Figures
Abstract
In the present project we assessed whether partisan news affects consumers’ views on polarizing issues. In Study 1 nationally representative cross-sectional data (N = 4249) reveals that right-leaning news consumption is associated with more right-leaning attitudes, and left-leaning news consumption is associated with more left-leaning attitudes. Additional three-wave longitudinal data (N = 484) in Study 2 reveals that right-leaning news is positively (and left-leaning news is negatively) associated with right-leaning issue stances three months later, even after controlling for prior issue stances. In a third (supplemental) study (N = 305), random assignment to right-leaning (but not left-leaning) news (vs. control) experimentally fostered more right-leaning stances, regardless of participants’ previously held political ideology. These findings suggest that partisan news, and particularly right-leaning news, can polarize consumers in their sociopolitical positions, sharpen political divides, and shape public policy.
Citation: Earle M, Hodson G (2022) News media impact on sociopolitical attitudes. PLoS ONE 17(3): e0264031. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264031
Editor: Jeff Galak, Carnegie Mellon Univeristy, UNITED STATES
Received: September 29, 2021; Accepted: January 31, 2022; Published: March 9, 2022
Copyright: © 2022 Earle, Hodson. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: Data for Study 1 can be obtained from the ANES website (https://electionstudies.org/data-center/2016-time-series-study/). We have provided code (see Supplemental File #6) that will allow the reader to understand the MPLUS syntax. The requested variable names from the ANES file are now also included at the beginning of the Supplemental File #6. The authors confirm that they did not have special access to the data. Data for Study 3, and the supplemental third study, can be obtained from https://osf.io/4k8s7/.
Funding: This study was supported by the Brock University Chancellor’s Chair for Research Excellence fund through funding awarded to GH (2015-1018).
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Early in the 20th century news organizations aimed for objectivity by providing both sides of political debates [1,2]. Now, with greater availability and more choices [3,4], many news organizations provide more overt ideological perspectives to differentiate themselves in a more competitive market [5–9]. Many speculate that this partisan news could be driving political polarization [10–12] and some scholars argue that increasing polarization in the United States may alter how people form attitudes [13], thus making them more inclined to uncritically accept arguments put forth by partisan news. As such, examining sociopolitical implications of news media is crucial [14], especially in times of heightened political divide in the United States and much of the western world.
According to diffusion theory, there are five stages by which people eventually come to adopt ideas [15,16]. This process begins with awareness, or knowledge, of an idea, primarily obtained through mass media. Arguably then, news media is a primary force in shaping knowledge about sociopolitical issues, and organizations can differ dramatically in how this “knowledge” is conveyed [17; for a real-world example see 18 compared to 19]. Content analyses show that right-leaning organizations (e.g., Fox News, Rush Limbaugh, Wall Street Journal) tend to use more “outrage language”, including insults, misrepresentative exaggeration, and character assassination [8,20], and framed immigrants more negatively after 9/11 [21], relative to left-leaning organizations (e.g., MSNBC, NPR, New York Times). Further, whereas content analyses largely focus on right-slanted news, such as Fox News [8,22–24], some find equally intense left-leaning slants and preferential treatment of Democratic presidential candidates on left-leaning outlets [24,25].
From an information processing perspective, partisan news is considered more persuasive than news providing multiple or balanced perspectives [26,27], in part because one-sided arguments (vs. information that includes counterarguments) are more likely to be accepted uncritically [28]. As such, partisan news exposure may result in congruent sociopolitical stances among consumers. For instance, the introduction of Fox News to a specific region coincided with increases in Republican candidate support in that area [14]. Right-wing news consumption is associated with more anti-immigrant attitudes [29] and climate change denial [30; see also ref. 31]. Moreover, partisan news exposure predicts views toward healthcare consistent with the news slant, regardless of the participants’ pre-existing ideology [26], increases opposition toward political candidates that disparaged by the outlet [24], and reinforces preferences for previously supported political candidate [32]. Moreover, there is debate as to whether left- and right-leaning news media exert an equivalent or a differing level of effect on consumers. Feldman and colleagues [31], for instance, found that that Fox News consumption is associated with greater climate change doubt and consumption of CNN and MSNBC is associated with greater acceptance of climate change, suggesting that right- and left-leaning media have similar degrees of influence on consumers. Moreover, Morris [33] found that audiences of both Fox News and CNN became increasingly polarized between 1998 and 2004. However, Hoewe and colleagues [34] found a stronger influence of right-leaning (vs. left-leaning) media, with Fox News consumption predicting endorsement of stricter anti-immigration policies, whereas neither MSNBC nor CNN exerted an effect on consumers’ views toward immigration.
Further, other work suggests some unique effects of media exposure. For instance, although disparaging news coverage of a political candidate may lead to more negative attitudes toward the candidate, it is less clear whether favorable coverage of a political candidate increases positive attitudes [24]. Others suggest that exposure to positive coverage of viewers’ non-preferred candidate may even backfire, leading to greater endorsement for their prior candidate preference [28,35,36]. Still others find no effect of Fox News on political attitudes among Republicans, Democrats, or Independents [37,38] or no effect of partisan newspaper consumption on voting [39].
