Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Between forest and croplands: Nocturnal behavior in wild chimpanzees of Sebitoli, Kibale National Park, Uganda

  • Camille Lacroux ,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – original draft

    camille.lacroux@mnhn.fr

    Affiliations UMR 7206 CNRS/MNHN/P7, Eco-anthropologie, Hommes et Environnements, Museum National d’Histoire Naturelle, Musée de l’Homme, Paris, France, Sebitoli Chimpanzee Project, Great Ape Conservation Project, Fort Portal, Uganda, UMR 7179 CNRS/MNHN, Ecologie et Gestion de la Biodiversité, Museum National d’Histoire Naturelle, Paris, France

  • Benjamin Robira,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations UMR 7206 CNRS/MNHN/P7, Eco-anthropologie, Hommes et Environnements, Museum National d’Histoire Naturelle, Musée de l’Homme, Paris, France, CEFE, CNRS, Université Montpellier, Université Paul Valéry Montpellier 3, EPHE, IRD, Montpellier, France

  • Nicole Kane-Maguire,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Sebitoli Chimpanzee Project, Great Ape Conservation Project, Fort Portal, Uganda

  • Nelson Guma,

    Roles Project administration

    Affiliation Uganda Wildlife Authority, Kibale, Uganda

  • Sabrina Krief

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations UMR 7206 CNRS/MNHN/P7, Eco-anthropologie, Hommes et Environnements, Museum National d’Histoire Naturelle, Musée de l’Homme, Paris, France, Sebitoli Chimpanzee Project, Great Ape Conservation Project, Fort Portal, Uganda

Abstract

Some animal species have been presumed to be purely diurnal. Yet, they show flexibility in their activity rhythm, and can occasionally be active at night. Recently, it has been suggested that chimpanzees may rarely engage in nocturnal activities in savannah forests, in contrast to the frequent nocturnal feeding of crops observed at Sebitoli, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Here we thus aimed to explore the factors that might trigger such intense nocturnal activity (e.g. harsher weather conditions during daytime, low wild food availability or higher diurnal foraging risk) in this area. We used camera-traps set over 18 km2 operating for 15 months. We report activities and group composition from records obtained either within the forest or at the forest interface with maize fields, the unique crop consumed. Maize is an attractive and accessible food source, although actively guarded by farmers, particularly during daytime. Out of the 19 156 clips collected, 1808 recorded chimpanzees. Of these, night recordings accounted for 3.3% of forest location clips, compared to 41.8% in the maize fields. Most nocturnal clips were obtained after hot days, and most often during maize season for field clips. At night within the forest, chimpanzees were travelling around twilight hours, while when at the border of the fields they were foraging on crops mostly after twilight and in smaller parties. These results suggest that chimpanzees change their activity rhythm to access cultivated resources when human presence and surveillance is lower. This survey provides evidence of behavioral plasticity in chimpanzees in response to neighboring human farming activities, and emphasizes the urgent need to work with local communities to mitigate human-wildlife conflict related to crop-feeding.

Introduction

The distribution of activities over the day is an important facet of animal behavior. Indeed, time itself is a currency that could be optimized, thereby optimizing an individual’s fitness. However, active time is often limited to a specific period of the day. As such, most species are assumed to be exclusively diurnal or nocturnal. However almost 8.5% of mammalian species do not fit within this usual dichotomous classification, and instead show variations in their activity time between day and night (mammals [1, 2]; primates [3, 4]). Such species, which can be active both during the day and at night, are termed cathemeral [5]. Among primates, only Malagasy strepsirrhines (Eulemur fulvus [6]; Eulemur macaco [7, 8]) are classified as such. Yet within the broad spectrum of diurnal species, there is some evidence of crepuscular or occasionally nocturnal activity: either through direct visual observations (Cercopithecus mitis albogularis [9]; Chlorocebus pygerythrus and Papio anubis [10]; Theropithecus gelada [11]; Macaca fuscata yakui [12]; Pongo pygmaeus [13]; Pan troglodytes [14, 15]) or acoustic signals (Procolobus rufomitratus tephrosceles and P. anubis [9]; Macaca sylvanus [16]; Gorilla beringei [17]; Pan troglodytes [9, 1821]). As these night observations are rare and considered opportunistic, despite their potential to provide valuable information, these primate species, especially the suborder Haplorhini, have only been studied during the day until now.

In primate species considered diurnal, occasional nocturnal activity is purported to be a response to environmental pressure and constraints. For instance, active time allocation to night periods has been seen as a means to counteract detrimental effect on individuals’ fitness due to harsh environmental conditions during daytime (e.g. by avoiding higher temperature during the day [22, 23]), due to inter-species resource competition (e.g. by avoiding foraging time overlap with a competitive diurnal species [22]), due to predation risk (e.g. by foraging or moving outside of diurnal predator’s active time [12, 24, 25]) or due to food scarcity or lower availability during day time [2628]. In addition, climate and weather conditions (e.g. temperature, rainfall, humidity and moonlight) can affect primate sleep duration and quality [29, 30].

Changes in human traditional lifestyle may also have been driven by a response to increased proximity between humans and non-human primates [31; for other taxon, e.g. Ursus arctos 32], itself the result of human population growth and expansion of anthropised areas. This is best illustrated with the current crisis in palm oil production: large parts of the Indonesian tropical forest have been logged to allow increased palm cultivation, which has reduced the habitat of orangutans (Pongo sp.) and thus affecting their survival [33]. In Africa too, cultivated crops are becoming more widespread, encroaching on forest habitat [34], increasing competition for resources. Crop-feeding may therefore be a by-product of both natural resources becoming less available due to considerable deforestation [35, 36] and the nutritional benefits of cultivated foods. These foods are more palatable and easily accessible for primates [37, 38] through increased proximity and thereby familiar [3941]. However, during daylight those resources are usually actively protected by farmers, sometimes leading to death or severe injuries for the primates [42]. Thus, primates appear to show some behavioral adaptations, such as crop foraging at specific hours when farmers reduced their vigilance against wildlife incursions (P. troglodytes [14]; Macaca maura [43]). In Kibale National Park, Uganda, primates are thought to be responsible for over 71% of crop damage [44, 45]. Nonetheless, the difficulty of directly detecting these foraging events means that, in many cases, crop damage is sometimes misattributed to other species. In the end, the actual impact of crop foraging by a given species is therefore almost always under or overestimated [46].

To compensate the limited direct assessments of the consequence of crop foraging, one can benefit from new technology, such as camera traps which have become a frequently used tool in ecology to survey wildlife day and night at low cost [47]. This recent development has opened new avenues of research, providing a time saving and easy way for indirect monitoring, revolutionizing our understanding of nocturnal activities. Indeed, camera-traps have helped to provide reliable data on crop-feeding events even at night [14]. In addition, they allow the study of elusive animals [48, 49], to document rare-case occurring patterns (e.g. extraction of honey from underground bee nests by wild chimpanzees (P. troglodytes) [5052]), ands to carry-on broad and intensive non-invasive sampling (e.g. in chimpanzees [53, 54]). For terrestrial primates specifically, it is also a mean to study non-habituated groups [55], and minimize zoonoses [5658] meanwhile limiting poaching exposure by minimizing habituation to human presence [59]. Recently, a study using compiled data obtained via camera trapping at 22 sites in Africa confirmed the existence of nocturnal activity in chimpanzees across savannah and forested sites, at a low but non-null rate (0.14%– 9.58% of the recorded videos according to sites due to different sampling effort and openness of the habitat). Nocturnal activity was more likely at locations with lower levels of human activity, higher average daily temperature the previous day, and/or with a larger percentage of forest composing the habitat [15]. Nocturnal events in the forest were carried out by different individuals, with immatures individuals being the most present, followed by lone adult males [15]. Previously, Krief et al. [14] documented for the first time frequent nocturnal activities of chimpanzees living in a home range bordered by cultivated fields in Sebitoli, in the northern part of Kibale National Park in Uganda. Such results contrast with Tagg et al.’s study [15]. In Sebitoli, chimpanzee nocturnal activity appears paradoxically linked to human presence (i.e farming activity). In addition, active chimpanzees were male and female of all ages. However, Krief et al’s study period was short and the survey focused on a single maize plantation.

