Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Air pollution and individuals’ mental well-being in the adult population in United Kingdom: A spatial-temporal longitudinal study and the moderating effect of ethnicity

  • Mary Abed Al Ahad ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    maaa1@st-andrews.ac.uk

    Affiliation School of Geography and Sustainable Development, University of St. Andrews, Scotland, United Kingdom

  • Urška Demšar,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Geography and Sustainable Development, University of St. Andrews, Scotland, United Kingdom

  • Frank Sullivan,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Medicine, University of St. Andrews, Scotland, United Kingdom

  • Hill Kulu

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Geography and Sustainable Development, University of St. Andrews, Scotland, United Kingdom

Abstract

Background

Recent studies suggest an association between ambient air pollution and mental well-being, though evidence is mostly fragmented and inconclusive. Research also suffers from methodological limitations related to study design and moderating effect of key demographics (e.g., ethnicity). This study examines the effect of air pollution on reported mental well-being in United Kingdom (UK) using spatial-temporal (between-within) longitudinal design and assesses the moderating effect of ethnicity.

Methods

Data for 60,146 adult individuals (age:16+) with 349,748 repeated responses across 10-data collection waves (2009–2019) from “Understanding-Society: The-UK-Household-Longitudinal-Study” were linked to annual concentrations of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants using the individuals’ place of residence, given at the local-authority and at the finer Lower-Super-Output-Areas (LSOAs) levels; allowing for analysis at two geographical scales across time. The association between air pollution and mental well-being (assessed through general-health-questionnaire-GHQ12) and its modification by ethnicity and being non-UK born was assessed using multilevel mixed-effect logit models.

Results

Higher odds of poor mental well-being was observed with every 10μg/m3 increase in NO2, SO2, PM10 and PM2.5 pollutants at both LSOAs and local-authority levels. Decomposing air pollution into spatial-temporal (between-within) effects showed significant between, but not within effects; thus, residing in more polluted local-authorities/LSOAs have higher impact on poor mental well-being than the air pollution variation across time within each geographical area. Analysis by ethnicity revealed higher odds of poor mental well-being with increasing concentrations of SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 only for Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other-ethnicities and non-UK born individuals compared to British-white and natives, but not for other ethnic groups.

Conclusion

Using longitudinal individual-level and contextual-linked data, this study highlights the negative effect of air pollution on individuals’ mental well-being. Environmental policies to reduce air pollution emissions can eventually improve the mental well-being of people in UK. However, there is inconclusive evidence on the moderating effect of ethnicity.

1. Introduction

Mental health problems are rising noticeably world-wide causing serious socio-economic losses to the societies manifested in diminished work productivity and contributing to higher rates of criminal activity and lack of trust in governments [1, 2]. The global burden of mental diseases is estimated at 32% of years lived with disability and 13% of disability-adjusted life-years [3]. Mental disorders are mainly triggered by genetics and/or by psycho-social risk factors [1]. However, recent literature has shown a relationship between environmental factors including exposure to ambient air pollution and mental well-being that ranges from subjective stress and anxiety to more severe depression and suicidal ideation; though most of the evidence is fragmented and inconclusive [1, 47].

Ambient air pollution is a mixture of particles (e.g., black carbon and particulate matter with diameters less than or equal to 10 μm: PM10 and to 2.5 μm: PM2.5) and gaseous chemicals (e.g., sulphur dioxide: SO2, nitrogen dioxide: NO2, carbon monoxide: CO, and ozone) that are released to the atmosphere from natural processes (e.g., windblown soil, volcano ashes, pollen, dust) or from man-made activities including energy production, livestock farming, traffic exhaust, and industrial and mining processes [8, 9]. Inhalation of air pollutants can have major consequences on the human central nervous system and neuro-behavioural mechanisms [1, 5, 9, 10]. For example, particulate matter of small diameter such as PM1 or PM2.5 might initiate oxidative stress and lead to the formation of inflammatory cytokines that infiltrate the blood-brain barrier causing neurodegeneration and neuroinflammation [11]. Specifically, exposure to ambient PM2.5 results in depressive responses and increased hippocampal pro-inflammatory cytokines [11], while exposure to PM1 leads to increased inflammation and reactive oxygen species (ROS) generation and impacts learning and memory [12]. In this context, traffic related air pollution (e.g., particulate matter and NO2) have been linked by observational research to increased rates of mental health problems including: autism spectrum disorders [13], schizophrenia [14], dementia [15], psychotic experiences [16, 17], cognitive disabilities [18], anxiety and major depressive disorders [19]. Moreover, higher rates of emergency hospital admissions for depressive disorders have been found on more polluted days in Canada [2022]; and self-reported mental well-being was associated with long-term exposure to NO2, PM10, and carbon monoxide pollution in Korea [6].

Air pollution can also affect mental well-being indirectly through nuisance and individuals’ coping behaviours. Air pollution can result in cognitive anxiety, stress, and loneliness leading to general fatigue and perceived symptoms of poor mental well-being due to aesthetic/odorous nuisance and inhibition of psychological-supporting outdoor activities [2325]. For example, people may prefer to stay indoors rather than enjoying outdoor activities during periods of high air pollution, especially when air pollution is characterised by visible signs (e.g., colour) and/or bad odours [26]. In a recent systematic literature review of 178 published articles, air pollution was shown to decrease happiness and life satisfaction substantially, and to increase anxiety, annoyance, mental problems, suicidal ideation, and coping approaches such as avoidance behaviour and migration [2].

Air pollution has been also linked to stress and experiential anxiety emerging from worrying feelings about one’s physical health and future [27]. Due to the more conclusive research about the effect of air pollution on physical health including cardiopulmonary, immune system and cancer diseases [8, 9, 28], people living in highly polluted areas might experience stress and worrisome feelings of getting physical illness, which impairs their mental well-being.

Despite the establishment of linkages between air pollution and mental well-being in the literature, results are generally inconclusive and suffer from methodological drawbacks related to the chosen study design and methods of estimating air pollution [16]. Most of the studies are either cross-sectional studies or longitudinal studies that lack spatial-temporal specificity and a lengthy follow-up time. To date, no study has tried to address the association between long-term (11 years) air pollution exposure and mental well-being using a spatial-temporal (between-within) longitudinal design. A between-within analysis can determine the spatial time-constant cross-sectional (average 11 years air pollution) effect of air pollution on mental well-being between different geographical areas (e.g., local authorities or census output areas) as well as the temporal time-varying longitudinal (yearly air pollution deviation from the 11 years average) effect of air pollution on mental well-being within each geographical area. In other words, this analysis reveals whether living in more polluted local authorities or census output area is the driving cause for poor mental well-being (between) or whether it is the fluctuation in air pollution across time within each local authority or census output area (within) that is causing poor mental well-being. This type of analysis combines both cross-sectional and longitudinal designs; thus establishing a more robust measurement of the effect of air pollution [29].

Furthermore, published research has not yet covered population subgroups and the potential moderating effect that key demographic groups might have on the association between air pollution and mental well-being. To date, only age (young vs elderly population) and gender have been reported as effect modifiers for the association between air pollution and mental well-being. In Korea, a more pronounced risk of stress and depression from air pollution exposure was observed among men than among women and among people aged less than 65 [6]. In China, exposure to increased concentrations of ambient air pollution showed a greater risk for mental health problems and general well-being among female college students [30]. Therefore, examining how the effect of air pollution on mental well-being varies by other key demographic characteristics such as ethnicity can provide a more conclusive explanation for the association between air pollution and mental well-being. Earlier literature has shown that ethnic minorities suffer from relatively higher levels of stress, depression, and self-harm [31, 32]. This could be attributed to their lower socio-economic status and to living in more deprived ethnic communities with poor housing and neighbourhood conditions [3335]. Ethnicity was also examined in the literature from the lens of migration and being a non-native resident. A systematic literature review in Sweden showed increased risks of depression and psychotic problems among immigrants compared to the native population [36]. Given the higher observed risk of mental problems among ethnic minorities and immigrants, the effect of air pollution on mental well-being among ethnic groups should be investigated.

