Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Greater volumes of a callosal sub-region terminating in posterior language-related areas predict a stronger degree of language lateralization: A tractography study

Abstract

Language lateralization is the most intriguing trait of functional asymmetry for cognitive functions. Nowadays, ontogenetic determinants of this trait are largely unknown, but there are efforts to find its anatomical correlates. In particular, a white matter interhemispheric connection–the corpus callosum–has been proposed as such. In the present study, we aimed to find the association between the degree of language lateralization and metrics of the callosal sub-regions. We applied a sentence completion fMRI task to measure the degree of language lateralization in a group of healthy participants balanced for handedness. We obtained the volumes and microstructural properties of callosal sub-regions with two tractography techniques, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and constrained spherical deconvolution (CSD). The analysis of DTI-based metrics did not reveal any significant associations with language lateralization. In contrast, CSD-based analysis revealed that the volumes of a callosal sub-region terminating in the core posterior language-related areas predict a stronger degree of language lateralization. This finding supports the specific inhibitory model implemented through the callosal fibers projecting into the core posterior language-related areas in the degree of language lateralization, with no relevant contribution of other callosal sub-regions.

Introduction

Functional asymmetry for some cognitive functions is one of the most prominent features of the brain [1]. Among other examples, there is a well-documented dominance of language in the left hemisphere in the majority of human beings [2, 3]. Only about 10–15% of individuals show atypical dominance of language processing in the right hemisphere or no clear hemispheric dominance, which is more frequently represented in left-handers and ambidexters than in right-handers [46]. Although language lateralization is associated with handedness, ontogenetic determinations of these traits differ [7]. While the causation of language lateralization remains largely unknown [8], efforts have been made to find its anatomical correlates in gray and white matter structures [9, 10].

Among gray matter structures, insular asymmetry has been suggested to predict language lateralization [11, 12]. However, although the insula is highly involved in different aspects of language processing, its role is not restricted to them [13], and as a consequence, an insular specific contribution to language lateralization is not still clear. In turn, asymmetry in clearly language-related areas, namely the planum temporale and Broca’s area, does not correlate with language lateralization [11, 14]. In contrast, interhemispheric white matter structures, mainly the corpus callosum (CC), which controls the functional interplay between hemispheres has been empirically associated with language lateralization [15]. Based on the fact of the less lateralized language functions in adults with agenesis of CC [16, 17], Adibpour, Dubois, Moutar, and Dehaene-Lambertz [18] explained it by the crucial role of the callosal fibers in infants with such disease. Therefore, the CC promotes the development of language lateralization in the early stages, and as a consequence, contributes to it in later life.

Two different models explain how the CC may contribute to language lateralization [19]. The excitatory model suggests the functional activation of both hemispheres through the CC because most of its fibers rely on excitatory glutamate neurotransmitters. The inhibitory model argues for the suppression of the subdominant hemisphere during language tasks by the dominant one through the inhibitory interneurons of the CC [20]. Depending on the different functions of the cortical areas involved in language processing, both excitation and inhibition might be ultimately carried out through the callosal sub-regions. Additionally, the functional diversity of the CC contributions might be even further enhanced by microstructural heterogeneity across callosal sub-regions [21]. Given that, the critical question is whether specific callosal sub-regions, rather than the CC as a whole, separately contribute to language lateralization and whether this contribution is excitatory or inhibitory [10].

Previous attempts to investigate the association between the CC and language lateralization used structural MRI and measured the midsagittal area of the CC based on it [10]. But firstly, structural MRI does not provide insight into microstructural properties, such as myelination, axonal diameter, and density of white matter fibers. Secondly, it does not properly assess individual variability in the overall shape of a tract [22] and, as a consequence, its volume, which is the closest proxy of the number of fibers [19]. This led to mixed results in previous structural MRI findings [22]. While Labache, Mazoyer, Joliot, Crivello, Hesling, and Tzourio-Mazoyer [23] and Bartha-Doering et al. [24] showed that a greater midsagittal area of the entire CC predicted decreased language lateralization, which was interpreted in favor of the excitatory model, Josse, Seghier, Kherif, and Price [25] showed the opposite effect and thus supported the inhibitory model.

In contrast to structural MRI, tractography allows researchers to reconstruct and quantify the volumes and microstructural properties of white matter tracts in a more direct way. Early tractography studies of language lateralization only investigated the microstructural properties obtained with diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). Using the DTI metric of fractional anisotropy (FA), Putnam, Wig, Grafton, Kelley, and Gazzaniga [26] and Steinman et al. [27] showed distinct contributions of the anterior and posterior callosal sub-regions to interhemispheric inhibition and excitation, respectively. However, both findings are inconsistent with Häberling, Badzakova-Trajkov, and Corballis [28], who revealed the excitation through the anterior callosal sub-region, and Westerhausen et al. [29], who, in turn, revealed the inhibition through the posterior callosal sub-region using another DTI metric, relative anisotropy. Of note, none of these studies analyzed the volumes of the callosal sub-regions. Thus, the application of the DTI technique did not result in consistent evidence about the contribution of callosal sub-regions to interhemispheric regulation in language processing. This may be because DTI cannot reliably quantify structural properties because of its poor ability to resolve multiple crossings of fibers in the CC [30]. Thus, DTI does not fully reconstruct all callosal fibers [31] and underestimates the volumes of callosal sub-regions. Likewise, DTI represents the microstructural properties of underlying tissue only to a limited extent [32, 33]. For example, the most frequently used DTI metric, FA, is believed to be sensitive to fiber density [34, 35] and myelin content of fibers [36]. But Friedrich et al. [37] demonstrated that FA does not coincide with fiber density in the posterior callosal sub-region and myelin content of fibers in the most callosal sub-regions.

To overcome the limitations of DTI, a more advanced tractography technique, constrained spherical deconvolution (CSD), is more appropriate for modeling the crossing fibers [38], as the latter allows a more precise evaluation of the volumes of callosal sub-regions [31]. But, as with DTI, CSD has not been used to investigate the association between volumes of the callosal sub-regions and language lateralization. To fill this gap, we applied both tractography techniques in the present study and tested the extent of the DTI limitations in comparison to CSD. In addition to extracting volumes using the two approaches, we followed previous DTI studies and examined the microstructural properties of the callosal fibers across sub-regions and their association with language lateralization. For that, we used FA in DTI [37] and hindrance modulated orientational anisotropy (HMOA) in CSD, which reflects axonal diameter, fiber density and dispersion and represents the microstructural properties of fibers more precisely than FA [39]. HMOA was previously used in association with spatial attention lateralization [40] but has not yet been applied to study language lateralization.

