Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The impact of providing hiding spaces to farmed animals: A scoping review

  • Hannah B. Spitzer,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Health Management, Atlantic Veterinary College, University of Prince Edward Island, Charlottetown, Canada

  • Rebecca K. Meagher,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Animal Science and Aquaculture, Dalhousie University, Truro, Canada

  • Kathryn L. Proudfoot

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    kproudfoot@upei.ca

    Affiliation Health Management, Atlantic Veterinary College, University of Prince Edward Island, Charlottetown, Canada

Abstract

Many wild animals perform hiding behaviours for a variety of reasons, such as evading predators or other conspecifics. Unlike their wild counterparts, farmed animals often live in relatively barren environments without the opportunity to hide. Researchers have begun to study the impact of access to hiding spaces (“hides”) in farmed animals, including possible effects on animal welfare. The aims of this scoping review were to: 1) identify the farmed species that have been most used in research investigating the provision of hides, 2) describe the context in which hides have been provided to farmed animals, and 3) describe the impact (positive, negative or neutral/inconclusive) that hides have on animals, including indicators of animal welfare. Three online databases (CAB Abstracts, Web of Science, and PubMed) were used to search for a target population of farmed animals with access to hiding spaces. From this search, 4,631 citations were screened and 151 were included in the review. Fourteen animal types were represented, most commonly chickens (48% of papers), cattle (9%), foxes (8%), and fish (7%). Relatively few papers were found on other species including deer, quail, ducks, lobsters, turkeys, and goats. Hides were used in four contexts: at parturition or oviposition (56%), for general enrichment (43%), for neonatal animals (4%), or for sick or injured animals (1%). A total of 218 outcomes relevant to our objectives were found including 7 categories: hide use, motivation, and/or preference (47% of outcomes), behavioural indicators of affective state (17%), health, injuries, and/or production (16%), agonistic behaviour (8%), abnormal repetitive behaviours (6%), physiological indicators of stress (5%), and affiliative behaviours (1%). Hiding places resulted in 162 positive (74%), 14 negative (6%), and 42 neutral/inconclusive (19%) outcomes. Hides had a generally positive impact on the animals included in this review; more research is encouraged for under-represented species.

Introduction

Over the last century, farm animal industries have shifted from traditional, extensive ways of raising animals to predominantly indoor housing systems, often using barren pens or cages [1, 2]. This shift, known as intensification, allowed for increased efficiency of animal production; however, intensification also sparked growing concerns about the welfare of farmed animals [1]. Concerns for the welfare of animals generally fall into three spheres: animals’ health and biological functioning, affective states, and ability to live in a natural way [3]. Animal producers and veterinarians tend to be most concerned about animals’ health and production as indicators of animals with good welfare [46]. On the other hand, citizens are generally concerned about animals’ affective states (feelings or emotions, [3]) and ability to live naturally [4, 6]. Indeed, naturalness is an aspect of welfare that is often overlooked in many animal production systems, as farmed animals have limited ability perform natural behaviours [7].

A common natural behaviour that is seen in many wild animals, but is often not available for farmed animals, is the ability to hide. In the natural environment, animals perform hiding behaviors for a variety of reasons and under different contexts. For example, free-roaming ungulates have been demonstrated to separate from the herd before parturition to find natural cover like trees or tall grass, including white-tailed deer [8], elk [9], Alaskan moose [10], wild and feral goats and sheep [11, 12], and semi-wild Maremma cattle [13]. Additionally, some (“hider” species) will leave their young hidden for several days after birth [14]. These peri-parturient hiding behaviours are thought to be both an anti-predatory strategy, as well as aiding in the dam and infant forming a bond before introduction to the herd [15]. Periparturient seclusion and neonatal hiding has also been documented in other wild counterparts to domesticated farm animals. For example, European rabbit does [16], mink [17] and red fox [18] dams hide their offspring to prevent predation and infanticide. Similarly, wild fowl, such as red jungle fowl [19], wild turkeys [20], and king eider ducks [21] will also find a covered laying site for to avoid nest predation.

Wild animals will hide in dens, burrows, or other naturally secluded areas at times other than parturition. For example, predators such as wild red foxes [22] and American mink [23] reside in dens, which often take the form of natural hollows in the earth or repurposed rabbit dens. These dens are typically situated under trees or other areas on natural cover as to act as shelter and protection from higher predators [22, 24], and the animals often move frequently between several different dens [23]. Similarly, prey species such as rabbits live in dens much like mink and foxes, although the rabbit typically builds the den themselves [25]. Use of the natural environment for sheltering is also an important antipredator behaviour for wild fish [26] and crustaceans [27], particularly for juveniles. Likewise, visual cover in the form of trees, bushes, and tall grass is also a major component of the natural environment for wild fowl, which birds use to evade predators or aggressive conspecifics, for example in the Red Jungle Fowl [28] and the North Island blue duck [29]. Although it is less reported in the literature, some wild animals as well as laboratory rodents have also been described to seek hiding places or seclusion for other reasons, including during sickness and injury [3032].

Giving farmed animals the opportunity to perform natural behaviours, such as hiding, is an important component of animal welfare for many stakeholders [4, 6]. Moreover, allowing the performance of some natural behaviors may also improve other aspects of welfare, such as affective states and biological functioning [36]. Because of these potential benefits, there has been increasing research on allowing farmed animals to perform natural hiding behaviours, despite few conventional management systems that currently allow for this behaviour. Thus, the overall goal of this scoping review was to describe the present literature exploring the provision of hiding spaces ("hides") to farmed animals, which we defined as a space to hide from sight by placing themselves or their young away from a group, pen mate, or an open area. The aims of this scoping review were to: 1) identify the farmed species that have been most used in research investigating the provision of hiding spaces, 2) describe the context in which hides have been provided to farmed animals, and 3) describe the impact (positive, negative, or neutral/inconclusive) that hides have on animals, including indicators of animal welfare.

Materials and methods

Protocol registration

No review protocol was registered for this paper.

Eligibility criteria

Populations and interventions.

To be included in the review, the paper must have met the following criteria: 1) the population of animals were those commonly farmed in North America (Table 1), 2) the intervention of the study included the provision of an artificial or natural hiding place or comparisons between hiding space design, placement, or timing of placement, or, the study was observational but one of the main outcomes was a measurement of hiding behaviour, and 4) the study must have had at least one outcome related to animal behavior and/or welfare. We considered a hiding place to be a space to hide from sight by placing themselves or their young away from a group, pen mate, or an open area (e.g., shelters, nesting boxes, natural cover, visual barriers, or spaces to isolate from other animals). Studies that provided animals with shelters with the primary goal of assessing animals’ sheltering from harsh weather were excluded. If a hide was included in the study but hiding behaviour was not included as an outcome, the study was excluded. Studies were also excluded if they reported nest building behaviours or interactions with nesting materials but hiding behaviour was not included as an outcome.

Comparators and outcomes.

Comparisons were made between species and stage of life (e.g., parturition, neonatal or other life stages). Outcomes investigated included any behavioral measurements well as other measurements that were related to animal welfare, including hide use, hide preference, indicators of physiological stress responses, health, and abnormal behaviour.

Limitations.

Only academic journal articles written in English were considered for inclusion. There were no limitations on year of publication or country of publication.

Search

With the assistance of a librarian experienced in scoping reviews and veterinary literature, searches were designed in three databases: CAB Abstracts (via EBSCOhost), PubMed, and Web of Science. See Table 1 for full details of the search terms and construction. Concepts were searched in title and abstract fields, and a single search for all terms was run in each database. Searches were conducted on June 4, 2021.

Three databases were searched (CAB Abstracts, PubMed, Web of Science) with the aim of searching for experimental papers with a population of farmed animals and an intervention of hiding place. Some search terms were repeated; this was done with the guidance of a librarian to reduce irrelevant search results.