Accordingly, this study investigates longitudinally the overall and the spatial-temporal (between-within) effects of long-term (11 years) exposure to NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollution in the UK on individuals’ reported mental well-being measured using the 12 items General Health Questionnaire (GHQ12) scale. Unlike other studies that assess the effect of air pollution on well-being using one geographical scale, our study aims to assess the effect of air pollution exposure on mental well-being at two geographical scales, coarse local authorities (council areas in Scotland) and detailed Lower Super Output Areas (LSOAs; data zones in Scotland). This will allow us to compare the results between the two geographical scales and explore in more detail the local-contextual patterns of the effect of air pollution on mental well-being. Additionally, our study aims to consider both space and time by determining whether living in more polluted geographical areas (local authorities and LSOAs) is the driving cause for poor mental well-being (between) or whether it is the variation in air pollution across time within each geographical area (within) that is causing poor mental well-being; thus providing detailed spatial-temporal evidence for policymaking decisions. Finally, we aim to investigate whether ethnic minorities including Pakistani/Bangladeshi, Indians, Black/African/Caribbean, mixed and other ethnicities as well as non-UK born individuals suffer from a more pronounced risk for mental well-being with increasing concentrations of the four air pollutants compared to British-White ethnicity and UK-born individuals, respectively.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Study design and population

This study utilises individual-level data from the “Understanding Society: The UK Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS)” [37]. The UKHLS is a rich longitudinal dataset that is composed of 10 data collection panels/waves over a period of 12 years from (2009–2020) with about 40,000 households enrolled at the first wave from the four UK nations: England, Wales, Scotland, and Northern Ireland.

The dataset contains yearly information on the self-reported general health and mental well-being of individuals and on their socio-demographic characteristics including gender, age, educational qualification, marital status, occupation, perceived financial situation, ethnicity, and country of birth. In addition, the dataset collects yearly information on individuals’ lifestyle factors such as cigarette smoking and on contextual factors such as the local authority/council area and the Lower Super Output Areas (LSOAs)/data zones where households are located [37].

The UKHLS main survey sample is composed of four data collection sub-samples as summarised in Fig 1 and described in detail in other publications [3739].

thumbnail
Fig 1. A diagram showing the four data collection sub-samples of the UKHLS main survey.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.g001

The present study utilises individual-level data from the UKHLS survey on 60,146 adult (age: 16+) individuals with 349,748 repeated responses (at least 2 repeated responses per individual) collected within 10 waves over a period of 11 years (2009–2019). It should be noted, however, that the initial UKHLS adult survey includes a total of 87,045 individuals with 444,181 repeated responses. We dropped 94,433 observations due to the reasons described in Fig 2.

thumbnail
Fig 2. A diagram showing the reasons for dropping survey responses from the UKHLS longitudinal panel data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.g002

2.2. Variables and measurements

2.2.1. Individuals’ reported mental well-being.

Individuals’ reported mental well-being was measured using the 12-items “General Health Questionnaire (GHQ12)” scale [40] which is widely used in the population health research to capture non-psychotic psychiatric illness. The GHQ12 scale is composed of 12 questions about the individuals’ experience of 12 symptoms related to mental well-being in the past few weeks preceding data collection date. The 12 questions are: 1) Ability to concentrate; 2) Losing much sleep; 3) Playing a useful part; 4) Capability of taking decisions; 5) Being under stress; 6) Inability to overcome difficulties; 7) Enjoying normal activities; 8) Being able to face up problems; 9) Feeling unhappy and depressed; 10) Losing confidence; 11) Thinking of self as worthless; and 12) Feeling reasonably happy [41]. Individuals are then asked to rate the negative questions as 0 = ‘not at all’, 1 = ‘no more than usual’, 2 = ‘rather more than usual’, 3 = ‘much more than usual’ and the positive questions as 0 = ‘better than usual’, 1 = ‘same as usual’, 2 = ‘less than usual’, and 3 = ‘much less than usual’ [42]. Two methods are used by relevant literature to construct the overall GHQ12 well-being score. The first and most used method is the (0-0-1-1) method whereby responses for each of the 12 questions of the GHQ12 scale are dichotomised (0 and 1 into 0; 2 and 3 into 1) and then the 12 items are summed up resulting in a general mental well-being score ranging from 0 to 12 with higher scores indicating poorer mental well-being [4246]. The second method aims to construct a Likert scale score (0-1-2-3) by adding up all the 12 items of the GHQ12 scale resulting in a total mental well-being score ranging from 0 to 36 with higher scores indicating poorer mental well-being [47, 48].

For the present study, we used both methods for the GHQ12 scale, the (0-0-1-1) and the (0-1-2-3) method. Given that the scores of the GHQ12 scale are right-skewed and based on relevant literature, we dichotomised the overall GHQ12 scale using two cut off points for the GHQ12 (0–12): our sample mean GHQ12 (0–12) score = 1.8 ∼ 2 (GHQ0-12 ≥ 2) and the GHQ12 (0–12) score of greater than or equal to 4 (GHQ0-12 ≥ 4) as an indication of poor mental well-being [4346, 49, 50]. The GHQ12 (0–36) score was dichotomised based on one cut off point of greater than or equal to 12 (GHQ0-36 ≥ 12) as an indication of poor mental well-being [47, 48]. Therefore, GHQ0-12 and GHQ0-36 total scores were treated as binary variables: GHQ0-12 ≥ 2 (0 = scores <2, good mental well-being; 1 = scores ≥2, poor mental well-being); GHQ0-12 ≥ 4 (0 = scores <4, good mental well-being; 1 = scores ≥4, poor mental well-being); and GHQ0-36 ≥ 12 (0 = scores <12, good mental well-being; 1 = scores ≥12, poor mental well-being).

2.2.2. Air pollution data.

Raster data of annual mean concentrations of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants available up to the year of 2019, measured in μg/m3, and estimated using air dispersion models at a 1x1 km spatial resolution were downloaded from the “Department for Environment Food and Rural Affairs (DEFRA)” online data repository [51]. These air pollution maps at 1x1 km resolution are modelled each year by DEFRA under the “Defra’s Modelling of Ambient Air Quality (MAAQ) contract” and are used to provide policy support in the UK and to fulfil the UK’s reporting obligations to Europe [51]. The 1x1 km air pollution raster data are the finest spatial resolution data that can be downloaded from DEFRA and are sufficient to obtain good modelling estimates [52, 53].

These data were first projected using the UK National Grid projection system in ArcGIS Pro software. Then for each of the 391 local authorities/council areas in the UK, we computed the average concentrations of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants from all the 1x1 km raster cells that fell within the boundaries of the respective local authorities/council areas for each year from 2009 up to 2019. Next, we linked the average concentrations of air pollution at the local authority level to the UKHLS dataset for each individual and each year of follow up (2009–2019).

To minimise exposure bias and establish more robust results from a spatial perspective, we also linked the 1x1 km raster air pollution data to the UKHLS data at the level of Lower Super Output Areas (LSOAs; data zones for Scotland and Super Output Areas for Northern Ireland), a finer geographical scale, for each individual and each year of follow up (2009–2019). LSOAs are used to decompose England and Wales based on the population size into areas with a minimum population size of 1000 people and are the lowest level of geography offered by the UKHLS dataset. The LSOAs in England and Wales are equivalent to data zones in Scotland and to Super Output Areas in Northern Ireland. For simplicity we refer to the joint LSOAs, data zones, and Super Output Areas as LSOAs. Using these smaller spatial units, we ran our analysis at a smaller geographic scale than local authorities, which allowed us to explore local patterns of the effect of air pollution.