Finally, in previous DTI studies aiming to reveal the contribution of the CC to language lateralization, the latter was measured with fMRI paradigms using word generation [26, 28, 29] or listening tasks [27]. These two tasks mainly activate the anterior or posterior language-related areas, respectively. Thus, each previous DTI study reported a result that was based on language lateralization in either the anterior or posterior language-related areas, but critically, not in both. Thus, distinctions in the associations of the structural properties of callosal sub-regions and language lateralization in the anterior or posterior language-related areas have been based on studies with different groups of participants, but not within the same group using the same task. In the present study, we measured language lateralization using a more comprehensive sentence completion task with fMRI that robustly activates the anterior and posterior language-related areas [4143] within the same individual. To ensure variability of the degree of language lateralization [1], we balanced our participants on handedness and recruited right-handed, left-handed, and ambidextrous participants.

Overall, the goal of the study was to measure both the volumes and microstructural metrics of callosal sub-regions using DTI and CSD, and to test their specific associations with the degree of language lateralization, as derived from a comprehensive fMRI task in a cohort of participants ensuring the variability of such asymmetry.

Materials and methods

Participants

Fifty neurologically healthy individuals participated in the study (32 females; mean age = 24.4, SD = 4.8, range 18–37 years). All participants were Russian native speakers with no history of psychiatric or neurological diseases. The handedness of each participant was estimated with ten questions of the Edinburgh inventory [44]. In our study, 20 participants with scores from +45 to +100 were right-handed (13 females; mean age = 24.9, SD = 5.7, range = 18–37 years), 20 participants with scores from -100 to -45 were left-handed (14 females; mean age = 23.8, SD = 4.0, range = 19–30 years), and ten participants with scores from -45 to +45 were ambidextrous (7 females; mean age = 23.2, SD = 3.1, range = 19–27 years). The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, all participants gave written informed consent, and the protocol was approved by the local ethical committee of the National Research Center “Kurchatov Institute” (Moscow, Russia).

Language task during fMRI

All participants performed a block-designed language task with alternating sentence completion and baseline blocks, lasting 21.3 s each. Each block consisted of three stimuli presented for 5 s, which were separated by an inter-stimulus interval with an exclamation mark on the screen presented for 2.1 s. In each trial of a sentence completion block, participants had to read aloud a visually presented Russian sentence and complete it with a semantically and grammatically appropriate final word (e.g., Teper’ ministr podpisyvaet vazhnoe… Now the minister in signing an important…). All sentences consisted of an adverb of time, a subject, and a predicate with an omitted direct object. One half of the sentences were expanded with an adjective before the object. The other half of the sentences were expanded with an adverb before the predicate. In each trial of the baseline block, participants had to read aloud a string of four syllables and repeat the same syllable one more time (e.g., Peeeee peeeeeeeeee peeeeeee peeeeeee…). The length of the strings of syllables, measured in letters and syllables, was matched to the length of the sentences in sentence completion blocks. The scanning session consisted of two runs, and each run included 120 trials (60 sentence completion and 60 baseline trials), lasting 14 min and 37 s. All participant responses were audio recorded.

MRI acquisition

We performed MRI data acquisition on a Siemens 3T Magnetom Vario MRI scanner. Functional T2* images were acquired using an EPI sequence with TR = 7000 ms; TE = 30 ms; flip angle = 90°. Each functional T2* image contained 40 axial slices (no gap) with FOV = 205×205 mm2, spatial resolution = 3×3×3 mm3. An additional field map for fMRI data was acquired using a dual-echo GRE sequence with TR = 400 ms; TE1 = 4.92 ms; TE2 = 7.38 ms; 30 slices (no gap); FOV = 205×205 mm2; spatial resolution = 3×3×3 mm3. Sparse sampling acquisition was used to record the participant’s overt responses in intervals that were equal to TR delay = 5 s. Structural T1 images were obtained using a 3D MP-RAGE sequence with TR = 1900 ms; TE = 2,2 ms; flip angle = 9°. Each T1-image contained 176 axial slices (no gap) with FOV = 320×320 mm2 and spatial resolution = 1×1×1 mm3. Diffusion-weighted images (DWIs) were acquired using single-shot spin echo EPI sequence with TR = 13700 ms; TE = 101ms; 65 axial slices (no gap); FOV = 240×240 mm2; spatial resolution = 2×2×2 mm3; b-value = 1500 s/mm2. Sixty-four diffusion directions and one image with b = 0 s/mm2 were collected in AP phase encoding direction, twice for 47 participants and once for 3 participants. An additional field map for the DWIs was acquired using a dual-echo GRE sequence with TR = 698 ms; TE1 = 4.92 ms; TE2 = 7.38 ms; 65 slices (no gap); FOV = 240×240 mm2; spatial resolution = 2×2×2 mm3.

DWI analysis

We pre-processed the DWI data in FMRIB Software Library (FSL) (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/FSL). No DWI data were removed after visual estimation of quality. For 47 participants, two diffusion-weighted sequences in the AP phase encoding direction were merged to improve the signal-to-noise ratio. For all participants, DWIs were corrected for the eddy-current and subject motion distortions by aligning to the images with b = 0 s/mm2, and for the EPI distortion by applying the field map.

For all participants, the DTI technique was applied in ExploreDTI software (http://www.exploredti.com); the CSD technique was applied in StarTrack software (https://www.mr-startrack.com). For DTI, the diffusion tensor was fitted at each voxel using the linear approach. For CSD, the damped Richardson-Lucy algorithm was applied with fiber response = 1.5×10−3 mm2/s−1, 400 iterations, relative threshold = 2×10−3, and geometric damping parameter = 16 [45] to obtain the fiber orientation distribution at each voxel. Both whole-brain tractographies were performed using the maximum angle = 30°, seed point resolution = 1 mm3, and step size = 1 mm. FA threshold = 0.2 was used for DTI; absolute threshold = 2×10−3 was used for CSD. In addition, FA in DTI and HMOA in CSD maps were obtained in the native space of each participant.

The CC was manually reconstructed from the whole-brain tractographies in native space of each participant in TrackVis software (http://trackvis.org). For both whole-brain tractographies of each participant, an inclusion region of interest (ROI) was drawn manually in the midsagittal slice of the CC on the FA map and divided into sub-regions according to Hofer’s scheme [46]. In contrast to other schemes, Hofer’s scheme is based on tractography results. Fig 1 presents the Hofer’s scheme of the CC. In addition, exclusion ROIs were manually placed for each participant to remove spurious fibers. The reconstructed callosal sub-regions in both DTI and CSD corresponded to CC-I, with fibers projecting into the prefrontal cortex (PFC); CC-II—premotor cortex and supplementary motor area (PM-SMA); CC-III—primary motor cortex (M1); CC-IV—primary somatosensory cortex (S1); CC-V—parietal, temporal, occipital lobes (PTOLs) in Hofer’s scheme [46]. Fig 2 presents the visualization of a single subject reconstruction of callosal sub-regions performed in Surf Ice (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/surfice/).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Hofer’s scheme of callosal sub-regions.

The image was adapted from [46].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Single subject reconstruction of callosal sub-regions.