Selection

Fig 1 outlines the screening procedure for paper inclusion using a modified PRISMA 2020 flow diagram [33]. From the search results, citations were uploaded to the Rayyan QRCI web application [34] to be manually screened for inclusion. Inclusion and exclusion criteria were flexible and were discussed between authors to reach consensus. One author (HBS) first screened papers based on title and abstract, and excluded papers based on incorrect population (animals farmed in North America) or incorrect intervention (provision of a hiding place; papers that only assessed nesting-building behaviours were excluded), incorrect publication type (reviews, books, and conference proceedings were not included), weather (studies that provided animals with shelters with the primary goal of assessing animals’ sheltering from harsh weather were excluded), and incorrect language (non-English languages). After the papers were screened by title and abstract, the same author (HBS) reviewed the remaining papers by full text using the same inclusion/exclusion criteria. After full text screening, the reference lists of the remaining papers were then hand searched for any additional relevant papers. Although the population of interest was animals commonly farmed in North America, the study did not have to be conducted in North America.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Screening procedure and exclusion process for scoping review.

The aim was to search for experimental papers with a population of farmed animals and an intervention of hiding place.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277665.g001

Coding and appraisal

The 151 papers included in the final review were coded to facilitate analysis of the aims of this paper. All papers were reviewed by full text and were coded in each of the following categories: Title, author, year of publication, animal category (ungulate, poultry, fur carnivore, small herbivore, fish, crustacean), animal type (cattle, deer, pigs, sheep, goats, chickens, quail, ducks, turkeys, mink, foxes, fish, lobsters, and rabbits), context of hide provision (e.g., timepoint in the animals’ life when the hide was provided), and study outcomes (described using notes). We chose to use these specific “animal types” due to their relevance to farm animal industries. We also opted to further group them into “animal categories” based on biological and behavioural similarities; for example, we used the broader category of ungulate to represent cattle, deer, pigs, sheep, and goats because these animals have similar natural histories. However, rabbits and lobsters were the only animal types found in our search that fit in the small herbivore and crustacean categories.

Study outcomes were first described by one author (HBS) using notes for each paper. These outcomes were then subjectively categorized by topics that emerged (e.g., specific behavioural or physiological data that were collected). The outcomes were then subjectively classified as representing the hide having “positive”, “negative” or “neutral/inconclusive” impacts on the study animals. Outcomes that matched at least one of the following criteria were considered to reflect a positive impact: 1) animals used the hides, were motivated to access the hides, or preferred hide design features that increased the level of seclusion compared to a “standard” hide (for example, a nest box with a curtain compared to a standard nest box without a curtain); 2) animals increased performance of behaviours that typically indicate positive welfare, such as affiliative behaviours or other behavioural indicators of affective state (e.g. preening); 3) animals reduced performance of behaviours that indicate negative welfare, such as agonistic behaviours, abnormal repetitive behaviours or other behavioural indicators of negative affective state (such as behavioural indicators of fear); or 4) there was a decrease in injuries or physiological indicators of stress, or an increase in health or productivity.

Study outcomes that matched at least one of the following criteria were considered to reflect a negative impact of hides on the animals: 1) animals decreased performance of behaviours that indicate positive welfare, such as affiliative behaviours or other behavioural indicators of positive affective state (such as preening); 2) animals increased performance of behaviours that indicate negative welfare, such as agonistic behaviours, abnormal repetitive behaviours, or other behavioural indicators of negative affective state (such as behavioural indicators of fear); or 3) there was an increase in injuries or physiological indicators of stress, or a decrease in health or productivity.

Some outcomes did not indicate that animals benefitted from the hides but also did not suggest that the animals were negatively impacted by the hides; these were classified as neutral or inconclusive and included the following: 1) animals did not use the hides, did not demonstrate motivation to access the hides, or did not show preference for hide design features that increased seclusion; 3) rate of injuries, agonistic behaviours, or abnormal repetitive behaviours were unaffected by hide provision; 2) conflicting behaviours of similar type were reported (for example, one type of abnormal repetitive behaviour was decreased when hides were provided but another abnormal repetitive behaviour was increased); or 3) conflicting physiological results were reported (for example, one type of injury was decreased when hides were provided but another type of injury was increased).

Results

Search results and exclusions

A total of 4,631 papers were retrieved (CAB: 1,236; WOS: 2,785; PubMed: 610). Duplicates (n = 1,548) were immediately detected and eliminated (Fig 1). A total of 3,083 papers were reviewed by title and abstract; 2,914 were excluded. Of these, 1,814 papers were initially excluded because the study population was not farmed animals. A further 944 were excluded because the intervention was unrelated to hiding behaviour and 106 were excluded because the publication was not a peer-reviewed journal article. Fifty papers were excluded because the primary aim was related to sheltering from harsh weather, and 1 foreign language paper was excluded. A total of 169 papers were reviewed by full text. A further 28 papers were excluded: 1 for wrong population, 26 for wrong intervention, and 1 for wrong publication type. The reference lists of the included papers were hand-searched for missing papers relevant to this review; 6 papers were added through initial hand searching. A reviewer suggested the inclusion of a further 2 papers; these were included, and the reference lists of these papers were hand-searched for relevant papers, leading to inclusion of a further 2 papers. After this process, a total of 151 papers were included in the final review. Publication years ranged from 1976–2021.

Aim 1: Animal types represented in the literature

Fourteen types of farmed animals were represented in the 151 papers (Fig 2). The most represented animals were chickens (48% of papers), cattle (9%), foxes (8%), and fish (all species pooled, 7% of the papers). Many animal types were also be split into subtypes, which reflected differences in hiding behaviour. For example, the group of chicken papers were composed of laying hens (84% of the chicken papers) as well as broiler chickens (15% of the chicken papers). The cattle papers were nearly all focused on dairy cattle (92% of the cattle papers), while only two papers (15% of the cattle papers) investigated beef cattle (one of which focused on both dairy and beef cattle, while the other investigated beef calves).

thumbnail
Fig 2. The number of studies per category and type of animal.

From the 151 papers included in this scoping review, there were 6 categories (e.g., “ungulate”) and 15 types (e.g., “cattle") of animals represented. This figure shows the number of studies for each of the animal categories and types. One paper represented both turkeys and ducks, so this paper was categorized as both a ‘duck’ paper and a ‘turkey’ paper, resulting in 152 total papers in the graph.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277665.g002

One paper represented both turkeys and ducks, so this paper was categorized as both a ‘duck’ paper and a ‘turkey’ paper, resulting in 152 total papers in Fig 2. There were 6 animal types included in the initial literature search for which no results were found: bison, elk, alpacas, llamas, horses, and donkeys.

Aim 2: Context of hide provision

Four categories of hide provision were found: 1) at parturition or oviposition (56% of the papers), 2) for general enrichment (43% of the papers), 3) for neonatal animals (4% of the papers), or 4) for sick or injured animals (1% of the papers). Some papers (n = 6) represented two categories of hide provision; these papers were included in both categories. The context of the hide use seemed to be dependent on the animal type (Fig 3). Most papers that focused on parturition and oviposition included ungulates and poultry, all papers that focused on neonatal animals and sickness or injury were focused on ungulates, and there were a variety of species included in papers using hides as general enrichment.

thumbnail
Fig 3. The number of studies per category of animal and context of hide provision.

From the 151 papers included in this scoping review, there were 6 categories of animals (e.g., “ungulate”) and 4 contexts of hide provision (e.g., “parturition and oviposition”) represented. This figure shows the number of studies for each of the animal categories and context of hide provision. Three papers represented two contexts of hide provision; these papers were included in both categories for a total of 157 papers represented in the figure.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277665.g003

Aim 3: Effect of hide provision on the animals

A total of 218 study outcomes were coded from the 157 studies, as many studies had more than one reported outcome. A total of 7 categories emerged from the study outcomes, including: 1) ‘hide use, motivation, and/or preference’, which included outcomes related to animals’ use of provided hides, their motivation to access to hides, or preference of certain hide features, 2) ‘affiliative behaviour’, such as allogrooming, 3) ‘agonistic behaviour’, including aggression, threatening behaviour, or retreating, 4) ‘abnormal repetitive behaviour’, such as repetitive pacing or feather pecking, 5) ‘health, injuries, and/or production’, such as rates of illness, injuries, or growth, 6) ‘physiological indicators of stress’ such as corticosterone levels, and 7) ‘behavioural indicators of affective state, such as preening or settled pre-laying or pre-parturient behaviour.

