Figures
Abstract
Cuticular hydrocarbons (CHC) are known to serve as discrimination cues and will trigger defence behaviour in a plethora of eusocial insects. However, little is known how about nestmate recognition ability selects for CHC diversification. In this study we investigate differences in CHC composition of four major honey bee species with respect to the differences in their nesting behavior. In contrast to A. mellifera, A. cerana and A. florea, the giant honey bee A. dorsata prefers to build their nests in aggregations with very small spatial distances between nests, which increases the probability of intrusions. Thus, A. dorsata exhibits a particularly challenging nesting behavior which we hypothesize should be accompanied with an improved nestmate recognition system. Comparative analyses of the worker CHC profiles indicate that A. dorsata workers exhibit a unique and more complex CHC profile than the other three honey bee species. This increased complexity is likely based on a developmental process that retains the capability to synthesize methyl-branched hydrocarbons as adults. Furthermore, two sets of behavioral experiments provide evidence that A. dorsata shows an improved nestmate discrimination ability compared to the phylogenetically ancestral A. florea, which is also open-nesting but does not form nest aggregations. The results of our study suggest that ecological traits like nesting in aggregation might be able to drive CHC profile diversification even in closely related insect species.
Citation: Maihoff F, Bohlke K, Brockmann A, Schmitt T (2022) Increased complexity of worker CHC profiles in Apis dorsata correlates with nesting ecology. PLoS ONE 17(7): e0271745. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271745
Editor: Wolfgang Blenau, University of Leipzig Faculty of Life Sciences: Universitat Leipzig Fakultat fur Lebenswissenschaften, GERMANY
Received: January 25, 2022; Accepted: July 6, 2022; Published: July 28, 2022
Copyright: © 2022 Maihoff et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting information files.
Funding: This work was supported by Deutscher Akademischer Austauschdienst, Bavarian- Indian Centre, NCBS-TIFR institutional funds (No. 12P4167) and the Department of Atomic Energy, Government of India (No. 12-R&D-TFR-5.04-0800 and 12-R&D-TFR-5.04-0900). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. This publication was supported by the Open Access Publication Fond of the University of Würzburg.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interest exist.
Introduction
Honey bees are a small group of closely related species showing distinct differences in their nesting behavior and ecology which affects the exposure to conspecific intruders (Fig 1) [1–4]. The dwarf honey bees (A. florea and A. andreniformis), which are the ancestral taxa, and the giant honey bees (A. dorsata, A. laboriosa and A. d. binghami) build open nests with single combs attached to the branches of bushes or trees and cliff overhangs. The workers of a colony form a flexible curtain, around the single comb which protects the brood but also allows free movement of the queen and workers between comb and curtain [2, 3, 5, 6]. The derived cavity-nesting species (species of the A. cerana group and A. mellifera) build their nests in tree trunks or other crevices with a small entrance hole. The different modes of nesting behavior require different strategies in colony defense [5]. While the cavity-nesting bees are protected by the cavity walls and a small entrance, the open-nesting bees are exposed to the environment, and the entire surface of the nest has to be protected against predators and intruders [7]. Besides these differences in nesting behavior, colonies of the giant honey bee species also tend to form nest aggregations. In A. dorsata it is common that 20 or even more colonies aggregate on one single tree or cliff site [8, 9]. Within an aggregation nests of individual colonies are in close vicinity to each other, sometimes even less than 1 m apart [10]. In contrast, nests of A. florea colonies are generally separated by larger distances of about 80–188 m [11]. Whether aggregation of nests in isolated tall trees are beneficial for A. dorsata by lowering the likelihood of being attacked by a predator, or aggregations are a consequence of the limited number of high quality nesting sites [7], the colonies within an aggregation are certainly at a higher risk of being intruded by drifting bees, robbers, and parasites. However, a very low frequency of drifted workers (0–6.25%) has been reported in aggregations [12, 13] suggesting an elaborate nestmate recognition system. Initially colonies respond to visual cues like irregular flight maneuvers of non-nestmates with shimmering behavior [14], whereas in close-range interactions after landing on the curtain olfactory cues like CHC profile differences are more likely used to identify the non-nestmate.
Bayesian consensus tree, graph modified according to Raffiudin and Crozier [15]. Species of the genus Apis can be divided into cavity-nesting bees, giant honey bees and dwarf honey bees. They respectively nest in caves or have open nests, where the colony is either attached to the bottom of the substrate or surrounds the branch they are attached to. Bayesian consensus tree was derived from the data set omitting the third codon position of the cox2 sequence. The credibility values shown were derived from 2000 trees after burnin. Trigona fimbriata, Melipona bicolor and Bombus terrestris are included as outgroups.
In addition to visual and acoustic cues that can be detected from a distance, chemical components of the insect cuticle such as cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs) function as important close-range recognition cues particularly distinguishing from conspecifics which are similar in appearance and behavior [16–18]. CHCs have been shown to serve as cues for species and mate recognition as well as for communication [19–23]. CHCs comprise a diverse set of substances, which vary in chain length and in number and positions of double bonds (alkenes, alkadienes) and methyl-branches (methyl-, dimethyl-, trimethyl-branched alkanes). These long-chain hydrocarbons form the lipophilic layer of the cuticle. While long-chain hydrocarbon profiles are apparently species- or even colony-specific, they also show also a clear individual specificity [18, 24]. However, the functional role of single compounds or substance classes among the chemically diverse profiles is still under debate. Cuticular alkenes alone are sufficient to trigger aggression in A. mellifera [25] whereas in Formica japonica only the entire CHC profile trigger aggressive behavior [16]. In general, alkenes and methyl-branched alkanes, but not alkanes seem to play a crucial role in recognition processes [26–29].