The visualization of a single subject reconstruction of callosal sub-regions according to Hofer’s scheme in DTI (A) and CSD (B). DTI = diffusion tensor imaging; CSD = constrained spherical deconvolution.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.g002

The volumes in DTI and CSD of each sub-region were extracted in TrackVis and normalized by the total volume of gray and white matter obtained from the T1 image of each participant. The normalized volume of a sub-region is further referred to as volume for simplicity. Also, for each sub-region, values of FA and HMOA across streamlines were extracted from the corresponding maps in TrackVis.

fMRI analysis

We analyzed fMRI data with SPM12 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/) in MATLAB R2017b (MathWorks; Natick, MA, USA). The first four volumes of each run corresponding to the task instructions were discarded. For preprocessing, voxel displacement maps were estimated based on the acquired field maps. Functional images were manually aligned along the anterior commissure-posterior commissure line. Using the voxel displacement maps, functional images were simultaneously unwarped to correct for magnetic field inhomogeneity and re-aligned to correct for head motion. The structural T1 image was co-registered to the mean functional volume. The structural T1 and functional T2* images were normalized into standardized stereotactic space (MNI 152-subject template) based on segmentation into gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid. The functional T2* images were spatially smoothed with an 8-mm FWHM isotropic Gaussian kernel.

After preprocessing, we conducted the first-level statistical analysis to obtain an individual map of language-related activation for each participant. The two conditions (sentence completion and baseline blocks) were modeled in an event-related design where each stimulus corresponded to an event of 7.1 s duration. The model also included regressors: six motion parameters obtained in realignment, as well as binary response accuracy scored independently by two raters. A canonical hemodynamic response function with no derivatives was used to model BOLD response. A high pass filter of 256 s was used to eliminate low-frequency scanner drift and a first-order autoregressive model was used to correct for autocorrelations. A group map of language activation was obtained by contrasting the sentence completion to the baseline condition with a one-sample t-test in Python, version 3.7 (https://www.python.org) using the “nilearn” package (https://nilearn.github.io). Results were considered significant at p < 0.05 with family-wise error rate correction for multiple comparisons. Fig 3 shows the group map of language activation.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Group map of language activation.

Colors indicate statistically significant language activation (family-wise error rate correction, p < 0.05): sentence completion block > baseline block.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.g003

The language lateralization index (LI) was calculated for multiple regions for each participant using the fMRI activation maps obtained in the first-level statistical analysis in the LI-toolbox [47] in SPM12 according to the formula: whereby AL and AR are language-related activation of a region in the left and right hemispheres, respectively. For each participant, the fMRI activation map was divided into cortical areas–PFC, PM-SMA, M1, S1, and PTOLs–obtained in the Brainnetome atlas [48] according to the callosal sub-regions [46]. Table 1 presents the correspondence between the callosal sub-regions and selected ROIs. The PM-SMA included the core language-related areas, such as the pars triangularis, pars opercularis [49], and supplementary motor area [50], whereas the PTOLs included the angular and supramarginal gyri, and posterior temporal cortex, which are also frequently associated with language processing [49]. In contrast, the PFC, M1, and S1 did not include core language-related areas, instead being associated with executive functions, general motor, and somatosensory processing, respectively [5153]. For each of these ROIs, LI calculations were based on voxel count and voxel values, using adaptive thresholding as implemented in the LI-toolbox and excluding 5 mm along the brain midline. The LI could range from -1 for strong right-hemispheric lateralization to +1 for strong left-hemispheric lateralization. Following [54], our study used absolute values of the resulting LI, so that stronger lateralization, regardless of the hemisphere, corresponded to values closer to +1, whereas weaker lateralization corresponded to values closer to 0. The absolute values indicating the degree of lateralization are further referred to as LIabs for simplicity. Regarding the hemispheric dominance, we, additionally, considered raw values of resulting LI, which are further referred to as LIraw for simplicity.

thumbnail
Table 1. Correspondence between the callosal sub-regions and cortical areas.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.t001

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were performed in JASP (https://jasp-stats.org) and RStudio, version 4.2.0 (https://www.rstudio.com) using package “BayesFactor” (https://github.com/richarddmorey/BayesFactor). Following recommendations [55], we reported the results of each analysis in both frequentist statistics and Bayesian statistics via Bayes factors (BF10). BF10 is a change from an odds ratio between prior probabilities of the alternative and null hypotheses to an odds ratio of their posterior probabilities driven by the observed data. While the frequentist statistics only either reject or not the null hypothesis, BF10 allows us to quantify evidence in favor of either the null hypothesis (BF10 < 1/3) or the alternative hypothesis (BF10 > 3) or, as an additional state, indicate no clear evidence in favor of either hypothesis (1/3 < BF10 < 3) [55, 56]. We calculated BF10 using a default prior probability distribution, a Cauchy distribution with a center location, x0  =  0; scale value, σ  =  √2/4 [57]. For analyses with multiple testing, we adjusted this distribution by decreasing a scale value proportionally to the number of tests [58, 59]. Thus, an effect of either difference between groups or predictors of multiple regressions became closer to 0. Details of the adjustment procedure are presented in [60].

To test whether CSD provided greater volume than DTI, the difference in the volumes of each callosal sub-region between DTI and CSD was assessed with a paired samples t-test. Due to multiple testing in this analysis, we adjusted the level of significance and scale value for five tests (Bonferroni correction, α = .05/5 = .01; σ  = 0.072). To compare the microstructural heterogeneity of the callosal fibers across all callosal sub-regions, one-way ANOVA tests were used separately for FA in DTI, and for HMOA in CSD (Bonferroni correction, α = .05/2 = .025; σ = 0.18). To assess the association of the structural properties of callosal sub-regions with LIabs, we used general multiple regression. For each pair of the callosal sub-region and respective LIabs in that region, two general multiple regressions using backward elimination were built with either the volume and FA in DTI, or the volume and HMOA in CSD, as independent variables. To obtain BF10 for the general multiple regressions, we additionally built Bayesian multiple regressions. BF10 were shown for the three possible models: a model with both independent variables, and two models with one of the two independent variables, compared to the null model without any independent variables [61]. For each of these three models and a null model, a prior probability of 0.25 was defined. Due to multiple testing of LIabs in this analysis across all callosal sub-regions in DTI and CSD, we adjusted the level of significance for (Bonferroni correction, α = .05/10 = .005) and scale value (σ = 0.036) for ten general and Bayesian multiple regressions.

Supplementary analyses

As a supplementary analysis, we explored the associations between the structural properties of the callosal sub-regions and LIraw, thus regarding the hemisphere. Due to multiple testing of LIraw in this analysis across all callosal sub-regions in DTI and CSD, we adjusted the level of significance (Bonferroni correction, α = .05/10 = .005) and scale value (σ = 0.036) for ten general and Bayesian multiple regressions.