Table 2 shows the distribution of study outcomes into positive, negative, and neutral/inconclusive categories, along with the number of outcomes that were found for each context of hide provision (aim 2) per animal category. Overall, there were 162 positive outcomes, 14 negative outcomes, and 42 neutral/inconclusive outcomes. Of 151 papers, 103 papers reported outcome related to hide use, motivation, and/or preference, 2 papers reported outcomes related to affiliative behaviour, 17 papers reported outcomes related to agonistic behaviour, 14 papers reported outcomes related to abnormal repetitive behaviour, 35 papers reported outcomes related to health, injuries, and/or production, 11 papers reported outcomes related to physiological indicators of stress, and 37 papers reported outcomes related to behavioural indicators of affective state.

thumbnail
Table 2. Categorization of 151 papers based on how provision of a hiding space impacted the animals.

The number of outcomes per context of hide provision is listed under each animal category. Missing categories indicates that there were no outcomes found in that context for that species; a blank cell indicates that no outcomes were found for that animal category and context of hide provision.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277665.t002

The number of papers reporting that hide provision resulted in a positive, negative, or neutral/inconclusive impact on the animals is listed for each category. A ‘+’ indicates that hide provision had a positive impact on the animals, a ‘-’ indicates that hide provision had a negative impact on the animals, and a ‘+/-’ indicates that hide provision did not impact animals or that the results were otherwise inconclusive.

Discussion

The overall goal of this scoping review was to describe the existing literature exploring the provision of hiding places to farmed animals. We identified that chickens were the most used animal in research exploring hide use, and that hides were used mainly for parturition/oviposition and for general enrichment. Further, we found that hides resulted in primarily positive outcomes related to animal behaviour and/or welfare.

Aim 1: Animal types represented in the literature

A wide variety of farmed species were included in this review, but much of the focus was on species most used for food production and for domestic animal welfare research. For example, about half (73/151) of the papers included in this review investigated the hiding behaviour of chickens, with a majority of these focused on laying hens. Chickens are not only one of the most widely used food animals globally but have also a long history of being the focus of animal welfare research (for example on laying hens, see [35]). A moderate number of studies (6 to 13 studies per species listed) were performed on other common farm animals such as cattle, foxes, fish, pigs, sheep, and mink.

Relatively little research (1 to 5 studies per species listed) has been conducted on other farm animal species including deer, quail, ducks, lobsters, turkeys, and goats. The lack of research on this topic for these animal types is a gap in the existing literature. Despite the lack of research in farm settings, these animals are still likely to have an instinct to hide. For example, ducks are known to have a natural drive to find a secluded nesting area to lay their eggs [21]. Wild lobsters also make great use of hiding places throughout their lives, especially in the juvenile stage when lobsters will near-continuously remain in or near a shelter to avoid predation [27]. Wild goats are known to seclude from their herd during the periparturient period, and neonatal kids will remain hidden for the first several days after birth [36].

Six animal types were included in the initial literature search as farmed species in North America, but no papers were found (bison, elk, alpacas, llamas, horses, and donkeys). Literature describing the natural behaviour of bison and elk indicates that they have many of the same natural hiding instincts as the other animals included in this review, particularly during the periparturient and neonatal periods [37, 38]. Despite their prevalence as farmed animals, little information is available on the natural hiding behaviour of horses, donkeys, alpacas, and llamas; more investigation is warranted. We encourage more research on providing hiding spaces to these animals, especially for those where there is evidence to suggest the species would use a hiding space in a more natural setting.

Aim 2: Context of hide provision

Parturition and oviposition.

More than half (84/151) of the papers focused on the time of parturition and oviposition; most of these papers (63/84) explored the use of nest boxes at oviposition by poultry. In these papers, common methodologies included investigation on whether hens make use of nest boxes, the factors that influence nest box use, and the impact that nest box provision has on the hens. For example, one early study [39] explored all three of these factors in caged laying hens by providing hens with various configurations of nest box and describing the chickens’ use of the nest boxes along with their behaviour around oviposition.

Several papers (18/84) investigated the use of hiding spaces around parturition in ungulates. Most of these (11/18) were focused on dairy cattle, typically describing cows’ use of spaces to seclude from pen-mates at parturition. Spaces to seclude before, during, or after calving were provided in several different ways; for example, creating enclosed box stalls with wide or narrow entrance gaps [40] or placing a hiding wall [41] in group maternity pens, or providing areas of natural cover [42]. The remaining ungulate papers described the periparturient seclusion behaviour of pigs [43] and sheep provided with artificial [4446] and natural hiding spaces [47], as well as farmed deer provided with both artificial covered areas [48] as well as areas of natural cover [49]. A further 3 papers explored the periparturient hide use of farmed foxes [50] and rabbits [51, 52] when provided with nest boxes that allowed for relatively more seclusion compared to a traditional nest box.

Allowing periparturient animals the space to seclude common in the presented literature. However, more research could be beneficial, particularly in animals that are not well-represented in the current literature. For example, few papers explored the provision of nest boxes to non-chicken poultry, particularly ducks and turkeys, despite evidence that these animals seek seclusion around oviposition in the natural environment [21, 53]. Likewise, non-cattle ungulates such as goats and pigs also demonstrate the natural urge to seclude around parturition [36, 54, 55]; however, no papers were found on this topic for goats and only two papers were found for pigs. Many papers have studied the expression of natural nest-building behaviours in farmed sows (for a review, see [54]), but more research investigating sows’ or piglets’ desire to physically hide or seclude could be beneficial. Four papers were found on the seclusion of ewes around lambing; however, all were published in or before 1985, thus, more research into current farming settings is encouraged. Mink, foxes, and rabbits are known to seclude during the periparturient period in the natural environment [18, 56, 57]; however, relatively few papers were found exploring this behaviour in the farmed counterparts of these animals.

General enrichment.

The second most common context for hides was their use for general enrichment for juvenile and adult animals (65/147), with all animal types represented. Many (20/65) of these papers explored the use of hiding spaces for poultry and were often placed in an outdoor range as a method to promote greater ranging behaviour in the birds. This was done using both artificial structures, such as vertical and/or horizontal walls [58], as well as natural vegetative cover [59]. Providing hiding shelters, including hay bales [60] and vertical walls [61], to poultry in indoor pens was also done to investigate their use to promote birds’ comfortable use of the space, particularly in the center of pens.

Fur carnivores, including farmed foxes and mink, were the next most represented animal type in this category of papers (18/65). Farmed foxes and mink are unique, as it is often considered standard for modern farming to provide these animals with hiding boxes in the cage [23]. Thus, the papers found for this review primarily focus on the impact of giving foxes and mink additional hiding places, often in combination with other enrichment items. These additional hiding places often took the form of an extra nest box [62] or even a mesh tunnel [63]. Some papers, particularly early papers, did investigate the benefits of providing these animals with a sole hiding place compared to a barren cage [64, 65]. Assessments of animals’ motivation to access nest boxes (along with other enrichment items) were also performed [66], as were descriptions of how much the animals’ interacted with hiding places compared to other enrichment items [66, 67] and how much the animals used the hiding places over time [68], which tended to be higher and remain high over time compared to other enrichment items.

Fish and crustaceans were also represented in this category (13/65). These papers described the animals’ use of hiding places in otherwise barren tanks and how the animals were affected by the hiding places. Hides were typically provided to group-housed fish in the form of tunnels [69], artificial plants [70], real plants [71], or other overhead cover [72]. Hides were provided to singly housed lobsters in the form of a dish with sides [73], a box [73], or a tunnel [74]. The benefit of rearing fish and lobsters with hiding places on survival rates upon release into the wild was also explored [74, 75], as this may be a useful method to “train” these animals to seek safety from potential predators as they would in the natural environment.

Several papers (9/65) explored the provision of hiding places for enrichment to ungulates, including pigs, goats, sheep, and cattle. These papers primarily focused on the provision of hiding walls [76] or cubicles [77] to group-housed animals to decrease agonistic behaviour between animals. Several more papers (5/65) explored the provision of hiding places to small herbivores, which explored group-housed rabbits’ use of hiding areas, including tubes [78], walls [79], and boxes [80], and the subsequent impact on the rabbits, particularly agonistic and abnormal behaviours. Overall, the provision of hiding places to animals as general enrichment has been well researched as a tactic to improve animal welfare across many types of animals, particularly in ungulates, fur carnivores, poultry, and fish.

Neonatal, sickness, and injury.