A map showing the local authorities in the UK (council areas in Scotland) and an enlarged subset of 20 local authorities in the south-east of UK with an example of PM10 concentrations at 1x1 km resolution for the year of 2017 for Tower Hamlets local authority and its corresponding LSOAs is used to clarify the process of linking air pollution to the UKHLS dataset at the two geographical scales, the coarse local authorities and the detailed LSOAs (Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 3. A map showing the local authorities in the UK and an enlarged subset of 20 local authorities in the south-east of UK with an example of PM10 concentrations at 1x1 km grid for the year of 2017 for Tower Hamlets local authority and its corresponding LSOAs.

The map was constructed by the authors in ArcGIS Pro software using PM10 air pollution shapefile for the year of 2017 downloaded from the DEFRA online data repository [51], local authorities UK boundaries shapefile downloaded from the Office for National Statistics [54], and LSOAs and data zones UK boundaries also downloaded from the Office for National Statistics, National Records of Scotland, and Northern Ireland Statistics [55]. Both DEFRA and Office for National Statistics shapefiles are governed under the Open Government Licence v.3.0.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.g003

2.2.3. Socio-demographic and lifestyle covariates.

A list of individual-level socio-demographic and lifestyle covariates summarised in Table 1 was selected a priori for this study based on what is available in the UKHLS dataset and based on the potential confounders considered by relevant literature. Most of the researchers in the field of air pollution and mental well-being have considered age [4, 6, 16], gender [2, 4, 6, 16], education [4, 6], marital status [6], socio-economic deprivation and occupation [16, 56], ethnicity [1, 7, 16], cigarette smoking [16, 56], alcohol drinking [4, 16], physical exercise [4, 16], and body mass index [4] as potential confounders. Specifically, poor mental well-being, stress, and depression have been associated with younger or older ages, women, cigarette smoking, alcohol drinking, physical inactivity, lower education, divorced/widowed individuals, lower household income, and belonging to an ethnic minority group [6, 3133], which in turn confounds the association between air pollution and mental well-being outcomes. In addition, environmental-contextual factors such as noise pollution [1, 16], neighbourhood disorder [16], green spaces availability [25, 57], air temperature [57], and seasonality [16, 58] have been also considered as potential confounders in the association between air pollution and mental well-being.

thumbnail
Table 1. The socio-demographics and lifestyle covariates selected for this study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.t001

2.3. Data analysis

Individuals’ socio-demographic and lifestyle factors were described using percentages for each wave (waves 1 to 10) of the UKHLS sample.

The mean of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 concentrations and the Pearson’s correlation between the four pollutants was computed at both geographical scales, the coarse local authorities and the detailed LSOAs. Given the high observed correlations between NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants (Pearson’s coefficient ≥ 0.7 [59]; Tables 3 and 4), the association of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 with individuals’ reported mental well-being was analysed in separate regression models. Nevertheless, low to moderate correlation was observed between SO2 and each of the other three pollutants which enabled the construction of bi-pollutant models adjusting the NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 models for the SO2 pollutant.

Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) were also calculated to assess the homogeneity in the mental well-being scores within individuals and household clusters; with an ICC of greater than 0.3 indicating moderate to fair homogeneity [60]. Given the presence of 42% and 49% homogeneity (ICC = 0.42 and ICC = 0.49; Table 5) for the mental well-being scores of GHQ0-12 and GHQ0-36 within individuals’ clusters across time, respectively, the mean score of mental well-being was computed from predictions of mixed effects linear models, adjusting for age in fixed effects and for the individual ID in random intercept.

The association between the three binary measures (GHQ0-36 ≥ 12; GHQ0-12 ≥ 2; GHQ0-12 ≥ 4) of individuals’ reported mental well-being and each of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants (linked at two geographical scales: once at the LSOAs and once at the local authorities level) was examined separately and in bi-pollutant models adjusted for SO2 using three-levels (repeated individual observations across time nested within LSOAs or local authorities) mixed-effect logit models, adjusting for the socio-demographic and lifestyle covariates and for the year (2009–2019) dummies. This type of analysis was chosen as it fits the longitudinal panel design of the study which involves repeated individual responses across time linked to air pollution data at the LSOA or local authority level whereby repeated individual responses are nested within LSOAs or local authorities. The individual-level random intercept is necessary in the multilevel models given the high homogeneity in the individual’s responses across time (ICC = 0.42 and ICC = 0.49; Table 5), while the local authority or LSOAs random intercept is needed to allow for less biased assessments of the contextual-linked air pollution effect on mental well-being [61]; resulting in three-levels mixed-effect logit models. In a supplementary analysis, we also show the association of individuals’ reported mental well-being with each of the socio-demographic and lifestyle covariates (Table 1 in S1 File). It should be noted, however, that our models did not account for the household clustering in random intercepts due to the low observed homogeneity in the well-being responses within each household cluster (ICC = 0.16 and ICC = 0.18; Table 5).

In additional analysis, we decomposed the overall effect of air pollution (linked at two geographical scales: LSOAs and local authorities) on mental well-being into between (spatial) and within (temporal) effects. Between effects were used to examine the spatial time-constant effect of air pollution between different geographical areas (local authorities and LSOAs) by calculating the average concentrations of NO2, SO2, PM10 and PM2.5 pollutants across the 11 years of follow up (2009–2019) for each local authority and each LSOA. On the other hand, within effects were used to assess the temporal time-varying effect of air pollution within each geographical area by calculating the annual air pollution deviation from the 11 years average air pollution for each local authority and LSOA. Therefore, two sets of four multilevel mixed effect logit models (one for each pollutant) were used to examine the overall (Eq 1) and the between-within (Eq 2) effects of air pollution on individuals’ mental well-being, respectively, at the two geographical scales (coarse local authorities and detailed LSOAs).

(1)(2)

Where Ytij is the mental well-being outcome for individual i, in local authority or LSOA j at year t; β1, β2 …. β12 are the slopes of fixed effects; β0 is the fixed intercept; U0ij is level 2 random intercept of individuals nested in local authorities or LSOAs; U0j is level 3 random intercept of local authorities or LSOAs.

To assess the effect modification of ethnicity and country of birth on the association between air pollution and mental well-being, we added interaction terms between ethnicity and each of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants and between country of birth and each of the four pollutants. These interaction terms were added into the models investigating the overall effect of air pollution and into the models examining the between and within effects of air pollution at the two geographical scales of local authorities and LSOAs, separately (i.e., one interaction term at a time, once with the between effect and once with the within effect). The interaction results were visualised with coefficient plots.

To estimate the cohort effect and to balance the follow up time, we repeated the same multilevel mixed effect logit modelling in a sensitivity analysis only for individuals recruited during the first wave of the UKHLS.

STATA software (StataCorp. 2015. Stata Statistical Software: Release 14. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP) was used for statistical analysis and ArcGIS Pro software was used for spatial pre-processing of air pollution data. Odd ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence (CIs) for every 10 μg/m3 increase in air pollution were used to report the study findings. Statistical significance was considered at a P-value < 0.05.

2.4. Ethical considerations

This paper was granted ethical approval on the 14th of May 2020 by the authors’ affiliated institution (School of Geography and Sustainable Development Ethics Committee, acting on behalf of the University Teaching and Research Ethics Committee (UTREC) at the University of St Andrews). The paper uses secondary adult (age 16+) fully anonymised data from the “Understanding Society: The UK Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS)” and authors did not have access to potentially identifying information; thus, obtaining participants’ informed consent is not applicable and was waved by the authors’ institution ethics committee. The University of Essex responsible for the UKHLS data collection and management has already obtained written informed consent from all the study participants [37]. Requesting consent for health record linkage was approved at Wave 1 by the National Research Ethics Service (NRES) Oxfordshire REC A (08/H0604/124), and at Wave 4 by NRES Southampton REC A (11/SC/0274). Approval for the collection of biosocial data by trained nurses in Waves 2 and 3 of the main survey was obtained from the National Research Ethics Service (Understanding Society—UK Household Longitudinal Study: A Biosocial Component, Oxfordshire A REC, Reference: 10/H0604/2).