Finally, to test whether obtained associations of the structural properties of callosal sub-regions with LIabs and LIraw depend on handedness, we conducted an analysis dividing all participants according to their handedness. We distinguished two groups, which corresponded to typical handedness (TH) and atypical handedness (AH) and consisted of right-handers and left-handers with ambidexters, respectively. For each pair of the callosal sub-region and respective either LIabs or LIraw, we built general multiple regressions with the structural properties in both DTI and CSD, as independent variables, additionally nested by the groups of handedness. BF10 were shown for these models based on comparing to a null model. Due to multiple testing of LIabs and LIraw in this analysis, we adjusted the level of significance (Bonferroni correction, α = .05/20 = .0025) and a scale value (σ = 0.018) for 20 general and Bayesian multiple regressions.

Results

Functional lateralization

Table 2 presents the LIraw and LIabs of the cortical areas corresponding to the callosal sub-regions. Detailed descriptive statistics of the LIraw and LIabs according to handedness of the participants are presented in the (S1 Table).

thumbnail
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the LIraw and LIabs in the selected ROIs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.t002

Left-hemispheric language lateralization was found across 40 in the PFC (12 left-, 19 right-handers, and 9 ambidexters), 39 in the PM-SMA (11 left-, 19 right-handers, and 9 ambidexters), 35 in the M1 (12 left-, 14 right-handers, and 9 ambidexters), 25 in the S1 (10 left-, 10 right-handers, and 5 ambidexters), and 39 in the PTOLs (12 left-, 18 right-handers and 9 ambidexters) from 50 participants.

Callosal metrics

For all callosal sub-regions, volumes were significantly greater in CSD than in DTI, with evidence in favor of the significant differences (BF10 > 105; Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Results of a paired samples t-test comparing the volumes of each callosal sub-region between DTI and CSD.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.t003

The one-way ANOVA tests revealed significant differences across callosal sub-regions in FA in DTI (F(4,245) = 94,38, p < 0.001) and in HMOA in CSD (F(4,245) = 86,41, p < 0.001). BF10 > 104 for both tests indicated evidence for these differences. Post hoc two-sample t-tests (Bonferroni correction, α = .05/10 = .005) showed that all callosal sub-regions significantly differed in FA, except for the comparisons between CC-II and CC-IV (t(49) = 2.12, p = 0.04) and between CC-III and CC-V (t(49) = -2.43, p = 0.02). BF10 = 1.40 and BF10 = 1.75 (σ = 0.036) for the comparisons between CC-II and CC-IV, and between CC-III and CC-V, respectively, showed no clear evidence in favor of difference. Post hoc two-sample t-tests (Bonferroni correction, α = .05/10 = .005) also showed that all callosal sub-regions significantly differed in HMOA, except for the comparison between CC-III and CC-IV (t(49) = -1.84, p = .071). BF10 = 0.74 (σ = 0.036) for this comparison showed no clear evidence in favor of no difference. Fig 4 presents FA and HMOA across all callosal sub-regions. Detailed statistics of the callosal metrics according to handedness of the participants are presented in the (S2 Table).

thumbnail
Fig 4. FA and HMOA across all callosal sub-regions.

The sub-regions significantly differed in FA (A) except for the comparisons between CC-II and CC-IV, as well as between CC-III and CC-V. The sub-regions significantly differed in HMOA (B) except for the comparisons between CC-III and CC-IV. CC = corpus callosum; FA = fractional anisotropy; HMOA = hindrance modulated orientational anisotropy; ns = non-significant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.g004

Relations of the LIabs to the structural properties of callosal sub-regions

Table 4 presents the results of the general linear regressions of LIabs. The LIabs in the cortical areas corresponding to the callosal sub-regions were not significantly related to DTI-based volumes nor to FA in these sub-regions. There was an association between stronger LIabs in the PTOLs and greater FA of CC-V (β = 3.65, SE = 1.67, t(48) = 2.18, p = 0.03), which did not reach Bonferroni corrected significance level (α = .005). Bayesian multiple regression showed no clear evidence for this relation (BF10 = 1.46).

thumbnail
Table 4. Results of the general multiple regressions in DTI and CSD examining the relations of LIabs to volumes and FA in DTI, and to volumes and HMOA in CSD.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.t004

There was an association of stronger LIabs in the PM-SMA and greater CSD-based volume of CC-II (β = 7.40, SE = 2.81, t(48) = 2.64, p = 0.01), which did not reach α = .005. Bayesian multiple regression showed no clear evidence for this relation (BF10 = 2.12). We also found a significant association between stronger LIabs in the PTOLs and greater CSD-based volume of CC-V (β = 4.45, SE = 1.41, t(48) = 3.15, p = 0.003). Bayesian multiple regression showed evidence for this relation (BF10 = 4.00). Relations of the other pairs of LIabs in the cortical areas to either CSD-based volumes or HMOA of any sub-regions were not significant. Detailed summaries of the results of Bayesian multiple regressions are presented in the (S3 Table).

Supplementary analyses

Relations of the LIraw to the structural properties of callosal sub-regions.

The supplementary analysis indicated a significant association between stronger LIraw in the PFC and greater CSD-based volume of CC-I (β = 19.65, SE = 6.33, t(48) = 3.10, p = 0.003). However, Bayesian multiple regression showed no clear evidence for this relation (BF10 = 2.97). There were associations of stronger LIraw in the PM-SMA and greater CSD-based volume of CC-II (β = 13.64, SE = 5.83, t(47) = 2.31, p = 0.03), and stronger LIraw in the PTOLs and greater CSD-based volume of CC-V (β = 5.35, SE = 2.16, t(47) = 2.56, p = 0.01), which did not reach α = .005. Bayesian multiple regression showed no clear evidence for these relations BF10 = 1.85 and BF10 = 1.91, respectively. Detailed summaries of the results of the general and Bayesian multiple regressions are presented in the (S4 and S5 Tables).

Analysis of the relations within handedness groups.

LIabs. We found a significant association for AH between stronger LIabs in PM-SMA and CSD-based volume of CC-II (β = 12.24, SE = 3.55, t(47) = 3.44, p = 0.001). But Bayesian multiple regression showed no clear evidence for this relation for AH (BF10 = 1.62). Also, for AH, there was an association between stronger LIabs in the PTOLs and CSD-based volume of CC-V (β = 5.23, SE = 2.0, t(47) = 2.6, p = 0.01), which did not reach Bonferroni corrected significance level (α = .0025). Bayesian multiple regression showed no clear evidence for this relation for AH (BF10 = 1.13).