To clarify this preliminary result, we conducted a follow-up study on an extended time period in Sebitoli. We monitored the nocturnal activity of this chimpanzee community over a larger part of their home range using up to 16 camera-trap set-ups over 18 km2 during 15 months. Camera-traps were located both inside and at the forest edge, in order to compare data from other sites and to explore the influence of anthropisation of the forest edge [15]. In the case where frequent nocturnal activities of chimpanzees have been confirmed inside and at edge of the forest, we aimed to understand whether environmental conditions might have driven such a change in lifestyle in both locations, and which individuals are more likely to be involved. We therefore formulated the following (but not mutually exclusive) hypotheses, following previous studies [14, 15]:

1) Harsh meteorological conditions reduction strategy

Nocturnal activities may be more likely when harsh conditions (high temperature or heavy rains) occurred during the previous day, having reduced the opportunity to practice any activity during the day. We hypothesized that chimpanzees may delay their activities until evening or night in order to face tempered conditions, such as cooler temperatures, and thus avoid physiological stress.

2) Human avoidance strategy

i. Nocturnal activities may be a result of risk associated with and thus avoidance of humans. In Sebitoli, the main life-or-death risk faced by immature and adult chimpanzees is humans (farmers and poachers). Farmers are working in their field and actively guarding them during the day, chasing wildlife detected in, or near the fields, sometimes with spears. However, they usually do not patrol more than once or twice during the night when they are mainly vigilant for elephant (Loxodonta sp.) incursion. As such, we predicted that the categories of chimpanzees engaging in nocturnal behaviors will differ in activity and according to the location. Adult males may engage more in dangerous nocturnal activities in crop fields. On the other hand, we expected adult females with infants and juveniles to be less engaged in nocturnal activities in the fields, likewise for other vulnerable individuals such as those with physical ailments/mutilations. Furthermore, group size could be smaller when entering the gardens as a strategy to be less easily spotted by humans [46].

ii. Light levels might also be a factor shaping nocturnal activity rate. Preliminary results of the first study relating to this community seemed to indicate that chimpanzees could favor moonless nights, potentially to decrease the risk of being seen by farmers [14] and in accordance with lunar phobia seen in other cathemeral mammals [3]. Indeed, human vision is more suited for light condition, and farmers or guards in Sebitoli are not affluent (Krief, personal communication) and do not often have the financial means to be equipped with light torches. Such a hypothesis might be valid in a crop context where the habitat is open, whereas one can consider moon illumination as a facilitating condition for navigation would hold particularly in a dense habitat such as forest context, where visual abilities are already greatly impeded [60]. Thus, we predicted that the impact of moonlight would differ depending on chimpanzees location, with higher nocturnal activity in forest during bright night and higher nocturnal activity in fields during dim and waning moonlight.

3) Complement dietary requirements strategy

i. Tropical forests have traditionally been considered permanently food-rich environments [61]. Botanical investigations however revealed a more nuanced picture: for most species, trees fruit in bursts, either sub-annually or pluri-annually [62, 63] creating seasonal and inter-annual fluctuations in fruit quantity. As such, food quantity alternates from scarce to plentiful for frugivorous dominant species, such as chimpanzees in Kibale [64]. In Kibale forest (Kanyawara and Ngogo research sites) for instance, fruit availability reaches a maximum after the first wet season ends, while it is at its lowest in the preceding months, with fruiting tree density about six times lower than during peak fruiting [62]. Such seasonal low availability of wild sources of energy food in the forest may result in night feeding of highly energetic resources, as chimpanzees fail to meet their food requirement during the day. Hence, we predicted that low wild fruit availability would trigger increased night activities in both forest and crop fields.

ii. The presence of a highly nutritious food items represented by maize crops could be a strong motivation for chimpanzees to forage at night. We predicted that the presence of maize would increase nocturnal activity within the forest, as chimpanzees will travel to reach the fields if they nested far from the park edge. In parallel, we predicted that nocturnal activity in the fields would increase during the maize season.

To test these predictions, we analyzed the 1 808 clips containing chimpanzees recorded over the time period. We described chimpanzee behavior, party composition, and timing of nocturnal activity. Next, we tested correlative patterns between the probability of nocturnal activity and a set of environmental predictors (temperature, rainfall, moon illumination and food availability) using a linear modelling approach [65] separately for each habitat type (forest or cultivated fields). Overall, by providing a detailed picture of nocturnal behaviors in chimpanzees, we finally hope to contribute to a better understanding of changes to chimpanzee ecology arising from habitat modification.

Material and methods

Field site

Sebitoli chimpanzees home range is located in the North part of Kibale National Park (KNP), Western Uganda (KNP: 795 km2, 0°13’-0°41’N and 30°19’-30°32’E [66]; Fig 1). The park is a medium-altitude moist tropical forest that is described as a biodiversity hotspot [67]. The climate of this equatorial zone is typical of rainforest areas, with two rainy and two dry seasons [62]. In the Northern part of the park, the forest border is surrounded by numerous agricultural plots such as small-scale farms with food crops (72%), tea (24%) and eucalyptus plantations (4%) [68]. The forest itself is composed of regenerating forest (35%), degraded forest (35%), followed by mature forest (14%), terrestrial herbaceous vegetation (14%) and patchy shrub/wetland vegetation (1%) [69]. A tarmac road with heavy traffic divides the community’s home-range into two parts: “Sebitoli North” and “Sebitoli South” [70, 71]. A trail system consisting of 120 km of transects directed north-south and east-west was created to facilitate movements of our teams. Chimpanzees also frequently use these trails.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Map of Sebitoli chimpanzee forest home range and surrounding cultivated fields, Kibale National Park (KNP), Uganda.

Republished from [73] under a CC BY license, with permission from Revue Francophone de Primatologie, original copyright 2012.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268132.g001

Chimpanzee community

Since 2009, the Sebitoli Chimpanzee Project (SCP) team has been monitoring the chimpanzee community of the Eastern subspecies (P. troglodytes schweinfurthii) on a daily basis, which numbers around 80 individuals, 66 of which have been identified by habituation. Nevertheless, some individuals are still in the process of habituation. Crop fields are only present in the western and northern parts of Sebitoli North (Fig 1). In “Sebitoli South” (East and West) and in the eastern part of “Sebitoli North”, the fields bordering the forest were mainly composed of tea gardens at the time of the study, which are not consumed by chimpanzees. Sebitoli chimpanzees exhibited a high level of limb anomalies almost certainly caused by wire snares [71]. Using camera traps, we recognised 62 individuals (94% of identified individuals) including 13 adult males (over 15 years), 23 adult females (over 15 years), 17 immature individuals (juveniles from 3 to 9.9 years and sub-adults from 10 to 14.9 years) and 9 infants (under 3 years and dependent of their mother) [72]. In the following analyses, the 9 females with clinging infants and the 6 mutilated individuals (2 adult males, 2 adult females and 2 immatures) were classified as “vulnerable” (less likely to respond as fast to a danger because of the infant or the injured leg or arm) and thus excluded from their age-sex categories.

Data collection

Camera trap and chimpanzee behavior.

Camera traps (CT) were set from the 3rd January 2017 to the 6th April 2018 within the home range of Sebitoli chimpanzees (forest) and at its border with cultivated fields of maize (henceforth fields). Camera traps consisted of HD Reconyx XR-6 Ultrafire™ (30s-long, 4s delay, sensed up to 21m) and Bushnell Trophy Cam HD Max™ (60s-long, 5s delay, sensed up to 18m) with a day/night auto sensor and sound recordings. The cameras were not placed randomly but with the objective of maximizing the probability of capturing images of chimpanzees from head to toe in order to identify them. Any information such as a missing ear, finger or toe can help in identification. The cameras were therefore placed 1 m above the ground and at least 1 m from an open area where our field teams, who followed them on a daily basis, estimated that they would pass. In the forest, CTs were set up on junctions of open trails frequently used according to long term data regarding the core area. At the edge between the forest and the maize fields, CTs were set up when chimpanzees were reported by farmers or by our team, who on a weekly basis surveyed for footprints, food remains (eaten corns as well as wadges of stems) and/or feces. They were positioned in different fields, so we assumed the clips to be independent as they do not show the same crop visiting event. Due to logistical challenges and technical issues (e.g. camera traps damaged by elephants or poachers; memory reaching full capacity), the entire study-period was not covered for some camera traps. In the forest, 14 378 relevant clips, obtained by on average 9 camera traps per day (min = 3, max = 13), spanning 448 days with functional camera traps, that is 4002 trap days, were analyzed. In the maize fields, 4 778 relevant clips obtained by on average 3 camera traps per day (min = 1, max = 4), spanning 197 days with functional camera traps, that is 607 trap days, were analyzed. Among them, only a part was triggered by chimpanzee presence (see Results).