3. Results

3.1. Individuals’ socio-demographic and lifestyle characteristics

A total of 60,146 adult individuals with 349,748 repeated responses over 11 years (2009–2019) of 10 data collection waves were included in this study. The mean of observations per individual was 5.81 (SD = 2.81) with a minimum of 2 observations per individual and the average follow up time was 5.58 (SD = 2.98) years.

Description of the individuals’ socio-demographic and lifestyle characteristics for the 10 data collection waves of the UKHLS are summarised in Table 2. For all waves, the majority of individuals were females, belonged to the middle-aged group (34–58 years), were married, had either a university or high school degree, were non-manual workers (if working), were living comfortably/doing alright financially, and were cigarette non-smokers (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Description of individual’s socio-demographic and lifestyle factors for each wave of the UKHLS data (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.t002

For ethnicity, most individuals were UK-born (86% in wave 1) and belonged to the British-white group (81% in wave 1). The description of other ethnic groups in wave 1 is as follows: Other-white (4%), Indians (3%), Pakistani/Bangladeshi (3.5%), Black/African/Caribbean (4%), mixed ethnicities (1.6%), and other ethnicities (2%) (Table 2).

3.2. Description of air pollution

3.2.1. Description of air pollution at the LSOAs level.

Fig 4 shows the average yearly concentrations of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants across the 42,619 LSOAs in the UK from 2009 to 2019. Air pollution showed fluctuations across time with lower concentrations seen in the last 5 years (2015–2019) of observation compared to previous years for all four pollutants (Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. The annual mean of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants at the LSOAs level in the UK from the year of 2009 to 2019 (N = 42,619 LSOAs).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.g004

We also observed high correlations (Pearson’s coefficient ≥ 0.7) between NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants (Table 3), which could be attributed to the source of emission and the atmospheric chemical reactions between these pollutants. For example, the major source of NO2 and particulate matter (PM10 and PM2.5) emissions is traffic exhaust [62, 63], while industrial processes and power plants are the major sources of SO2 pollution [64].

thumbnail
Table 3. Correlation matrix of air pollutants at the LSOAs level (N = 42,619 LSOAs).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.t003

3.2.2. Description of air pollution at the local authority level.

Similar to the air pollution at the LSOAs level, air pollution at the local authority level also showed fluctuations across time with lower concentrations seen in the last 5 years (2015–2019) of observation compared to previous years (2009–2014) for all four pollutants (Fig 5). Likewise, high correlations (Pearson’s coefficient ≥ 0.7) between NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants were noticed (Table 4).

thumbnail
Fig 5. The annual mean of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants at the local authority level in the UK from the year of 2009 to 2019 (N = 391 local authorities).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.g005

thumbnail
Table 4. Correlation matrix of air pollutants at the local authority level (N = 391 local authorities).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.t004

3.3. Description of individuals’ reported mental well-being

The mean score for mental well-being GHQ12 (0–12) scale was 1.8 (SD = 0.2) with 30% of responses having a GHQ12 score of 2 or more and 19% having a GHQ12 score of 4 or more. The mean score for GHQ12 (0–36) Likert scale was 11.14 (SD = 0.05) with 36% of responses having a score of 12 or more. The ICC was 0.42 and 0.49 for GHQ12 (0–12) and GHQ12 (0–36), respectively, indicating a moderate homogeneity in the well-being responses within the individual clusters over time, whilst low homogeneity (ICC = 0.16 and ICC = 0.18) was detected within the household clusters (Table 5).

thumbnail
Table 5. Intraclass correlation coefficient for within individual and household clusters.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.t005

3.4. The effect of air pollution on individuals’ mental well-being

3.4.1. The effect of air pollution on individuals’ mental well-being at the LSOAs level.

Higher odds of poor mental well-being were observed with every 10 μg/m3 increase in NO2 (ORs ranging between 1.12–1.19), SO2 (ORs ranging between 1.29–1.49), PM10 (ORs ranging between 1.19–1.34), and PM2.5 (ORs ranging between 1.30–1.53) air pollutants (Table 6).

thumbnail
Table 6. The association of individuals’ mental well-being with each of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants linked at the LSOAs level in separate models (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.t006

We noticed similar results of higher odds of poor mental well-being with every 10 μg/m3 increase in NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants in bi-pollutant models adjusted for SO2 (Table 7).

thumbnail
Table 7. The association of individuals’ mental well-being with each of NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants linked at the LSOAs level in bi-pollutant models adjusted for SO2 pollutant (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.t007

Decomposing the overall effect of air pollution on mental well-being into between (spatial: across LSOAs) and within (temporal: across years within each LSOA) effects, revealed significant associations with poor mental well-being for the between effects for all the four pollutants; while no significant associations were noted for the within effects despite the sign of the odds ratios being largely as expected. An exception was PM10 and PM2.5 pollutants which showed significant within effects on poor mental well-being (GHQ0-12) only in model 1; yet these significant effects disappeared after controlling for the sociodemographic and lifestyle covariates (Table 6). Therefore, living in more polluted LSOAs was the driving cause for poor mental well-being (between) rather than the variation in air pollution across time within each LSOA.

In a sensitivity analysis, results remained the same for the overall and for the between-within effects of the four pollutants on individuals’ mental well-being for individuals starting at wave 1 of the UKHLS survey (Table 2 in S1 File).

3.4.2. The effect of air pollution on individuals’ mental well-being at the local authority level.

Similar to the air pollution results at the LSOAs geographical scale, higher odds of poor mental well-being were also observed with every 10 μg/m3 increase in NO2 (ORs ranging between 1.10–1.16), SO2 (ORs ranging between 1.27–1.51), PM10 (ORs ranging between 1.13–1.22), and PM2.5 (ORs ranging between 1.21–1.36) pollutants at the local authority level (Table 8).

thumbnail
Table 8. The association of individuals’ mental well-being with each of NO2, SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants linked at the local authority level in separate models (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.t008

Higher odds of poor mental well-being were also observed at the local authority level with every 10 μg/m3 increase in NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants in bi-pollutant models adjusted for SO2 (Table 9).

thumbnail
Table 9. The association of individuals’ mental well-being with each of NO2, PM10, and PM2.5 air pollutants linked at the local authority level in bi-pollutant models adjusted for SO2 pollutant (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.t009

The between-within (spatial-temporal) analysis of the effect of air pollution on mental well-being at the local authority level revealed similar results to the between-within analysis at the LSOAs level (Table 8). Thus, living in more polluted local authorities was also the driving cause for poor mental well-being (between) rather than the variation in air pollution across time within each local authority.

In a sensitivity analysis for individuals starting at wave 1 of the UKHLS survey, results remained unchanged for the overall and for the between-within effects of the four pollutants on individuals’ mental well-being at the local authority level (Table 3 in S1 File).

3.5. The association of air pollution with individuals’ mental well-being by ethnicity and country of birth

3.5.1. The association of air pollution at the LSOAs level with individuals’ mental well-being by ethnicity and country of birth.