Of note, most partisan news research has relied on cross-sectional correlational data drawing on convenience samples or on quasi-experimental work that relies on pre-existing groups (e.g., viewers of a particular broadcast). Further, most partisan media research relies exclusively on political candidate preference or voting patterns, which do not capture the complexity of polarization in the sociopolitical landscape (e.g., gun control, women’s rights, military support, immigration). Psychological research on media effects have also been criticized for frequent reliance on “ecologically invalid research materials, such as artificial laboratory-produced stimuli, rather than actual media” [40 (p.376); see also 41]. Indeed, experimental manipulations often rely on artificial text-based news articles that can be far-removed from the actual news content that people experience in the real-world [e.g., 42]. Other limitations include asking participants to select a single source from a limited range of options (e.g., do you watch Fox News or MSNBC?), and none, to our knowledge, has attempted to quantify the variety of news outlets consumed or length of exposure. As the diversity of news sources grows, such narrow focus on one or a few sources is unlikely to capture an ecologically valid reflection of partisan news use. Moreover, several attempts have been made to examine partisan news effects on political positions over time [e.g., 9,32] but lack statistical control for prior political positions. Therefore, it remains largely unclear whether, or to what extent, partisan media causally impact viewers’ positions on many polarizing issues.
Further, news organizations may modify their content to adapt to increasingly polarized audiences [2,4–7,43], and people may simply select news media that fits pre-existing preferences rather than partisan news impacting viewers over time. Rooted in selective exposure theory, this position posits that people prefer communications that are consistent with their pre-existing opinions [44,45]. Thus, right-leaning news consumption may be associated with right-leaning attitudes, not necessarily because media shapes consumer views over time, but because people right-leaning views choose these media. Indeed, people perceive themselves as largely impervious to the influence of mass media [46,47], and people on both sides of the political spectrum selectively expose themselves to attitude-consistent information, even when it comes at a cost [9,48–52]. For instance, Arendt and colleagues found that both explicit and implicit attitudes toward news outlets predict later choice in news articles, even when headlines from those sources were nearly identical [53], and Knobloch-Westerwick and Meng [54] found evidence for selective exposure to news media, regardless of the topic or political issue presented.
Underpinning these media effects one may expect to find motivated reasoning or motivated social cognition. Jost and colleagues [55], for instance, argue that ideologies may differentially be served by epistemic motives (i.e., to know and understand the world), existential motives (i.e., to survive and thrive in the world), and so-called ideological motives (i.e., rationalizing the status quo, attaining group or personal dominance). As such, people may be more or less drawn to certain types or flavors of media to satisfy psychological needs, and outlets that satisfy such needs are likely to subsequently shape attitudes and opinions. As such, people can be motivated by goals for accuracy but also by “directional” needs, that is, to reach particular conclusions. In the words of Kunda [56], “people are more likely to arrive at conclusions that they want to arrive at, but their ability to do so is constrained by their ability to construct seemingly reasonable justifications for these conclusions” (p. 480). From a motivated cognition viewpoint, media thus has the potential to draw in audiences seeking to reach particular conclusions, but also to provide the rationale for holding particular conclusions or forming opinions/attitudes. Thus, although partisan news may widen political gaps on polarizing issues, consistent with diffusion theory and information processing or motivated social cognition accounts, methodological limitations and the plausibility of selective exposure restrict our understanding of the causal direction between partisan news and viewers’ attitudes. As such, high quality data and rigorous methodologies are needed to determine the impact of partisan news on consumers.
Present investigation
Here we examine relations between partisan news use, ideology, and attitudes, asking whether partisan news exposure can shape sociopolitical positions. We take a multi-method approach to address limitations in previous work and present results from cross-sectional (nationally representative), longitudinal, and experimental designs. In Study 1 we use nationally representative U.S. data to capture accurate and generalizable estimates of relations. In Study 2 we employ a 3-wave longitudinal method to test effects as they play out across time, isolating the unique effects of political news bias on future sociopolitical positions above pre-existing attitudes or ideologies. We also assess whether pre-existing attitudes predict future news use (i.e., a bidirectional model), consistent with selective exposure theory. Using an experiment in a supplemental study (S2 File), we assess whether exposure to real-world partisan news causally impacts sociopolitical positions, and whether this depends on viewers’ pre-existing political ideology. By experimentally manipulating exposure to real world news, this study addresses limitations of determining causality in past research and reliance on fabricated news content.
Nationally representative cross-sectional associations (Study 1)
We predicted that more right-leaning news consumption would be associated with more right-leaning sociopolitical stances, and more left-leaning news consumption would be associated with more left-leaning sociopolitical stances. Attitudes toward immigrants, refugee admittance, the U.S. military, Muslims, feminists, gun control, and general political ideology were explored.
