Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Neural responses associated with attentional engagement and disengagement from threat in high socially anxious children: Evidence from temporal-spatial PCA

  • Erika Wauthia ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    erika.wauthia@umons.ac.be

    Affiliations Laboratory of Cognitive Psychology and Neuropsychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Mons, Mons, Belgium, Interdisciplinary Research Center in Psychophysiology and Cognitive Electrophysiology, Mons, Belgium, National Fund for Human Science Research, National Fund for Scientific Research (FNRS), Brussels, Belgium

  • Fabien D’Hondt,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Univ. Lille, INSERM U1172, CHU Lille, Centre Lille Neuroscience & Cognition, Lille, France, CHU Lille, Clinique de Psychiatrie, Unité CURE, Lille, France, Centre national de ressources et de résilience Lille-Paris (CN2R), Lille, France

  • Wivine Blekic,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Laboratory of Cognitive Psychology and Neuropsychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Mons, Mons, Belgium, Interdisciplinary Research Center in Psychophysiology and Cognitive Electrophysiology, Mons, Belgium, National Fund for Human Science Research, National Fund for Scientific Research (FNRS), Brussels, Belgium

  • Laurent Lefebvre,

    Roles Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Laboratory of Cognitive Psychology and Neuropsychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Mons, Mons, Belgium, Interdisciplinary Research Center in Psychophysiology and Cognitive Electrophysiology, Mons, Belgium

  • Laurence Ris,

    Roles Conceptualization, Software, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Interdisciplinary Research Center in Psychophysiology and Cognitive Electrophysiology, Mons, Belgium, Neurosciences Laboratory, Faculty of Medicine, University of Mons, Mons, Belgium

  • Mandy Rossignol

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Laboratory of Cognitive Psychology and Neuropsychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Mons, Mons, Belgium, Interdisciplinary Research Center in Psychophysiology and Cognitive Electrophysiology, Mons, Belgium

Abstract

Background

Cognitive models indicated that social anxiety disorder (SAD) would be caused and maintained by a biased attentional processing of threatening information. This study investigates whether socially anxious children may present impaired attentional engagement and disengagement from negative emotional faces, as well as their underlying event-related potential responses.

Methods and findings

Fifteen children with high levels of social anxiety (HSA; 9 boys; mean age = 9.99y; SD = 1.14) and twenty low socially anxious children (LSA; 16 boys; mean age = 10.47y; SD = 1.17) participated in a spatial cueing task in which they had to detect targets following neutral/disgusted faces in a valid or invalid location. No group effect was reported on reaction times [p>.05]. However, electrophysiological data showed lower P3a amplitude in HSA children compared with the LSA group when processing facial stimuli. They also reported larger N2 amplitudes for valid-disgusted targets and a larger P3a amplitude for the invalid-disgusted ones.

Conclusion

In terms of electrophysiological data, our results validated, the hypothesis of attentional disengagement difficulties in SAD children. We also confirm the idea that high levels of social anxiety are associated with cognitive control impairments and have a greater impact on the processing efficiency than on the performance effectiveness.

1. Introduction

Social anxiety disorder (SAD) refers to a condition in which individuals experience fear when they are performing a specific social task under the scrutiny of others [1]. SAD affects up to 10% of the pediatric population [25]. It can develop as early as 8 years old [6] and is associated with several negative consequences on social, academic and familial life, that can predict a more severe and persistent form of the disorder in adulthood [7, 8].

Recent research on the cognitive factors of SAD highlighted that the onset and maintenance of the disorder may arise from a biased attentional system in favor of threat-related stimuli such as human faces showing signs of disapproval or criticism [913]. Three mechanisms acting at different information processing stages are likely to contribute to this enhanced attentional processing, known as attentional bias (AB) for threat [14]: (i) a lower detection threshold for threatening stimuli resulting in an initial hypervigilance; (ii) a sustained attention to threatening cues leading to attentional disengagement impairments and to a stronger cognitive elaboration of the threat; (iii) a subsequent attentional avoidance when the threat is displayed for longer periods [15, 16]. The most widely used paradigm to assess AB is the visual dot-probe task [17] in which paired stimuli (e.g. a threat-neutral word pair or an angry-neutral face pair) are presented simultaneously on a computer screen. The stimuli then disappear and are followed by a visual target (e.g. a dot), presented in the spatial location previously occupied by the threatening (valid condition) or the neutral (invalid condition) stimulus. AB for threat classically infer from shorter reaction times (RTs) to detect targets in the valid condition and by longer RTs to detect targets in the invalid condition [18].