Providing hiding spaces to neonatal and sick/injured animals was less common in the literature, with only 6 and 2 papers existing on each these topics, respectively. All 8 of these papers focused on ungulates. Four of the papers exploring hide use in neonatal animals focused on newborn, extensively housed deer calves, and described calves’ use of both artificial [81] and natural [82] hiding spaces in the days immediately following birth. Two papers described how indoor-housed dairy cow-calf pairs utilized barriers in the pen as a place to seclude in the hours after birth [40, 83]. More research is encouraged to assess neonatal hiding behavior in farmed species, particularly ungulates that are considered to be “hider” species, such as cattle and goats, where mothers will hide their young depending on their available resources [15]. Oftentimes “follower” species, such as sheep, are thought to perform fewer neonatal hiding behaviours as other “hider” species [84]. However, investigation into neonatal hiding is still warranted, as the hider-follower dynamic has been criticized as overly simplistic (see [15]) and free-ranging sheep also show similar seclusion seeking behaviours to cattle in the peri-parturient and neonatal stages [55]. Both papers exploring hiding during illness and injury were focused on dairy cattle, including cows’ patterns of seclusion during times of periparturient sickness [85], and calves’ use of hiding spaces after disbudding [86]. In many farm environments, animals may be isolated in hospital pens which may give them some level of seclusion; seclusion may be worth considering when designing hospital areas for sick and injured animals. We encourage further research on this topic, as separation from conspecifics is considered a “sickness behaviour” in laboratory animals [31], and may have valuable applications to animal production industries.

Aim 3: Effect of hide provision on the animals

Hide use, motivation, and/or preference.

Overall, it appears that most authors reported positive outcomes for animals in their studies using hides. Many of these papers focused on use, preference, or motivation for a hiding space. Hide use was a common outcome in many species, particularly around parturition and oviposition. For example, nearly all papers reported that laying chicken (both cage [39] and aviary [87] housed), quail [88], and duck [89] hens used provided nest boxes to lay their eggs. Likewise, extensively housed deer [49], extensively housed cattle [90], indoor-housed dairy cows [41], dairy cow-calf pairs [83], semi-extensively housed dairy cattle [42], indoor-housed ewes [45, 46], and outdoor-housed ewes [44] were reported to retreat to secluded hiding areas before, during, and after parturition. Indoor-housed dairy cows increased use of hiding spaces when experiencing illness during the post-parturient period.

Some inconclusive/neutral results for dairy cows, sows, and ewes were also reported, as some papers reported that most cows [91, 92], sows [48], and ewes [47] did not make use of provided hiding areas for parturition. It is possible that there are also other factors influencing the dams’ choice of calving, farrowing, or lambing site; for example, one study found that cows appeared to choose calving sites that were close in proximity to the location of a previous calving [92]. Additionally, some poultry papers report that a minority of hens who do not use nest boxes (“floor layers”) may find provided nest boxes to be unsatisfactory [93, 94]. However, others posit that these floor layers may simply prefer more open nest sites and are equally motivated to access these open nest sites compared to nest layers [95]. Research into how to design satisfactory facilities for all hens could help improve the welfare of the animals as well as improve productivity, as floor laying is a concern for aviary housing systems in which the eggs are collected via the nest boxes.

Many papers reported animals used hiding structures for times other than oviposition/parturition. For example, newborn, extensively housed deer calves were also reported to make great use of both natural [82] and artificial [96] hiding places to hide in the days following birth. Indoor-housed dairy calves also used a secluded hiding area in a group pen, and increased hiding behaviour when experiencing injuries after disbudding [86]. Group-housed pigs make extensive use of hiding partitions in group pens, both during feeding and resting [97]. Both farmed mink [63] and foxes [67] have been reported to make use of hiding places and use remained high over time [68]. However, some papers reported that farmed foxes did not use provided hiding spaces [98], and that the roof of the nest was more often used as a platform instead. For example, one paper reported that mink preferred to rest in a mesh, non-opaque overhead tunnel that connected the main pen to the enrichment pen, rather than in the opaque hiding areas provided in the enrichment pen and in the home pen [63].

Only two outcomes related to hide use in rabbits were reported, both of which were inconclusive. One paper reported that rabbits did not use a provided hiding tube [78], and another reported that rabbits did use a provided shelter, but it was mostly used as a platform rather than as a hiding place [80]. More research into the provision of hiding places for rabbits is warranted to be able to provide structural enrichment that is meaningful for the animals. For example, wild rabbits dig their own burrows [25] whereas the studies found in this review provided rabbits with pre-made hiding places. The evidence that foxes, mink, and rabbits may prefer to rest on tall places rather than inside enclosed hiding areas also warrants further research into preferred hiding place design for these animals.

Papers involving fish and crustaceans reported 7 outcomes related to hide use, all of which were positive. Papers reported that many species of fish, including char [99], wrasse [100], bream [75], catfish [101], and cod [102], as well as lobsters [73, 74], used hiding places that were provided to them. Since the results demonstrated that the studied fish made use of hiding structures when provided, continued research on other fish species as well as on preferred hide design features is encouraged.

Juvenile and adult poultry also utilized hiding structures for times other than oviposition: Broiler chicks were reported to cluster around straw bales provided for shelter [60, 103], chickens clustered around both vertical and overhead hiding structures in an outdoor range [58, 104], and caged quail made use of provided hiding walls [105]. Despite this, some poultry papers reported inconclusive findings. For example, placing visual barriers around dust baths did not improve use of them [106], a few papers reported that hiding places in the range did not improve use of the range [107], and placing vertical hiding walls in the middle of a pen did not encourage birds to use the center of the pen [108]. Although most research suggests that poultry make extensive use of hiding structures for general enrichment, it remains unclear why some hiding structures are not used.

Motivation tests were also used to assess animals’ desire to access hiding places. Chicken [95, 109] and duck [89] hens demonstrated motivation to access nesting boxes for oviposition by pushing through increasingly narrow gaps and weighted push doors to access the nest boxes. Foxes also demonstrated motivation to access hiding areas by pushing weighted doors [66, 110]. However, dairy cows did not demonstrate motivation to access secluded calving areas around parturition; the authors posited that the cows could not demonstrate the learned push-door task in combination with the desire to access the secluded area during calving [91].

Factors influencing animals’ preference of various hide features were also explored, particularly in poultry. Laying hens most often preferred the most secluded nest available. This included top rack or corner nests in aviaries [111] and nests with enclosed sides or curtained entrances [112, 113]. Extensively housed deer calves also demonstrated a preference for natural hiding places over artificial hiding places when offered simultaneously, although the artificial hides were also used [81]. Studies have also investigated dairy cows’ preference for narrow or wide hiding walls; the wider walls were preferred by both cows and cow-calf pairs [40]. This research suggests that animals will often prefer hiding areas with relatively greater degrees of seclusion.

Affiliative and agonistic behaviours.

Some papers measured the impact of hide use on social behaviors, such as increasing affiliative behaviours and reducing agonistic behaviour. Two papers (1 positive and 1 neutral) reported outcomes related to affiliative behaviours: a hiding wall increased affiliative behaviours between group-housed calves [114], while a hiding structure decreased overall social contact between group-housed rabbits [115].

Regarding agonistic behaviour, 9 positive outcomes, 5 negative outcomes, and 3 neutral outcomes were reported. Hiding places were demonstrated to decrease agonistic behaviours between socially housed goats [116], pigs [97, 117], calves [114], quail [118], foxes [119], and fish [70]. However, several papers reported that providing group-housed animals with hiding places increased agonistic behaviours, this was reported to have resulted from animals competing for the hiding spaces [69, 100, 120] or because the hide formation subsequently restricted open space in the pen [76, 121]. Based on the existing literature on this topic, it appears that giving group-housed animals spaces to hide can improve the social interactions between the animals, but only if implemented in a way that does not cause strain on the space allowances of the living space.

Abnormal repetitive behaviours.

Fourteen outcomes related to abnormal repetitive behaviours were reported, all of which were positive, indicating that these behaviors were reduced when a hide was provided. For example, providing animals with hiding areas for general enrichment or nest boxes for oviposition decreased abnormal repetitive behaviours in pigs [121], laying hens [122], foxes [68], mink [123], rabbits [80], and fish [70]. It was commonly concluded that the reduction of abnormal repetitive behaviours was because hiding spaces helped reduce affective and physiological states that are presumed to be negative, such as perceived boredom, distress, or fear.

Health, injuries, and/or production.

