 5 for χ2/df, and ≤ .08 for RMSEA, and .90 for CFI and TLI indicate good fit of the model to the data [47, 48].

There was no missing data since all questions were required in the Google form link. The social media use disorder score was considered normally distributed (skewness and kurtosis values between -2 and +2) [49]. The SPSS software v.22 was used to conduct the bivariate analysis of factors associated with SMD; the Student t test was used to compare two means (i.e. sex, marital status and education level), whereas the Pearson correlation test was used to correlate SMD with other continuous scores (impulsivity, desire thinking, etc.). The PROCESS SPSS Macro version 3.4, model four [50] was used to calculate all pathways (Pathway A from the independent variable to the mediator, Pathway B from the mediator to the dependent variable and Pathway C from the independent to the dependent variable). Pathway AB calculated the indirect effect; the latter was deemed significant when the macro generated bias-corrected bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals (CI) did not pass by zero [50]. The covariates that were included in the mediation model were age, body mass index, household crowding index, physical activity (variables that showed a p<0.25 in the bivariate analysis).

Results

Sociodemographic and other characteristics of participants

A total of 414 non-clinical participants were included in this study. They had a mean age of 38.58 ± 18.01 years, and 51.2% of them were females. Additional sample characteristics are summarized in Table 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. Sociodemographic and other characteristics of the participants (N = 414).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277884.t001

Prevalence of social media use disorder

The results showed that 162 participants (39.1%) had social media use disorder as they scored above the cut-off for the pathology.

Factor analysis

DTQ scale.

Bartlett’s test of sphericity, χ2(45) = 2450.3, p < .001, and KMO (.923) indicated that the DTQ items had adequate common variance for factor analysis. The results of the EFA revealed a two-factor solution, which explained 91.53% of the common variance (item-factor loadings ≥ .80). The WRMR value was also adequate (= .047; 95% CI .032-.058), indicating good fit of the model.

CFA of the two-factor model obtained in the EFA indicated a good fit: χ2/df = 165.78/34 = 4.88, RMSEA = .141 (90% CI .120, .162), CFI = .966, and TLI = .955. The convergent validity for this model was adequate, as AVE = 1.06.

WBSI scale.

Bartlett’s test of sphericity, χ2(105) = 2431.1, p < .001, and KMO (.929) indicated that the WBSI items had adequate common variance for factor analysis. The results of the EFA revealed a two-factor solution, which explained 89.51% of the common variance (item-factor loadings ≥ .80). The WRMR value was also adequate (= .013; 95% CI .010-.015), indicating good fit of the model.

CFA of the two-factor model obtained in the EFA indicated a modest fit: χ2/df = 667.70/90 = 7.42, RMSEA = .181 (90% CI .168, .194), CFI = .873, and TLI = .852. When adding a correlation between items 2–10, 3–4 and 9–13, the fit indices improved as follows: χ2/df = 466.38/87 = 5.36, RMSEA = .149 (90% CI = .136, .163), CFI = .917, and TLI = .900. The convergent validity for this model was adequate, as AVE = .92. The standardized estimates of factor loadings of both scales were all adequate (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Items of the DTQ and WBSI in English and factor loadings derived from the Exploratory Factor Analyses (EFA) with women and men in the first split-half subsample, and standardized estimates of factor loadings from the Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) in the second split-half subsample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277884.t002

Bivariate analysis.