For each clip, we identify all animal species observed, the respective number of individuals, sex, age class, identity whenever possible. For all the individuals, we also recorded all occurrences of the following behaviors: travelling, sitting, standing, resting, grooming, feeding on wild food and cropfeeding. We summarized it in ethogram available in (S1 File). The longest activity of an individual or the most frequent activity among several individuals was considered to determine the main activity for a given clip.

Food availability.

To estimate wild food availability, we used monthly phenological surveys collected during the study period. These included 373 trees of 39 species along 10 trails of 500 m located within the chimpanzee home range. We calculated a food availability index (FAI) by quantifying fruit abundance per month, using a ‘percent basal area fruiting/ha’ method [see 69]. Finally, to determine the presence of maize, a team patrolled at the edge of the chimpanzees’ home range and made weekly estimates of the maize maturity.

Data analysis.

Two viewings of each clip (one by SCP field assistant dedicated to CT project and one by CL) were performed (S2 File). Clips were considered non-independent if they were recorded less than 15 minutes apart: they formed one “event” [15]. We estimated the party size by compiling the number of different identified individuals seen across all clips of a same event. Then, we obtained the number of chimpanzee occurrences by adding the party size estimated for each event. To each event, we associated to the given date the following descriptive variables: 1) time of sunset, sunrise and civil twilights -defined as the period before the sun reaches 6° under horizon when human eye clearly distinguishes terrestrial objects- (using the web tool https://www.timeanddate.com/sun/). We defined true night as the period of time occurred between sunset and sunrise, it was split into 4 phases (see legend Fig 2, following [15] classification), 2) the moon illumination (the measure of the incident moon light illuminating a surface (using the U.S Navy web tool http://aa.usno.navy.mil/data/index.php) 3) the FAI per day which was linearly extrapolated from the monthly value 4) the presence of maize crops in the fields 5) the mean temperature from the previous day (Chapman, personal communication) 6) the rainfall from the previous day in Kibale National Park (Chapman, personal communication) (S3 File).

thumbnail
Fig 2.

Number of chimpanzees observed with camera traps located nearby fields and within the forest of the Kibale National Park, Uganda in Sebitoli area according to the phase of the night (A) or moon illumination (B). A: To be comparable between fields and forest and times of year, the recording time of the clips was scaled from 0 to 1 based on the time difference from sunset on that day (or the previous day for clips captured after midnight) and night duration (Time of the night = (Tvideo—Tsunset)/night duration). Each dot represents a video with sized by the maximum number of individuals observed, respectively for field or forest habitats. The vertical gray line indicates approximately an hour. Night time was separated into four phases represented by the horizontal black bars at the bottom: phase 1 (1 h after/before sunset/sunrise), phase 2 (within 2 h of twilight); phase 3 (2 h of early morning and late night), phase 4 (2 h around midnight represented by a dotted line). B: Each dot represents a video with and is sized by the maximum number of individuals observed, respectively for field or forest locations. The vertical gray lines indicate the moon illumination, while the bottom illustration depicts their equivalence in “moon phase”.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268132.g002

Statistical analyses

In order to investigate whether environmental, risk avoidance or food constraints influenced chimpanzee nocturnal behavior (hypotheses 1, 2 and 3 respectively, see Introduction), we analyzed the joint effect of variables describing these constraints on the probability of observing nocturnal activity in the forest and in the fields separately (over 448 nights for the forest model and 197 nights for the garden model). As such, we used a generalized linear model approach (GLM) [74, 75], with binomial structure and a logit link error function [75] since the output variable was whether we observed nocturnal behavior on a given night. To control for sampling effort, we integrated the number of camera trap active at the observation data as an offset variable in the model. In order to test for the harsh conditions reduction strategy hypothesis (hypothesis 1), we included as explanatory variables, rainfall intensity (total mm of rain) and mean daytime temperature (in Celsius) of the day preceding the night as tested predictors. In order to test for the human avoidance strategy hypothesis (hypothesis 2), we included the moon illumination of the night of interest as a supplementary tested predictor since this variable might represent perceptibility. In order to test the food-complementation strategy hypothesis (hypothesis 3), we included the FAI per day for the period corresponding to the night of interest, as a tested predictor. For the presence of maize (binary variable, “yes” or “no”), it was only integrated into the forest model, as chimpanzee’ visits to the fields only happened when maize crops were present. Furthermore, we considered that foraging strategy in a given environment depends simultaneously on food availability and human risk. Hence, we put initially the variables moon illumination and FAI in interaction. Similarly, as we expected raid frequency to be related to environmental conditions, e.g by increasing concurrent with severity of conditions, we initially interacted maize presence with temperature and rainfall for the forest model.

To allow comparison among estimates, all continuous variables were scaled to a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1 (i.e. z-transformation [76]). Previously, when data were skewed, we applied transformations in order to reach distributions that are more symmetrical. This was notably true for rainfall that was log transformed. However, the variable remained right-queued. As removing extreme (but still reliable) high rainfall values did not substantially change the output, we present results from the entire data. We confirmed there was no major internal correlation issues based on the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) (max VIF < 2.06 in the forest model and < 1.13 in the field model [77]), using the “vif” function from the “car” package [78].

While all models initially included all the above-mentioned interactions, we presented the latest version of the models with only the significant interactions.

We assessed the joint effect of variables by comparing the full model, including the tested predictors as well as the control variables, to the null model, including only control variables or a constant if none [79], using a likelihood-ratio test (R-function “anova” set to “Chisq” [75]). In case this comparison was significant, we then tested for the singular effect of each variable by comparing the full model deviance and the deviance of a reduced model [79] excluding the variable of interest one at a time, using “drop1” function [80]. In order to account for multiple testing downside, we maintained the false discovery rate to the nominal value of 0.05 using Benjamini and Liu’s second procedure [81]. For each model analysis separately, that is considering p-values given by the “drop1” function and the likelihood ratio test between the full and null model, non-significant interactions were discarded once. We qualify a variable as significant only if it occurred below the corrected threshold following this last procedure.

We visually checked the models’ assumption (e.g. homogeneous distribution of residuals vs fitted values), and assessed the models stability using various parameters such as leverage values (forest model: 0.031; fields model: 0.061), the maximum Cook’s distance (forest model: 0.079; garden model: 0.088), the maximum Dffits (forest model: 0.569; fields model: 0.801) and the maximum DFBetas (forest model: 0.102; fields model: 0.168). It indicated no obviously influential cases [82] (S4 File). Furthermore, overdispersion was not an issue in the forest model (overdispersion test, parameter = 1.832, χ2 = 809.715, df = 442, p-value<0.001) or the fields model (overdispersion test, parameter = 1.439, χ2 = 276.236, df = 192, p-value<0.001).

All statistical analyses were performed in R software, version 3.3.2 [83].

Ethical guidelines

This research was conducted in the context of the Memorandum of Understanding MNHN/UWA/Makerere University SJ 445–12 following the guidelines of the Uganda Wildlife Authority (UWA).

Results

Active time distribution

Out of the 19 156 relevant clips recorded during the study period, 1 808 (9.4%) clips involved chimpanzees were pooled in 939 events. In the forest, out of the 796 events involving chimpanzees, 36 (4.6%) happened during night time. In parallel, at the edge of the fields, out of 143 chimpanzees events, 57 (39.9%) were recorded at night (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Number of clips and events recorded in the forest and at the edge of the gardens during day and night.

A total of 14 camera-traps were used over 2828 days from 03 January 2017 to 06 April 2018 in Sebitoli area, Kibale National Park, Uganda.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268132.t001

At the forest-fields edge, nocturnal activity occurred mainly in the early hours of the true night, especially in the phase 2 of the night (i.e. between one and two hours after sunset). No event occurred after 22h and no chimpanzees were detected in the early morning at the edge of the fields (Fig 2A). Inside the forest, nocturnal activities recorded were more frequent in twilight than in true night (26 vs 10 out of 36 events) (Table 1). Most nocturnal activity occurred in the early morning, within an hour before sunrise. In addition, some nocturnal activities occurred during the deep phase of the night (at 00h38 and at 01h40) (Fig 2A).