Method
Participants and procedure.
Nationally representative data from Americans (N = 4249) were provided by the 2016 American National Election Studies (ANES) Time Series Study (Mage = 49.31, SD = 17.33; 52.3% female; 71.1% White, 9.3% Black, 3.5% Asian/native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, 10.5% Hispanic, 4.7% other race/multiple races). Participants indicated news outlet use and sociopolitical attitudes. Data can be obtained from the ANES website (https://electionstudies.org/data-center/2016-time-series-study/).
Partisan news use.
Across studies, news source political leanings were determined by a Pew Research Center report [17]. ANES assessed consumption of the following right-leaning news organizations: Fox News, Sean Hannity Show, Rush Limbaugh Show, and Glenn Beck Program, and the following left-leaning news organizations: New York Times, Huffington Post, Washington Post, CNN, NBC, MSNBC, PBS, BBC, and Buzzfeed. This classification is consistent with previous classifications (e.g., refs. 8, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 29, 30). Participants indicated which news sources they use at least once a month (1 = use, 0 = do not use), with responses summed to create indices of right-leaning and left-leaning news use.
Lean of news outlets can also be determined by AllSides (www.allsides.com), an organization that tracks bias in news media and incorporates editorial review, survey research, third-party data, and community feedback to classify American news sources as left-leaning, right-leaning, or centrist. These classifications are largely consistent with classifications made in the current report based on nationally representative data from Pew Research, with a few exceptions. Specifically, whereas Pew Research suggests that BBC and NPR are left-leaning, AllSides considers these sources as centrist. As such, analyses from Study 1 and Study 2 were re-run excluding these sources. Results suggest that removing these sources did not influence the results in any meaningful way, and as such we report findings using classifications based on Pew Research.
Political ideology.
Assessing political ideology, participants classified themselves as extremely liberal to extremely conservative, and rated strength of identification as Democrat or Republican (both 7-point scales). These two correlated items (r = .70) were averaged into a single measure, with higher scores reflecting greater conservatism.
Anti-immigrant attitudes.
Participants responded to the following: “Immigrants are generally good for America’s economy”, “America’s culture is generally harmed by immigrants”, and “Immigrants increase crime rates in the United States” (1 = agree strongly, 5 = disagree strongly). Responses were reversed and averaged, with higher scores reflecting more anti-immigrant attitudes (α = .79).
Anti-refugee attitudes.
Participants indicated the extent to which they favor or oppose allowing Syrian refugees to come to the U.S. (1 = favor a great deal, 7 = oppose a great deal).
Military support.
Participants indicated support for the U.S. sending ground troops to fight Islamic militants, such as ISIS, in Iraq and Syria (1 = oppose a great deal, 7 = favor a great deal).
Anti-muslim attitudes.
Participants indicated how warmly (vs. coldly) they feel toward Muslims using a feeling thermometer (0–100). Scores were reversed, with higher scores reflecting more anti-Muslim attitudes.
Anti-feminist attitudes.
Participants indicated how warmly (vs. coldly) they feel toward feminists using a feeling thermometer (0–100). Scores were reversed, with higher scores reflecting more anti-feminist attitudes.
Permissive gun attitudes.
Participants indicated whether the government should make it easier to obtain a gun (coded as 3), keep the rules the same as they are now (coded as 2), or more difficult to obtain a gun (coded as 1). Higher scores indicate more permissive gun control attitudes.
Statistical approach.
Examination of model variables revealed 15 outliers (0.4% of all data points), or values greater than three SD from the mean, on left-leaning news use, and 136 outliers (3.2% of all data points) on right-leaning news use. Outliers were winsorized (converted to the value at 3 SD from the mean). Missing data (0.4–16.2% for each variable) were estimated using FIML in Mplus v7.4 [57]. Regression analyses were conducted in Mplus v.7.4 using robust maximum likelihood estimation. Left-leaning news use and right-leaning news use were entered as simultaneous predictors of sociopolitical attitudes. Assumptions (normality, homoscedasticity, linearity, multicollinearity) were assessed via visual inspection of plots and statistical values (e.g., skewness, kurtosis, VIF) and were determined to be met for analyses in the current study and the following studies. Threshold for significance for all analyses for all studies was set at p = .05. Mplus code used to conduct this analysis and analyses in the following studies can be found in the S4 File.
Results
See Table 1 for bivariate correlations. Regression results (Table 2) suggest that for all attitudes assessed, greater right-leaning news consumption was associated with more right-leaning stances and greater left-leaning news consumption was associated with more left-leaning stances. Of note, most associations between right-leaning news consumption and attitudes are as strong as those between left-leaning news consumption and attitudes, indicating some symmetry. However, right-leaning (vs. left-leaning) news consumption exhibited a stronger association with military support and conservatism, and left-leaning (vs. right-leaning) news consumption had a stronger association with lowered anti-immigrant and anti-Muslim prejudices. Overall, these findings demonstrate preliminary evidence that partisan news exposure may influence consumers’ views and contribute to political polarization. However, longitudinal and experimental investigations are needed to bolster this causal proposition.