The bivariate analysis results are shown in Tables 3 and 4. Older age (r = -0.19) was significantly associated with less social media use disorder, whereas higher verbal perseveration (r = 0.24), imaginal prefiguration (r = 0.21), urgency (r = 0.28), lack of premeditation (r = 0.15), lack of perseverance (r = 0.20) and suppression (r = 0.20) were significantly associated with more social media use disorder.

thumbnail
Table 3. Bivariate analysis of the categorical variables associated with social media use disorder score.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277884.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Correlation of continuous variables associated with social media use disorder.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277884.t004

Mediation analysis

The results of the mediation analysis are summarized in Table 5. Suppression (Fig 1) and lack of premeditation (Fig 2) mediated the association between verbal perseveration and social media use disorder. Suppression (Fig 3) and urgency (Fig 4) mediated the association between imaginal prefiguration and social media use disorder. More verbal perseveration was significantly associated with more suppression and more social media disorder, whereas more suppression was significantly associated with more social media disorder. More imaginal prefiguration was significantly associated with more suppression, which was associated with higher social media disorder. More verbal perseveration was significantly associated with less lack of premeditation and more social media disorder, whereas more lack of premeditation was significantly associated with more social media disorder. Finally, more imaginal prefiguration was significantly associated with more urgency and more social media disorder, with higher urgency being significantly associated with more social media disorder.

thumbnail
Fig 1.

(a) Relation between verbal perseveration and suppression (R2 = 20.38%); (b) Relation between suppression and social media use disorder (R2 = 13.92%); (c) Total effect of the relation between verbal perseveration and social media use disorder (R2 = 9.55%); (c’) Direct effect of the relation between verbal perseveration and social media use disorder. Numbers are displayed as regression coefficients (standard error). ***p<0.001; *p<0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277884.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2.

(a) Relation between verbal perseveration and lack of premeditation (R2 = 20.38%); (b) Relation between lack of premeditation and social media use disorder (R2 = 13.92%); (c) Total effect of the relation between verbal perseveration and social media use disorder (R2 = 9.55%); (c’) Direct effect of the relation between verbal perseveration and social media use disorder. Numbers are displayed as regression coefficients (standard error). ***p<0.001; *p<0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277884.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3.

(a) Relation between imaginal prefiguration and suppression (R2 = 20.38%); (b) Relation between suppression and social media use disorder (R2 = 13.92%); (c) Total effect of the relation between imaginal prefiguration and social media use disorder (R2 = 9.55%); (c’) Direct effect of the relation between imaginal prefiguration and social media use disorder. Numbers are displayed as regression coefficients (standard error). ***p<0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277884.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4.

(a) Relation between imaginal prefiguration and urgency (R2 = 20.38%); (b) Relation between urgency and social media use disorder (R2 = 13.92%); (c) Total effect of the relation between imaginal prefiguration and social media use disorder (R2 = 9.55%); (c’) Direct effect of the relation between imaginal prefiguration and social media use disorder. Numbers are displayed as regression coefficients (standard error). ***p<0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277884.g004

thumbnail
Table 5. Mediation analyses results, taking verbal perseveration / imaginal prefiguration as the independent variables, the impulsivity subscales and the suppression score as mediators and social media use disorder score as the dependent variable.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277884.t005

Discussion

Prevalence

Our study investigated the correlation between desire thinking and problematic social media use, and further illustrated differential mediation models of impulsivity facets and thought suppression on the relation between the two dimensions of desire thinking and social use in Lebanese adults.

Our results indicate that 40% of the current sample fit the criteria for pathological social media use. These results are in line with a meta-analysis assessing social media addiction, suggesting that in collectivist countries, such as Lebanon, 31% of individuals are addicted to social media [51]. In neighboring Gulf countries, almost 50% of surveyed people reported perceiving themselves as addicted to social media [52]. Results in Saudi Arabia, another Arab country, indicate an overall rate of 48% of Internet addictions [53], whereas the rate in the Philippines is approximately 25% [54].

Desire thinking and problematic social media use

As hypothesized, our results show that desire thinking was significantly correlated with problematic social media use, which is similar to previous findings documenting that desire thinking plays a significant role in enhancing addictive behaviors including problematic social media use [55]. Furthermore, desire thinking was not only associated with problematic social media use but is also thought to be an underlying key factor connecting other psychological variables with problematic social media [22]. As such, desire thinking was integrated into a more structured model focusing on behavioral disorders related to the Internet such as problematic social media use disorder and was found to directly influencing addiction and cravings [7].