Activity allocation during night time

Locomotion and foraging activities were the most frequent behavior recorded at night. 144 out of 284 (50.7%) recorded behavior occurrences corresponded to travelling and 129 (45.4%) corresponded to foraging- (Table 2). Nocturnal activities were significantly different according to the habitat (χ2 = 135.280, df = 3, p-value<0.001) with “travelling” as the most frequent activity in the forest. 93 out of 99 (93.3%) nocturnal activities were travelling occurrences in the forest and 51 out of 185 (27.6%) in the garden. No feeding activities occurred in the forest while cropfeeding represented129 (69.7%) of behavior occurrences in the garden). In fact, foraging behaviors on wild food was not observed at night in the forest. Foraging behaviors were only observed near fields and were related to maize consumption.

thumbnail
Table 2. Frequency of occurrence of chimpanzee nocturnal activity in function age-class and sex.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268132.t002

Group size and composition during night events

Party size observed within the forest or at the forest-field interface did not differ between night and day, whether or not we removed those involving only one individual (all t-test had a p-value > 0.05, Table 3). However, when distinguishing habitats, there were more lone individuals at night in the forest (chi-square test, χ2 = 10.872, df = 3, p-value = 0.012, Table 3). Despite this, events in the fields (day and night combined) included significantly fewer chimpanzees on average than in the forest (t-test, with lone individuals: t = 1.737, df = 208.75, p-value = 0.083; without lone individuals: t = 2.094, df = 140.53, p-value = 0.038), which suggests considerably lower party-size in the field on average. To determine if a given category, and more particularly vulnerable individuals such as mutilated chimpanzees or females with dependent offspring and immatures, were recorded more often on CT during night event in the gardens to prevent risks of being detected we determined the rate at which each chimpanzee category (see Methods) was seen (Fig 3) and compared it to the community composition (z-test with the Benjamini’s False Decovery Rate (FDR) second correction [82], the significance is therefore indicated relative to the new threshold of 0.002 to maintain the FDR at the nominal value of 0.05). On all camera clips, adult males were the most highly encountered category of individuals, especially at night in the forest compared to their proportion in the community (z-test, χ2 = 61.174, df = 1, p-value<0.001). Immature individuals of both sexes, adult females with clinging infants and mutilated individuals were equivalently seen day and night or in both habitats compared to their proportion in the community (z-tests, all p-value>0.002). Interestingly, adult females without clinging infants were less represented in clips compared to their distribution in the community (z-tests, all p-value<0.002) with the exception of clips in the forest during daytime (z-test, χ2 = 5.265, df = 1, p-value = 0.022).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Ratio of occurrences during nocturnal activity of chimpanzees in function age-class, sex and mutilations according to the daytime and the habitat (the individuals not identified have been excluded).

Bars represent the percentage of occurrences of age-sex classes during day (light gray) and night (dark gray) time. The dotted bars depict the percentage of an age-sex class in the Sebitoli chimpanzees’ community. Adults are non-mutilated individuals of over 15 years [84]; infants (under 3 years old) are dependent of their mother and counted together; immatures are healthy juveniles (from 3 to 9.9 years old) and sub-adults (from 10 to 14.9 years old) [84]; mutilated individuals of all age and class excluded from the other categories.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268132.g003

thumbnail
Table 3. Party size and mean group size in function of time condition (day or night) and habitat.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268132.t003

Environmental factors influencing nocturnal activity

Here we tested using a generalized linear model whether temperature, rainfall, moon illumination and/or food availability (wild and domestic) influenced the probability of capturing chimpanzee nighttime activity, according to the habitat type (forest or fields) and taking into account the sampling effort represented by the number of active camera traps.

The full model differed significantly from the null model in the forest habitat (χ2 = 16.024, df = 5, p-value = 0.007) and at the interface between forest and fields (χ2 = 13.159, df = 4, p-value = 0.011). All initially included interactions, whether triple or pairwise, did not have a significant impact on the model response and were therefore removed (Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 4. Results of the GLM on the presence of chimpanzees at night within the forest (model 1) or then the fields surrounding the national park (model 2).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268132.t004

Harsh meteorological conditions hypothesis.

Nocturnal activities both in the forest and fields were more frequent when daytime temperatures were higher (forest estimate±sd: 0.464±0.227, χ2 = 4.189, df = 1, p-value = 0.041; garden estimate±sd: 0.674±0.235, χ2 = 8.769, df = 1, p-value = 0.003) (Table 4 and Fig 4). On the other hand, rainfall during the day had no impact on the probability to detect chimpanzees at night (forest: χ2 = 2.391, df = 1, p-value = 0.122, fields: χ2 = 0.478, df = 1, p-value = 0.489) (Table 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Influence of temperature (°C) on the probability of observing nocturnal activity by camera traps in chimpanzees from Kibale National Park, Uganda according to the habitat (forest or fields).

Left is forest and right is field habitat. Points represent the raw data either under binary form (white points) or averaged (black points). The plain black line depicts the response of the model. It depicts the influence of the temperature while averaging the effects of all other variables. Namely, continuous z-transformed variables were set to 0, while categorical predictor habitat type was dummy coded and z-transformed, and finally also set to 0. The gray polygon indicates the 95% confidence interval, borders are depicted by the black dashed lines.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268132.g004

Human avoidance or inter-species competition.

Moon illumination had no influence on chimpanzee’s nocturnal activity rate in either the forest or the fields (forest: χ2 = 0.066, df = 1, p-value = 0.798, garden: χ2 = 0.109, df = 1, p-value = 0.742) (Table 4). Chimpanzees showed nocturnal activities for all possible lunar phases (Fig 2B).

Complement dietary requirements hypothesis.

i. The FAI, a proxy for estimating food abundance, varied between 0.49 and 2.30 during the study period (mean±sd = 1.28±0.48). It did not significantly improve our model fit and was not correlated with nighttime events (forest: χ2 = 0.116, df = 1, p-value = 0.734, fields: χ2 = 0.915, df = 1, p-value = 0.339; Table 4 and Fig 5).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Monthly representation of meteorological factors, food abundance and nocturnal events of chimpanzees according to the location of the camera trap (forest or garden) in Kibale National Park, Uganda.

A: Bars represent the precipitation in mm per month. The plain black line depicts the mean temperature per month over the study period. B: Bars represent the monthly ratio obtained with the number of nocturnal events by chimpanzees taking into account the sampling effort with trap days (CTDay) according to their location (dark gray represents gardens and light gray represents forest). The dashed black line depicts the food availability index of ripe fruits (FAI) each month over the study period. The maize pictogram illustrates the monthly presence of maize in fields.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268132.g005

ii. Data obtained from SCP patrols on maize availability (7 months out of the 15 of the study period) and chimpanzee presence in the fields showed that they visited the fields only during the maize season. However, within the forest the presence of maize did not influence the rate of nocturnal activity (χ2 = 1.238, df = 1, p-value = 0.266; Table 4).

Discussion

Our study aimed to document the nocturnal behavior of wild chimpanzees in the context of a forested ecosystem in a protected area surrounded by agriculture. Among the plantations, maize is a highly nutritious and attractive crop, but farmers actively protect their fields from wildlife incursion during daylight hours. We investigated whether this context may explain the high frequency of nocturnal activity observed in a preliminary study in this particular location [14] compared to surveys conducted in 22 other sites [15] where nocturnal activities were less frequent. Our data collection spanned 15 consecutive months, using camera-traps set up within the core area and at the forest-maize fields interface. The data showed that the proportion of nocturnal activity within the forest zone (3.30%) was within the range of that observed in other sites also using camera traps (1.80% the mean of 22 African sites [15]. However, when examining the camera traps set at the forest-field edge, we discovered a very high proportion of nocturnal activity (41.80%), raising the total proportion of nocturnal occurrences within and at the edge of the Sebitoli’s home range to 12.90%, the highest proportion ever recorded in the chimpanzee sites studied (9.58% was the highest frequency in Kayan, Senegal [15]). Our results partially validated some initial predictions: 1) the harsh meteorological conditions reduction strategy where more nocturnal activity in forest and garden occurred after hot days but not after heavy rainfalls, 2) the human avoidance strategy where smaller groups entered the garden at night but nocturnal activity overall was not related to moon illumination but 3) the complement dietary requirements strategy was not confirmed as nocturnal activities in the forest was not correlated to the presence of maize, and nocturnal activities in both locations were not correlated with the wild fruit availability.