Cross-lagged longitudinal model (Study 2)
Next we used a longitudinal approach predicting that, controlling for pre-existing sociopolitical stances, greater right-leaning news consumption would predict more right-leaning stances and greater left-leaning news consumption would be associated with more left-leaning stances, over time. Moreover, some scholars have suggested a “spiral” effect of news media, whereby news media both has the capacity to influence identity and attitudes, and identity and attitudes, in turn, influence choice in news media [58,59]. For instance, Westerwick and colleagues [60] found that people generally choose to view attitude-consistent information (selective exposure), and that exposure to attitude-consistent information reinforced these pre-existing attitudes. As such, we also assessed whether pre-existing sociopolitical positions predicts partisan news use over time (i.e., a bidirectional model; see Fig 1) to compare hypotheses regarding partisan news impacts (i.e., partisan news polarizes consumers) and selective exposure (i.e., polarized viewers select partisan news). We assessed attitudes toward Muslims, immigrants, terrorism, women’s rights, and gun control, all relevant to the news cycle during data collection.
Correlations between variables (residuals for W2 and W3) within waves were modelled but not shown here for brevity.
Method
Participants and procedure
Data were collected from 650 American residents via Amazon Mechanical Turk (using Cloud Research) at three time points each spaced three months apart. To better ensure high quality data, Cloud Research includes verification of residency, the blocking of suspicious geocode locations, and the blocking of duplicate IP addresses. These checks were in place for the current study in addition to attention checks embedded within each survey to better ensure legitimate and reliable responses. Study 2 (and Study 3, see S2 File) sample sizes were pre-determined via power analysis conducted in Mplus v.7.4 [61] in which Monte Carlo simulations were used with effects estimated using results from Study 1, threshold significance set at p = .05, and power set at .80. In Study 2 and Study 3, ethics approval was obtained from the Brock University Research Ethics Board and written consent was obtained from each participant prior to participation in the study. At each wave, participants indicated news use and sociopolitical positions (see S3 File for full materials in Studies 2 & 3). Data can be obtained from https://osf.io/4k8s7/. Participants who failed attention checks at two or three waves were excluded (n = 16), as were participants self-categorizing as Muslim, immigrant, or a women’s rights activist (i.e., target groups), as per common practice in intergroup research (e.g., refs. [62,63]). After exclusions, the sample at Wave 1 (W1) comprised 484 participants (42.9% female, 57.1% male, Mage = 36.98, SD = 11.72; 80.6% White, 11.6% Black, 5.8% Asian, 5.8% Hispanic/Latino/South American, 1.6% another race/ethnicity; 55.2% Christian, 35.8% atheistic/agnostic, 6.6% practiced another religion). Of the full sample, 69.4% completed Wave 2 (W2), and 65.1% completed all three waves. Little’s test suggested that data were not missing completely at random (χ2 (680) = 762.49, p = .015). However, t-tests suggest that participants who completed all three waves did not differ from those who dropped out after W1 on any of the model variables (ps range from .093 to .993).
Partisan news use.
Participants rated how often they used news sources over the last month (1 = would never use, 8 = used daily). New York Times, Huffington Post, Washington Post, NPR, CNN, NBC, MSNBC, PBS, BBC, Politico, Al Jazeera America, and New Yorker were averaged to create a measure of left-leaning news use (W1 α = .90, W2 α = .90, W3 α = .90). Use of Fox News, The Blaze, Rush Limbaugh Show, Glenn Beck Program, Drudge Report, and Breitbart were averaged to create a measure of right-leaning news use (W1 α = .83, W2 α = .84, W3 α = .83).
Political ideology.
Participants indicated how liberal or conservative they are in social policy, economic policy, and in general, using 7-point Likert scales (ref. [64]; 1 = very liberal, 7 = very conservative; W1 α = .92, W2 α = .92, W3 α = .93).
Anti-muslim attitudes.
The 7-item Modern Racism Scale [65] was adapted to tap anti-Muslim attitudes (e.g., “Muslims have more influence on government policies than they ought to have”; 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree; W1 α = .92, W2 α = .93, W3 α = .93).
Anti-immigrant attitudes.
Participants indicated anti-immigrant attitudes via a 5-item measure adapted from Pereira and colleagues [66] (e.g. “America is made a better place to live by people coming to live here from other countries”, reverse coded; 1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree; W1 α = .92, W2 α = .93, W3 α = .93).
Terrorism imminence beliefs.
Beliefs in imminent terrorism were assessed using a 4-item measure adapted from Cox and Cheyne [67] (e.g., “I’m sure it’s only a matter of time before our country experiences another terrorist attack”; 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree; W1 α = .73, W2 α = .79, W3 α = .73).
Anti-feminist attitudes.