Mediation of suppression and lack of premeditation between verbal perseveration and problematic social media use

We found that suppression and lack of premeditation moderated the relation between verbal perseveration and problematic social media use. Previous studies have focused on desire thinking as a moderator variable [22], overlooking differential investigation of its two dimensions and further highlighting the complexity and interdependence of various constructs in determining behavioral addictions. The verbal perseveration dimension of desire thinking indeed consists of self-talk in order to convince oneself to engage in a desirable activity [3]. Verbal perseveration was found to significantly predict a variety of addictive behavior including problematic social media use [21]. Most importantly, it was found to expedite the pathway from normal use of the Internet to addictive use [56], however, it failed to show a direct path to craving online activities, which is known to pave the way to subsequent social media addiction [57]. This suggests the possibility of an indirect pathway between desire thinking, notably verbal perseveration and problematic social media use. Lack of premeditation consists of disregarding the consequences of one’s actions, in addition to intentionally yet inadequately suppressing and ignoring one’s unwanted thoughts that would moderate the backlash of the verbal perseveration aspect of focusing on an unwanted arousing behavior, ultimately reinforcing problematic social media use.

Mediation of suppression and urgency between imaginal prefiguration and problematic social media use

In the current study, suppression and urgency mediated the association between imaginal prefiguration and problematic social media use. The urgency facet of impulsivity was already found to be a prominent predictor of social media addiction [58]. Similarly, urgency was evidenced as a predictor of smartphone addiction, which was positively correlated with high social media use [59]. Within the same context, higher suppression was associated with increased and more severe problematic smartphone use [60]. Although significant, the contribution of thought suppression to the model is very modest and within range of previous work, and might suggest an alternative route where suppression influences addictive behavior. Desire thinking, specifically imaginal prefiguration, was significantly related to problematic social media use [55]. It could be that imagining the positive results of a desired behavior elicits arousing emotions, in turn triggering a sense of urgency that ultimately increases the focus on the behavioral addiction.

Thought suppression, impulsivity and problematic social media

Our results indicate that higher suppression was significantly associated with higher social media disorder. First, research has shown that thought suppression has an ironic rebound effect where higher cognitive engagement in suppressing thoughts results in higher probability of engaging in that behavior [61]. Such could be the case that supports the current findings: the higher the thought suppression relating to social media, the higher the possibility of eventually engaging in problematic social media behavior.

Also, two sub-factors of impulsivity, lack of premeditation and higher urgency, had positive relationships with social media disorder. This is consistent with a recent study that showed a positive association between impulsivity and a social media platform, Facebook [62]. Similar results were found in another study where higher impulsivity correlated to higher social media addiction [63]. A breakdown of all subsets of impulsivity further evidenced that all were connected with higher problematic social media use [23]. Moreover, intervention programs targeting lack of premeditation specifically successfully alleviated behavioral addictions overall [64], such as social media use disorder. These connections could be explained by the fact that impulsivity facets were shown to share neural pathways with behavioral addictions [65], with common neurocircuitry for addiction and urgency recently elucidated [66].

Recommendations

At a time when social media is becoming a seemingly essential element for the daily lives of many people, and whilst reposts documenting its problematic use are exponentially rising, the current results have important implications to better optimize its use. First, it could help profile vulnerable adults whose social media use could become problematic. Second, it could help clinicians better target problematic social media use by employing interventions targeting maladaptive coping strategies in desire thinking, thought suppression and impulsivity. Lastly, taken together, these factors would inform policies to address this rising public health threat. It is indeed noteworthy in that regard that awareness campaigns and behavioral interventions effectively decreased the negative effects of problematic social media use, such as the life skills enhancement program [67].