In this study, we analysed and compared the variation in CHC profiles among major open- and cavity-nesting honey bee species. In particular, we were interested to explore whether the CHC composition of A. dorsata foragers differs specifically from all studied non-aggregating honey bee species (A. florea, A. cerana, and A. mellifera). Due to their nesting in aggregations, we hypothesized that the CHC profile of A. dorsata should contain more components relevant for recognition, e.g., alkenes and methylalkanes, which would increase the probability to distinguish non-nestmates from nestmates. Furthermore, we analysed the CHC profiles of worker pupae and compared them to those of the worker bees to investigate whether changes in the CHC profiles during the development may provide information that would help to reconstruct the evolutionary history of the CHC diversification in the genus Apis. This approach is based on theories of Darwin and the developmental constraints hypothesis [30–32], which assume a gradient of species divergence rates across ontogeny (embryonic < larval/pupal < adult).
In addition to the chemical analyses, we performed two types of behavioral assays, a dummy test and an experiment releasing nestmates and non-nestmates in front of a colony, to test differences in nestmate recognition abilities between colonies of the two open-nesting species A. dorsata and A. florea. We tested the hypothesis whether the colonies of both species can distinguish visually similar objects (dummies) using olfactory CHC cues, and whether A. dorsata worker show an improved discrimination between nestmates and non-nestmates than workers of A. florea.
Material and methods
Study region and time
All experiments were performed on honey bee colonies located on the National Centre for Biological Sciences (NCBS) and University of Agricultural Sciences UAS-GKVK campus in the North of Bangalore, India. A. dorsata and A. florea colonies and worker bees were obtained from bee removers that were called to remove colonies of A. dorsata from buildings and A. florea residential gardens. City residents are afraid of A. dorsata colonies nesting close to their apartments and it has become a quite common practice to eradicate A. dorsata colonies nesting at such buildings [33–35]. A. florea and A. cerana are not necessarily recognized as threats but requests to remove colonies from residential gardens have increased in recent years. Sample collections and behavioral assays were conducted during the dry seasons of 2015, 2017 and 2018. As resource availability may affect behavioral responses of bees, all experiments were conducted within the same season since high and equal food resource availability across experimental years can be assumed.
Chemical analysis of cuticular hydrocarbon profiles
To explore possible phylogenetic changes in the CHC development among honey bee species, we analyzed the development of the CHC profiles from pupae to foragers. Six days old pupae were collected from brood combs kept in incubators. Following Groh and Rössler [36] pupal stages were identified with respect to color of the cuticle and the eyes. CHC profiles were extracted from flash-frozen pupae and foragers from two colonies per species. Each honey bee was extracted individually in n-hexane for 10 min. Extracts were evaporated in the fume hood, stored at -20°C and shipped to the University of Würzburg. Resuspended CHC extracts were analyzed with HP 6890 gas chromatograph coupled with a HP 5975 Mass Selective Detector (GC-MS, Agilent, Waldbronn, Germany): The GC (split/ splitless injector in splitless mode for 1 min, injected volume 1 μl at 300°C) was equipped with a DB-5 Fused Silica capillary column (30 m x 0.25 mm ID, df = 0.25 μm; J&W Scientific, Folsom, USA). Helium served as carrier gas at a constant flow of 1 ml/min. The following temperature program was used: Start temperature 60°C, temperature increase by 5°C per min up to 300°C, isotherm at 300°C for 10 min. The electron ionization mass spectra (EI-MS) were acquired at an ionization voltage of 70 eV (source temperature: 230°C). Chromatograms and mass spectra were recorded and quantified via integrated peak areas with the software HP Enhanced ChemStation G1701AA (version A.03.00; Hewlett Packard). CHC compounds were identified by the compound specific retention indices and their detected diagnostic ions [37].
We identified double-bound position in alkenes by DMDS-derivatization following Dunkelblum [38]. The GC settings were the same as described before, but oven temperature increased by 5°C per min up to 325°C and isotherm at 325°C for 10 min.
Comparative CHC analysis.
We compared the relative abundances of compounds of pupae and worker bees of all four investigated Apis species. Only CHC compounds which contributed to at least 1% to the total profile were selected for the analysis. CHC profile similarity was assessed by Non-metric Multidimensional Scaling (NMDS) and agglomerative cluster analysis. Dissimilarities were calculated using Bray-Curtis distances. We revealed profile composition differences between bee species and developmental stages by performing permutational multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). All analyses were performed in R using the packages vegan [39] and pairwise Adonis [40]. Permutations were set on 10000 or 999 respectively.
Comparisons of the relative proportion of alkadienes, alkenes, alkanes, monomethyl alkanes and dimethyl alkanes were performed using Kruskal-Wallis rank sum test followed by Dunn test as post-hoc test (stats package, dunn.test package [41].