Negative attitudes towards women’s rights activists were assessed via a 5-item measure created for this study (e.g. “Women’s rights protesters are just complainers.” 1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree; W1 α = .92, M = 3.30, SD = 1.72; W2 α = .93, M = 3.41, SD = 1.78; W3 α = .94, M = 3.33, SD = 1.82).
Permissive gun attitudes.
Permissive gun attitudes were assessed via a 5-item measure adapted from Igielnik and Brown [68] (e.g. “Being allowed to own a gun is essential to one’s sense of freedom:” 1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree; W1 α = .85, W2 α = .85, W3 α = .86).
Statistical approach.
Outliers (27 values 3 SD from the mean) were winsorized. Missing data were estimated using FIML in Mplus v7.4. Using robust maximum likelihood estimation with Mplus we tested a cross-lagged panel model which included all lag-1 autoregressive paths (e.g., W1 political ideology predicts W2 political ideology, and W2 political ideology predicts W3 political ideology), all lag-1 cross-lagged paths (i.e., from news use to sociopolitical positions, and from sociopolitical positions to news use), and correlations between residuals within each wave. There was no significant decrement when constraining W1-W2 paths equal to W2-W3 paths for any given variable relative to when these paths were freely estimated (see S1 Table). We therefore retained the most parsimonious (i.e., constrained) model (for a similar procedure see refs. [69,70]).
Results
Results for the crossed-lagged paths can be seen in Table 3, with autoregressive paths found in S1 Table. Controlling for pre-existing attitudes, greater right-leaning news consumption predicted more right-leaning stances on every issue assessed except terrorism imminence. Greater left-leaning news use predicted more left-leaning stances on all attitudes except terrorism imminence. Thus, in support of diffusion and information processing theories, the consumption of partisan news pulls consumers’ attitudes in the direction of the news slant over time. Moreover, associations for right- and left-leaning news are similarly strong, consistent with the general symmetry in Study 1. However, there are two notable exceptions; right-leaning (vs. left-leaning) news use was a particularly strong predictor of anti-women and anti-Muslim attitudes.
In contrast, there is less evidence that prior positions impacted choice in partisan news (after controlling for pre-existing news choice). More pro-gun attitudes predicted less use of left-leaning (but not right-leaning) news, and more anti-Muslim attitudes predicted greater consumption of right-leaning (but not left-leaning) news. However, no other attitude significantly predicted later partisan news consumption. Overall, rather than viewers selecting media suiting their pre-existing attitudes, partisan news appears to lead viewers to adopt perspectives increasingly consistent with the media they consume over time.
In the S2 File we provide details on an experiment among Canadian university undergraduates (largely White and female, approximately half identifying as Christian). We randomly assigned participants to view a 10-minute clip of actual media footage covering ISIS and Syrian refugees from either left-leaning or right-leaning news sources; those in the control viewed sports news. Only the right-leaning news coverage generated more anti-refugee attitudes, military support, and terrorism imminence beliefs (with no effects on Muslim attitudes). Data can be obtained from https://osf.io/4k8s7/.
General discussion
As daily news can be markedly partisan and powerful in disseminating knowledge, thorough investigation of its effects is crucial, particularly against a backdrop of mixed findings. Our findings suggest that partisan news media can alter perceptions of reality, shape worries and sociopolitical positions, and widen political divides. First, large-scale cross-sectional data showed that greater consumption of right-leaning news was associated with greater conservatism and more right-leaning sociopolitical positions. In contrast, left-leaning news was associated with less conservatism and more left-leaning stances on politically polarizing issues. As nationally representative data, these findings offer high-quality point-estimates on the magnitude of these relations.
Next, using a longitudinal approach we found that, controlling for pre-existing sociopolitical positions, right-leaning news led to more negative attitudes toward immigrants, women’s rights, Muslims, gun control, and greater conservatism months later. Moreover, left-leaning news independently led people to adopt more positive attitudes toward immigrants, women’s rights, Muslims, gun control, and less conservatism months later. These findings suggest that political news leanings influence ideologies and attitudes of viewers, and that these effects are evidenced several months downstream. These effects (news → attitudes & ideology) were more reliable and robust than the reverse pathway. Partisan news appears able to shift one’s general political ideology to be more consistent with the political leaning of the outlet, which may result in greater polarization over time. In experimentally manipulating news exposure in the third study we found that right-leaning news (vs. control) caused increased terrorism imminence beliefs, anti-refugee attitudes, and support for military action against terrorism. The results from this Canadian experiment, however, warrant follow up with a nationally representative sample in the U.S., with equivalent numbers of men and women in the sample.