Primate sleep patterns are highly variable across species, and might be the result of compromises between necessary good sleep quality and favorable foraging conditions, for instance with reduced predation risk or clement environmental conditions [85, 86]. At night, the main activity happened at the beginning of the night, this activity being feeding on maize crops, a high calorific resource at the edge of the forest [14]. This resource is not easily accessible during the day without risk to life, due to farmers actively protecting their fields. This activity pattern was unmatched in the forest. There, chimpanzees mostly travelled around twilight, presumably to reach their nesting site. For both habitats, we observed that nocturnal behaviors coincided with high day-time temperatures, which has been noted in other chimpanzee communities (Fongoli community in Senegal [24]; 22 communities across Africa [15]) and for other cathemeral species (Eulemur macao [7]; Eulemur rubriventer and Eulemur fulvus rufus [87]; Eulemur collaris [88]). However, this propensity for being active at night remained unaffected by rainfall. Although these results closely resemble Taggs et al. [15], they are puzzling since heavy rains inhibits chimpanzee activities during the day as they are reluctant to move on wet ground [89], thus compressing their foraging window. However, the many arboreal supports in this forest can offer protection and the opportunity to move around despite heavy rainfalls. Conversely, the potential stress of low rainfall has been demonstrated in a dry savanna context [9092] and may also occur in the future for this studied community, but quite unlikely. Indeed, Uganda had experienced a temperature increase of 0.15–0.5°C every decade since 1960 [93, 94], rainfall had decreased by 28% over the last 32 years and the dry season is becoming longer in the Northern part of Kibale National Park [95]. Nonetheless, in this mid-altitude forest crossed by two main rivers, the stress of low rainfall may not be felt by chimpanzees as water sources are present.

Since visibility decreases at night for farmers guarding the fields, primates’ nocturnal activity has also been assumed to be a strategy to reduce the risk associated with human interaction. Because they also rely primarily on vision for foraging (e.g illumination intensity ranges between 1 to 85 lux for feeding activity and full-moon nights offer 0.3 lux [96]), the frequency of such nocturnal activity should not be equal across all nights, but positively correlated with moon illumination [14, 18, 19, 25, 97, 98]. Here, contradicting with our predictions, we did not observe a linear variation in the frequency of nocturnal activity in the forest or in the fields, with moon illumination. In deep forest, it is certain that even a full-moon would not facilitate activities such as feeding, which usually requires substantial illumination to be easily performed [96]. In fields, an open habitat, although chimpanzees might benefit more significantly from higher lunar illumination, the lack of variation in nocturnal activity with moon phases could be due to the fact that the main attraction driver is maize maturity, for which there is no reason to correlate with lunar phases. In addition, our data could be biased by the fact that clouds were not taken into account and could have greatly influenced the actual light transmitted to the ground.

Despite the common misconception that the tropical environment is permanently food rich, there are fluctuations in overall food availability [63]. It can result in variation in calorific intakes for some species unless they diversify their diet over time (e.g. in western lowland gorillas [99]). In the case of chimpanzees, they have generally diversified their diet to the point of incorporating crop foods [35, 40] as they have been exposed to agriculture for a significant amount of time (earliest evidence for cultivation in western Uganda reported at 4,800 BP [100]). Chimpanzees following the optimal foraging strategies can switch from one food source to another to maintain their energy balance, and we therefore expected that chimpanzees would rely more on crops when wild fruits are less available [39, 40, 45, 101]. Yet, as in Tweheyo et al. [102], crop-foraging events occurred when plenty of fruits were available. Indeed, the FAI, the index used to estimate availability of fruits, had no impact on the likelihood of chimpanzees foraging maize. If the quantity argument does not seem that significant, it could ultimately be explained by the fact that maize fields are a reliable, more palatable and nutritive food sources [37, 38] since they contain less non-digestible fibers, more protein [103] and are more energy dense [14] while necessitating less handling overall and no climbing when compared to wild food [104]. No matter the availability of such wild food, feeding from these more nutrient-rich crops would still offset the cost associated with foraging in the fields to a much greater extent than, wild food items [105]. In our study, evidence of chimpanzees in the fields only appeared during maize season. More raids happened during the true night, as opposed to the twilight hours, which is consistent with the estimated reduced vigilance of farmers. In addition, reducing the cost associated with foraging can be mitigated not only by better timing incursions to the crop fields, but also by minimizing the travelled distance to access these areas. Given that chimpanzees favor night field raids, and generally sleep in nests that they build at the end of the day [106], we hypothesize that they would actually nest closer to maize gardens. This should explain why we do not witness an increase of nocturnal activity in the forest during the maize season. The decision to sleep a full night or to wake up for nocturnal activity appeared to be a trade-off [107, 108] and can be altered depending upon changing environment, as the opportunity to feed become advantageous at night by avoiding human threats (confirming [14]).

In Sebitoli, the major risk to chimpanzees comes from spears of farmers that may injure them defending their crops if they see them [104], as they are not involved in active hunting and tracking of prey in the forest. This situation can be associated to a predation risk for the chimpanzees. To minimize this risk, grouped individuals can form larger group making it less likely that any given individual will be preyed upon (dilution effect) [109, 110]. However, in the specific case of these raids, having more individuals equates to a greater chance of being spotted by patrolling people [44]. Here, we did observe that in areas where the risk was highest (e.g gardens, day or night), party size was significantly reduced compared to party size in the forest (5 vs 4 individuals without the lone individuals) despite lone individuals were more frequent in the forest, especially adult males [in accordance with 15]. This may be due to them being more likely to engage in nightly territory patrols [111]. This suggests that small groups of two to three individuals attempted to forage in these crops, particularly males who accounted for 31% of individuals observed in garden whereas they account for less than 18% of community’s individuals and are avoided by adult females in general (21% of individuals in garden vs 48% in the community). It has been suggested that it could be a delayed food-for-sex strategy where they showed prowess and enhance affiliative relationships by sharing the crops with females inside the forest [112]. It has also been shown that males occupy a wider range than females [113]. Further investigation has shown that some females who occupy smaller home range away from the peripherical gardens have lower access to them, and this could be the drivers of such a pattern [Couturier et al, in press]. On the other hand, mutilated individuals and immatures were represented according to their distribution in the community and equally during the day and in both habitats. What we therefore assumed to be an increase in their vulnerability, notably to farmers, did not in fact impact on their pattern of space-use or activity, as has been shown for their activities inside the forest. The risk perception may differ across age and/or sex [114].

In conclusion, human activities influence the behavior, the ecology and the sociology of species in many ways [115]. For example, morphological differences and altered life history traits between wild and more urban populations of certain species have been observed [116, 117]. Here, we provide some evidence that behaviors can be impacted as well, based on the day-night foraging pattern of chimpanzees. Behavioral plasticity may in fact buffer habitat disturbances, and might explain why wild chimpanzee populations persist in anthropized environments despite a slow lifecycle [118]. Nonetheless, if we focus here on how wild chimpanzees derive nutritional benefit from human crops, a short-term view presents living in the vicinity of humans as beneficial, however medium to long term views present the downsides of increased exposure to contamination by pathogens from farmers [119] and their livestock, the risks of injury or death by farmers protecting their crops [105]. In addition, in Sebitoli, in the northern part of Kibale National Park, the use of pesticides in the surrounding crops and tea plantations may also have serious consequences for primate health, with numerous possibly related malformations in baboons (P. anubis) and chimpanzees [120, 121]. Indeed, 13 pesticides were found in Sebitoli water samples that impact on the hormonal system and are possible endocrine disruptors [120, 122]. Further studies need to investigate the impact on the fertility and development of the Sebitoli community over a longer period. In order to prevent crop feeding by chimpanzees, farmers could strengthen their vigilance during the night not only for elephant intrusions but also for chimpanzees, and/or develop effective 24-hour deterrence methods. In addition, we recommend rethinking land-use near the forest interface to reconcile wildlife conservation and human development by relocating maize fields and growing unpalatable crops at the park’s edge to prevent both economic losses and injuries or kills of endangered species. This survey emphasizes the urgent need to work with local communities on the issue of human-wildlife conflict.