At the LSOAs level, Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other-white, and other ethnicities group as well as non-UK born individuals showed higher odds of poor mental well-being compared to British-white and UK-born individuals, respectively, with every 10 μg/m3 increase in SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants only with the GHQ0-12 well-being measure (Fig 6). Nevertheless, most of these significant differences disappeared in a cohort sub-analysis for only individuals recruited at wave 1 of the UKHLS survey (Fig 1 in S1 File). In addition, we observed no significant ethnic differences for the between or within effects of air pollution at the LSOAs level on mental well-being with exception of between effects for PM10 and PM2.5 acting as protective factors (ORs<1) against poor mental well-being for people from Indian, Black/African/Caribbean and mixed ethnicities origin (Figs 3 and 4 in S1 File).

thumbnail
Fig 6. The overall effect of air pollution linked at the LSOAs level on individuals’ mental well-being by ethnicity and country of birth (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).

The dashed line is placed at OR = 1 as a cut-off for statistically insignificant results; The solid line separates between the air pollution-ethnicity interaction models and the air pollution-country of birth interaction models; Air pollution-ethnicity interaction models are adjusted for country of birth, age, gender, marital status, education, occupation, perceived financial situation, smoking status, and year dummies (2009 to 2019); Air pollution-country of birth interaction models are adjusted for ethnicity, age, gender, marital status, education, occupation, perceived financial situation, smoking status, and year dummies (2009 to 2019).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.g006

It is worth to note that examining the association between ethnicity and mental well-being revealed higher odds of poor mental well-being among people from Pakistani/Bangladeshi (GHQ0-36 ≥ 12: OR = 1.17, 95%CI = 1.06–1.29; GHQ0-12 ≥ 2: OR = 1.09, 95%CI = 1.00–1.19; GHQ0-12 ≥ 4: OR = 1.13, 95%CI = 1.02–1.25) and mixed ethnicities (GHQ0-36 ≥ 12: OR = 1.15, 95%CI = 1.01–1.32; GHQ0-12 ≥ 2: OR = 1.21, 95%CI = 1.08–1.37; GHQ0-12 ≥ 4: OR = 1.26, 95%CI = 1.10–1.43) origin in comparison to the British-white, even after adjusting for socio-demographics and lifestyle covariates (Table 1 in S1 File).

3.5.2. The association of air pollution at the local authority level with individuals’ mental well-being by ethnicity and country of birth.

Similar to the LSOAs level, analysis at the local authority level showed higher odds of poor mental well-being with increasing concentrations of SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants among people from Pakistani/Bangladeshi and other ethnicities origin compared to British-white and among non-UK born people compared to natives. People from an Indian origin also showed higher odds of poor mental well-being than the British-white with every 10 μg/m3 increase in SO2 pollution at the local authority level (Fig 7). Yet, these significant differences disappeared in a cohort sub-analysis for only individuals recruited at wave 1 of the UKHLS survey (Fig 2 in S1 File). Similarly, no significant ethnic differences for the between or within effects of air pollution at the local authority level on mental well-being were observed with exception of between effects for PM10 and PM2.5 acting as protective factors (ORs<1) against poor mental well-being for people from Indian, Black/African/Caribbean and mixed ethnicities origin (Figs 5 and 6 in S1 File).

thumbnail
Fig 7. The overall effect of air pollution linked at the local authority level on individuals’ mental well-being by ethnicity and country of birth (N = 349,748 surveys from 60,146 individuals).

The dashed line is placed at OR = 1 as a cut-off for statistically insignificant results; The solid line separates between the air pollution-ethnicity interaction models and the air pollution-country of birth interaction models; Air pollution-ethnicity interaction models are adjusted for country of birth, age, gender, marital status, education, occupation, perceived financial situation, smoking status, and year dummies (2009 to 2019); Air pollution-country of birth interaction models are adjusted for ethnicity, age, gender, marital status, education, occupation, perceived financial situation, smoking status, and year dummies (2009 to 2019).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.g007

4. Discussion

This study showed the negative impact of NO2, SO2, PM10 and PM2.5 air pollution (linked at two geographical scales: coarse local authorities and detailed LSOAs) on mental well-being in the UK for individuals followed from the year 2009 up to 2019. These results are supported by relevant literature whereby exposure to ambient air pollution has been shown to affect negatively individuals’ mental well-being and contribute to increased rates of mental health problems such as autism spectrum disorders [13], schizophrenia [14], dementia [15], psychotic experiences [16, 17], cognitive disabilities [18], anxiety and major depressive disorders [19]. Evidence from China revealed an elevation in the rate of depressive symptoms and poorer self-reported mental well-being with long-term exposure to air pollution [6567]. Similarly, exposure to NO2 and particulate matter (PM10 and PM2.5) air pollution in the Netherlands was positively associated with poor mental health and prescription of anti-anxiety drugs [68]. In South Korea, increased exposure to NO2, SO2, and PM10 pollutants resulted in higher hazards for suicide death [69]. In addition, a recent systematic literature review and meta-analysis of 22 articles revealed that exposure to PM2.5 pollution increases the risk for depression and anxiety with a pooled odd ratio estimate of 1.10 for every 10 μg/m3 increase in PM2.5 concentration [70].

The observed positive association between air pollution and poor mental well-being in this study can be explained by four factors. The first explanation is through the biological mechanisms of air pollutants on the human central nervous system and neuro-behavioural processes [1, 5, 9, 10]. Air pollution particles of small diameters such as PM2.5 are capable of initiating oxidative stress and forming inflammatory cytokines that infiltrate the blood-brain barrier causing neurodegeneration and neuroinflammation [11]. The second explanation is through the aesthetic and odorous nuisance caused by air pollution, which results in avoidance behaviour and inhibition of psychological-supporting outdoor activities and sports. This in turn leads to reduced happiness and life satisfaction and to elevated levels of stress, anxiety, loneliness, and poor mental well-being [2, 2325]. The third explanation is related to experiential anxiety and worrying feelings about one’s physical health and future [27]. Through substantive research, people are made aware of the negative impact of air pollution on human’s physical health and the higher risk for acute and chronic diseases such as cardiovascular, respiratory, cancer and immune system diseases [8, 9, 28]. People living in highly polluted areas might experience stress and anxiety about physical illness, which is reflected in poorer mental well-being. The last explanation is manifested in the indirect effect of air pollution on mental well-being through the physical health of individuals. People who already suffer from physical illness often also suffer from poor mental well-being. Based on an international study that utilises data from the World Health Organisation, 9% to 23% of patients with one or more chronic physical health conditions displayed symptoms of depression [71]. Thus, air pollution might be associated with poor mental well-being because individuals are also suffering from a physical health condition. Conducting a simple t-test on the UKHLS data showed a higher GHQ12(0–12) mean score (mean = 2.44, SD = 3.46) among individuals with at least one physical health condition (e.g., asthma, arthritis, coronary heart disease, cancer, liver illness, chronic bronchitis, diabetes, blood pressure) in comparison to a GHQ12(0–12) mean score of 1.68 (SD = 2.92) for those with non-reported physical health condition (t-test P-value = 0.000).

Despite the existence of literature on the topic of air pollution and poor mental well-being, this study went a further step in analysing the spatial-temporal effects of air pollution on mental well-being using a between-within longitudinal design. Additionally, we carried out the analysis at two geographical scales, coarse local authorities and detailed LSOAs, which forms another novelty of the present study. The between-within analysis is extensively used in the fields of economics, behavioural finance, and strategic management [29], yet little used in health research [72]; and no previous study has examined the between-within effects of air pollution on mental well-being. Our study revealed significant between effects for NO2, SO2, PM10 and PM2.5 pollutants on poor mental well-being at both the LSOAs and the local authority geographical scales, while no significant within effects were noted. Thus, individuals residing in LSOAs or local authorities with higher average concentrations of the four pollutants across the 11 years of follow up exhibited poorer mental well-being than individuals residing in LSOAs or local authorities with lower pollution concentrations. This shows the importance of the spatial dimension in the association between air pollution and mental well-being whether at the coarse local authorities or at the detailed LSOAs geographical level given that both geographical scales resulted in similar findings. Nevertheless, the non-significant within (temporal) effects could be related to the low variation of yearly air pollution concentrations across the 11 years of follow up, particularly for SO2 pollutant as shown in Fig 4 for LSOAs and in Fig 5 for local authorities. Therefore, further research with longer follow up time is needed to allow for more temporal variation in air pollution which might result in significant within effects.