LIraw. For AH, we found significant associations between stronger LIraw in the PFC and greater CSD-based volume (β = 36.45, SE = 8.43, t(47) = 4.32, p < 0.001) and HMOA (β = 57.16, SE = 17.58, t(47) = 3.25, p = 0.002) of CC-I. Bayesian multiple regression showed clear evidence for this relation (BF10 = 4.11). Also, for AH, there were associations between stronger LIraw in the PM-SMA and greater CSD-based volume (β = 18.51, SE = 7.07, t(47) = 2.62, p = 0.01) and HMOA volume (β = 88.04, SE = 30.2, t(47) = 2.91, p = 0.006) of CC-II. Both relations did not reach α = .0025 with no clear evidence (BF10 = 1.73). Finally, we found an association between stronger LIraw in the PTOLs and greater CSD-based volume of CC-V (β = 6.57, SE = 2.98, t(47) = 2.2, p = 0.03), which did not reach α = .0025, with no clear evidence (BF10 = 1.03). Detailed summaries of the results of general and Bayesian multiple regressions nested by the groups of handedness are presented in the (S6S8 Tables).

Discussion

The current study aimed at verifying the link between the structural properties of each callosal sub-region and the degree of language lateralization in the corresponding cortical area. Hofer’s scheme was applied to segment the CC into five functionally specialized sub-regions, whose microstructural properties are also distinct [46]. In addition to previously reported DTI-based microstructural properties, the volumes of the callosal sub-regions, for the first time, were explored in relation to the degree of language lateralization. Also, to avoid the limitations of DTI, we additionally applied the advanced CSD technique for CC modelling and directly compared the results of the two approaches. Language lateralization was measured using the sentence completion task, which activates both anterior and posterior language-related areas within the same paradigm, unlike in previous studies [4143]. Following [54] we focused on the degree of language lateralization regardless of the hemisphere. However, we also conducted supplementary analyses given the hemisphere of language lateralization and handedness.

Structurally, for each callosal sub-region, we revealed a significantly greater volume in CSD than in DTI. This is in line with Steventon et al. [31] and emphasizes that CSD can provide fuller reconstruction of the crossing callosal fibers, which is particularly relevant for lateral fibers projecting to language-related areas. To study the microstructural properties of callosal sub-regions, we used FA in DTI and HMOA in CSD. It is suggested that HMOA mirrors changes in the axonal diameter, fiber density and dispersion across the distinct callosal sub-regions more reliably than FA [39]. Both FA and HMOA were significantly different across the callosal sub-regions, except for the comparisons in FA between CC-II and CC-IV, between CC-III and CC-V; and in HMOA between CC-III and CC-IV. Thus, using both metrics we confirmed the microstructural heterogeneity of fibers across callosal sub-regions. We found the highest FA in CC-V, which is in line with [28, 46, 62]. But, in contrast to those studies using FA within a voxel, we extracted FA in streamlines of each callosal sub-region. That led to the discrepancy in the lowest FA that was shown in CC-I in our study but was previously reported in CC-III and CC-IV [28, 46, 62]. Similarly to FA, the highest value of HMOA was in CC-V and the lowest value of HMOA was also observed in CC-I. However, the lowest values of both FA and HMOA in CC-I are consistent with Friedrich et al. [37], who showed myelin content and axonal density based on neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging (NODDI) decreasing in the frontal segments of the CC.

The variability of the microstructural properties and potential functional specialization of the callosal sub-regions led us to consider the degree of language lateralization separately in each of the cortical areas corresponding to the terminations of the callosal sub-regions. The five resulting areas were not restricted to the core language-related regions [49], but a significant effect was found only for the area that included the posterior language-related regions. This obtained positive relation in the CSD analysis points to their inhibition of the activity in the homologous areas in the subdominant hemisphere through the callosal fibers of CC-V, which is consistent with the results for the callosal fibers projecting into the temporal regions in Josse et al. [25]. However, this study also reported that the anterior language-related areas inhibit activity of the homologous areas through the corresponding callosal fibers, whereas, in our study, this effect was not significant after multiple testing corrections. Based on Bayesian statistics of no clear evidence in favor of either a significant or null effect, we did not reject a relation between greater CSD-based volumes of CC-II and a stronger degree of language lateralization in the PM-SMA and, thus, pointed to a need to verify this relation in further studies.

The link between the volume of CC-V and the degree of language lateralization might be explained by the role of the posterior callosal sub-region in language comprehension [63]. Patients with complete or partial agenesis of CC performed a language comprehension task with lower scores than healthy participants. Moreover, for these patients, weaker language lateralization and interhemispheric connectivity were more common [64]. Although both these findings were considered within the excitation through the CC, we assume that the inhibitory model might also explain it by associating weaker language lateralization with reduced interhemispheric connectivity. Hinkley et al. [16] and Ocklenburg, Ball, Wolf, Genç, and Güntürkün [17], using auditory and dichotic listening tasks, respectively, also found a weaker degree of language lateralization in such patients, thus, confirming our result. By contrast, in healthy participants, Westerhausen et al. [22] and Bartha-Doering et al. [24] confirmed excitation of both hemispheres through the auditory callosal projections and the posterior callosal sub-region, respectively. However, those studies were limited by using the midsagittal size to represent the CC volume. Notably, the midsagittal size was used as a proxy for the volume of callosal fibers in most previous studies, whereas the present study was the first to apply tractography techniques. The fact that this effect was only found in the CSD analysis again emphasizes the advantage of CSD over DTI to feed more anatomically adequate white matter reconstructions that correspond to functional brain metrics. To our knowledge, there is only one CSD study that supports the opposite–an excitation through of the CC for the degree of language lateralization [54]. However, we suggest that our study has methodological advantages and thus provides more reliable results. First, in [54] the authors calculated the degree of functional asymmetry based on several cognitive functions, which is not specific [65]. Furthermore, their measurement of the whole body of the CC eliminated potential specific associations between each callosal sub-region and the degree of language lateralization in the corresponding cortical areas [16]. Following the idea in [10], we separated the CC into sub-regions and showed a specific association of the volume and degree of language lateralization in the area that contained the posterior language-related regions (angular and supramarginal gyri, posterior temporal cortex). Respectively, we found no link between the volume of the CC and degree of language lateralization in the PFC, in M1, nor in S1, which are not specialized for language processing but are rather involved in executive control, primary motor, and somatosensory processing, respectively [5153].

Previous studies on language reorganization revealed that the relation of the homologous areas through the callosal subregions is not limited to inhibition. Regarding the excitatory model, greater callosal connectivity based on FA was associated with decreased language lateralization in patients with brain tumors [66] and arteriovenous malformations [67]. Despite this conflict in the results, all together seem to be explained by the switching of inhibition or excitation according to the state of the brain. Because the dominant hemisphere affected by pathology tends to recruit other regions in patients, it is believed that excitation through the callosal fibers is more efficient in performing language tasks than inhibition, which is the opposite implemented under a healthy state [68]. But a shift to the sub-dominant hemisphere as language reorganization was shown to be ineffective for better recovery in post-stroke patients and appeared to be more frequent for early strokes [69]. As a result, we can assume that the inhibitory model is regular for language tasks under a healthy or close-to-healthy state, which is in line with the results for the callosal fibers of the CC-V in our study.