CHC diversity was calculated by the number of components contributing to the profiles (Component Richness) and calculating Shannon-Wiener-Indices. Peak areas including more than one compound were divided by the number of identified compounds. We calculated the Shannon-Wiener index per species based on the abundance values per component, averaged over all individuals of a species.
Nestmate odor recognition assay.
We conducted dummy experiments to test a potential function of CHCs for nestmate recognition in A. florea and A. dorsata. The dummies consisted of pale beige Teflon tapes wrapped around pieces of a drinking straw (size adjusted to the tested species) impregnated with CHC extract from nestmates or non-nestmates respectively. The CHC extract was produced by immersing two bees in n-hexane for 10 min. The extract was concentrated to a volume of 150 ± 50μl and was applied drop by drop to the Teflon tape using a microliter glas pipet. Bee dummies were presented on a rod to the test colony for 6 seconds. To avoid that the dummy itself would cause a colony response by increasing a general alert [42] the dummy, attached to a thread and self-made fishing rod (made from a tree branch), was positioned on the surface of the bee curtain by a colony-specific adjusted speed and angle of movement [42]. The exact position on the curtain varied, but corresponded to the spots where nestmates returning in parallel to the experiment could be observed. Due to the restrictions in observation on the surface of the bee curtain, the behavior of workers was categorized as either high-aggressiveness, which was characterized by persisting stinging and biting, or low-aggressiveness/neutral which was characterized by brief gentle tactile and visual contact with the dummy (based on categorization in Harrison, Palmer & Rittschof [43].
Dummies treated with solvent (n-hexane) served as controls. The behavioral assay was performed blind in a random block design (each n = 40). Before the experiments, dummies without solvent or CHC treatment were presented ten times to the colonies to habituate the colony members successfully, thereby avoiding any responses elicited by presenting the dummy itself. During the experiments two focal colonies per species were used. To test whether colonies’ response differed between dummies with different odors and between species we modeled colony response (high-aggressiveness or low-aggressiveness) as a function of the interaction between species and dummy type using a GLMM with a binomial distribution. The test colony was modeled as random effect. We tested the interaction using a likelihood ratio test (ANOVA (fullmodel, model without interaction)). Data were analyzed using generalized linear mixed-effects models (GLMM) in the R language using the glmmTMB function [44]. Post-hoc tukey tests were performed with the emmeans function [45] (for summary statistics see S1 Table).
Worker releasing experiments.
To test intruding attempts and intruder acceptance in nestmates and non-nestmates in A. florea and A. dorsata under natural condition, we collected workers from foreign colonies located at least 6 km away from the test colonies. Here, the distances exceed the expected gathering distances of both species, as resource availability in the landscape was very high at the time of our experiment, which is why at the same site and at the same phenological time even the larger A. dorsata forages only up to 3 km [46]. Workers were either collected directly from the bee curtain or from a feeder where bees were trained. Note that automated model selection based on the Akaike information criterion has shown that the models are not improved when this sampling type is integrated in one of the following analyses. Cohorts of around 30 foreign conspecific workers were marked and released in approximately 1.5 m from the test colony. Attempts to land on the bee curtain surface were observed. Bees were considered as accepted if they were able to move freely within the colony and stayed for at least 10 seconds without being expelled. As control we released cohorts of nestmates (originating from the test colony). Each of the four experimental combinations (2 species x nestmates/ non-nestmates) was repeated 10 to 13 times using a total of 4 test colonies per species (Table 1). To test whether foreign and nestmate workers differ in their tendency to enter the test colonies we modeled the number of intruding events (count data) as a function of the interaction between species and worker origin (nestmate vs non-nestmate) and the number of released individuals as an offset variable using GLMM with Conway-Maxwell-Poisson distribution, because it can handle both over- and underdispersion (Brooks et al. 2019). Colony was set as random effect. As we hypothesised that species would differ in their reaction towards nestmates and non-nestmates we tested the interaction using a likelihood ratio test (ANOVA (fullmodel, model without interaction)). We used the function emmeans to perform a post hoc tukey test to analyse pairwise differences between the four test categories (2 species x nestmates/non-nestmates). Note, that since we observed cohorts of released bees and not individual marked bees, the number of intruding attempts could exceed the total number of released bees. To test wether test colonies reacted differently to nestmates or foreign bees we modeled the number of observed accepting events (tolerance) as a function of the interaction between species and worker origin (non nestmates vs nestmates) and the total number of intruding attempts as an offset using a GLMM with Conway-Maxwell-Poisson distribution [47]. The test colony was modeled as random effect. Data were analyzed using generalized linear mixed-effects models (GLMM) in the R language using the glmmTMB function [44]. Post-hoc tukey tests were performed with the emmeans function [45] (for summary statistics see S1 Table).
Results
Cuticular hydrocarbon profile comparison
The CHC profiles of the four investigated species A. florea, A. dorsata, A. cerana and A. mellifera revealed species and developmental stage specific differences (see NMDS) (Fig 2, Tables 2 and 3).
CHC profiles of pupae (unfilled) and foragers (filled) of the four honey bee species A. florea (circle), A. dorsata (square), A. mellifera (triangle) and A. cerana (diamond) displayed in a two-dimensional graph by non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS) of Bray-Curtis distances.