Limitations

This study has some limitations. The data was collected through an online questionnaire, which introduces respondent bias. Plus, the snowball technique followed to collect the data predisposes us to a selection bias. Additionally, the Desire Thinking Questionnaire and WBSI are not yet validated in Lebanon; the psychometric properties of those scales in the current study revealed modest results, therefore, findings should be interpreted with caution. Some variables had a low direct effect on social media use disorder. Although the White Bear Suppression Inventory is old, however it is consistently used in recent literature and had a very high Cronbach’s alpha in the current study. The analyses conducted reflect correlational relationships between variables and therefore do not infer causation. Lastly, the lack of active governmental literacy in Lebanon on such novel psychopathological variables makes it difficult to promote effective policies that can be applied in various sectors exposed to excessive social media use including industrial, educational and public health domains.

Conclusion

This study provides new insight on factors associated with problematic social media use, including desire thinking, impulsivity and thought suppression. This is especially relevant to the Lebanese context, with a population faced with multiple crises, including a global pandemic and a severe socio-economic recession, exposing people to extreme chronic stress. It adds to the literature of a global modern public health issue that is massively present in the daily lives of Lebanese people. The association between desire thinking and problematic social media use disorder being mediated by impulsivity and suppression offers potential targets for future scientific investigations.

Future studies

The current findings show the pervasiveness of social media use disorder and the need to further investigate factors contributing to it. Although cognitive modalities such as desire thinking and thought suppression are misleadingly defined as inhibiting unwanted thoughts with the goal of controlling unwanted behaviors, this study adds to the panoply of evidence showing an inverse effect of exacerbating maladaptive social media use. Impulsivity, notably with its lack of premeditation and sense of urgency also mediated the association between desire thinking and problematic social media use. These differential factors could be selectively targeted in various interventions to diminish the deleterious mental health burden of behavioral addictions.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all participants.