The attentional control theory [19, 20] postulates that attentional control plays a central role in the occurrence of AB for threat in anxiety disorders and has been supported by behavioral [21, 22] and neuroimaging results [23, 24]. Attentional control is known to be critical for inhibition and attentional shifting [19, 20, 25]. Inhibition enables the withstanding of interference from task-irrelevant stimuli or responses, while attentional shifting underlies attention flexibility abilities and the attentional focus on task-relevant stimuli of importance [20, 25]. Because of the inhibition and shifting deficits, anxious individuals would present a less efficient top-down attentional control, leading to an imbalance between the goal-oriented and the stimulus-driven attentional systems. Consequently, anxious individuals would display a hyperactive stimulus-driven system associated with the facilitated detection of threatening cues and attentional engagement towards them.

While AB for threat has been well-documented regarding anxious adults [9], there are inconsistent results among children. On the one hand, there is evidence for attentional hypervigilance for threat in children with various types of anxiety disorders (for a review, see [26]). In particular, Roy et al. (2008) [27] demonstrated a greater AB for angry faces in children aged between 7 and 18 years old and suffering from a generalized anxiety disorder, SAD and/or separation anxiety disorder, in comparison to their healthy peers. Results from the study conducted by Waters and colleagues (2011) [28] on socially anxious children aged between 5 and 12 years old revealed an AB for angry faces only in children with high symptom severity, while children with low symptom severity showed a bias away from the threat. On the other hand, several studies failed to show any AB for threat in children suffering from various types of anxiety disorders [9, 29]. The model proposed by Eysenck et al. (2007) also posits that group differences between anxious and non-anxious individuals for threat processing may not appear at behavioral level because anxiety acts more on processing efficiency (i.e. the amount of cognitive efforts needed) than on performance effectiveness (i.e. the performance quality). In other words, anxious individuals may recruit more neural resources during task performance to compensate for their reduced attentional control and their inhibition impairments, and finally behave as non-anxious individuals. Therefore, behavioral measures that focus on performance effectiveness may be not sensitive enough to assess efficiency deficit and detect AB. The application of this model to children suffering from social anxiety could therefore justify the inconsistencies observed in the literature.

The event-related potentials (ERP) technique seems particularly relevant to measure these two dimensions of efficiency and effectiveness. Indeed, because of its high temporal resolution, it allows to examine the time course of cerebral activations and to infer on cognitive processing steps leading to behavioral performance. Accordingly, ERPs may allow disentangling whether AB for threat is due to a faster engagement for threat and/or to a difficulty in disengaging from threat [30, 31]. They can also identify whether anxiety influences information processing and examine whether compensatory mechanisms appear, even in the absence of group differences between anxious and non-anxious individuals at a behavioral level. Finally, this noninvasive method is well-suited to experimentation with young children [32].