We identified five substance classes: alkadienes, alkenes, alkanes, monomethyl alkanes and dimethyl alkanes, which differ in their relative abundance in the CHC profiles of the studied honey bee species (Fig 3(A), Table 4). In the pupal stage workers of all species have qualitatively similar profiles. All pupal profiles consist of alkenes, alkanes and monomethyl alkanes, but the relative compositions of the substance classes differ among the species. The pupal profiles of A. florea and A. dorsata are more similar to each other compared to A. mellifera and A. cerana. The pupal profile of A. mellifera is characterized by a relatively high proportion of monomethyl alkanes. In contrast, the pupal profile of A. cerana shows a high proportion of alkenes compared to the other species.
(A) Relative proportions of hydrocarbon substance classes (alkadienes: dark green, alkenes: light green, alkanes: yellow, monomethyl alkanes: orange and dimethyl alkanes: red) in the pupal and forager stages in A. mellifera, A. cerana, A. florea and A. dorsata are presented. Each bar refers to one individual per species. (B): Cluster dendrogram of the forager CHC profile based on individual substances is shown.
In contrast to the pupal profiles, the forager profiles show strong qualitative differences in the composition of the substance classes among species. Alkenes and alkanes are represented in all species. However, monomethyl alkanes and dimethyl alkanes occur only in A. dorsata forager profiles whereas small amounts of alkadienes occur exclusively in A. mellifera forager profiles. Overall, A. florea, A. mellifera and A. cerana forager profiles are more similar with respect to the proportions of alkanes and alkenes compared to the profiles of A. dorsata.
There is a shift from relatively high to low proportions of monomethyl alkanes from pupae to forager stage in all species except A. dorsata. In contrast, A. dorsata increases the proportion of monomethyl alkanes during development from pupae to forager and adds dimethyl alkanes to the profile (Fig 3, Table 4).
A cluster dendrogram analysis of CHC profiles of foragers of all species based on the comparison of single components revealed that A. mellifera and A. cerana have the most similar CHC profiles. Most interestingly, the foragers of the open-nesting and aggregating A. dorsata have the most distinct CHC profile (Fig 3(B)), separating it from all the other studied species. Consequently, the cluster dendrogram based on CHC profiles deviates from the well-established phylogeny of honey bees (Raffiudin & Crozier, 2007 see Fig 1). In addition, a NMDS of the CHC profiles of 28 workers from three different colonies of A. dorsata and of 31 workers from three different colonies of A. florea, show a separation of each colony with some overlap (S1 File).
Averaged CHC profile diversity per species was estimated in forager profiles by calculating (1) the number of components contributing to the profile (component richness) and (2) the Shannon-Wiener Index, which accounts for the abundances of components. With regard to the total CHC profiles, A. dorsata has the highest component richness (28 components), which was around twice as high as in the other species. A. mellifera (15 components) had a higher richness than A. cerana (12 components) and A. florea (11 components), but differences between the three latter species are small. Considering Shannon-Wiener diversity, A. dorsata has the most diverse profile (2.62) followed by A. mellifera (2.17), A. florea (1.83) and A. cerana (1.64) (Fig 4).
Honey bee species differ in their mean component richness and diversity (calculated according to Shannon-Wiener-Index). Additionally, abundances of single component are presented. The mean proportion of each component per species is given.
Behavioral approach: Cuticular hydrocarbons as nestmate recognition cues
By exposing bee dummies covered with CHC extracts from nestmates or non-nestmates in front of colonies, we tested whether CHCs alone are sufficient to trigger differential response of A. florea and A. dorsata colonies towards nestmates and non-nestmates. The three dummy types (nestmate, non-nestmate and solvent control) elicited different degrees of high-level aggressive responses (factor dummy type: χ2 (2) = 89.84, p<0.001). In addition, the effect of the dummy did not significantly differ the behavior in both species (interaction: χ2 (2) = 4.5, p = 0.105) (Fig 5). Colony aggresiveness was lowest towards solvent control in both species, although A. florea workers showed an aggressive behavior against the solvent control dummy in a few cases. Colonies showed a significantly higher proportion of high-level aggressive responses towards the dummies with the non-nestmate CHCs than those with the nestmate CHCs. While in A. florea high-level aggressive behavior towards non-nestmates odor dummies was shown twice as often as towards nestmate odor dummies (75% vs 37.5%), in A. dorsata high-level aggressive responses towards the dummies with non-nestmate was more than four times higher (77.5% vs 17.5%).
The behavioral response of a colony (high-aggressive: dark grey: low-aggressive: light grey) to odor dummies is shown. Dummies applied with solvent served as control. Letters are indicating significant differences in the proportion of high-level aggressive and low-level aggressive colony responses achieved via post-hoc Tukey test. For all tests related to species x dummy type, 40 individual dummies were tested.
Behavioral approach: Intruder acceptance
By releasing workers in front of conspecific colonies we determined the willingness of workers to enter their own or a foreign colony as well as the responses of the colonies towards the entering nestmates and non-nestmates. Apis florea and A. dorsata workers differed in their attempts to enter conspecific colonies (Glmm: χ2 (1) = 30.763, p < 0.001) (Fig 6(A)). In A. florea, it was observed that both colony members and foreign workers entered the test colony, with no significant differences in their behaviour when in contact with the colony (0.83 vs 0.78, Tukey: p = 0.96), while in A. dorsata foreign workers were observed to enter the test colony less frequently than nestmates (0.1 vs. 0.75, Tukey: p < 0.001).