Supporting information

S1 File. Ethogram of the chimpanzee main activity recorded in the clip.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268132.s001

(DOCX)

S2 File. Raw data of video clips information collected and the chimpanzees identified.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268132.s002

(XLSX)

S3 File. List of the daily environmental information.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268132.s003

(CSV)

S4 File. Graphics of the residual diagnostics for the two generalized linear models and the DFBetas results.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268132.s004

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the Uganda Wildlife Authority, Uganda National Council for Science and Technology for permission to conduct research at Sebitoli in Kibale NP, Uganda. We offer profound thanks to Jean-Michel Krief, co-director of Great Ape Conservation Project, Nelson Fawoh and John Paul Okimat, the managers, the field assistants of the Sebitoli Chimpanzee Project: Deogratius Kiomuhangi in charge of the phenology data collection, Emmanuel Balinda, Joseph Alinaitwe, Ibrahim Nyakana, Wilson Muzahura, Edward Kalyegira, Nelson Tugume and in particular Robert Asyimwe, Robert Nyakahuma and Daniela Birungi for their precious work with the camera-traps. We thank Clovice Alikonyera, Philip Musinguzi and Charles Twesige for their weekly maize patrols. We thank Sarah Bortolamiol for providing the layers of the map and Chloé Couturier for the compilation of Food Avalaibility Index. Finally, we thank Petra Spirhanzlova for her comments and insights.