Only a few studies used ERPs to assess the cerebral correlates of threat processing in children with SAD [33] or at high risk for SAD [34, 35] and results showed increased amplitudes in early and late ERP components. Notably, these studies found an enhancement of the P1 amplitude in response to angry faces in these children. The P1 is a positive deflection that peaks approximately 90–110 ms after the occurrence of the stimulus [36, 37]. It is elicited from the extrastriate areas of the visual cortex when a stimulus is detected [38] and indexes early, pre-attentive processes [39]. Its amplitude increases for attended stimuli and can be modulated by top-down attention processes [40]. The results obtained by Bechor and colleagues (2019) [33] and by Pollak and Tolley-Schell (2003) [34] suggest that anxious children may devote more attentional resources associated with early orienting processes towards threatening facial stimuli. Bechor et al. (2019) [33] also found a larger N170 amplitude in anxious children by comparison to a control group, particularly when exposed to threatening cues. The N170 is a negative early component peaking around 140 and 180 ms after stimulus onset at occipito-parietal sites [41, 42] and is related to the processing of facial structures [41, 43]. The third interesting ERP component to investigate in anxious children would be the N2. The N2 is a fronto-central negativity generated by the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the orbito-frontal cortex [44, 45] which occurs 200–300 ms post-stimulus and reflects cognitive control [46] and conflict monitoring [24, 47]. As for that component, only Thai, Taber-Thomas and Pérez-Edgar (2016) [35] reported that, during facial cues processing, increased amplitudes were associated with attentional bias as measured through RTs. Interestingly, these authors also found that only non-anxious children had higher P1 amplitudes for targets replacing angry faces, suggesting that non-anxious children may also be prone to show a hypervigilance towards threat-cued locations. Finally, ERP studies conducted in pediatric populations also investigated two higher-order cognitive ERP components, namely the P3a, which is associated with the detection and involuntary attentional orienting towards an incoming distractor stimuli [4851] and the P3b, which is thought to reflect involuntary switching of attention towards distractors and their inhibition, according to the specific contents to the task at hand [50, 52]. Both components peak between 300 and 500 ms after stimulus onset [53] but while the P3a is thought to be maximal at fronto-central sites, the P3b is maximal at centro-parietal sites [49, 52, 54]. Bechor and colleagues (2019) [33] found reduced P3b in response to all facial stimuli in a group of anxious children aged between 8 and 12 by comparison to a control group. Authors have interpreted this result as indexing a poorer attentional control in pediatric anxiety. Furthermore, Pollak and Tolley-Schell [34] found, in children at higher risk of anxiety disorder, larger P3b amplitudes regarding the processing of targets following invalid angry faces comparing with invalid happy faces, suggesting an increased need for resources to disengage attention from a location previously occupied by a threat.

However, two major difficulties have been outlined in studies measuring ERP in pediatric populations. First, the child’s brain is still developing and components may present particularities in terms of topography or temporality, which complicates their identification and functional understanding [5557]. Secondly, as outlined by Kujawa, Weinberg, Hajcak and Klein (2013) [58], EEG data in children are more likely to include noise and artifacts. These two considerations point to the need for a more refined analysis method to obtain more consistent ERP data [58]. In this perspective, temporal-spatial PCA have been proposed as a useful and more reliable measurement data for separating sources of variability in components and differentiating latent components from unsystematic sources of noise in adults [59, 60] and children [60, 61].

The aim of the current study was to assess the cognitive correlates of the processing of emotional stimuli in children with social anxiety and to provide support for the phenomenon of AB by recording evoked potentials and subjecting the data to a temporal-spatial PCA. To this end, we used an emotional spatial-cueing task, in which children had to detect a target preceded by a single cue presented in the left or the right hemifield. Cues may orient attention toward a valid location (when the target will appear on the same hemifield) or an invalid one (when targets appear on the opposite hemifield). Unlike the visual dot-probe task using a pair of cues, this paradigm allows to postulate that participants focus their attention on the single cue, and allows distinguishing between attentional engagement and disengagement processes [62]. Moreover, we chose to use faces expressing disgust instead of anger because disgust faces convey a negative evaluation of rejection and avoidance and are rated even more negatively than angry faces by anxious individuals [6367].

We hypothesized that all children would display an attentional preference for disgusted faces by comparison to neutral faces because of the increased arousal of these kinds of stimuli. However, we also postulated that this effect would be more prominent in HSA children, since social anxiety increases attention towards social threat. Following the main assumptions of the attentional control theory, we postulated that more group differences were to be observed at the electrophysiological level than at the behavioral level because of the supposed greater impact of high levels of anxiety on processing efficiency than on performance effectiveness. In other words, we did not expect to find significant group differences on the RTs associated with the processing of targets, but we predicted to observe such differences on the ERP (e.g. increased amplitudes) during both the processing of threatening facial cues and targets following them.