Some papers found that the provision of the hide improved measurements of health, reduced injuries, or improved productivity. Of the papers that were found, 24 positive outcomes, 4 negative outcomes, and 6 neutral outcomes were reported. Many papers reported that animals, including pigs, hens, foxes, mink, and fish provided with hiding places had increased feed intake [75], improved growth [99, 123], higher egg production [124], improved mating success [125], fewer injuries (including injuries from the environment as well as conspecific-inflicted and self-inflicted mutilation) [77, 126, 127], and decreased mortality [50, 99]. Two papers also reported that providing group-housed ewes with lambing cubicles reduced incidences of lamb stealing [45, 46]. However, other papers reported that providing hides increased injuries due to competition [69], resulted in lower fox fur quality [64], decreased feed intake [65], and decreased growth [78]. Research is still needed to address these issues while still allowing the animals to express their natural hiding behaviours.

Physiological indicators of stress.

Several papers reported outcomes related to physiological indicators of stress, including 8 positive outcomes, 1 negative, and 2 neutral outcomes. For example, farrowing sows had decreased heart rate when housed in a pen that allowed for seclusion [43], as did mink when housed with a nest box [123] and fish when given hiding enrichment [126]. Chicken [127] and quail [118] hens had reduced plasma and fecal glucocorticoid levels and decreased tonic immobility when housed with nest boxes, while duck hens experienced “stress-induced hyperthermia” when unable to access their nest boxes [89]. However, two papers reported that providing hens with nest boxes did not have an impact on physiological indicators of stress, such as corticosterone levels [128, 129]. An additional study reported that providing fish with hiding places increased cortisol due to the accompanying increase in competition and aggression [69]. Development of methods to practically provide sufficient hiding spaces and reduce the accompanying agonism and stress may help resolve some of these issues.

Behavioural indicators of affective state.

Implications about animals’ affective state based on behavioural measures were also reported in many studies. From these, 23 positive outcomes, 4 negative outcomes, and 8 neutral outcomes were reported. Most of these papers used poultry; for example, it was documented that providing hens with nests increased settled laying behaviours [130, 131], reduced behavioural signs of frustration (pacing and sham dustbathing) [132, 133], and increased comfort behaviours (preening) [134]. One paper demonstrated the offspring of quail hens housed with hiding walls had lower emotional reactivity, even though the offspring never experienced the hides themselves [105]. Hiding places improved maternal care in both fox [50], mink [62], and sheep [46] dams, and foxes showed decreased fear of humans [135]. Rabbit dams given a more secluded nest box performed fewer nest-disturbing behaviours, and rabbits housed with a hiding shelter performed fewer abnormal behaviours (restlessness, excessive grooming, bar-gnawing, and timidity) [80]. Additionally, group-housed dairy cows who were provided with a hiding wall showed more settled behaviour when close to calving, including paying less attention to other cows and fewer position changes [136]. Group-housed ewes who were provided with lambing cubicles also demonstrated more settled pre-lambing behaviours, and ewes who eventually selected to lamb inside a cubicle spent less time traveling and investigating birth sites. However, two papers reported that housing foxes with hiding places increased fearfulness [64, 137], and group-housed pigs’ resting behaviour was disturbed when housed with hiding walls [121]. Most studies that included these outcomes reported a positive effect of the hiding place, but more research is encouraged to understand the behavioural indicators of negative affective state, such as increased fearfulness.

Limitations

The search criteria required that papers include “behaviour” or “welfare” in the title or abstract, thus, papers focusing on physiological or production measures may be underrepresented in this paper. Moreover, several papers in this review (37/151), particularly for fur carnivores and poultry, provided the animals with hiding places in combination with other enrichment items, thus it is not clear if the outcomes were a result of the hide alone [138]. These papers still provide valuable insight into the housing needs of farmed animals, especially since several papers have reported that when animals are given several enrichment items, the hiding enrichment item is among the most interacted with [67], and their use remains high over time [68]. However, it is a limitation of this review that the impact of the hiding spaces in these studies cannot be separated from the other enrichment items also provided.

Conclusion

In this scoping review, we investigated the existing research into the provision of hiding spaces to farmed animals, including the types of animals represented in this literature, the context of hide provision, and the reported impact that hides had on the animals. Many animal types were represented, with the most being laying hens. Some animals were underrepresented in the literature and others were missing entirely, despite evidence that many of these animals perform hiding behaviors in their natural habitat. Common situations in which animals were provided hides included at parturition and oviposition, for general enrichment, for neonatal animals, or for injured/sick animals; the latter two categories were underrepresented and require further investigation. We also categorized the outcomes reported in the papers and assessed their implications for how hiding places impact animals. Although most outcomes indicate that providing farmed animals is beneficial for their welfare, health or productivity, some conflicts exist in the literature. In cases where outcomes indicate that hiding places may have a negative impact on the animals, the authors often reported competition among group-housed animals for the hiding places. We emphasize that while providing farmed animals with hiding places appears to be beneficial to their welfare in many circumstances, it should not be considered a replacement to more systematic housing concerns such as adequate space allowance.

Supporting information

S1 Checklist. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR) checklist.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0277665.s001

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to acknowledge Kim Mears for her assistance in developing the literature search.