Additionally, according to our results, there was no significant relation between the degree of language lateralization and either FA (in DTI) or HMOA (in CSD) of the callosal sub-regions. We found a positive association between the degree of language lateralization in the PTOLs and FA in CC-V, which did not survive a multiple testing correction. Thus, we failed to replicate the findings of Häberling et al. [28] and Steinmann et al. [27], who showed a negative link between FA in the CC and whole-brain lateralization for language and, in more detail, between FA of the posterior callosal sub-region and language lateralization in the secondary auditory cortex. One may consider that DTI-based FA indirectly represents the white-matter microstructural properties [37], but the analysis of the CSD-based HMOA of the callosal sub-regions was not associated with the degree of language lateralization either. This is seemingly in contrast to the results of Chechlacz et al. [40], who used HMOA to show the inhibition through the posterior parietal callosal fibers in enabling lateralization of spatial attention. However, this discrepancy may be due to different contributions of the callosal fibers in two different tasks, namely language and spatial attention [19, 20]. Thus, overall, the microstructural properties of the callosal fibers did not yield any effect in relation to the degree of language lateralization in our study, irrespective of the tractography approach.

While we specifically explored the absolute values of the degree of language lateralization regardless of the hemisphere [54], we additionally verified it regarding the hemispheric dominance, that is, considered both negative and positive values. In such a case, values closer to -1 represent weaker language lateralization compared to values of 0. It means that both models implemented through the CC, excitation and inhibition, occur as the functional activation and suppression of the right hemisphere by the left, dominant hemisphere, but never vice versa. Based on this view, surprisingly, we observed a link of greater CSD-based volumes and HMOA of CC-I to a stronger language lateralization in the PFC for left-handed with ambidextrous participants. However, there were no such associations for the callosal sub-regions terminating in the areas with the core language-related areas for any groups of the participants. Therefore, inhibition through CC-V was shown when we ignored the hemisphere, considering the absolute values of the degree only. Furthermore, our main result was significant for all participants and not replicated within the groups divided by handedness, separately. Given that we did not initially design our study to consider the groups of handedness, further studies should address it using larger samples.

The study had several limitations. Firstly, the results of Hofer’s scheme suggest clear boundaries between the callosal sub-regions. But such clear boundaries are not anatomically likely, and non-homologous projections of the CC are reported [70]. However, at least to a certain degree, our approach seems to be an appropriate approximation. Another limitation concerns the inclusion of the occipital lobe into PTOLs along with the core language-related areas–the angular and supramarginal gyri, and the posterior temporal cortex. Thus, the degree of language lateralization and CC metrics in the PTOLs reflect both high- and low-order language-related processes, despite the possibility that the link between the structural properties of the callosal sub-region and the degree of language lateralization in the high- and low-order aspects might be different. But the inclusion of the occipital lobe was motivated by the highly overlapping temporal, parietal, and occipital callosal fibers of CC-V [46], which would make it difficult to separate the occipital callosal fibers from the other fibers of CC-V. Overall, all these limitations should be addressed in further studies.

Conclusion

In conclusion, this is the first tractography study that investigated the relation between the volumes and microstructural properties of callosal sub-regions and the degree of language lateralization, using both DTI and CSD. We showed no effect of the microstructural properties of callosal fibers on the degree of language lateralization, irrespective of the tractography method. In line with the inhibitory model, greater volumes in CSD, although not in DTI, predicted a stronger degree of language lateralization in the area containing posterior language-related regions–posterior parietal/temporal/occipital lobes. Thus, the influence of callosal fibers on the degree of language lateralization is not equipotential, but rather anatomically specific. Also, CSD was confirmed to be a more appropriate tractography approach, when lateral crossing projections are in focus, as is the case in language studies.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Descriptive statistics of the LIraw and LIabs in the cortical areas according to handedness of the participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Descriptive statistics of the callosal metrics according to handedness of the participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Results of Bayesian multiple regressions in DTI and CSD examining the relations of LIabs to volumes and FA in DTI, and to volumes and HMOA in CSD.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.s003

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Results of the general multiple regressions in DTI and CSD examining the relations of LIraw to volumes and FA in DTI, and to volumes and HMOA in CSD.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.s004

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Results of Bayesian multiple regressions in DTI and CSD examining the relations of LIraw to volumes and FA in DTI, and to volumes and HMOA in CSD.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.s005

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Results of the general multiple regressions in DTI and CSD examining the relations of LIabs to volumes and FA in DTI, and to volumes and HMOA in CSD within handedness groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.s006

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Results of the general multiple regressions in DTI and CSD examining the relations of LIraw to volumes and FA in DTI, and to volumes and HMOA in CSD within handedness groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.s007

(DOCX)

S8 Table. Results of the Bayesian multiple regressions in DTI and CSD examining the relations of LIabs and LIraw to volumes and FA in DTI, and to volumes and HMOA in CSD within handedness groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276721.s008

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Sergey Kartashov for MRI technical assistance; Rosa Vlasova and Maria Ivanova for their help at initial stages of the project; Irina Sekerina for her valuable text revisions; and Angela Dugarova for preparation of the figures. We are grateful to the participants of the present and following related studies, who visited the lab multiple times.