2. Method

2.1 Participants

Fifteen children (6 girls) aged between 8 and 12 years old (mean age = 9.99; SD = 1.17) and presenting high levels of social anxiety were selected to participate in this study. We selected this age range because of evidence suggesting that more than 50% of socially anxious children develop their symptoms before 13 years old [68] and that symptoms can already be seen in children as young as 8 years old [6]. Children were included in the high social anxiety (HSA) group if they showed a score of 18 or higher at the Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory for Children (SPAI-C; [69]). They were paired to twenty low socially anxious (LSA) children (4 girls; mean age = 10.47; SD = 1.14), reporting a score below 18 at the SPAI-C. All children were also assessed for state and trait anxiety using the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (STAI-C; [70]). The sample characteristics are presented in Table 1. All children were free from learning, neurologic or other psychiatric disorders as assessed with the Child Behavior Checklist [71, 72]. The ethical board of the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences of the University of Mons approved this study. All participants and their legal guardian gave written informed consent. The work described has been carried out in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the Declaration of Helsinki. Children received a €20 gift-card for their participation.

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic and psychopathological data for the LSA and HSA groups (** p-values < .01; * p-values < .05) (M = male; F = female).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261172.t001

2.2 Task and materials

The emotional spatial-cueing task was adapted from the protocol used by Bar-Haim, Morag and Glickman (2011) [30]. The task was administered using E-Prime 2.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA) on an Asus X756U system (screen of 17 inches) in the Cognitive Psychology and Neuropsychology research laboratory of the University of Mons. Each trial began with a fixation cross appearing at the center of the screen for 500 milliseconds. Then, two rectangular frames (600 mm X 1000 mm) appeared on the right and the left side of the fixation cross for 500 milliseconds, and one of them showed a face depicting either a disgusted or a neutral expression. The pictures shown were faces of 10 different children (5 boys and 5 girls) from the Radboud Faces Database [73]. After the faces disappeared, a target (a star) (400 mm X 400 mm) appeared at the center of one of the rectangles. In 50% of the trials, targets appeared in the previous location of the face (valid trials) and in the remaining 50%, targets appeared on the opposite side of the screen (invalid trials). Participants were told to indicate as fast as possible the location of the target by pressing the corresponding button of the mouse. Targets remained on the screen until a response was made. A new trial began 500 milliseconds after the target offset. Before the test, participants received 16 randomly presented practice trials, 4 per condition (Valid/Invalid Neutral; Valid/Invalid Disgust). The entire task comprised 512 trials, presented in 8 blocks of 64 trials each. Within each block, 50% of the trials presented disgusted faces and 50% presented neutral faces. Trials were presented in a randomized order.

2.3 Behavioral data

RTs for each response corresponded to the time between the presentation of the target and the button press. Only RTs for correct responses were analyzed. Trials with RT longer than 2000 milliseconds and shorter than 150 milliseconds and then those with RT higher than 2.5 SDs above or lower than -2.5 SDs below the participants’ mean were discarded in order to reduce the influence of outliers. Individual mean RTs were computed for each experimental condition on the remaining trials (93.88% of the data).