References

  1. 1. Fraser D. Animal Welfare and the Intensification of Animal Production: An Alternate Interpretation. Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations; 2005.
  2. 2. Fraser D. Toward a global perspective on farm animal welfare. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;113: 330–339.
  3. 3. Fraser D, Weary DM, Pajor EA, Milligan BN. A scientific conception of animal welfare that reflects ethical concerns. Animal welfare. 1997;6: 187–205.
  4. 4. Spooner JM, Schuppli CA, Fraser D. Attitudes of Canadian citizens toward farm animal welfare: A qualitative study. Livest Sci. 2014;163: 150–158.
  5. 5. Spooner JM, Schuppli CA, Fraser D. Attitudes of Canadian beef producers toward animal welfare. Animal Welfare. 2012;21: 273–283.
  6. 6. Vanhonacker F, Verbeke W, Van Poucke E, Tuyttens FAM. Do citizens and farmers interpret the concept of farm animal welfare differently? Livest Sci. 2008;116: 126–136.
  7. 7. Fraser D. Animal welfare and the intensification of animal production. In: Thompson PB, editor. The Ethics of Intensification. Dordrecht: Springer; 2008. pp. 167–190.
  8. 8. Ozoga JJ, Verme LJ, Bienz CS. Parturition behavior and territoriality in white-tailed deer: Impact on neonatal mortality. J Wildl Manage. 1982;46: 1–11. Available: https://www.jstor.org/stable/3808402
  9. 9. Barbknecht AE, Fairbanks WS, Rogerson JD, Maichak EJ, Scurlock BM, Meadows LL. Elk parturition site selection at local and landscape scales. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2011;75: 646–654.
  10. 10. Bowyer RT, Ballenberghe AN, Kie JG, Maier JAK. Birth-site selection by Alaskan moose: Maternal strategies for coping with a risky environment. J Mammal. 1990;80: 1070–1083. Available: https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/80/4/1070/851824
  11. 11. Shackleton DM, Shank CC. A review of the social behavior of feral and wild sheep and goats. J Anim Sci. 1984;58: 500–509.
  12. 12. Rudge MR. Mother and kid behaviour in feral goats (Cupru hircus L.). Ethology. 1970;27: 687–692.
  13. 13. Vitale AF, Tenucci M, Papini M, Lovari S. Social behaviour of the calves of semi-wild Maremma cattle, Bos primigenius taurus. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1986;16: 217–231.
  14. 14. Lent PC. Mother-infant relationships in ungulates. The Behaviour of Ungulates and its Relation to Management. 1974. pp. 14–55.
  15. 15. Rørvang MV, Nielsen BL, Herskin MS, Jensen MB. Prepartum maternal behavior of domesticated cattle: A comparison with managed, feral, and wild ungulates. Front Vet Sci. 2018;5. pmid:29594159
  16. 16. Rödel HG, Starkloff A, Seltmann MW, Prager G, von Holst D. Causes and predictors of nest mortality in a European rabbit population. Mammalian Biology. 2009;74: 198–209.
  17. 17. Malmkvist J, Palme R. Periparturient nest building: Implications for parturition, kit survival, maternal stress and behaviour in farmed mink (Mustela vison). Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;114: 270–283.
  18. 18. Macdonald DW, Zimen E. Social factors affecting reproduction amongst red foxes (Vulpes vulpes L., 1758). In: Zimen E, editor. Biogeographica, Vol 18: The Red Fox. Dordrecht: Springer; 1980. pp. 123–175.
  19. 19. Collias NE, Collias EC. Social organization of a red junglefowl, Gallus gallus, population related to evolution theory. Anim Behav. 1996;51: 1337–1354.
  20. 20. Thogmartin WE. Landscape attributes and nest-site selection in wild turkeys. Auk. 1999;116: 912–923. Available: https://academic.oup.com/auk/article/116/4/912/5172411
  21. 21. Bentzen RL, Powell AN, Suydam RS. Strategies for nest‐site selection by king eiders. J Wildl Manage. 2009;73: 932–938.
  22. 22. Tryjanowski P, Jankowiak L, Antczak M. Habitat use, food and the importance of poultry in the diet of the red fox Vulpes vulpes in extensive farmland in Poland. World Appl Sci J. 2008;4: 886–890.
  23. 23. Nimon AJ, Broom DM. The welfare of farmed mink (Mustela vision) in relation to housing and management: A review. Animal Welfare. 1998;8: 205–228.
  24. 24. Larivière S. Mustela vison. Mammalian Species. 1999. pp. 1–9.
  25. 25. Moreno S, Villafuerte R, Delibes M. Cover is safe during the day but dangerous at night: the use of vegetation by European wild rabbits. Can J Zool. 1996;74: 1656–1660.
  26. 26. Therrien CA, Morbey YE, Neff BD. Hide or seek: presence of stream shelter influences the foraging behaviour of juvenile Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar, Linnaeus 1758). Environ Biol Fishes. 2022;105.
  27. 27. Lawton P, Lavalli KL. Postlarval, juvenile, adolescent, and adult ecology. In: Factor JR, editor. Biology of the Lobster Homarus americanus. San Diego: Academic Press; 1995. pp. 47–88.
  28. 28. Collias NE, Collias EC. A field study of the Red Jungle Fowl in north-central India. Condor. 1967;69: 360–386. Available: https://about.jstor.org/terms
  29. 29. Baillie BR, Glaser AB. Roost habitat of a North Island blue duck (Hymenolaimus malacorhynchos) population. Notornis. 2005;52: 1–5.
  30. 30. van Hooff J. On the ethology of pain, its experience and expression. In: Beynen A, Solleveld H, editors. New developments in biosciences: Their implications for laboratory animal science. Dordrecht: Springer; 1988. pp. 42–46.
  31. 31. Arakawa H, Arakawa K, Deak T. Sickness-related odor communication signals as determinants of social behavior in rat: A role for inflammatory processes. Horm Behav. 2010;57: 330–341. pmid:20079357
  32. 32. Loehle C. Social barriers to pathogen transmission in wild animal populations. Ecology. 1995;76: 326–335.
  33. 33. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. International Journal of Surgery. 2021;88. pmid:33789826
  34. 34. Ouzzani M, Hammady H, Fedorowicz Z, Elmagarmid A. Rayyan-a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Syst Rev. 2016;5. pmid:27919275
  35. 35. Towards Dawkins M. and objective method of assessing welfare in domestic fowl. Applied Animal Ethology. 1976;2: 245–254.
  36. 36. Allan CJ, Hinch GN, Holst PJ. Behaviour of parturient Australian bush goats. II. Spatial relationships and activity patterns. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1991;32: 65–74.
  37. 37. Altmann M. Social behavior of elk, Cervus canadensis Nelsoni, in the Jackson Hole area of Wyoming. Source: Behaviour. 1952;4: 116–143.
  38. 38. Green WCH. The development of independence in bison: pre-weaning spatial relations between mothers and calves. Anim Behav. 1992;43: 759–773.
  39. 39. Appleby MC, Hughes BO. Cages modified with perches and nests for the improvement of bird welfare. Worlds Poult Sci J. 1990;46: 38–40.
  40. 40. Jensen MB, Rørvang MV. The degree of visual cover and location of birth fluids affect dairy cows’ choice of calving site. J Dairy Sci. 2018;101: 9483–9492. pmid:30122418
  41. 41. Creutzinger KC, Dann HM, Krawczel PD, Habing GG, Proudfoot KL. The effect of stocking density and a blind on the behavior of Holstein dairy cattle in group maternity pens. Part I: Calving location, locomotion, and separation behavior. J Dairy Sci. 2021;104: 7109–7121. pmid:33773795
  42. 42. Edwards EM, Krawczel PD, Dann HM, Schneider LG, Whitlock B, Proudfoot KL. Calving location preference and changes in lying and exploratory behavior of preparturient dairy cattle with access to pasture. J Dairy Sci. 2020;103: 5455–5465. pmid:32278561
  43. 43. Damm BI, Lisborg L, Vestergaard KS, Vanicek J. Nest-building, behavioural disturbances and heart rate in farrowing sows kept in crates and schmid pens. Livest Prod Sci. 2003;80: 175–187.
  44. 44. Stevens D, Alexander G, Lynch JJ. Do Merino ewes seek isolation or shelter at lambing? Applied Animal Ethology. 1981;7: 149–155.
  45. 45. Gonyou HW, Stookey JM. Use of lambing cubicles and the behavior of ewes at parturition. J Anim Sci. 1983;56: 787–791. pmid:6853379
  46. 46. Gonyou H, Stookey J. Behavior of parturient ewes in group-lambing pens with and without cubicles. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1985;14: 163–171.
  47. 47. Alexander G, Lynch JJ, Mottershead BE. Use of shelter and selection of lambing sites by shorn and unshorn ewes in paddocks with closely or widely spaced shelters. Applied Animal Ethology. 1979;5: 51–69.
  48. 48. Damm BI, Heiskanen T, Pedersen LJ, Jørgensen E, Forkman B. Sow preferences for farrowing under a cover with and without access to straw. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2010;126: 97–104.
  49. 49. Birtles T, Goldspink CR, Gibson S, Holland RK. Calf site selection by red deer (Cervus elaphus) from three contrasting habitats in northwest England: Implications for welfare and management. Animal Welfare. 1998;7: 427–443.
  50. 50. Braastad BO. Behaviour of silver foxes in traditional breeding boxes and in boxes with an entrance tunnel. Animal Welfare. 1996;5: 155–166.
  51. 51. Baumann P, Oester H, Stauffacher M. The use of a cat-flap at the nest entrance to mimic natural conditions in the breeding of fattening rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Animal Welfare. 2005;14: 135–142.