References

  1. 1. Caselli G, Spada MM. Metacognitions in desire thinking: A preliminary investigation. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy. 2010;38(5):629–37. pmid:20667168
  2. 2. Spada MM, Caselli G, Wells A. A triphasic metacognitive formulation of problem drinking. Clinical psychology & psychotherapy. 2013;20(6):494–500. pmid:22589026
  3. 3. Caselli G, Spada MM. Desire thinking: What is it and what drives it? Addictive Behaviors. 2015;44:71–9. pmid:25128328
  4. 4. Caselli G, Nikcevic A, Fiore F, Mezzaluna C, Spada MM. Desire thinking across the continuum of nicotine dependence. Addiction Research & Theory. 2012;20(5):382–8.
  5. 5. Caselli G, Ferla M, Mezzaluna C, Rovetto F, Spada MM. Desire thinking across the continuum of drinking behaviour. European Addiction Research. 2012;18(2):64–9. pmid:22178806
  6. 6. Mansueto G, Martino F, Palmieri S, Scaini S, Ruggiero GM, Sassaroli S, et al. Desire thinking across addictive behaviours: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Addictive behaviors. 2019;98:106018. pmid:31233946
  7. 7. Brandtner A, Antons S, Cornil A, Brand M. Integrating Desire Thinking into the I-PACE Model: a Special Focus on Internet-Use Disorders. Current Addiction Reports. 2021;8(4):459–68.
  8. 8. Spada MM, Caselli G, Slaifer M, Nikčević AV, Sassaroli S. Desire thinking as a predictor of problematic Internet use. Social Science Computer Review. 2014;32(4):474–83.
  9. 9. Marino C, Caselli G, Lenzi M, Monaci MG, Vieno A, Nikčević AV, et al. Emotion regulation and desire thinking as predictors of problematic Facebook use. Psychiatric Quarterly. 2019;90(2):405–11. pmid:30710196
  10. 10. Moss AC, Erskine JA, Albery IP, Allen JR, Georgiou GJ. To suppress, or not to suppress? That is repression: controlling intrusive thoughts in addictive behaviour. Addictive behaviors. 2015;44:65–70. pmid:25648574
  11. 11. Rømer Thomsen K, Callesen MB, Hesse M, Kvamme TL, Pedersen MM, Pedersen MU, et al. Impulsivity traits and addiction-related behaviors in youth. Journal of behavioral addictions. 2018;7(2):317–30. pmid:29642723
  12. 12. Wartberg L, Zieglmeier M, Kammerl R. An empirical exploration of longitudinal predictors for problematic internet use and problematic gaming behavior. Psychological Reports. 2021;124(2):543–54. pmid:32237970
  13. 13. Garland EL, Roberts-Lewis A. Differential roles of thought suppression and dispositional mindfulness in posttraumatic stress symptoms and craving. Addictive behaviors. 2013;38(2):1555–62. pmid:22385734
  14. 14. Wenzlaff RM, Wegner DM. Thought suppression. Annual review of psychology. 2000;51(1):59–91. pmid:10751965
  15. 15. Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. American journal of psychiatry. 2001;158(11):1783–93. pmid:11691682
  16. 16. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of clinical psychology. 1995;51(6):768–74. pmid:8778124
  17. 17. Cyders MA, Littlefield AK, Coffey S, Karyadi KA. Examination of a short English version of the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale. Addictive behaviors. 2014;39(9):1372–6. pmid:24636739
  18. 18. Derefinko K, DeWall CN, Metze AV, Walsh EC, Lynam DR. Do different facets of impulsivity predict different types of aggression? Aggressive behavior. 2011;37(3):223–33. pmid:21259270
  19. 19. Efrati Y, Kolubinski DC, Caselli G, Spada MM. Desire thinking as a predictor of compulsive sexual behaviour in adolescents: Evidence from a cross-cultural validation of the Hebrew version of the Desire Thinking Questionnaire. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2020;9(3):797–807. pmid:33006958
  20. 20. Khosravani V, Bastan FS, Ghorbani F, Kamali Z. Difficulties in emotion regulation mediate negative and positive affects and craving in alcoholic patients. Addictive Behaviors. 2017;71:75–81. pmid:28273489
  21. 21. Khosravani V, Spada MM, Bastan FS, Ardestani SMS. The desire thinking questionnaire-Persian version (DTQ-P) and its association with addictive behaviors in individuals with alcohol use disorder, nicotine dependence, and problematic social media use. Addictive Behaviors. 2022;125:107144. pmid:34688122
  22. 22. Sharifi Bastan F, Spada MM, Khosravani V, Samimi Ardestani SM. The independent contribution of desire thinking to problematic social media use. Current Psychology. 2022:1–10. pmid:35540371