(A) Differences in the proportion of intruding attempts (measured as the proportion of observed intruding individuals to released individuals) between nestmates and non-nestmates in A. florea and A. dorsata are presented. (B) Differences in the proportion of accepted individuals between released nestmates and non-nestmates and between species (A. florea and A. dorsata) are shown. Letters indicate differences in the proportion of accepted individuals achieved via post-hoc test (Tukey). The number of observed cohorts is indicated with n.
Correspondingly, colonies of A. florea and A. dorsata differed in their response towards conspecific intruders (Glmm: χ2 (1) = 10.427, p < 0.01) (Fig 6(B)). Acceptance of non-nestmates (88%) and nestmates (100%) did not significantly differ in A. florea colonies (Tukey, p = 0.07), whereas A. dorsata colonies showed a significantly lower acceptance rate towards non-nestmates (58%) compared to nestmates (99%) (Tukey: p < 0.001).
Discussion
The results of our study indicate that A. dorsata workers exhibit a more complex and qualitatively more diverse CHC profile than foragers of A. florea, A. cerana, and A. mellifera. This mirrors the nesting ecology of the studied species with A. dorsata beeing the only species that nests in aggregations, while all other species do not nest in such close proximity. In addition, A. dorsata workers avoid entering foreign colonies more often and A. dorsata guards recognize and repel intruders to a higher degree than A. florea. The results of our behavioral experiments indicate that colonies of A. dorsata, are better at maintaining their social integrity than colonies of A. florea. The correlation between a more complex CHC profile and more selective behavioral response toward non-nestmates suggests that A. dorsata might have evolved a more fine-tuned nestmate recognition system in parallel with the evolution of building their nests in close aggregations.
The variation of CHC profiles among all analysed honey bee species is driven by differences in the occurrence of alkenes, alkadienes, monomethyl alkanes and dimethyl alkanes. All these components have been demonstrated to play major roles in nestmate recognition, as insects are able to detect and differentiate CHCs with different number and positions of double bonds and methyl-branches [26, 29]. A. dorsata is the only honey bee species exhibiting methyl-branched alkanes (mono- and dimethyl alkanes) in the forager profile with a proportion of about 25% of the amount of CHCs. Thus, we propose that methyl-branched alkanes found in the forager profile likely play a functional role for the fine-tuned recognition system in A. dorsata. In contrast, alkanes, which are present in the forager profiles of all species, do not provide cues as good as unsaturated or methyl-branched hydrocarbons due to the lack of steric complexity [25, 26, 48, 49].
Interestingly, we found complex CHC profiles with a large proportion of methyl-branched alkanes on the pupae of all four investigated species of honey bees. Indeed, we show that all honey bee species produce alkanes, methyl-branched alkanes and unsaturated hydrocarbons at some point during their development. However, A. mellifera, A. cerana and A. florea reduce the abundance of methyl-branched alkanes when they develop into foragers. Genes encoding enzymes for monomethyl alkane biosynthesis might be downregulated or silenced at some point in the development. Only A. dorsata increases the proportion of methyl-branched alkanes during the development from the pupal to the adult stage. Thus, the CHC profile of A. dorsata foragers is more similar to the pupal stage of all four studied honey bee species than the forager profiles of A. mellifera, A. cerana and A. florea. Interestingly, the divergence pattern of the CHCs mirror the molecular phylogenetic tree except for the position of A. dorsata [3, 4, 15, 50]. This finding suggests that the CHC profiles in A. dorsata with a high content of methyl-branched alkanes are subject to selection. In contrast, the diversification of the CHC profiles in the other Apis species are more likely the result of neutral evolution [51].
Honey bee colonies can use several behavioral strategies to avoid the reciprocal intrusion of foreign individuals. Drifting, where a forager visits a foreign colony instead of the mother colony, is particularly likely in nest aggregations. To minimise drifting, foragers must be able to identify the position of the mother colony via landmarks even in a dense nest aggregation. A potential colony-specific nest odour could be quite helpful as a strategy to confirm the identification of their own nest via landmarks. In addition, colonies can effectively prevent the intrusion of non-nestmates through their guards. Considering the behavior of drifters, we observed that in A. dorsata non-nestmates were less motivated to approach a foreign conspecific colony than A. florea non-nestmates, suggesting that in A. dorsata homing workers are more cautious when approaching a colony. Considering guarding behavior, we found that both A. dorsata and A. florea workers were able to recognize and repel non-nestmates. This finding is in contrast to an earlier study that used a lab assay with individual workers and could not show non-nestmate discrimination in A. dorsata and A. florea [52]. Our CHC dummy experiments using colonies of A. dorsata and A. florea under natural conditions showed that this two honey bee species could use CHCs profiles for nestmate discrimination. Furthermore, in accordance with our hypothesis, A. dorsata workers rejected intruding conspecific non-nestmates at a much higher rate than A. florea workers. Our findings are supported by Weihmann et al. [14] who also reported that A. dorsata colonies fend off intruding foreign workers under natural conditions. This study showed that A. dorsata colonies are capable of detecting non-nestmates by aberrant flight approaches and respond to this with shimmering behavior. We did not record shimmering responses in our experiments. However, both findings do not contradict each other, instead suggest different stages of defensive responses, the first, a long-range response based on visual cues, and the second, a short-range response based on olfactory cues. In this case, increased information content from methyl-branched alkanes may be beneficial. Alternatively, larger amounts of methyl-branched alkanes could help counteract possible desiccation stress in A. dorsata individuals hanging exposed on tall trees [53].