In the second part of this study, we attempted to examine the moderating effect of ethnicity and country of birth on the association between air pollution and individuals’ mental well-being. We hypothesised that the lower socio-economic status of ethnic minorities and living in more disadvantaged neighbourhoods, near major roads and transportation networks can result in higher levels of stress, anxiety, and mental health problems among ethnic minority groups; thus, moderating the association between air pollution and mental well-being. However, our findings did not reveal much difference in the overall and in the between and within effects of air pollution on mental well-being across the ethnic groups; with exception for Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other-white (only at the LSOAs level but not at the local authority level), Indians (only for SO2 pollution at the local authority level), other ethnicities, and non-UK born individuals who showed higher odds of poor mental well-being than the British-White and UK-born individuals with increasing concentrations of SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollutants. The poorer mental well-being observed among the Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other ethnicities group, and non-UK born individuals could be related to the socio-economic and lifestyle differences or to place-related contextual differences. Literature on ethnic inequalities in health has shown that ethnic minorities often live in more disadvantaged communities, and have lower socio-economic status, lower healthcare coverage and higher job/income insecurity [34, 73, 74], which increases their risk of physical and mental illness. Nevertheless, our analysis adjusted for the main socio-economic and lifestyle factors of individuals including age, gender, marital status, education, occupation, financial situation, and cigarette smoking. Therefore, the most likely explanation for the more pronounced effect of SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollution on poor mental well-being among Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other ethnicities, and non-UK born individuals could be related to the contextual factors and place of residence. Ethnic minorities and immigrants often choose to reside in large cities and highly urbanised regions, near major roads and key transportation networks to simplify their commuting and working conditions [75]. In addition, ethnic minorities often live in low-priced social housing offered by local authorities, which is often situated in more deprived ethnic concentration neighbourhoods [35]. These place-related factors can result in more pronounced effect of air pollution on mental well-being among ethnic minorities and immigrants due to greater exposure to air pollution resulting from vehicles, factories, and burning of fossil fuels. In a supplementary analysis, we show through Chi2 square tabulation that a very high percentage of non-UK born individuals (93%) and of ethnic minorities including Pakistani/Bangladeshi (99%) reside in urban areas; whereas this percentage is much lower for British-white (71%) and UK-born (75%) individuals (Table 4 in S1 File). It should be noted, however, that decomposing the overall effect of air pollution into between (spatial) and within (temporal) effects at both the LSOAs and local authority levels did not show significant differences among the ethnic groups, not even for the Pakistani/Bangladeshi and non-UK born individuals. Thus, we cannot be conclusive that residing in more polluted areas is the key explanation for the more pronounced effect of SO2, PM10, and PM2.5 pollution on mental well-being among Pakistani/Bangladeshi and non-UK born individuals. Furthermore, in a cohort sub-analysis for individuals recruited at wave 1 of the UKHLS survey (Figs 1 and 2 in S1 File), the differences noted with respect to the Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other ethnicities group, and non-UK born individuals disappeared. Therefore, our study shows no conclusive evidence of ethnicity or nativity differences in the association between air pollution and poor mental well-being.

Despite the new insights provided by this study, it is important to discuss its limitations. The first limitation is related to the design of the study in which individual-level data from the UKHLS survey was linked to yearly air pollution contextual data at the local authority level. Therefore, individuals residing within a respective local authority and a respective year were assigned to the same value of air pollution exposure. However, we also linked the air pollution data to the UKHLS at the LSOAs level (the lowest available geography level at the UKHLS due to ethical considerations) which minimised the exposure bias. Air pollution linked at both, the local authority and the LSOAs level, revealed similar results. For future research, we recommended the usage of data sources that allow linkages of air pollution at the postcode level, the lowest available geography in the UK. Second, our analysis included all individuals recruited at different waves of the UKHLS survey, that had at least two observations through the follow up time (2009–2019). Hence, the time window for follow up differed between the study participants, some were followed for 11 years across all the 10 waves while others entered the study at later waves and were followed for a shorter period of time. It should be noted, however, that similar results were observed in a sensitivity analysis on wave 1 cohort, except for the analysis by ethnic groups whereby Pakistani/Bangladeshi, other ethnicities, and non-UK born individuals did not show significant differences in the association of air pollution with mental well-being at both the LSOAs and local authority geographical scales (Tables 2, 3 and Figs 1 and 2 in S1 File). This can be explained by the UKHLS sample design which involved ethnic minority boost samples at waves 1 and 6 of data collection to enable ethnicity-focused research. Therefore, by conducting analysis on only wave 1 cohort, we are missing the second ethnic minority boost sample at wave 6 which resulted in the observed differences. Finally, the UKHLS survey included longitudinal weights that adjust for the overrepresentation of some groups, such as the ethnic minority groups; thus, allowing for greater generalisation of the estimates. However, we could not add the longitudinal weights into our analysis as this requires that all individuals be followed until the last wave (wave 10) of the survey, which was not the case.

5. Conclusion

Using a longitudinal panel design that involves linking individual to context-level data at two geographical scales (coarse local authorities and detailed LSOAs) and a between-within analysis, this study highlights the negative effect of air pollution on individuals’ mental well-being over space and time and emphasises the importance of the spatial dimension in the shaping of this association. Thus, environmental policies to reduce air pollution emissions with a core of spatial planning can eventually improve the mental well-being of people residing in the UK. There is, however, less conclusive evidence on the moderating effect of ethnicity.

Supporting information

S1 File. Additional analysis and sensitivity check-ups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264394.s001

(DOCX)