References

  1. 1. Corballis MC. The evolution and genetics of cerebral asymmetry. Philos Trans R So Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364(1519):867–79. pmid:19064358
  2. 2. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Seghier ML. The neural bases of hemispheric specialization. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93(Pt B):319–324. pmid:27756694
  3. 3. Josse G, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Hemispheric specialization for language. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2004;44(1):1–12. pmid:14739000
  4. 4. Knecht S, Deppe M, Dräger B, Bobe L, Lohmann H, Ringelstein E, et.al. Language lateralization in healthy right-handers. Brain. 2000;123 (Pt 1):74–81. pmid:10611122
  5. 5. Somers M, Aukes MF, Ophoff RA, Boks MP, Fleer W, de Visser KC, et.al. On the relationship between degree of hand-preference and degree of language lateralization. Brain Lang. 2015;144:10–5. pmid:25880901
  6. 6. Packheiser J, Schmitz J, Arning L, Beste C, Güntürkün O, Ocklenburg S. A large-scale estimate on the relationship between language and motor lateralization. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):13027. pmid:32747661
  7. 7. Schmitz J, Lor S, Klose R, Güntürkün O, Ocklenburg S. The Functional Genetics of Handedness and Language Lateralization: Insights from Gene Ontology, Pathway and Disease Association Analyses. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1144. pmid:28729848
  8. 8. Güntürkün O, Ocklenburg S. Ontogenesis of Lateralization. Neuron. 2017;94(2):249–263. pmid:28426959
  9. 9. Vingerhoets G. Phenotypes in hemispheric functional segregation? Perspectives and challenges. Phys Life Rev. 2019;30:1–18. pmid:31230893
  10. 10. Ocklenburg S, Friedrich P, Güntürkün O, Genç E. Intrahemispheric white matter asymmetries: the missing link between brain structure and functional lateralization? Rev Neurosci. 2016;27(5):465–80. pmid:26812865
  11. 11. Greve DN, Van der Haegen L, Cai Q, Stufflebeam S, Sabuncu MR, Fischl B, et.al. A surface-based analysis of language lateralization and cortical asymmetry. J Cogn Neurosci. 2013;25(9):1477–92. pmid:23701459
  12. 12. Keller SS, Roberts N, García-Fiñana M, Mohammadi S, Ringelstein EB, Knecht S, et.al. Can the language-dominant hemisphere be predicted by brain anatomy? J Cogn Neurosci. 201;23(8):2013–29. pmid:20807056
  13. 13. Nieuwenhuys R. The insular cortex: a review. Prog Brain Res. 2012;195:123–63. pmid:22230626
  14. 14. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Crivello F, Mazoyer B. Is the planum temporale surface area a marker of hemispheric or regional language lateralization? Brain Struct Funct. 2018;223(3):1217–1228. pmid:29101522
  15. 15. Gazzaniga MS. Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain. 2000;123 (Pt 7):1293–326. pmid:10869045
  16. 16. Hinkley LB, Marco EJ, Brown EG, Bukshpun P, Gold J, Hill S, et.al. The Contribution of the Corpus Callosum to Language Lateralization. J Neurosci. 2016;36(16):4522–33. pmid:27098695
  17. 17. Ocklenburg S, Ball A, Wolf CC, Genç E, Güntürkün O. Functional cerebral lateralization and interhemispheric interaction in patients with callosal agenesis. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(5):806–15. pmid:25798664
  18. 18. Adibpour P, Dubois J, Moutard ML, Dehaene-Lambertz G. Early asymmetric inter-hemispheric transfer in the auditory network: insights from infants with corpus callosum agenesis. Brain Struct Funct. 2018;223(6):2893–905. pmid:29687282
  19. 19. Bloom JS, Hynd GW. The role of the corpus callosum in interhemispheric transfer of information: excitation or inhibition? Neuropsychol Rev. 2005;15(2):59–71. pmid:16211466
  20. 20. van der Knaap LJ, van der Ham IJ. How does the corpus callosum mediate interhemispheric transfer? A review. Behav Brain Res. 201;223(1):211–21. pmid:21530590
  21. 21. Aboitiz F, Scheibel AB, Fisher RS, Zaidel E. Fiber composition of the human corpus callosum. Brain Res. 1992;598(1–2):143–53. pmid:1486477
  22. 22. Westerhausen R, Grüner R, Specht K, Hugdahl K. Functional relevance of interindividual differences in temporal lobe callosal pathways: a DTI tractography study. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(6):1322–9. pmid:18842665
  23. 23. Labache L, Mazoyer B, Joliot M, Crivello F, Hesling I, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Typical and atypical language brain organization based on intrinsic connectivity and multitask functional asymmetries. Elife. 2020;9:e58722. pmid:33064079
  24. 24. Bartha-Doering L, Kollndorfer K, Schwartz E, Fischmeister FPS, Alexopoulos J, Langs G, et al. The role of the corpus callosum in language network connectivity in children. Dev Sci. 2021;24(2):e13031. pmid:32790079
  25. 25. Josse G, Seghier ML, Kherif F, Price CJ. Explaining function with anatomy: language lateralization and corpus callosum size. J Neurosci. 2008;28(52):14132–9. pmid:19109495
  26. 26. Putnam MC, Wig GS, Grafton ST, Kelley WM, Gazzaniga MS. Structural organization of the corpus callosum predicts the extent and impact of cortical activity in the nondominant hemisphere. J Neurosci. 2008;28(11):2912–8. pmid:18337422
  27. 27. Steinmann S, Amselberg R, Cheng B, Thomalla G, Engel AK, Leicht G, et.al. The role of functional and structural interhemispheric auditory connectivity for language lateralization—A combined EEG and DTI study. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):15428. pmid:30337548
  28. 28. Häberling IS, Badzakova-Trajkov G, Corballis MC. Callosal tracts and patterns of hemispheric dominance: a combined fMRI and DTI study. Neuroimage. 2011;54(2):779–86. pmid:20920586
  29. 29. Westerhausen R, Kreuder F, Dos Santos Sequeira S, Walter C, Woerner W, Wittling RA, et.al. The association of macro- and microstructure of the corpus callosum and language lateralisation. Brain Lang. 2006;97(1):80–90. pmid:16157367
  30. 30. Tournier JD, Mori S, Leemans A. Diffusion tensor imaging and beyond. Magn Reson Med. 2011;65(6):1532–56. pmid:21469191
  31. 31. Steventon JJ, Trueman RC, Rosser AE, Jones DK. Robust MR-based approaches to quantifying white matter structure and structure/function alterations in Huntington’s disease. J Neurosci Methods. 2016;265:2–12. pmid:26335798
  32. 32. Wheeler-Kingshott CA, Cercignani M. About "axial" and "radial" diffusivities. Magn Reson Med. 2009;61(5):1255–60. pmid:19253405
  33. 33. Vos SB, Jones DK, Jeurissen B, Viergever MA, Leemans A. The influence of complex white matter architecture on the mean diffusivity in diffusion tensor MRI of the human brain. Neuroimage. 2012;59(3):2208–16. pmid:22005591
  34. 34. Roberts TP, Liu F, Kassner A, Mori S, Guha A. Fiber density index correlates with reduced fractional anisotropy in white matter of patients with glioblastoma. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2005;26(9):2183–6. pmid:16219820
  35. 35. Grazioplene RG, Bearden CE, Subotnik KL, Ventura J, Haut K, Nuechterlein KH, et al. Connectivity-enhanced diffusion analysis reveals white matter density disruptions in first episode and chronic schizophrenia. Neuroimage Clin. 2018;18:608–16. pmid:29845009