  23. 23. TUTAL Ö, ERENSOY H, Melek L. Relationship between psychological symptoms, impulsivity and social media use problems. Bağımlılık Dergisi. 2021;22(1):43–52.
  24. 24. Efrati Y, Kolubinski DC, Marino C, Spada MM. Modelling the contribution of metacognitions, impulsiveness, and thought suppression to behavioural addictions in adolescents. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021;18(7):3820. pmid:33917425
  25. 25. Luo T, Chen W, Liao Y. Social media use in China before and during COVID-19: Preliminary results from an online retrospective survey. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2021;140:35–8. pmid:34090101
  26. 26. Perlis RH, Green J, Simonson M, Ognyanova K, Santillana M, Lin J, et al. Association between social media use and self-reported symptoms of depression in US adults. JAMA network open. 2021;4(11):e2136113–e. pmid:34812844
  27. 27. Dennis EE, Martin JD, Hassan F. Media use in the Middle East, 2019: A seven-nation survey. Northwestern University in Qatar. 2019.
  28. 28. Poushter J, Bishop C, Chwe H. Social media use continues to rise in developing countries but plateaus across developed ones. Pew research center. 2018;22:2–19.
  29. 29. Malaeb D, Salameh P, Barbar S, Awad E, Haddad C, Hallit R, et al. Problematic social media use and mental health (depression, anxiety, and insomnia) among Lebanese adults: Any mediating effect of stress? Perspectives in psychiatric care. 2021;57(2):539–49. pmid:32633428
  30. 30. Fritz MS, MacKinnon DP. Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychological science. 2007;18(3):233–9. pmid:17444920
  31. 31. Melki I, Beydoun H, Khogali M, Tamim H, Yunis K. Household crowding index: a correlate of socioeconomic status and inter-pregnancy spacing in an urban setting. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health. 2004;58(6):476–80.
  32. 32. Weary-Smith KA. Validation of the physical activity index (PAI) as a measure of total activity load and total kilocalorie expenditure during submaximal treadmill walking: University of Pittsburgh; 2007.
  33. 33. Caselli G, Spada MM. The desire thinking questionnaire: Development and psychometric properties. Addictive Behaviors. 2011;36(11):1061–7. pmid:21741178
  34. 34. Wegner DM, Zanakos S. Chronic thought suppression. Journal of personality. 1994;62(4):615–40. pmid:7861307
  35. 35. Awad E, Rogoza R, Gerges S, Obeid S, Hallit S. Association of Social Media Use Disorder and Orthorexia Nervosa among Lebanese university students: The indirect effect of loneliness and factor structure of the Social Media Use Disorder short form and the Jong-Gierveld loneliness scales. Accepted, under press. Psychol Reports. 2022. pmid:36245332
  36. 36. Van den Eijnden RJ, Lemmens JS, Valkenburg PM. The social media disorder scale. Computers in human behavior. 2016;61:478–87.
  37. 37. Awad E, Salameh P, Sacre H, Malaeb D, Hallit S, Obeid S. Association between impulsivity and orthorexia nervosa/healthy orthorexia: any mediating effect of depression, anxiety, and stress? BMC psychiatry. 2021;21(1):1–14.
  38. 38. Swami V, Barron D. Translation and validation of body image instruments: Challenges, good practice guidelines, and reporting recommendations for test adaptation. Body image. 2019;31:204–20. pmid:30220631
  39. 39. Lorenzo-Seva U, Ferrando PJ. FACTOR: A computer program to fit the exploratory factor analysis model. Behavior research methods. 2006;38(1):88–91. pmid:16817517
  40. 40. Clark LA, Watson D. Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale development. 2016.
  41. 41. Hair JF. Multivariate data analysis. 2009.
  42. 42. Timmerman ME, Lorenzo-Seva U. Dimensionality assessment of ordered polytomous items with parallel analysis. Psychological methods. 2011;16(2):209. pmid:21500916
  43. 43. Yu CY, editor Evaluation of model fit indices for latent variable models with categorical and continuous outcomes. Paper presented at the annual conference of the American Educational Research Association, April 4, 2002, New Orleans; 2002.
  44. 44. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS, Ullman JB. Using multivariate statistics: pearson Boston, MA; 2007.
  45. 45. Mundfrom DJ, Shaw DG, Ke TL. Minimum sample size recommendations for conducting factor analyses. International journal of testing. 2005;5(2):159–68.
  46. 46. Malhotra N, Dash S. Marketing Research: An Applied Orientation (; Pearson, Ed.). Delhi; 2011.