References

  1. 1. Bennie JJ, Duffy JP, Inger R, Gaston KJ. Biogeography of time partitioning in mammals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111(38):13727–13732. pmid:25225371
  2. 2. Halle S, Stenseth NC. Activity patterns in small mammals: an ecological approach. Springer. 2012;141.
  3. 3. Bearder SK, Nekaris KAI, Curtis DJ. A re-evaluation of the role of vision in the activity and communication of nocturnal primates. Folia Primatol. 2006;77(1–2):50–71. pmid:16415577
  4. 4. Curtis DJ, Rasmussen MA. The evolution of cathemerality in primates and other mammals: a comparative and chronoecological approach. Folia Primatol. 2006;77(1–2):178–193. pmid:16415585
  5. 5. Tattersall I. Cathemeral activity in primates: a definition. Folia Primatol. 1987.
  6. 6. Tattersall I. Patterns of activity in the mayotte lemur, Lemur fulvus mayottensis. J Mammal. 1979;60:314–323.
  7. 7. Andrews JR, Birkinshaw CR. A comparison between the daytime and night time diet, activity and feeding height of the black lemur, Eulemur macaco (primates: Lemuridae), in lokobe forest, Madagascar. Folia Primatol. 1998;69(Suppl1):175–182.
  8. 8. Colquhoun IC. Cathemeral behavior of Eulemur macaco macaco at Ambato Massif, Madagascar. Folia Primatol. 1998;69(Suppl1):22–34.
  9. 9. Struhsaker TT. The Red Colobus Monkey. Chicago: Univ; 1975.
  10. 10. Isbell LA, Bidner LR, Crofoot MC, Matsumoto‐Oda A, Farine DR. GPS‐identified, low‐level nocturnal activity of vervets (Chlorocebus pygerythrus) and olive baboons (Papio anubis) in Laikipia, Kenya. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2017;164(1):203–211. pmid:28573721
  11. 11. Kawai M, Iwamoto T. Nomadism and activities. In: Kawai M(ed) Ecological and sociological studies of gelada baboons. Karger, Basel; 1979. p. 251–278.
  12. 12. Nishikawa M, Mochida K. Coprophagy-related interspecific nocturnal interactions between Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) and sika deer (Cervus nippon yakushimae). Primates. 2010;51(2):95–99. pmid:20052604
  13. 13. MacKinnon J. The behaviour and ecology of wild orang-utans (Pongo pygmaeus). Anim Behav. 1974;22(1):3–74.
  14. 14. Krief S, Cibot M, Bortolamiol S, Seguya A, Krief JM, Masi S. Wild chimpanzees on the edge: nocturnal activities in croplands. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e109925. pmid:25338066
  15. 15. Tagg N, McCarthy M, Dieguez P, Bocksberger G, Willie J, Mundry R, et al. Nocturnal activity in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): evidence for flexible sleeping patterns and insights into human evolution. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2018;166(3):510–529. pmid:29989158
  16. 16. Hammerschmidt K, Ansorge V, Fischer J, Todt D (1994). Dusk calling in Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus): demand for social shelter. Am J Primatol. 1994;32(4):277–289. pmid:32070077
  17. 17. Schaller GE. The mountain gorilla: ecology and behavior. 1963.
  18. 18. Izawa K, Itani J. Chimpanzees in Kasakati Basin. Tanganyika(I): Eco. 1966.
  19. 19. van Lawick-Goodall J. The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Anim Behav monogr. 1968;1:161–IN12.
  20. 20. Tutin CE, McGrew WC, Baldwin PJ. Responses of wild chimpanzees to potential predators. In Primate behavior and sociobiology. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg; 1981. p. 136–141.
  21. 21. Zamma K. What makes wild chimpanzees wake up at night?. Primates. 2014;55(1):51–57. pmid:23817693
  22. 22. Curtis DJ, Zaramody A, Martin RD. Cathemerality in the mongoose lemur, Eulemur mongoz. Am J Primatol. 1999;47(4):279–298. pmid:10206207
  23. 23. Pruetz JD, Bertolani P. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) behavioral responses to stresses associated with living in a savanna-mosaic environment: implications for hominin adaptations to open habitats. PaleoAnthropology. 2009;252.
  24. 24. Colquhoun IC. Predation and cathemerality. Folia Primatol. 2006;77(1–2):143–165. pmid:16415583
  25. 25. Hill RA. Why be diurnal? Or, why not be cathemeral?. Folia Primatol. 2006;77(1–2):72–86.
  26. 26. Tan CL, Yang Y, Niu K. Into the night: camera traps reveal nocturnal activity in a presumptive diurnal primate, Rhinopithecus brelichi. Primates. 2013;54(1):1–6. pmid:22843040
  27. 27. Engqvist A, Richard A. Diet as a possible determinant of cathemeral activity patterns in primates. Folia Primatol. 1991;57(3):169–172. pmid:1665469
  28. 28. Donati G, Baldi N, Morelli V, Ganzhorn JU, Borgognini-Tarli SM. Proximate and ultimate determinants of cathemeral activity in brown lemurs. Anim Behav. 2009;77(2):317–325.
  29. 29. Aquino R, Encarnación F. Characteristics and use of sleeping sites in Aotus (Cebidae: Primates) in the Amazon lowlands of Peru. Am J Primatol. 1986;11(4):319–331. pmid:31979438
  30. 30. Videan EN. Bed‐building in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the importance of early rearing. Am J Primatol. 2006;68(7):745–751. pmid:16786520
  31. 31. Estrada A, Garber PA, Rylands AB, Roos C, Fernandez-Duque E, Di Fiore A, et al. Impending extinction crisis of the world’s primates: why primates matter. Sci Adv. 2017;3(1):e1600946. pmid:28116351
  32. 32. Lamb CT, Ford AT, McLellan BN, Proctor MF, Mowat G, Ciarniello L, et al. The ecology of human–carnivore coexistence. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2020;117(30):17876–17883. pmid:32632004
  33. 33. Seaman DJ, Bernard H, Ancrenaz M, Coomes D, Swinfield T, Milodowski DT, et al. Densities of Bornean orang‐utans (Pongo pygmaeus morio) in heavily degraded forest and oil palm plantations in Sabah, Borneo. Am J Primatol. 2019;81(8):e23030. pmid:31328289
  34. 34. Acheampong EO, Macgregor CJ, Sloan S, Sayer J. Deforestation is driven by agricultural expansion in Ghana’s forest reserves. Sci Afr. 2019;5:e00146.
  35. 35. Hiser KL. Crop raiding and conflict: farmers’ perceptions of human-wildlife interactions in Hoima district, Uganda. Ph.D. dissertation, Oxford Brookes University. 2012.
  36. 36. McLennan MR, Hill CM. Troublesome neighbours: changing attitudes towards chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in a human-dominated landscape in Uganda. J Nat Conserv. 2012;20(4):219–227.
  37. 37. Campbell‐Smith G, Simanjorang HV, Leader‐Williams N, Linkie M. Local attitudes and perceptions toward crop‐raiding by orangutans (Pongo abelii) and other nonhuman primates in northern Sumatra, Indonesia. Am J Primatol. 2010;72(10):866–876. pmid:20301138
  38. 38. Seiler N, Robbins MM. Factors influencing ranging on community land and crop raiding by mountain gorillas. Anim Conserv. 2016;19(2):176–188.
  39. 39. Hockings KJ, Anderson JR, Matsuzawa T. Use of wild and cultivated foods by chimpanzees at Bossou, Republic of Guinea: feeding dynamics in a human‐influenced environment. Am J Primatol. 2009;71(8):636–646. pmid:19431200
  40. 40. McLennan MR. Diet and feeding ecology of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in Bulindi, Uganda:foraging strategies at the forest–farm interface. Int J Primatol. 2013;34(3):585–614.
  41. 41. McLennan MR, Hockings KJ. Wild chimpanzees show group differences in selection of agricultural crops. Sci Rep. 2014;4:5956. pmid:25090940
  42. 42. Hyeroba D, Apell P, Otali E. Managing a speared alpha male chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) in Kibale National Park, Uganda. Vet Rec. 2011;169(25):658–658. pmid:21984563
  43. 43. Zak AA, Riley EP. Comparing the use of camera traps and farmer reports to study crop feeding behavior of moor macaques (Macaca maura). Int J Primatol. 2017;38(2):224–242.
  44. 44. Hill CM. Conflict of interest between people and baboons: crop raiding in Uganda. International journal of primatology. 2000;21(2):299–315.
  45. 45. Naughton‐Treves L, Treves A, Chapman C, Wrangham R. Temporal patterns of crop‐raiding by primates: linking food availability in croplands and adjacent forest. J Appl Ecol. 1998;35(4):596–606.
  46. 46. Wallace GE. Monkeys in maize: primate crop-raiding behaviour and developing on-farm techniques to mitigate human-wildlife conflict. Ph.D. dissertation, Oxford Brookes University. 2010.
  47. 47. O’Connell AF, Nichols JD, Karanth KU. Camera traps in animal ecology: methods and analyses. Springer Science & Business Media; 2010.
  48. 48. Sanderson JG, Trolle M. Monitoring elusive mammals: unattended cameras reveal secrets of some of the world’s wildest places. Am Sci. 2005;93(2):148–155.
  49. 49. Kelly MJ. Design, evaluate, refine: camera trap studies for elusive species. Anim Conserv. 2008;11(3):182–184.
  50. 50. Estienne V, Mundry R, Kühl HS, Boesch C. Exploitation of underground bee nests by three sympatric consumers in Loango National Park, Gabon. Biotropica. 2017;49(1):101–109.
  51. 51. Estienne V, Stephens C, Boesch C. Extraction of honey from underground bee nests by central African chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) in Loango National Park, Gabon: techniques and individual differences. Am J Primatol. 2017;79(8):e22672. pmid:28464365
  52. 52. Estienne V, Robira B, Mundry R, Deschner T, Boesch C. Acquisition of a complex extractive technique by the immature chimpanzees of Loango National Park, Gabon. Anim Behav. 2019;147:61–76.
  53. 53. Després‐Einspenner ML, Howe EJ, Drapeau P, Kühl HS. An empirical evaluation of camera trapping and spatially explicit capture‐recapture models for estimating chimpanzee density. Am J Primatol. 2017;79(7):e22647. pmid:28267880
  54. 54. Cappelle N, Després‐Einspenner ML, Howe EJ, Boesch C, Kühl HS. Validating camera trap distance sampling for chimpanzees. Am J Primatol. 2019;81(3):e22962. pmid:30811079
  55. 55. Head JS, Robbins MM, Mundry R, Makaga L, Boesch C. Remote video-camera traps measure habitat use and competitive exclusion among sympatric chimpanzee, gorilla and elephant in Loango National Park, Gabon. J Trop Ecol. 2012;28(6):571–583.
  56. 56. Epstein JH, Price JT. The significant but understudied impact of pathogen transmission from humans to animals. Mt Sinai J Med. 2009;76(5):448–455. pmid:19787650
  57. 57. Köndgen S, Kühl H, N’Goran PK, Walsh PD, Schenk S, Ernst N, et al. Pandemic human viruses cause decline of endangered great apes. Curr Biol. 2008;18(4):260–264. pmid:18222690
  58. 58. Woodford MH, Butynski TM, Karesh WB. Habituating the great apes: the disease risks. Oryx. 2002;36(2):153–160.
  59. 59. Robbins MM, Boesch C. Among African apes: Stories and photos from the field. Univ of California Press; 2011.
  60. 60. Pariente G. The role of vision in prosimian behavior. The study of prosimian behavior; 1979. p. 411–459.
  61. 61. Terborgh J. Community aspects of frugivory in tropical forests. In Frugivores and seed dispersal. Springer, Dordrecht; 1986. p. 371–384.