The evolution of a more fine-tuned nestmate recognition ability at close range in A. dorsata may represent an adaptation for nesting in close aggregations which is unique among honey bee species [8]. Naturally, nests of A. mellifera are widely spaced in the environment so that drifting events are unlikely to occur [1, 54]. In apiaries however, beekeepers artificially aggregate colonies of these species by placing hives next to each other. It is well documented that under such unnatural conditions drifting of foragers between nests is common. Up to 40% of drifted bees can be found in artificially aggregated colonies in apiaries [55]. The extremely high tolerance towards non-nestmates observed in apiaries with A. mellifera colonies might be due to guard bees which lack the ability to discriminate non-nestmates from nestmates using olfactory cues. The lack of fine-tuned nestmate discrimination is not as important in species such as A. mellifera, which under natural conditions singularise their nests in the field and arrange them further apart from each other. On the contrary, the ability to distinguish foreign bees from nestmates, especially at short distances, allows a dense arrangement of colonies while maintaining colony integrity, which, as observed in nest aggregations of A. dorsata, are even arranged side by side on thick tree branches or on rocky niches.
Conclusions
In our study, we show that foragers of the giant honey bee A. dorsata have a complex cuticular hydrocarbon profile compared to other honey bee species, such as A. mellifera, A. cerana and A. florea. This complex profile might provide a better distinguishable olfactory signature to identify non-nestmates. These findings are in congruence with the nesting ecology of A. dorsata, whose colonies aggregate in large numbers on single trees often only a few centimeters away from each other. Thus, it allows A. dorsata to maintain their social integrity to keep non-nestmates efficiently out of their colony which is in line with studies using behavioral and molecular markers [12, 13]. Although, we have evidence that A. dorsata uses chemical cues from the cuticle to discriminate between nestmates and non-nestmates, we are aware that this honey bee species also uses visual cues for nestmate recognition [14].
There are two possible evolutionary scenarios to explain the present CHC phenotypes: First, methyl-branched alkanes in the forager stage represent the ancestral profile and A. florea, A. mellifera, and A. cerana experienced an evolutionary loss. Second, and this might be the more parsimonious scenario, the ancestral forager phenotype expressed only alkanes and unsaturated hydrocarbons as is found in A. florea and the other two investigated honey bee species. In this case, A. dorsata might have reactivated the biosynthesis of methyl-branched alkanes, which was present in the pupal stage, in the adult stage to establish a more sensitive nestmate recognition system as a consequence of nesting in close aggregation with conspecific colonies. The results of our study suggest that the species-specific behavioural trait of nesting in aggregations also results in a species-specific diversification of the CHC profile.
Supporting information
S1 Table. Summary of post hoc-test statistics.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271745.s001
(XLSX)
S3 Table. Raw data colony response against cuticular hydrocarbon odor.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271745.s003
(CSV)
S4 Table. Raw data colony response on intruding attempts.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271745.s004
(CSV)
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Hanumantha Raju, Santosh Kumar F, Mahes Kumar, Prabhudev M.V., Yogesh Pandey and Franziska Bandorf for their help with the organization and preparation of the field experiments and Antonia Konle for the preliminary analysis of colony specificity in A. dorsata. We appreciate the support by Benjamin Rutschmann and Patrick Kohl by providing camera equipment and their expertise in handling of field colonies. Further, we are grateful to Patrick Kohl for inspiring discussions and comments on an earlier version of the manuscript as well as the supply of the cavity nest picture. We are pleased that Hemalatha Bhagavan provided her beautiful A. florea illustration and that Simon Heider is accelerating CHC analysis through his programming skills. Finally, we thank Sruthi Unnikrishnan for proofreading the final version.
References
- 1.
Seeley TD. Honeybee Ecology: A Study of Adaptation in Social Life. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press; 1985.
- 2.
Oldroyd BP, Wongsiri S. Asian honey bees: biology, conservation, and human interactions. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press; 2006.
- 3.
Hepburn R, Radloff S, editors. Honeybees of Asia. Berlin Heidelberg: Springer; 2011. 669 p.
- 4.
Smith DR. Biogeography of honey bees. In: Encyclopedia of Social Insects. Springer International Publishing; 2020.
- 5. Dyer FC, Seeley TD. Nesting Behavior and the Evolution of Worker Tempo in Four Honey Bee Species. Ecology. 1991 Feb;72(1):156–70.
- 6. Bhagavan H, Muthmann O, Brockmann A. Structural and temporal dynamics of the bee curtain in the open-nesting honey bee species, Apis florea. Apidologie. 2016 Nov;47(6):749–58.