  52. 52. Rommers JM, Boiti C, de Jong I, Brecchia G. Performance and behaviour of rabbit does in a group-housing system with natural mating or artificial insemination. Reprod Nutr Dev. 2006;46: 677–687. pmid:17169314
  53. 53. Wood JD, Cohen BS, Conner LM, Collier BA, Chamberlain MJ. Nest and brood site selection of eastern wild turkeys. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2019;83: 192–204.
  54. 54. Wischner D, Kemper N, Krieter J. Nest-building behaviour in sows and consequences for pig husbandry. Livestock Science. 2009. pp. 1–8.
  55. 55. Fisher A, Matthews L. The Social Behaviour of Sheep. In: Keeling L, Gonyou H, editors. Social Behaviour in Farm Animals. Wallingford: CABI; 2001. pp. 211–239.
  56. 56. González-Mariscal G. Neuroendocrinology of maternal behavior in the rabbit. Hormones and Behavior. Academic Press Inc.; 2001. pp. 125–132. https://doi.org/10.1006/hbeh.2001.1692 pmid:11534972
  57. 57. Hovland A, Ahola L, Malmkvist J. Behaviour of foxes and mink kept for fur production. In: Jensen P, editor. The Ethology of Domestic Animals: An Introductory Text, 3rd Edition. Oxfordshire: CABI; 2017. pp. 255–271.
  58. 58. Larsen H, Rault JL. Preference for artificial range enrichment design features in free-range commercial laying hens. Br Poult Sci. 2021;62: 311–319. pmid:33480293
  59. 59. Dawkins MS, Cook PA, Whittingham MJ, Mansell KA, Harper AE. What makes free-range broiler chickens range? In situ measurement of habitat preference. Anim Behav. 2003;66: 151–160.
  60. 60. Baxter M, Bailie CL, O’Connell NE. Evaluation of a dustbathing substrate and straw bales as environmental enrichments in commercial broiler housing. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2018;200: 78–85.
  61. 61. Cornetto T, Estevez I. Behavior of the domestic fowl in the presence of vertical panels. Poult Sci. 2001;80: 1455–1462. pmid:11599705
  62. 62. Díez-León M, Mason G. Effects of environmental enrichment and stereotypic behavior on maternal behavior and infant viability in a model carnivore, the American mink (Neovison vison). Zoo Biol. 2016;35: 19–28. pmid:26536278
  63. 63. Dallaire JA, Meagher RK, Mason GJ. Individual differences in stereotypic behaviour predict individual differences in the nature and degree of enrichment use in caged American mink. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2012;142: 98–108.
  64. 64. Harri M, Mononen J, Rekilä T, Korhonen H, Niemelä P. Effects of top nest box on growth, fur quality and behaviour of blue foxes (Alopex lagopus) during their growing season. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica A: Animal Sciences. 1998;48: 184–191.
  65. 65. Hansen SW, Hansen BK, Berg P. The effect of cage environment and ad libitum feeding on the circadian rhythm, behaviour, and feed intake of farm mink. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica A: Animal Sciences. 1994;44: 120–127.
  66. 66. Koistinen T, Korhonen HT, Hämäläinen E, Mononen J. Blue foxes’ (Vulpes lagopus) motivation to gain access and interact with various resources. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2016;176: 105–111.
  67. 67. Koistinen T, Korhonen HT. Complex housing environment for farmed blue foxes (Vulpes lagopus): Use of various resources. Animal. 2013;7: 1354–1361. pmid:23481620
  68. 68. Korhonen HT, Eskeli P. A study on multi-enriched housing environment in blue foxes. Open J Anim Sci. 2015;05: 77–85.
  69. 69. Boerrigter JGJ, van den Bos R, van de Vis H, Spanings T, Flik G. Effects of density, PVC-tubes and feeding time on growth, stress and aggression in African catfish (Clarias gariepinus). Aquac Res. 2016;47: 2553–2568.
  70. 70. Favero JN, Giaquinto PC. Environmental enrichment techniques and tryptophan supplementation used to improve the quality of life and animal welfare of Nile tilapia. Aquac Rep. 2020;17: 100354.
  71. 71. Berejikian BA, Tezak PE. Rearing in enriched hatchery tanks improves dorsal fin quality of juvenile steelhead. N Am J Aquac. 2005;67: 289–293.
  72. 72. Champneys T, Castaldo G, Consuegra S, Garcia De Leaniz C. Density-dependent changes in neophobia and stress-coping styles in the world’s oldest farmed fish. R Soc Open Sci. 2018;5. pmid:30662751
  73. 73. van der Meeren GI. Effects of experience with shelter in hatchery-reared juvenile European lobsters Homarus gammarus. Mar Freshw Res. 2001;52: 1487–1493.
  74. 74. Carere C, Nascetti G, Carlini A, Santucci D, Alleva E. Actions for restocking of the European lobster (Homarus gammarus): a case study on the relevance of behaviour and welfare assessment of cultured juveniles. Rendiconti Lincei. 2015;26: 59–64.
  75. 75. Rae DL, Mos B, Scott A, Dworjanyn SA. Training fish for restocking: refuge and predator training in the hatchery has limited benefits for a marine fish. J Fish Biol. 2020;97: 172–182. pmid:32278329
  76. 76. Jørgensen GHM, Andersen IL, Bøe KE. The effect of different pen partition configurations on the behaviour of sheep. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2009;119: 66–70.
  77. 77. Wang LH, Li YZ. Effect of continuous access to feeding stalls during mixing on behavior, welfare, and performance of group-housed gestating sows in different social ranks. Can J Anim Sci. 2016;96: 386–396.
  78. 78. Trocino A, Zomeño C, Filiou E, Birolo M, White P, Xiccato G. The use of environmental enrichments affects performance and behavior of growing rabbits housed in collective pens. Animals. 2019;9: 1–13. pmid:31394853
  79. 79. Rommers J, de Greef KH. Are combi parks just as useful as regular parks for fatteners for part-time group housing of rabbit does? World Rabbit Science. 2018;26: 299–305.
  80. 80. Hansen LT, Berthelsen H. The effect of environmental enrichment on the behaviour of caged rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2000;68: 163–178. pmid:10771324
  81. 81. Wass JA, Pollard JC, Littlejohn RP. Observations on the hiding behaviour of farmed red deer (Cervus elaphus) calves. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2004;88: 111–120.
  82. 82. Endicott-Davies DR, Barrie AN, Fisher MW. Differences in the hiding behaviour of new-born red deer and hybrid 1/4 Pére David’s X 3/4 red deer calves. Animal Science. 1996;62: 363–367.
  83. 83. Zobel G, Proudfoot K, Cave V, Huddart F, Webster J. The use of hides during and after calving in New Zealand dairy cows. Animals. 2020;10: 1–13. pmid:33266194
  84. 84. Lyons DM, Price EO, Moberg GP. Social grouping tendencies and separation-induced distress in juvenile sheep and goats. Dev Psychobiol. 1993;26: 251–259. pmid:8339864
  85. 85. Proudfoot KL, Jensen MB, Weary DM, von Keyserlingk MAG. Dairy cows seek isolation at calving and when ill. J Dairy Sci. 2014;97: 2731–2739. pmid:24630650
  86. 86. Gingerich KN, Choulet V, Miller-Cushon EK. Disbudding affects use of a shelter provided to group-housed dairy calves. J Dairy Sci. 2020;103: 10519–10529. pmid:32896400
  87. 87. Oliveira JL, Xin HW, Wang KL, Zhao Y. Evaluation of nesting behavior of individual laying hens in an enriched colony housing by using RFID technology. International Journal of Agricultural and Biological Engineering. 2019;12: 7–15.
  88. 88. Schmid I, Wechsler B. Identification of key nest site stimuli for Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica). Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1998;57: 145–156.
  89. 89. Barrett LA, Maloney SK, Blache D. Pekin ducks are motivated to access their nest site and exhibit a stress-induced hyperthermia when unable to do so. Animal. 2021;15: 100067. pmid:33515988
  90. 90. Lidfors LM, Moran D, Jung J, Jensen P, Castren H. Behaviour at calving and choice of calving place in cattle kept in different environments. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1994;42: 11–28.
  91. 91. Rørvang MV, Herskin MS, Jensen MB. The motivation-based calving facility: Social and cognitive factors influence isolation seeking behaviour of Holstein dairy cows at calving. PLoS One. 2018;13: 1–19. pmid:29346399
  92. 92. Rørvang M v., Nielsen BL, Herskin MS, Jensen MB. Short communication: Calving site selection of multiparous, group-housed dairy cows is influenced by site of a previous calving. J Dairy Sci. 2017;100: 1467–1471. pmid:27939545
  93. 93. Cooper JJ, Appleby MC. Individual variation in prelaying behaviour and the incidence of floor eggs. Br Poult Sci. 1996;37: 245–253. pmid:8773834
  94. 94. Cooper JJ, Appleby MC. Motivational aspects of individual variation in response to nestboxes by laying hens. Anim Behav. 1997;54: 1245–1253. pmid:9398377
  95. 95. Kruschwitz A, Zupan M, Buchwalder T, Huber-Eicher B. Nest preference of laying hens (Gallus gallus domesticus) and their motivation to exert themselves to gain nest access. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;112: 321–330.
  96. 96. Hodgetts B v., Waas JR, Matthews LR. Use of different artificial shelter types by farmed red deer (Cervus elaphus) calves. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2002;79: 43–52.