Overall, our findings demonstrate that associations between partisan news consumption and attitudes are not entirely due to selective exposure to belief-confirming information, but rather that partisan news sources can shift positions on politically polarizing issues in ways that can widen political divides on key sociopolitical issues. We observed some symmetries in the strength of associations between right-leaning news use and sociopolitical stance and between left-leaning news use and sociopolitical stances, but there were some notable and important exceptions. In particular, left-leaning (vs. right-leaning) news use had a particularly strong negative association with anti-immigrant and anti-Muslim attitudes, but only in Study 1. In contrast, right-leaning (vs. left-leaning) news use had a particularly strong association with some sociopolitical stances including military support and conservatism in Study 1, anti-Muslim attitudes in Study 2, and in Study 3 only the right-leaning (not left-leaning) news condition differed from the control. Thus, right-leaning (vs. left-leaning) news may have a particularly strong effect on certain sociopolitical attitudes. Future research on news media effects may explore the conditions under which left-leaning and right-leaning news consumption show symmetry or asymmetry in their impact on attitudes. Future research may also explore the effects of partisan media in other countries more culturally dissimilar to the United States or Canada, to assess cultural differences in news media effects or compare effects across different types of samples.
Closing remarks
Deepening political polarization is cause for concern; solutions to contemporary problems such as the climate crisis and refugee resettlement require collaborative if not consensual views on the basic facts involved. Based on the present results we encourage news organizations to be mindful of their role in shaping social attitudes and beliefs about imminent harm. Consumers in turn must recognize differences between news outlets and appreciate how news shapes their reactions, something that does not come naturally [46,47]. If seeking to reduce political polarization, exposure to a variety or plurality of news outlets may be beneficial. Indeed, some theorists suggest that the effect of any single partisan news source may diminish when in the presence of other, competing news sources, and that such variety may balance to create an unbiased perspective [71–73]. With migration, gun rights, women’s rights, and political polarization representing pressing concerns in the 21st century, reducing polarization may be critical in fostering global stability.
Supporting information
S1 Fig. Experimental effect of left-leaning news and right-leaning news exposure on social attitudes (Study 3).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264031.s001
(PNG)
S1 Table. W1-W2 and W2-W3 autoregressive paths from cross-lagged model.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264031.s002
(DOCX)
S1 File. Additional details regarding constraining paths (Study 2-longitudinal model).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264031.s003
(DOCX)
S2 File. Experimental manipulation (Study 3).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264031.s004
(DOCX)
References
- 1.
Bennett LW. News: The politics of illusion. 3rd ed. White Plains (NY): Longman; 1996.
- 2.
West DM. The rise and fall of the media establishments. Boston (MA): St. Martin’s Press; 2001.
- 3. Baum MA, Kernell S. Has cable ended the golden age of presidential television? Am Polit Sci Rev. 1999; 93: 99–114.
- 4. Hollander BA. Tuning out or tuning elsewhere? Partisanship, polarization and media migration from 1998 to 2006. Journal Mass Commun Q. 2008; 85: 957–977.
- 5. Bernhardt D, Krasa S, Polborn M. Political polarization and the electoral effect of media bias. J Public Econ. 2008; 92: 1092–1104.
- 6. Gasper JT. Reporting for sale: The market for news coverage. Public Choice. 2009; 141: 493–508.
- 7. Gentzkow M, Shapiro JM. Media bias and reputation. J Polit Econ. 2006; 114: 280–316.
- 8.
Jamieson K, Cappella J. Echo chamber: Rush Limbaugh and the conservative media establishment. New York: Oxford University Press; 2008.
- 9. Stroud NJ. Media use and political predispositions: Revisiting the concept of selective exposure. Polit Behav. 2008; 30: 341–366.
- 10. Baum MA. Red state, blue state flu state: Media self-selection and partisan gaps in swine flu vaccinations. J Health Polit Policy Law. 2011; 36: 1021–1059. pmid:21948819
- 11. Hoewe J, & Peacock C. The power of media in shaping political attitudes. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2020; 34: 19–24.
- 12.
Prior M. Post-broadcast democracy: How media choice increases inequality in political involvement and polarizes elections. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2007.
- 13. Druckman JN, Peterson E, Slothuus R. How elite partisan polarization affects public opinion formation. Am Polit Sci Rev. 2013; 107: 57–79.
- 14. Della Vigna S, & Kaplan E. The Fox News effect: Media bias and voting. Q J Econ, 2007; 122, 1187–1234.
- 15.
Rogers EM. Diffusion of Innovations. 1st ed. New York: Free Press; 1962.
- 16.
Rogers EM. Diffusion of Innovations. 5th ed. New York: Free Press; 2003.
- 17. Mitchell A, Gottfried J, Kiley J, Matsa KE. Where news audiences fit on the political spectrum. Pew Research Center. 2014 Oct 21 [Cited 2021 Aug 4]. Available from: https://www.journalism.org/2014/10/21/political-polarization-media-habits/.
- 18. McKay H. The troubling US post-prison experiment to ‘rehabilitate’ an ISIS supporter. Fox News. 2017 Jul 18 [Cited 2021 Aug 4]. Available from: https://www.foxnews.com/us/the-troubling-us-post-prison-experiment-to-rehabilitate-an-isis-supporter.