  62. 62. Chapman CA, Wrangham RW, Chapman LJ, Kennard DK, Zanne AE. Fruit and flower phenology at two sites in Kibale National Park, Uganda. J Trop Ecol. 1999;15(2):189–211.
  63. 63. Adamescu GS, Plumptre AJ, Abernethy KA, Polansky L, Bush ER, Chapman CA, et al. Annual cycles are the most common reproductive strategy in African tropical tree communities. Biotropica. 2018;50(3):418–430.
  64. 64. Watts DP, Potts KB, Lwanga JS, Mitani JC. Diet of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda, 1. Diet composition and diversity. Am J Primatol. 2012;74(2):114–129. pmid:22109938
  65. 65. Zuur A, Ieno EN, Walker N, Saveliev AA, Smith GM. Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. Springer Science & Business Media; 2009.
  66. 66. Chapman CA, Lambert JE. Habitat alteration and the conservation of African primates: case study of Kibale National Park, Uganda. Am J Primatol. 2000;50(3):169–185. pmid:10711532
  67. 67. Treves A, Mwima P, Plumptre AJ, Isoke S. Camera-trapping forest–woodland wildlife of western Uganda reveals how gregariousness biases estimates of relative abundance and distribution. Biol Conserv. 2010;143(2):521–528.
  68. 68. Bortolamiol S, Krief S, Jiguet F, Palibrk M, Rwaburindore P, Kasenene J, et al. Spatial analysis of natural and anthropogenic factors influencing chimpanzee repartition in Sebitoli (Kibale National Park, Uganda). International cartographic conference proceedings. 2013.
  69. 69. Bortolamiol S, Cohen M, Potts K, Pennec F, Rwaburindore P, Kasenene J, et al. Suitable habitats for endangered frugivorous mammals: small-scale comparison, regeneration forest and chimpanzee density in Kibale National Park, Uganda. PloS one. 2014;9(7):e102177. pmid:25033459
  70. 70. Cibot M, Bortolamiol S, Seguya A, Krief S. Chimpanzees facing a dangerous situation: a high‐traffic asphalted road in the Sebitoli area of Kibale National Park, Uganda. Am J Primatol. 2015;77(8):890–900. pmid:25864720
  71. 71. Krief S, Iglesias-Gonzalez A, Appenzeller BMR, Okimat JP, Fini JB, Demeneix B, et al. Road impact in a protected area with rich biodiversity: The case of the Sebitoli road in Kibale National Park, Uganda. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. Int. 2020.
  72. 72. Cibot M, Krief S, Philippon J, Couchoud P, Seguya A, Pouydebat E. Feeding consequences of hand and foot disability in wild adult chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Int J Primatol. 2016;37(4–5):479–494.
  73. 73. Bortolamiol S, Cohen M, Palibrk M, Krief S. La répartition des chimpanzés à Sebitoli (Parc National de Kibale, Ouganda): influence des facteurs naturels et anthropiques. Rev. primatol. 2012;4.
  74. 74. McCullagh P, Nelder JA. Generalized Linear Models. 2nd ed. New York: Chapman and Hall; 1989.
  75. 75. Dobson AJ, Barnett AG. An introduction to generalized linear models. Chapman and Hall/CRC. 2018.
  76. 76. Schielzeth H. Simple means to improve the interpretability of regression coefficients. Methods Ecol Evol. 2010;1(2):103–113.
  77. 77. Field A. Discovering statistics using SPSS. London UK: Sage Publications; 2005.
  78. 78. Fox J, Weisberg S, Adler D, Bates D, Baud-Bovy G, Ellison S, et al. Package ‘car’. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. 2012.
  79. 79. Forstmeier W, Schielzeth H. Cryptic multiple hypotheses testing in linear models: overestimated effect sizes and the winner’s curse. Behav Ecol and Sociobiol. 2011;65(1):47–55. pmid:21297852
  80. 80. Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily HJ. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. J Mem Lang. 2013;68(3):255–278. pmid:24403724
  81. 81. Benjamini Y, Drai D, Elmer G, Kafkafi N, Golani I. Controlling the false discovery rate in behavior genetics research. Behav Brain Res. 2001;125(1–2):279–284. pmid:11682119
  82. 82. Queen JP, Quinn GP, Keough MJ. Experimental design and data analysis for biologists. Cambridge University Press; 2002.
  83. 83. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. 2016.
  84. 84. Altman J. Baboon mothers and infants. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA; 1980.
  85. 85. Pontzer H, Wrangham RW. Ontogeny of ranging in wild chimpanzees. Int J Primatol. 2006;27(1):295.
  86. 86. Li D, Ren B, Grueter CC, Li B, Li M. Nocturnal sleeping habits of the Yunnan snub‐nosed monkey in Xiangguqing, China. Am J Primatol. 2010;72(12):1092–1099. pmid:20665714
  87. 87. Overdorff DJ, Rasmussen MA. Determinants of nighttime activity in “diurnal” lemurid primates. In Creatures of the Dark. Springer, Boston, MA; 1995. p. 61–74.
  88. 88. Donati G, Bollen A, Borgognini-Tarli SM, Ganzhorn JU. Feeding over the 24-h cycle:dietary flexibility of cathemeral collared lemurs (Eulemur collaris). Behav Ecol and Sociobiol. 2007;61(8):1237–1251.
  89. 89. Nishida T. Local differences in responses to water among wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatol. 1980;33(3):189–209. pmid:7429373
  90. 90. Pruetz JD. Evidence of cave use by savanna chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Fongoli, Senegal:implications for thermoregulatory behavior. Primates. 2007;48(4):316–319. pmid:17624495
  91. 91. Lapuente J, Hicks TC, Linsenmair KE. Fluid dipping technology of chimpanzees in Comoé National Park, Ivory Coast. Am J Primatol. 2017;79(5):e22628. pmid:28002878
  92. 92. Lanjouw A. Behavioural adaptations to water scarcity in Tongo chimpanzees. Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos; 2002. p. 52–60.
  93. 93. Patz JA, Hulme M, Rosenzweig C, Mitchell TD, Goldberg RA, Githeko AK, et al. Regional warming and malaria resurgence. Nature. 2002;420(6916):627–628. pmid:12478282
  94. 94. Taylor RG, Mileham L, Tindimugaya C, Majugu A, Muwanga A, Nakileza B. Recent glacial recession in the Rwenzori Mountains of East Africa due to rising air temperature. Geophys Res Lett. 2006;33(10).
  95. 95. Diem JE, Hartter J, Salerno J, McIntyre E, Grandy AS. Comparison of measured multi-decadal rainfall variability with farmers’ perceptions of and responses to seasonal changes in western Uganda. Reg Environ Change. 2017;17(4):1127–1140.
  96. 96. Erkert HG. Diurnality and nocturnality in nonhuman primates: comparative chronobiological studies in laboratory and nature. Biol Rhythm Res. 2008;39(3):229–267.
  97. 97. Donati G, Lunardini A, Kappeler PM. Brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus rufus) in the Kirindy forest/CFPF. New Directions in Lemur Studies; 2012. p. 119.
  98. 98. Vessey SH. Night observations of free‐ranging rhesus monkeys. Am J Phys Anthropol. 1973;38(2):613–619. pmid:4632112
  99. 99. Masi S, Mundry R, Ortmann S, Cipolletta C, Boitani L, Robbins MM. The influence of seasonal frugivory on nutrient and energy intake in wild western gorillas. PLoS One. 2015;10(7). pmid:26154509
  100. 100. Hamilton A, Taylor D, Vogel JC. Early forest clearance and environmental degradation in south-west Uganda. Nature. 1986;320(6058):164–167.
  101. 101. Mikich SB, Liebsch D. Damage to forest plantations by tufted capuchins (Sapajus nigritus): too many monkeys or not enough fruits?. For Ecol Manage. 2014;314:9–16.
  102. 102. Tweheyo M, Hill CM, Obua J. Patterns of crop raiding by primates around the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda. Wildlife Biol. 2005;11(3):237–247.
  103. 103. Rode KD, Chiyo PI, Chapman CA, McDowell LR. Nutritional ecology of elephants in Kibale National Park, Uganda, and its relationship with crop-raiding behaviour. J Trop Ecol. 2006;22(4):441–449.
  104. 104. Strum SC. Prospects for management of primate pests. Revue d’écologie. 1994.
  105. 105. Hill CM. Primate crop feeding behavior, crop protection, and conservation. Int J Primatol. 2017;38(2):385–400.
  106. 106. Goodall JM. Nest building behavior in the free ranging chimpanzee. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1962;102(2):455–467. pmid:13949060
  107. 107. Siegel JM. Clues to the functions of mammalian sleep. Nature. 2005;437(7063):1264–1271. pmid:16251951
  108. 108. Starr C, Nekaris KAI, Leung L. Hiding from the moonlight: luminosity and temperature affect activity of Asian nocturnal primates in a highly seasonal forest. PLoS One. 2012;7(4).
  109. 109. Delm MM. Vigilance for predators: detection and dilution effects. Behav Ecol and Sociobiol. 1990;26(5):337–342.
  110. 110. Hamilton WD. Geometry for the selfish herd. J Theor Biol. 1971;31(2):295–311. pmid:5104951
  111. 111. Boesch C, Head J, Tagg N, Arandjelovic M, Vigilant L, Robbins MM. Fatal chimpanzee attack in Loango National Park, Gabon. Int J Primat. 2007;28:1025–1034.
  112. 112. Hockings KJ. The crop-raiders of the sacred hill. In The chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba. Springer, Tokyo.2011:211–220.
  113. 113. Williams JM, Pusey AE, Carlis JV, Farm BP, Goodall J. Female competition and male territorial behaviour influence female chimpanzees’ ranging patterns. Anim Behav. 2002;63(2):347–360.
  114. 114. Wallace MT, Barry CT, Zeigler‐Hill V, Green BA. Locus of control as a contributing factor in the relation between self‐perception and adolescent aggression. Aggress Behav. 2012;38(3):213–221. pmid:22531997
  115. 115. Hendry AP, Gotanda KM, Svensson EI. Human influences on evolution, and the ecological and societal consequences. 2017.
  116. 116. Chamberlain DE, Cannon AR, Toms MP, Leech DI, Hatchwell BJ, Gaston KJ. Avian productivity in urban landscapes: a review and meta‐analysis. Ibis. 2009;151(1):1–18.
  117. 117. Meillere A, Brischoux F, Parenteau C, Angelier F. Influence of urbanization on body size, condition, and physiology in an urban exploiter: a multi-component approach. PLoS One. 2015;10(8).
  118. 118. Hockings KJ, McLennan MR, Carvalho S, Ancrenaz M, Bobe R, Byrne RW, et al. Apes in the Anthropocene: flexibility and survival. Trends Ecol Evol. 2015;30(4):215–222. pmid:25766059
  119. 119. Cibot M, Guillot J, Lafosse S, Bon C, Seguya A, Krief S. Nodular worm infections in wild non-human primates and humans living in the Sebitoli area (Kibale National Park, Uganda): do high spatial proximity favor zoonotic transmission?. PLoS Negl Trop Dis. 2015;9(10):e0004133. pmid:26451592
  120. 120. Krief S, Berny P, Gumisiriza F, Gross R, Demeneix B, Fini JB, et al. Agricultural expansion as risk to endangered wildlife: pesticide exposure in wild chimpanzees and baboons displaying facial dysplasia. Sci Total Environ. 2017;598:647–656. pmid:28454037
  121. 121. Lacroux C, Guma N, Krief S. Facial dysplasia in wild forest olive baboons (Papio anubis) in Sebitoli, Kibale National Park, Uganda: use of camera traps to detect health defects J Med Primatol. 2019;48(3):143–153. pmid:30941780
  122. 122. Spirhanzlova P, Fini JB, Demeneix B, Lardy-Fontan S, Vaslin-Reimann S, Lalere B, et al. Composition and endocrine effects of water collected in the Kibale National Park in Uganda. Environ Pollut. 2019;251:460–468. pmid:31103006