  47. 47. Byrne BM. Structural equation modeling with EQS and EQS/Windows: Basic concepts, applications, and programming: Sage; 1994.
  48. 48. Lt Hu, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural equation modeling: a multidisciplinary journal. 1999;6(1):1–55.
  49. 49. George D. SPSS for windows step by step: A simple study guide and reference, 17.0 update, 10/e: Pearson Education India; 2011.
  50. 50. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach: Guilford publications; 2017.
  51. 51. Cheng C, Lau Y-c, Chan L, Luk JW. Prevalence of social media addiction across 32 nations: Meta-analysis with subgroup analysis of classification schemes and cultural values. Addictive Behaviors. 2021;117:106845. pmid:33550200
  52. 52. Allahverdi FZ. The relationship between the items of the social media disorder scale and perceived social media addiction. Current Psychology. 2022;41(10):7200–7.
  53. 53. Vadivu SV, Chupradit S. Psychosocial and occupational impact assessment due to internet addiction: a critical review. Systematic Reviews in Pharmacy. 2020;11(7):152–5.
  54. 54. Cleofas JV. Social media disorder during community quarantine: A mixed methods study among rural young college students during the COVID-19 pandemic. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing. 2022;40:97–105. pmid:36064253
  55. 55. Solem S, Pedersen H, Nesse F, Garvik Janssen A, Ottesen Kennair LE, Hagen R, et al. Validity of a Norwegian version of the Desire Thinking Questionnaire (DTQ): Associations with problem drinking, nicotine dependence and problematic social media use. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy. 2021;28(3):615–22. pmid:33090540
  56. 56. Aydın O, Ünal-Aydın P, Caselli G, Kolubinski D, Marino C, Spada M. Psychometric validation of the desire thinking questionnaire in a Turkish adolescent sample: Associations with internet gaming disorder. Addictive Behaviors. 2022;125:107129. pmid:34649063
  57. 57. Brandtner A, Brand M. Fleeing through the mind’s eye: Desire thinking as a maladaptive coping mechanism among specific online activities. Addictive Behaviors. 2021;120:106957. pmid:33932838
  58. 58. Willie C, Gill PR, Teese R, Stavropoulos V, Jago A. Emotion-driven problem behaviour: The predictive utility of positive and negative urgency. Brain and Neuroscience Advances. 2022;6:23982128221079573. pmid:35237728
  59. 59. Liang J, Leung L. Comparing smartphone addiction: The prevalence, predictors, and negative consequences in Hong Kong and Mainland China. The Journal of Social Media in Society. 2018;7(2):297–322.
  60. 60. Rozgonjuk D, Elhai JD. Emotion regulation in relation to smartphone use: Process smartphone use mediates the association between expressive suppression and problematic smartphone use. Current Psychology. 2021;40(7):3246–55.
  61. 61. Wang D, Hagger MS, Chatzisarantis NL. Ironic effects of thought suppression: a meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2020;15(3):778–93. pmid:32286932
  62. 62. Sindermann C, Elhai JD, Montag C. Predicting tendencies towards the disordered use of Facebook’s social media platforms: On the role of personality, impulsivity, and social anxiety. Psychiatry Research. 2020;285:112793. pmid:32058879
  63. 63. Sheinov VP, Dziavitsyn AS. Relations of social media addiction with impulsivity, narcissism and assertiveness in Belarusian and Ukrainian university students. RUDN Journal of Psychology and Pedagogics. 2022;19(2):233–52.
  64. 64. Martínez Loredo V, Fernández Hermida JR. Impulsivity-targeted selective preventive interventions and treatments in addictive behaviors. Revista de Psicología Clínica con niños y adolescentes, 6. 2019.
  65. 65. Um M, Whitt ZT, Revilla R, Hunton T, Cyders MA. Shared neural correlates underlying addictive disorders and negative urgency. Brain sciences. 2019;9(2):36. pmid:30744033
  66. 66. Zorrilla EP, Koob GF. Impulsivity derived from the dark side: Neurocircuits that contribute to negative urgency. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience. 2019;13:136. pmid:31293401
  67. 67. Kumkronglek J, Sirisatayawong P, Chupradit S. EFFECTS OF LIFE SKILL ENHANCEMENT PROGRAM ON LIFE SKILLS AND RISK BEHAVIORS OF SOCIAL MEDIA ADDICTION AMONG EARLY ADOLESCENCE: PILOT STUDY. PSAKU International Journal of Interdisciplinary Research. 2022;11(2):72–7.