- 7. Seeley TD, Seeley RH, Akratanakul P. Colony Defense Strategies of the Honeybees in Thailand. Ecol Monogr. 1982 Feb;52(1):43–63.
- 8. Deodikar G, Ghatge A, Phadke R, Mahindre D, Kshirsagar K, Muvel K, et al. Nesting behaviour of Indian honeybees III. Nesting behaviour of Apis dorsata Fab. Indian Bee J. 1977;(39):1–12.
- 9. Paar J, Oldroyd BP, Kastberger G. Giant honeybees return to their nest sites. Nature. 2000 Aug;406(6795):475–475. pmid:10952300
- 10. Moritz RFA, Kryger P, Koeniger G, Koeniger N, Estoup A, Tingek S. High degree of polyandry in Apis dorsata queens detected by DNA microsatellite variability. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1995 Nov 1;37(5):357–63.
- 11. Wattanachaiyingcharoen W, Wongsiri S, Oldroyd BP. Aggregations of unrelated Apis florea colonies. Apidologie. 2008 Sep;39(5):531–6.
- 12. Paar J, Oldroyd BP, Huettinger E, Kastberger G. Drifting of workers in nest aggregations of the giant honeybee Apis dorsata. Apidologie. 2002 Nov;33(6):553–61.
- 13. Paar J. Genetic Structure of an Apis dorsata Population: The Significance of Migration and Colony Aggregation. J Hered. 2004 Mar 1;95(2):119–26. pmid:15073227
- 14. Weihmann F, Waddoup D, Hötzl T, Kastberger G. Intraspecific Aggression in Giant Honey Bees (Apis dorsata). Insects. 2014 Sep 18;5(3):689–704. pmid:26462834
- 15. Raffiudin R, Crozier RH. Phylogenetic analysis of honey bee behavioral evolution. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2007 May;43(2):543–52. pmid:17123837
- 16. Akino T, Yamamura K, Wakamura S, Yamaoka R. Direct behavioral evidence for hydrocarbons as nestmate recognition cues in Formica japonica (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Appl Entomol Zool. 2004;39(3):381–7.
- 17. Ozaki M. Ant Nestmate and Non-Nestmate Discrimination by a Chemosensory Sensillum. Science. 2005 Jul 8;309(5732):311–4. pmid:15947139
- 18.
Blomquist GJ, Bagnères AG. Insect hydrocarbons: biology, biochemistry, and chemical ecology. Cambridge University Press; 2010.
- 19. Shu S, Swedenborg PD, Jones RL. A kairomone forTrichogramma nubilale (Hymenoptera: Trichogrammatidae) isolation, identification, and synthesis. J Chem Ecol. 1990 Feb;16(2):521–9. pmid:24263508
- 20. Gibbs AG. Lipid melting and cuticular permeability: new insights into an old problem. J Insect Physiol. 2002 Apr;48(4):391–400. pmid:12770088
- 21. Ruther J, Stahl LM, Steiner S, Garbe LA, Tolasch T. A male sex pheromone in a parasitic wasp and control of the behavioral response by the female’s mating status. J Exp Biol. 2007 Jun 15;210(12):2163–9.
- 22. Ruther J, Döring M, Steiner S. Cuticular hydrocarbons as contact sex pheromone in the parasitoid Dibrachys cavus: Contact sex pheromone in Dibrachys cavus. Entomol Exp Appl. 2011 Jul;140(1):59–68.
- 23. Yusuf AA, Pirk CWW, Crewe RM. Mandibular gland pheromone contents in workers and queens of Apis mellifera adansonii. Apidologie. 2015 Sep 1;46(5):559–72.
- 24. Falcón T, Ferreira-Caliman MJ, Franco Nunes FM, Tanaka ÉD, do Nascimento FS, Gentile Bitondi MM. Exoskeleton formation in Apis mellifera: Cuticular hydrocarbons profiles and expression of desaturase and elongase genes during pupal and adult development. Insect Biochem Mol Biol. 2014;50(1). pmid:24813723
- 25. Dani FR, Jones GR, Corsi S, Beard R, Pradella D, Turillazzi S. Nestmate Recognition Cues in the Honey Bee: Differential Importance of Cuticular Alkanes and Alkenes. Chem Senses. 2005 Jul 1;30(6):477–89. pmid:15917370
- 26. Dani FR, Jones GR, Destri S, Spencer SH, Turillazzi S. Deciphering the recognition signature within the cuticular chemical profile of paper wasps. Anim Behav. 2001 Jul;62(1):165–71.
- 27. Steiger S, Schmitt T, Schaefer HM. The origin and dynamic evolution of chemical information transfer. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2011 Apr 7;278(1708):970–9. pmid:21177681
- 28. Lorenzi CM, Cervo R, Bagnères AG. Facultative social parasites mark host nests with branched hydrocarbons. Anim Behav. 2011 Nov;82(5):1143–9.
- 29. Kleeberg I, Menzel F, Foitzik S. The influence of slavemaking lifestyle, caste and sex on chemical profiles in Temnothorax ants: insights into the evolution of cuticular hydrocarbons. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2017 Mar 15;284(1850):20162249. pmid:28298345
- 30.
Arthur W. A theory of the evolution of development. John Wiley & Sons Incorporated; 1988.