  36. 36. Chang EH, Argyelan M, Aggarwal M, Chandon TS, Karlsgodt KH, Mori S, et al. The role of myelination in measures of white matter integrity: Combination of diffusion tensor imaging and two-photon microscopy of CLARITY intact brains. Neuroimage. 2017;147:253–61. pmid:27986605
  37. 37. Friedrich P, Fraenz C, Schlüter C, Ocklenburg S, Mädler B, Güntürkün O, et.al. The Relationship Between Axon Density, Myelination, and Fractional Anisotropy in the Human Corpus Callosum. Cereb Cortex. 2020;30(4):2042–2056. pmid:32037442
  38. 38. Dell’Acqua F, Tournier JD. Modelling white matter with spherical deconvolution: How and why? NMR Biomed. 2019;32(4):e3945. pmid:30113753
  39. 39. Dell’Acqua F, Simmons A, Williams SC, Catani M. Can spherical deconvolution provide more information than fiber orientations? Hindrance modulated orientational anisotropy, a true-tract specific index to characterize white matter diffusion. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34(10):2464–83. pmid:22488973
  40. 40. Chechlacz M, Humphreys GW, Sotiropoulos SN, Kennard C, Cazzoli D. Structural Organization of the Corpus Callosum Predicts Attentional Shifts after Continuous Theta Burst Stimulation. J Neurosci. 2015;35(46):15353–68. pmid:26586822
  41. 41. Salek KE, Hassan IS, Kotrotsou A, Abrol S, Faro SH, Mohamed FB, et al. Silent Sentence Completion Shows Superiority Localizing Wernicke’s Area and Activation Patterns of Distinct Language Paradigms Correlate with Genomics: Prospective Study. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1): 12054. pmid:28935966
  42. 42. Wilson SM, Bautista A, Yen M, Lauderdale S, Eriksson DK. Validity and reliability of four language mapping paradigms. Neuroimage Clin. 2016;16:399–408. pmid:28879081
  43. 43. Elin K, Malyutina S, Bronov O, Stupina E, Marinets A, Zhuravleva A, et al. A New Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Localizer for Preoperative Language Mapping Using a Sentence Completion Task: Validity, Choice of Baseline Condition, and Test-Retest Reliability. Front Hum Neurosci. 2022;16:791577. pmid:35431846
  44. 44. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113. pmid:5146491
  45. 45. Dell’acqua F, Scifo P, Rizzo G, Catani M, Simmons A, Scotti G, et.al. A modified damped Richardson-Lucy algorithm to reduce isotropic background effects in spherical deconvolution. Neuroimage. 2010;49(2):1446–58. pmid:19781650
  46. 46. Hofer S, Frahm J. Topography of the human corpus callosum revisited—comprehensive fiber tractography using diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage. 2006;32(3):989–94. pmid:16854598
  47. 47. Wilke M, Lidzba K. LI-tool: a new toolbox to assess lateralization in functional MR-data. J Neurosci Methods. 2007;163(1):128–36. pmid:17386945
  48. 48. Fan L, Li H, Zhuo J, Zhang Y, Wang J, Chen L, et.al. The Human Brainnetome Atlas: A New Brain Atlas Based on Connectional Architecture. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26(8):3508–26. pmid:27230218
  49. 49. Friederici AD. The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(5):262–8. pmid:22516238
  50. 50. Dragoy O, Zyryanov A, Bronov O, Gordeyeva E, Gronskaya N, Kryuchkova O, et al. Functional linguistic specificity of the left frontal aslant tract for spontaneous speech fluency: Evidence from intraoperative language mapping. Brain Lang. 2020;208:104836. pmid:32673898
  51. 51. Yuan P, Raz N. Prefrontal cortex and executive functions in healthy adults: a meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;42:180–92. pmid:24568942
  52. 52. Lemon R. Recent advances in our understanding of the primate corticospinal system. F1000Res. 2019;8:F1000 Faculty Rev-274. pmid:30906528
  53. 53. Hertrich I, Dietrich S, Ackermann H. The Margins of the Language Network in the Brain. Front Commun. 2020;5:93.
  54. 54. Karolis VR, Corbetta M, Thiebaut de Schotten M. The architecture of functional lateralisation and its relationship to callosal connectivity in the human brain. Nat Commun. 2019;10(1):1417. pmid:30926845
  55. 55. Keysers C, Gazzola V, Wagenmakers EJ. Using Bayes factor hypothesis testing in neuroscience to establish evidence of absence. Nat Neurosci. 2020 Jul;23(7):788–799. pmid:32601411
  56. 56. Dienes Z, Mclatchie N. Four reasons to prefer Bayesian analyses over significance testing. Psychon Bull Rev. 2018;25(1):207–218. pmid:28353065
  57. 57. Gronau QF, Van Erp S, Heck DW, Cesario J, Jonas KJ, Wagenmakers EJ. A Bayesian model-averaged meta-analysis of the power pose effect with informed and default priors: the case of felt power. Compr. Results Soc. Psychol. 2017;2(1):123–38.
  58. 58. Gelman A. Prior distributions for variance parameters in hierarchical models. Bayesian Anal. 2006;1(3):515–34.
  59. 59. de Jong T. A Bayesian Approach to the Correction for Multiplicity. PsyArXiv. 2019.
  60. 60. Han H. Implementation of Bayesian multiple comparison correction in the second-level analysis of fMRI data: With pilot analyses of simulation and real fMRI datasets based on voxelwise inference. Cogn Neurosci. 2020;11(3):157–169. pmid:31855500
  61. 61. Bergh DVD, Clyde MA, Gupta ARKN, de Jong T, Gronau QF, et.al. A tutorial on Bayesian multi-model linear regression with BAS and JASP. Behav Res Methods. 2021. pmid:33835394
  62. 62. Rajan S, Brettschneider J, Collingwood JF; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Regional segmentation strategy for DTI analysis of human corpus callosum indicates motor function deficit in mild cognitive impairment. J Neurosci Methods. 2020;345:108870. pmid:32687851
  63. 63. Friederici AD, von Cramon DY, Kotz SA. Role of the corpus callosum in speech comprehension: interfacing syntax and prosody. Neuron. 2007;53(1):135–45. pmid:17196536
  64. 64. Bartha-Doering L, Schwartz E, Kollndorfer K, Fischmeister FPS, Novak A, Langs G, et al. Effect of corpus callosum agenesis on the language network in children and adolescents. Brain Struct Funct. 2021;226(3):701–713. pmid:33496825
  65. 65. Friedrich P, Forkel SJ, Thiebaut de Schotten M. Mapping the principal gradient onto the corpus callosum. Neuroimage. 2020;223:117317. pmid:32882387
  66. 66. Tantillo G, Peck KK, Arevalo-Perez J, Lyo JK, Chou JF, Young RJ, et al. Corpus Callosum Diffusion and Language Lateralization in Patients with Brain Tumors: A DTI and fMRI Study. J Neuroimaging. 2016;26(2):224–31. pmid:26258653
  67. 67. Li M, Wu J, Jiang P, Yang S, Guo R, Yang Y, et al. Corpus Callosum Diffusion Anisotropy and Hemispheric Lateralization of Language in Patients with Brain Arteriovenous Malformations. Brain Connect. 2021;11(6):447–456. pmid:33356845
  68. 68. Heiss WD, Thiel A, Kessler J, Herholz K. Disturbance and recovery of language function: correlates in PET activation studies. Neuroimage. 2003;20 Suppl 1:S42–9. pmid:14597295
  69. 69. Szaflarski JP, Allendorfer JB, Banks C, Vannest J, Holland SK. Recovered vs. not-recovered from post-stroke aphasia: the contributions from the dominant and non-dominant hemispheres. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2013;31(4):347–60. pmid:23482065
  70. 70. Rudy KL, Leemans A, Carson RG. Transcallosal connectivity of the human cortical motor network. Brain Struct Funct. 2017 Apr;222(3):1243–1252. pmid:27469272