References

  1. 1. Dzhambov AM, Markevych I, Tilov B, Arabadzhiev Z, Stoyanov D, Gatseva P, et al. Pathways linking residential noise and air pollution to mental ill-health in young adults. Environmental Research. 2018;166:458–65. pmid:29940479
  2. 2. Lu JG. Air pollution: A systematic review of its psychological, economic, and social effects. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2020;32:52–65. pmid:31557706
  3. 3. Vigo D, Thornicroft G, Atun R. Estimating the true global burden of mental illness. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(2):171–8. pmid:26851330
  4. 4. Lim Y-H, Kim H, Kim Jin H, Bae S, Park Hye Y, Hong Y-C. Air Pollution and Symptoms of Depression in Elderly Adults. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2012;120(7):1023–8. pmid:22514209
  5. 5. Ventriglio A, Bellomo A, di Gioia I, Di Sabatino D, Favale D, De Berardis D, et al. Environmental pollution and mental health: a narrative review of literature. CNS Spectr. 2021;26(1):51–61. pmid:32284087
  6. 6. Shin J, Park JY, Choi J. Long-term exposure to ambient air pollutants and mental health status: A nationwide population-based cross-sectional study. PLoS One. 2018;13(4):e0195607. pmid:29630645
  7. 7. Power MC, Kioumourtzoglou M-A, Hart JE, Okereke OI, Laden F, Weisskopf MG. The relation between past exposure to fine particulate air pollution and prevalent anxiety: observational cohort study. BMJ: British Medical Journal. 2015;350:h1111. pmid:25810495
  8. 8. Abed Al Ahad M, Sullivan F, Demšar U, Melhem M, Kulu H. The effect of air-pollution and weather exposure on mortality and hospital admission and implications for further research: A systematic scoping review. PLoS One. 2020;15(10):e0241415. pmid:33119678
  9. 9. Manisalidis I, Stavropoulou E, Stavropoulos A, Bezirtzoglou E. Environmental and Health Impacts of Air Pollution: A Review. Front Public Health. 2020;8:14–. pmid:32154200
  10. 10. Genc S, Zadeoglulari Z, Fuss SH, Genc K. The Adverse Effects of Air Pollution on the Nervous System. Journal of Toxicology. 2012;2012:782462. pmid:22523490
  11. 11. Calderón-Garcidueñas L, Calderón-Garcidueñas A, Torres-Jardón R, Avila-Ramírez J, Kulesza RJ, Angiulli AD. Air pollution and your brain: what do you need to know right now. Primary health care research & development. 2015;16(4):329–45.
  12. 12. Morris RH, Counsell SJ, McGonnell IM, Thornton C. Early life exposure to air pollution impacts neuronal and glial cell function leading to impaired neurodevelopment. BioEssays. 2021;43(9):2000288. pmid:33751627
  13. 13. Volk HE, Lurmann F, Penfold B, Hertz-Picciotto I, McConnell R. Traffic-related air pollution, particulate matter, and autism. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(1):71–7. pmid:23404082
  14. 14. Pedersen CB, Raaschou-Nielsen O, Hertel O, Mortensen PB. Air pollution from traffic and schizophrenia risk. Schizophrenia Research. 2004;66(1):83–5. pmid:14693358
  15. 15. Yuchi W, Sbihi H, Davies H, Tamburic L, Brauer M. Road proximity, air pollution, noise, green space and neurologic disease incidence: a population-based cohort study. Environmental Health. 2020;19(1):8. pmid:31964412
  16. 16. Bakolis I, Hammoud R, Stewart R, Beevers S, Dajnak D, MacCrimmon S, et al. Mental health consequences of urban air pollution: prospective population-based longitudinal survey. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2020. pmid:33097984
  17. 17. Newbury JB, Arseneault L, Beevers S, Kitwiroon N, Roberts S, Pariante CM, et al. Association of Air Pollution Exposure With Psychotic Experiences During Adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76(6):614–23. pmid:30916743
  18. 18. Kilian J, Kitazawa M. The emerging risk of exposure to air pollution on cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease—Evidence from epidemiological and animal studies. Biomed J. 2018;41(3):141–62. pmid:30080655
  19. 19. Roberts S, Arseneault L, Barratt B, Beevers S, Danese A, Odgers CL, et al. Exploration of NO(2) and PM(2.5) air pollution and mental health problems using high-resolution data in London-based children from a UK longitudinal cohort study. Psychiatry Res. 2019;272:8–17. pmid:30576995
  20. 20. Szyszkowicz M. Air pollution and emergency department visits for depression in Edmonton, Canada. Int J Occup Med Environ Health. 2007;20(3):241–5. pmid:17932013
  21. 21. Szyszkowicz M, Kousha T, Kingsbury M, Colman I. Air Pollution and Emergency Department Visits for Depression: A Multicity Case-Crossover Study. Environmental Health Insights. 2016;10:EHI.S40493. pmid:27597809
  22. 22. Szyszkowicz M. Ambient sulfur dioxide and female ED visits for depression. Air Quality, Atmosphere & Health. 2011;4(3):259–62.
  23. 23. Claeson A-S, Lidén E, Nordin M, Nordin S. The role of perceived pollution and health risk perception in annoyance and health symptoms: a population-based study of odorous air pollution. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health. 2013;86(3):367–74. pmid:22526088
  24. 24. Klæboe R, Amundsen AH, Fyhri A. Annoyance from vehicular air pollution: A comparison of European exposure–response relationships. Atmospheric Environment. 2008;42(33):7689–94.
  25. 25. Dzhambov A, Hartig T, Markevych I, Tilov B, Dimitrova D. Urban residential greenspace and mental health in youth: Different approaches to testing multiple pathways yield different conclusions. Environmental Research. 2018;160:47–59. pmid:28961469
  26. 26. von Lindern E, Hartig T, Lercher P. Traffic-related exposures, constrained restoration, and health in the residential context. Health & Place. 2016;39:92–100.
  27. 27. Lu JG, Lee JJ, Gino F, Galinsky AD. Polluted Morality: Air Pollution Predicts Criminal Activity and Unethical Behavior. Psychological Science. 2018;29(3):340–55. pmid:29412050
  28. 28. Ab Manan N, Noor Aizuddin A, Hod R. Effect of Air Pollution and Hospital Admission: A Systematic Review. Ann Glob Health. 2018;84(4):670–8. pmid:30779516
  29. 29. McNeish D, Kelley K. Fixed effects models versus mixed effects models for clustered data: Reviewing the approaches, disentangling the differences, and making recommendations. Psychol Methods. 2019;24(1):20–35. pmid:29863377
  30. 30. Sui G, Liu G, Jia L, Wang L, Yang G. The association between ambient air pollution exposure and mental health status in Chinese female college students: a cross-sectional study. Environmental Science and Pollution Research. 2018;25(28):28517–24. pmid:30088250
  31. 31. Gater R, Tomenson B, Percival C, Chaudhry N, Waheed W, Dunn G, et al. Persistent depressive disorders and social stress in people of Pakistani origin and white Europeans in UK. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2008;44(3):198. pmid:18726242
  32. 32. Brown G, Marshall M, Bower P, Woodham A, Waheed W. Barriers to recruiting ethnic minorities to mental health research: a systematic review. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2014;23(1):36–48. pmid:24474683
  33. 33. Smith NR, Lewis DJ, Fahy A, Eldridge S, Taylor SJC, Moore DG, et al. Individual socio-demographic factors and perceptions of the environment as determinants of inequalities in adolescent physical and psychological health: the Olympic Regeneration in East London (ORiEL) study. BMC Public Health. 2015;15(1):150. pmid:25884502
  34. 34. Su JG, Jerrett M, de Nazelle A, Wolch J. Does exposure to air pollution in urban parks have socioeconomic, racial or ethnic gradients? Environmental Research. 2011;111(3):319–28. pmid:21292252
  35. 35. van Ham M, Manley D. Social housing allocation, choice and neighbourhood ethnic mix in England. Journal of Housing and the Built Environment. 2009;24(4):407–22.
  36. 36. Gilliver SC, Sundquist J, Li X, Sundquist K. Recent research on the mental health of immigrants to Sweden: a literature review. European Journal of Public Health. 2014;24(suppl_1):72–9. pmid:25108001
  37. 37. UniversityofEssex. Understanding Society: Waves 1–10, 2009–2019 and Harmonised BHPS: Waves 1–18, 1991–2009. In: University of Essex IfSaER, NatCen Social Research, Kantar Public, editor. 13th Edition ed. UK Data Service2020.
  38. 38. Lynn P. Sample design for understanding society. Underst Soc Work Pap Ser. 2009;2009.