  97. 97. Petherick JC, Bodero DA v, Blackshaw JK. The use of partial barriers along the feed trough in a group housing system for non-lactating sows. Farm Buildings & Engineering. 1987;4: 32–36.
  98. 98. Alasuutari S, Korhonen H. Environmental enrichment in relation to behaviour in farmbred blue foxes. Norwegian Journal of Agricultural Sciences. 1992;9: 569–573.
  99. 99. Benhaïm D, Leblanc CA, Lucas G. Impact of a new artificial shelter on Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus, L.) behaviour and culture performance during the endogenous feeding period. Aquaculture. 2009;295: 38–43.
  100. 100. Deady S, Varian SJA, Fives JM. The use of cleaner-fish to control sea lice on two Irish salmon (Salmo salar) farms with particular reference to wrasse behaviour in salmon cages. Aquaculture. 1995;131: 73–90.
  101. 101. Slavík O, Maciak M, Horký P. Shelter use of familiar and unfamiliar groups of juvenile European catfish Silurus glanis. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2012;142: 116–123.
  102. 102. Zimmermann EW, Purchase CF, Fleming IA. Reducing the incidence of net cage biting and the expression of escape-related behaviors in Atlantic cod (Gadus morhua) with feeding and cage enrichment. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2012;141: 71–78.
  103. 103. Bergmann S, Schwarzer A, Wilutzky K, Louton H, Bachmeier J, Schmidt P, et al. Behavior as welfare indicator for the rearing of broilers in an enriched husbandry environment—A field study. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research. 2017;19: 90–101.
  104. 104. Rault JL, van de Wouw A, Hemswortha P. Fly the coop! Vertical structures influence the distribution and behaviour of laying hens in an outdoor range. Aust Vet J. 2013;91: 423–426. pmid:30049059
  105. 105. Guesdon V, Bertin A, Houdelier C, Lumineau S, Formanek L, Kotrschal K, et al. A place to hide in the home-cage decreases yolk androgen levels and offspring emotional reactivity in Japanese quail. PLoS One. 2011;6: 17–22. pmid:21980338
  106. 106. Baxter M, O’Connell NE. Does grouping environmental enrichments together affect the way they are used by commercially housed broiler chickens? Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2019;210: 52–59.
  107. 107. Chielo LI, Pike T, Cooper J. Ranging behaviour of commercial free-range laying hens. Animals. 2016;6. pmid:27128946
  108. 108. Estevez I, Mallapur A, Miller C, Christman MC. Short- and long-term movement patterns in complex confined environments in broiler chickens: The effects of distribution of cover panels and food resources. Poult Sci. 2010;89: 643–650. pmid:20308395
  109. 109. Cooper JJ, Appleby MC. Nesting behaviour of hens: Effects of experience on motivation. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1995;42: 283–295.
  110. 110. Koistinen T, Jauhiainen L, Korhonen HT. Relative value of a nest box, sand floor and extra space during the breeding season in adult blue fox males. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2009;120: 192–200.
  111. 111. Lentfer TL, Gebhardt-Henrich SG, Fröhlich EKF, von Borell E. Nest use is influenced by the positions of nests and drinkers in aviaries. Poult Sci. 2013;92: 1433–1442. pmid:23687137
  112. 112. Sherwin CM, Nicol CJ. Behaviour and production of laying hens in three prototypes of cages incorporating nests. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1992;35: 41–54.
  113. 113. Buchwalder T, Fröhlich EK. Assessment of colony nests for laying hens in conjunction with the authorization procedure. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2011;134: 64–71.
  114. 114. Ninomiya S, Sato S. Effects of “five freedoms” environmental enrichment on the welfare of calves reared indoors. Animal Science Journal. 2009;80: 347–351. pmid:20163647
  115. 115. Buijs S, Keeling LJ, Tuyttens FAM. Behaviour and use of space in fattening rabbits as influenced by cage size and enrichment. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2011;134: 229–238.
  116. 116. Aschwanden J, Gygax L, Wechsler B, Keil NM. Loose housing of small goat groups: Influence of visual cover and elevated levels on feeding, resting and agonistic behaviour. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2009;119: 171–179.
  117. 117. McGlone JJ, Curtis SE. Behaviour and performance of weanling pigs in pens equipped with hide areas. J Anim Sci. 1985;60: 20–24.
  118. 118. Nordi WM, Yamashiro KCE, Klank M, Locatelli-Dittrich R, Morais RN, Reghelin AI, et al. Quail (Coturnixcoturnix japonica) welfare in two confinement systems. Arq Bras Med Vet Zootec. 2012;64: 1001–1008.
  119. 119. Akre AK, Hovland AL, Bakken M. The effects of resource distribution on behaviour in pair housed silver fox vixens (Vulpes vulpes) subsequent to mixing. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2010;126: 67–74.
  120. 120. Hunniford ME, Torrey S, Bédécarrats G, Duncan IJH, Widowski TM. Evidence of competition for nest sites by laying hens in large furnished cages. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2014;161: 95–104.
  121. 121. Bulens A, van Beirendonck S, van Thielen J, Buys N, Driessen B. Hiding walls for fattening pigs: Do they affect behavior and performance? Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2017;195: 32–37.
  122. 122. Shi HP, Zheng WC, Tu J, Li BM. Reducing feather pecking and cloacal cannibalism by providing layer breeders with nest boxes in colony cages for natural mating. International Journal of Agricultural and Biological Engineering. 2018;11: 27–32.
  123. 123. Hansen SW, Hansen BK, Damgaard BM. The effects of cage environment of the welfare of mink. Norwegian Journal of Agricultural Sciences. 1992; 536–544.
  124. 124. Sütô Z, Budai Z, Almási A, Milisits G, Ujváriné J. The effect of different housing systems on traits of experimental crossbred leghorn and brown type layers until 96 weeks of age. Hungarian Agriculture Research. 2018;27: 15–19.
  125. 125. Díez-León M, Bowman J, Bursian S, Filion H, Galicia D, Kanefsky J, et al. Environmentally enriched male mink gain more copulations than stereotypic, barren-reared competitors. PLoS One. 2013;8: 1–11. pmid:24282547
  126. 126. Näslund J, Rosengren M, del Villar D, Gansel L, Norrgård JR, Persson L, et al. Hatchery tank enrichment affects cortisol levels and shelter-seeking in Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar). Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences. 2013;70: 585–590.
  127. 127. Shi H, Tong Q, Zheng W, Tu J, Li B. Effects of nest boxes in natural mating colony cages on fear, stress, and feather damage for layer breeders. J Anim Sci. 2019;97: 4464–4474. pmid:31545381
  128. 128. Barnett JL, Tauson R, Downing JA, Janardhana V, Lowenthal JW, Butler KL, et al. The effects of a perch, dust bath, and nest box, either alone or in combination as used in furnished cages, on the welfare of laying hens. Poult Sci. 2009;88: 456–470. pmid:19211513
  129. 129. Tactacan GB, Guenter W, Lewis NJ, Rodriguez-Lecompte JC, House JD. Performance and welfare of laying hens in conventional and enriched cages. Poult Sci. 2009;88: 698–707. pmid:19276411
  130. 130. Struelens E, van Nuffel A, Tuyttens FAM, Audoorn L, Vranken E, Zoons J, et al. Influence of nest seclusion and nesting material on pre-laying behaviour of laying hens. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;112: 106–119.
  131. 131. Hunniford ME, Widowski TM. Curtained nests facilitate settled nesting behaviour of laying hens in furnished cages. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2018;202: 39–45.
  132. 132. Sherwin CM, Nicol CJ. A descriptive account of the pre-laying behaviour of hens housed individually in modified cages with nests. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1993;38: 49–60.
  133. 133. Engel JM, Widowski TM, Tilbrook AJ, Butler KL, Hemsworth PH. The effects of floor space and nest box access on the physiology and behavior of caged laying hens. Poult Sci. 2019;98: 533–547. pmid:30165652
  134. 134. Pohle K, Cheng HW. Furnished cage system and hen well-being: Comparative effects of furnished cages and battery cages on behavioral exhibitions in White Leghorn chickens. Poult Sci. 2009;88: 1559–1564. pmid:19590069
  135. 135. Korhonen H, Niemelä P. Temperament and reproductive success in farmbred silver foxes housed with and without platforms. Journal of Animal Breeding and Genetics. 1996;113: 209–218.
  136. 136. Creutzinger KC, Dann HM, Krawczel PD, Moraes LE, Pairis-Garcia MD, Proudfoot KL. The effect of stocking density and a blind on the behavior of Holstein dairy cows in group maternity pens. Part II: Labor length, lying behavior, and social behavior. J Dairy Sci. 2021;104: 7122–7134. pmid:33773786
  137. 137. Pedersen V, Moeller NH, Jeppesen LL. Behavioural and physiological effects of post-weaning handling and access to shelters in farmed blue foxes (Alopex lagopus). Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2002;77: 139–154.
  138. 138. Appleby MC, Smith SF, Hughes BO. Nesting, dust bathing, and perching by laying hens in cages: Effects of design on behaviour and welfare. Br Poult Sci. 1993;34: 835–847. pmid:8156422