- 19. Walters J. An incredible transformation’: how rehab, not prison, worked for a US Isis convert. The Guardian. 2018 Jan 4 [Cited 2021 Aug 4]. Available from: https://www.theguardian.com/us-news/2018/jan/04/american-isis-abdullahi-yousuf-rehabilatation.
- 20. Sobieraj S, Berry JM. From incivility to outrage: Political discourse in blogs, talk radio, and cable news. Polit Commun. 2011; 28: 19–41.
- 21. Woods J, Arthur C. The threat of terrorism and the changing public discourse on immigration after September 11. Sociol Spectr. 2014; 34: 421–441.
- 22. Aday S. Chasing the bad news: An analysis of 2005 Iraq and Afghanistan war coverage on NBC and Fox News Channel. J Commun. 2010; 60: 144–164.
- 23. Morris JS, Francia PL. Cable news, public opinion, and the 2004 party conventions. Polit Res Q. 2010; 63: 834–849.
- 24. Smith G, Searles K. Who let the (attack) dogs out? New evidence for partisan media effects. Public Opin Q. 2014; 78: 71–99.
- 25. Steinberg J. Cable channel nods to ratings and leans left. The New York Times. 2007 Nov 6 [Cited 2021 Aug 4]. Available from: https://www.nytimes.com/2007/11/06/business/media/06msnb.html.
- 26. Feldman L. The opinion factor: The effects of opinionated news on information processing and attitude change. Polit Commun. 2011; 28: 163–181.
- 27. Page BI, Shapiro RY, Dempsey GR. What moves public opinion? Am Polit Sci Rev. 1981; 81: 23–44.
- 28. Taber CS, Cann D, Kucsova S. The motivated processing of political arguments. Polit Behav. 2009; 31: 137–155.
- 29. de Zúñiga HG, Correa T, Valenzuela S. Selective exposure to cable news and immigration in the U.S.: The relations between Fox News, CNN, and attitudes toward Mexican immigrants. J Broadcast Electron Medi. 2012; 56: 597–615.
- 30. Li N, Hilgard J, Scheufele DA, Winneg KM, Jamieson KH. Cross-pressuring conservative Catholics? Effects of Pope Francis’ encyclical on the U.S. public opinion on climate change. Clim Change. 2016; 139: 367–380.
- 31. Feldman L, Maibach EW, Roser-Renouf C, Leiserowitz A. Climate on cable: The nature and impact of global warming coverage on Fox News, CNN, and MSNBC. Int J Press/Polit. 2012; 17: 3–31.
- 32. Levendusksy MS. Why do partisan media polarize viewers? Am J Pol Sci. 2013; 57: 611–623.
- 33. Morris JS. The Fox News factor. Int J Press/Polit. 2005; 10: 56–79.
- 34. Hoewe J, Peacock C, Kim B, Barnidge M. The relationship between Fox News use and Americans’ policy preferences regarding refugees and immigrants. Int J Commun-US. 2020; 14: 2036–2056.
- 35. Redlawsk DP. Hot cognition or cool consideration? Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision making. J Polit. 2002; 64: 1021–1044.
- 36. Taber CS, Lodge M. Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American J Polit Sci. 2006; 50: 755–769.
- 37. Hopkins DJ, Ladd JM. The consequences of broader media choice: Evidence from the expansion of Fox News. Quart J Polit Sci. 2012; 9: 115–135.
- 38. Hainmueller J. Entropy balancing for causal effects: A multivariate reweighting method to produce balanced samples in observational studies. Polit Anal. 2012; 20:25–46.
- 39. Gentzkow M, Shapiro JM, Sinkinson M. The effect of newspaper entry and exit on electoral politics. Am Econ Rev. 2011; 101:2980–3018.
- 40. Giles D, Shaw R. The psychology of news influence and the development of media framing analysis. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2009; 3/4: 375–393.
- 41.
Gauntlett D. Moving Experiences: Media Effects and Beyond. 2nd ed. London (UK): John Libbey; 2005.
- 42. Brescoll V, LaFrance M. The correlates and consequences of newspaper reports of research on sex differences. Psychol Sci. 2004; 15: 515–520. pmid:15270995
- 43. Mullainathan S, Shleifer A. The market for news. Am Econ Rev. 2005; 95: 1031–1052.
- 44. Sears DO, Freedman JL. Selective exposure to information: A critical review. Public Opin Q. 1967; 31: 194–213.
- 45.
Stroud NJ, Muddiman A. The American media system today: Is the public fragmenting? In: Ridout TN, editor. New directions in media and politics. New York: Routledge; 2012. pp. 6–23.
- 46. Duck JM, Mullin B. The perceived impact of the mass media: Reconsidering the third person effect. Eur J Soc Psychol. 1995; 25:77–93.
- 47. Lambe JL, McLeod DM. Understanding third-person perception processes: Predicting perceived impact on self and others for multiple expressive contexts. J Comm. 2005; 55: 277–291.