  39. 39. Lynn P, Nandi A, Parutis V, Platt L. Design and implementation of a high-quality probability sample of immigrants and ethnic minorities: Lessons learnt. Demographic Research. 2018;38:513–48.
  40. 40. Goldberg DP. User’s guide to the General Health Questionnaire. Windsor. 1988(NFER/Nelson).
  41. 41. Zulkefly SN, Baharudin R. Using the 12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) to Assess the Psychological Health of Malaysian College Students. Global Journal of Health Science. 2010;2:73.
  42. 42. Daly M, Sutin AR, Robinson E. Longitudinal changes in mental health and the COVID-19 pandemic: evidence from the UK Household Longitudinal Study. Psychological Medicine. 2020:1–10. pmid:33183370
  43. 43. Hori D, Tsujiguchi H, Kambayashi Y, Hamagishi T, Kitaoka M, Mitoma J, et al. The associations between lifestyles and mental health using the General Health Questionnaire 12-items are different dependently on age and sex: a population-based cross-sectional study in Kanazawa, Japan. Environmental Health and Preventive Medicine. 2016;21(6):410–21. pmid:27270412
  44. 44. Nerdrum P, Rustøen T, Rønnestad MH. Student Psychological Distress: A psychometric study of 1750 Norwegian 1st‐year undergraduate students. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research. 2006;50(1):95–109.
  45. 45. Godinho EL, Farias LC, Aguiar JC, Martelli-Júnior H, Bonan PR, Ferreira RC, et al. No association between periodontal disease and GHQ-12 in a Brazilian Police population. Med Oral Patol Oral Cir Bucal. 2011;16(6):e857–63. pmid:21743420
  46. 46. Puustinen PJ, Koponen H, Kautiainen H, Mäntyselkä P, Vanhala M. Psychological distress measured by the GHQ-12 and mortality: a prospective population-based study. Scand J Public Health. 2011;39(6):577–81. pmid:21752849
  47. 47. Berglund E, Lytsy P, Westerling R. Health and wellbeing in informal caregivers and non-caregivers: a comparative cross-sectional study of the Swedish general population. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes. 2015;13(1):109. pmid:26216099
  48. 48. Wrede O, Löve J, Jonasson JM, Panneh M, Priebe G. Promoting mental health in migrants: a GHQ12-evaluation of a community health program in Sweden. BMC Public Health. 2021;21(1):262. pmid:33530971
  49. 49. Maheswaran H, Kupek E, Petrou S. Self-reported health and socio-economic inequalities in England, 1996–2009: Repeated national cross-sectional study. Social Science & Medicine. 2015;136–137:135–46. pmid:26004207
  50. 50. Holi MM, Marttunen M, Aalberg V. Comparison of the GHQ-36, the GHQ-12 and the SCL-90 as psychiatric screening instruments in the Finnish population. Nord J Psychiatry. 2003;57(3):233–8. pmid:12775300
  51. 51. Modelled background pollution data [Internet]. 2020. Available from: https://uk-air.defra.gov.uk/data/pcm-data.
  52. 52. Santiago JL, Rivas E, Gamarra AR, Vivanco MG, Buccolieri R, Martilli A, et al. Estimates of population exposure to atmospheric pollution and health-related externalities in a real city: The impact of spatial resolution on the accuracy of results. Science of The Total Environment. 2021:152062.
  53. 53. Lehtomäki H, Geels C, Brandt J, Rao S, Yaramenka K, Åström S, et al. Deaths Attributable to Air Pollution in Nordic Countries: Disparities in the Estimates. Atmosphere. 2020;11(5).
  54. 54. Local Authority Districts (December 2017) Full Clipped Boundaries in United Kingdom [Internet]. 2017. Available from: https://data.gov.uk/dataset/f8f57716-72b4-4c93-a50d-30b2a443c2c1/local-authority-districts-december-2017-full-clipped-boundaries-in-united-kingdom-wgs84.
  55. 55. 2011 Census Geography boundaries (Lower Layer Super Output Areas and Data Zones) [Internet]. 2011. Available from: https://statistics.ukdataservice.ac.uk/dataset/2011-census-geography-boundaries-lower-layer-super-output-areas-and-data-zones.
  56. 56. Yang Z, Song Q, Li J, Zhang Y, Yuan X-C, Wang W, et al. Air pollution and mental health: the moderator effect of health behaviors. Environmental Research Letters. 2021;16(4):044005.
  57. 57. Xue T, Zhu T, Zheng Y, Zhang Q. Declines in mental health associated with air pollution and temperature variability in China. Nature Communications. 2019;10(1):2165. pmid:31092825
  58. 58. Oudin A, Åström DO, Asplund P, Steingrimsson S, Szabo Z, Carlsen HK. The association between daily concentrations of air pollution and visits to a psychiatric emergency unit: a case-crossover study. Environmental Health. 2018;17(1):4. pmid:29321054
  59. 59. Schober P, Boer C, Schwarte LA. Correlation Coefficients: Appropriate Use and Interpretation. Anesth Analg. 2018;126(5):1763–8. pmid:29481436
  60. 60. Lajos GJ, Haddad SM, Tedesco RP, Passini R Jr, Dias TZ, Nomura ML, et al. Intracluster correlation coefficients for the Brazilian Multicenter Study on Preterm Birth (EMIP): methodological and practical implications. BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2014;14(1):54. pmid:24755392
  61. 61. Jerrett M, McConnell R, Wolch J, Chang R, Lam C, Dunton G, et al. Traffic-related air pollution and obesity formation in children: a longitudinal, multilevel analysis. Environmental Health. 2014;13(1):49. pmid:24913018
  62. 62. Grazuleviciene R, Maroziene L, Dulskiene V, Malinauskiene V, Azaraviciene A, Laurinaviciene D, et al. Exposure to urban nitrogen dioxide pollution and the risk of myocardial infarction. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment & Health. 2004;30(4):293–8. pmid:15458012
  63. 63. Jyethi DS. Air Quality: Global and Regional Emissions of Particulate Matter, SOx, and NOx. In: Kulshrestha U, Saxena P, editors. Plant Responses to Air Pollution. Singapore: Springer Singapore; 2016. p. 5–19.
  64. 64. Vestreng V, Myhre G, Fagerli H, Reis S, Tarrasón L. Twenty-five years of continuous sulphur dioxide emission reduction in Europe. Atmos Chem Phys. 2007;7(13):3663–81.
  65. 65. Zhang X, Zhang X, Chen X. Happiness in the air: How does a dirty sky affect mental health and subjective well-being? Journal of Environmental Economics and Management. 2017;85:81–94. pmid:29081551
  66. 66. Liu H, Chen Y, Ma L. Effect of time-varying exposure to air pollution on subjective well-being. Journal of Cleaner Production. 2021;281:125364.
  67. 67. Zu D, Zhai K, Qiu Y, Pei P, Zhu X, Han D. The Impacts of Air Pollution on Mental Health: Evidence from the Chinese University Students. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(18). pmid:32947810
  68. 68. Klompmaker JO, Hoek G, Bloemsma LD, Wijga AH, van den Brink C, Brunekreef B, et al. Associations of combined exposures to surrounding green, air pollution and traffic noise on mental health. Environment International. 2019;129:525–37. pmid:31158598
  69. 69. Min J-y Kim H-J, Min K-b. Long-term exposure to air pollution and the risk of suicide death: A population-based cohort study. Science of The Total Environment. 2018;628–629:573–9. pmid:29454198
  70. 70. Braithwaite I, Zhang S, Kirkbride James B, Osborn David PJ, Hayes Joseph F. Air Pollution (Particulate Matter) Exposure and Associations with Depression, Anxiety, Bipolar, Psychosis and Suicide Risk: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2019;127(12):126002. pmid:31850801
  71. 71. Moussavi S, Chatterji S, Verdes E, Tandon A, Patel V, Ustun B. Depression, chronic diseases, and decrements in health: results from the World Health Surveys. The Lancet. 2007;370(9590):851–8.
  72. 72. Dieleman JL, Templin T. Random-Effects, Fixed-Effects and the within-between Specification for Clustered Data in Observational Health Studies: A Simulation Study. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e110257. pmid:25343620
  73. 73. Cézard G, Finney N, Kulu H, Marshall A. Ethnic differences in self-assessed health in Scotland: The role of socio-economic status and migrant generation. Population, Space and Place. 2020;n/a(n/a):e2403.
  74. 74. Egede LE. Race, ethnicity, culture, and disparities in health care. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2006;21(6):667. pmid:16808759
  75. 75. Lucas K, Stokes G, Bastiaanssen J, Burkinshaw J. Inequalities in Mobility and Access in the UK Transport System. Future of Mobility: Evidence Review, Government Office for Science. 2019. pmid:31048482