- 31.
Raff RA. The shape of life: genes, development, and the evolution of animal form. University of Chicago Press; 2012.
- 32.
Gould SJ. Ontogeny and phylogeny. Harvard University Press; 1985.
- 33. Saravanan S, Kamaladhasan N, Maruthupandian J, Jothi Basu M, Chandrasekaran S. Beehive removal practices in urban India: Bane of bee’s life? Mater Today Proc. 2020 Dec;S2214785320376276.
- 34. Nagaraja DN. Effect of Insecticide Poisoning on Mortality of Giant Honeybee, Apis dorsata Colonies. Pesqui Int J Res. 2016;2(1):96–100.
- 35.
Walter S, Brockmann A. Pollinators in the urban age Living with wild giant honey bees in the Indian megacity Bengaluru [Internet]. 2021. https://in.boell.org/en/2021/01/07/pollinators-urban-age
- 36. Groh C, Rössler W. Caste-specific postembryonic development of primary and secondary olfactory centers in the female honeybee brain. Arthropod Struct Dev. 2008 Nov;37(6):459–68. pmid:18621587
- 37. Carlson DA, Bernier UR, Sutton BD. Elution Patterns from Capillary GC for Methyl-Branched Alkanes. J Chem Ecol. 1998 Nov 1;24(11):1845–65.
- 38. Dunkelblum E, Tan SH, Silk PJ. Double-bond location in monounsaturated fatty acids by dimethyl disulfide derivatization and mass spectrometry: Application to analysis of fatty acids in pheromone glands of four lepidoptera. J Chem Ecol. 1985 Mar;11(3):265–77. pmid:24309959
- 39.
Oksanen J, Blanchet GF, Friendly M, Kindt R, Legendre P, McGlinn D, et al. vegan: Community Ecology Package [Internet]. 2020. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=vegan
- 40.
Martinez Arbizu P. pairwiseAdonis: Pairwise multilevel comparison using adonis. R package [Internet]. 2020. https://github.com/pmartinezarbizu/pairwiseAdonis
- 41.
Dinno A. dunn.test: Dunn’s Test of Multiple Comparisons Using Rank Sums. [Internet]. 2017. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=dunn.test
- 42. Koeniger N, Fuchs S. Zur Kolonieverteidigung der asiatischen Honigbienen. Z Für Tierpsychol. 1975;37(1):99–106.
- 43. Harrison JW, Palmer JH, Rittschof CC. Altering social cue perception impacts honey bee aggression with minimal impacts on aggression-related brain gene expression. Sci Rep. 2019 Oct 10;9(1):14642. pmid:31601943
- 44. Brooks ME, Kristensen K, van Benthem KJ, Magnusson A, Berg CW, Nielsen A, et al. glmmTMB Balances Speed and Flexibility Among Packages for Zero-inflated Generalized Linear Mixed Modeling. R J. 2017;9(2):378.
- 45.
Lenth R. emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares. Means. 2021; https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans
- 46. Young AM, Kohl PL, Rutschmann B, Steffan-Dewenter I, Brockmann A, Dyer FC. Temporal and spatial foraging patterns of three Asian honey bee species in Bangalore, India. Apidologie. 2021 Apr 1;52(2):503–23.
- 47. Brooks ME, Kristensen K, Darrigo MR, Rubim P, Uriarte M, Bruna E, et al. Statistical modeling of patterns in annual reproductive rates. Ecology. 2019;100(7):e02706. pmid:30916779
- 48. Châline N, Sandoz JC, Martin SJ, Ratnieks FLW, Jones GR. Learning and Discrimination of Individual Cuticular Hydrocarbons by Honeybees (Apis mellifera). Chem Senses. 2005 May 1;30(4):327–35. pmid:15788713
- 49. van Wilgenburg E, Sulc R, Shea KJ, Tsutsui ND. Deciphering the Chemical Basis of Nestmate Recognition. J Chem Ecol. 2010 Jul;36(7):751–8. pmid:20556636
- 50. Arias MC, Sheppard WS. Phylogenetic relationships of honey bees (Hymenoptera: Apinae: Apini) inferred from nuclear and mitochondrial DNA sequence data. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2005 Oct;37(1):25–35. pmid:16182149
- 51. Kimura M. The neutral theory of molecular evolution: a review of recent evidence. Idengaku Zasshi. 1991 Aug;66(4):367–86. pmid:1954033
- 52. Breed MD, Deng XB, Buchwald R. Comparative nestmate recognition in Asian honey bees, Apis florea, Apis andreniformis, Apis dorsata, and Apis cerana. Apidologie. 2007 Sep;38(5):411–8.
- 53. Gibbs A, Pomonis JG. Physical properties of insect cuticular hydrocarbons: The effects of chain length, methyl-branching and unsaturation. Comp Biochem Physiol B Biochem Mol Biol. 1995 Oct 1;112(2):243–9.
- 54. Kohl PL, Rutschmann B. The neglected bee trees: European beech forests as a home for feral honey bee colonies. PeerJ. 2018 Apr 6;6:e4602. pmid:29637025
- 55. Pfeiffer KJ, Crailsheim K. Drifting of honeybees. Insectes Sociaux. 1998 May 1;45(2):151–67.