- 48. Barberá P, Jost JT, Nagler J, Tucker JA, Bonneau R. Tweeting from left to right: Is online political communication more than an echo chamber? Psycol Sci. 2015; 26: 1531–1542. pmid:26297377
- 49. Frimer JA, Skitka LJ, Motyl M. Liberals and conservatives are similarly motivated to avoid exposure to one another’s opinions. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2017; 72: 1–12.
- 50. Iyengar S, Hahn KS, Krosnick JA, Walker J. Selective exposure to campaign communication: The role of anticipated agreement and issue public membership. J Polit. 2008; 70: 186–200.
- 51. Iyengar S, Hahn KS. Red media, blue media: Evidence of ideological selectivity in media use. J. Commun. 2009; 59: 19–39.
- 52. Nam HH, Jost JT, Van Bavel JJ. (2013). “Not for all the tea in China!” Political ideology and the avoidance of dissonance-arousing situations. PLOS ONE. 2013; 8: 1–8. pmid:23620724
- 53. Arendt F, Northup T, Camaj L. Selective exposure and news media brands: Implicit and explicit attitudes as predictors of news choice. Media Psychol. 2009; 22: 526–543.
- 54. Knobloch-Westerwick S, Meng J. Looking the other way: Selective exposure to attitude-consistent and counterattitudinal political information. Comm Res. 2009; 36: 426–448.
- 55. Jost JT, Kruglanski AW, Sulloway FJ. Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychol. Bull. 2003;129: 339–375. pmid:12784934
- 56. Kunda Z. The case for motivated reasoning. Psychol. Bull. 1990; 108: 480–498. pmid:2270237
- 57.
Múthen LK, & Múthen BO. Mplus user’s guide 7th ed. Los Angeles: Múthen & Múthen; 2015.
- 58. Slater MD. Reinforcing spirals: The mutual influence of media selectivity and media effects and their impact on individual behavior and social identity. Commun Theor. 2007; 17: 281–303.
- 59. Slater MD. Reinforcing spirals model: Conceptualizing the relationships between media content exposure and the development and maintenance of attitudes. Media Psychol. 2015; 18: 370–395. pmid:26366124
- 60. Westerwick A, Johnson BK, Knobloch-Westerwisk S. Confirmation biases in selective exposure to political online information: Source bias vs content bias. Commun Monogr. 2017; 84: 343–364.
- 61. Thoemmes F, MacKinnon DP, & Resier MR. (2010). Power analysis for complex mediational designs using Monte Carlo methods. Struct. Equ. Model. 2010; 17: 510–534. pmid:23935262
- 62. Christ O, Schmid K, Lolliot S, Swart H, Stolle D, Tausch N, et al. Contextual effect of positive intergroup contact on outgroup prejudice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2014; 111: 3996–4000. pmid:24591627
- 63. Hässler T, Ullrich J, Bernardino M, Schnabel N, Valdenegro D, Van Laar C, et al. A large-scale test of the link between intergroup contact and support for social change. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020; 4: 380–386. pmid:31988440
- 64. Skitka LJ, Mullen E, Griffin T, Hutchinson S, & Chamberlin B. Dispositions, scripts or motivated correction? Understanding ideological differences in explanations for social problems. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2002; 83: 470–487. pmid:12150241
- 65. McConahay JG, Hardee BB, Batts V. Has racism declined in America? It depends on who’s asking and what is asked. J. Confl. Resolut. 1981; 25: 563–579.
- 66. Pereira C., Vala J., & Costa-Lopes R. From prejudice to discrimination: The legitimizing role of perceived threat in discrimination against immigrants. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2010; 40: 1231–1250.
- 67. Cox S. J., & Cheyne A. J. T. (2000). Assessing safety culture in offshore environments. Saf. Sci. 2000; 34: 111–129.
- 68.
Igielnik R, Brown A. Key takeaways on Americans’ views of guns and gun ownership. 2017 June 22 [Cited 2021 Sep 23] Pew Research Center. Available from: https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2017/06/22/key-takeaways-on-americans-views-of-guns-and-gun-ownership/.
- 69. Del Toro J, Lloyd T, Buchanan KS, Robins SJ, Bencharit LZ, Smiedt MG, et al. The criminogenic and psychological effects of police stops on adolescent black and Latino boys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2019; 116: 8261–8268. pmid:30962370
- 70. Swart H, Christ O, Hewstone M, Voci A. Affective mediators of intergroup contact: A three-wave longitudinal study in South Africa. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2011; 101: 1221–1238. pmid:21728450
- 71. Chong D, Druckman JD. Dynamic public opinion: Communication effects over time. Am. Political Sci. Rev. 2010; 104: 663–680.
- 72. Mullainathan S, Shleifer A. The market for news. Am. Econ. Rev. 2005; 95: 1031–1052.
- 73.
Zaller J. The myth of massive media impact revived: New support for a discredited idea. In: Multz DC, Sniderman PM, & Brody RA, editors. Political persuasion and attitude change. Anne Arbor: University of Michigan Press; 1996. p. 17–28.