2.4 EEG acquisition, preprocessing and analysis

EEG data were recorded at a sampling rate of 500 Hz (V-Amp, Brain Products, GmbH, Munich, Germany: 0–500 Hz bandwidth, 24-bit A/D conversion) from 64 electrodes sites (AF3, AF4, AF7, AF8, AFz, C1, C2, C3, C4, C5, C6, CP1, CP2, CP3, CP4, CP5, CP6, CPz, Cz, F1, F2, F3, F4, F5, F6, F7, F8, FC1, FC2, FC3, FC4, FC5, FC6, FCz, Fp1, Fp2, FT10, FT7, FT8, FT9, Fz, O1, O2, Oz, P1, P2, P3, P5, P6, P7, P8, PO3, PO4, PO7, PO8, POz, Pz, T7, T8, TP10, TP7, TP8, and TP9), with an actiCap acquisition system arranged in a standard 10–20 layout (Brain Products, GmbH, Munich, Germany). One electrode (P4) has been excluded from further analyzes due to poor functioning during data acquisition. The reference electrode was located at FCz throughout the recording. The ground electrode was placed on the forehead between Fp1 and Fp2. Electrode impedances were kept below 10 kΩ. Off-line analysis was performed using Brain Vision Analyzer 2 software (Brain Products, GmbH, Munich, Germany). We applied a band-pass IIR filter (0.1 to 30 Hz) and then, to correct eye blink artifacts, we used a semi-automatic ocular correction function implemented in the Analyzer 2.0 that used a simplified version of the independent component analysis (ICA) to correct ocular artifact specifically in the EEG signal [74]. This method has been proven to be useful to separate neural activity from muscle and blink artifacts in EEG data (e.g. [75]) and has been shown to present several advantages compared to other artifact removal methods (see [74]). All data were re-referenced to the average of all scalp electrodes. Finally, both for faces and targets processing, we created epochs by segmenting data from 200 milliseconds before and 500 milliseconds after stimulus onset. Epochs were baseline corrected using the mean voltage calculated from the 200 milliseconds preceding the event. Each epoch of 700 milliseconds with at least one channel showing a maximal amplitude difference of 200 μV was excluded from further analysis. Stimulus-locked ERPs were averaged separately for faces and targets.

ERP averages for each participant in each experimental conditions were submitted to temporal-spatial principal component analyses (PCA) using the ERP PCA Toolkit, version 2.7 [76] and respected the published guidelines for applying PCA to ERP datasets [77, 78]. PCA analyses are used to extracts linear combinations of data that distinguish patterns of electro-cortical activity across all-time points and recording sites [58]. Separated analyses were conducted for faces and targets processing. Firstly, a temporal PCA was performed to capture the variance across time points and to maximize the initial separation of ERP components [79]. All-time points were used as variables, and we considered all subjects, conditions and recording sites as observations. Simulation studies previously conducted suggested that Promax rotations are the most effective for temporal analyses since they do not force orthogonality amongst the components [76, 78]. Therefore, a Promax rotation was used, and following this first rotation, a parallel test [80] was conducted on the resulting Scree plot [81], in which the Scree of our dataset is compared to a Scree plot derived from a fully random dataset. The number of factors retained is thus based on the largest number of factors that account for a greater proportion of variance of the fully random data set (see [76] for more information). Based on this criterion and on the resulting Scree plots [81], seventeen temporal factors were extracted for faces processing (total variance explained = 97.3%), and eighteen temporal factors were extracted for targets processing (total variance explained = 91.5%). Each temporal factor may be considered to be a virtual epoch and can be described both by its factor loading (which describes the time course of that factor) and its factor scores (which give that factor’s value for each combination of subject, picture type, and recording site). Spatial information is preserved by temporal PCA; scalp topography can be reconstructed for any time point, subject, and condition by multiplying the corresponding electrode scores by the factor loading and standard deviation [82].

A spatial PCA was then performed on each of the temporal factors to synthesize the spatial dimensions of the dataset. Since simulation studies suggest that Infomax rotations are the most effective for spatial analyses (52, 54), an Infomax rotation was used for faces and targets processing separately. Recording sites were used as variables and all subjects, conditions and temporal scores were used as observations, thus a spatial PCA was conducted on each temporal factor retained in the previous step. Based on the average Scree plots resulting from the parallel test [80], six spatial factors were extracted for each of the 17 temporal factors identified for faces processing (total variance explained = 83.7%), resulting in 102 unique combinations of factors for faces processing. Following the same procedure for targets processing, six spatial factors were extracted for each of the 17 temporal factors identified (total variance explained = 84.3%), resulting in 108 combinations of factors. The covariance matrix and Kaiser normalization were used for each PCA [83]. To assess timing and spatial voltage distributions directly, we translated all factors back into voltages (μV) by multiplying factor scores by their corresponding loadings and standard deviations; in this way, both the time course and scalp topography of the electrocortical activity captured by that temporospatial factor combination can be directly assessed [59].

The temporal-spatial factors that accounted for at least 0.5% unique variance, i.e. 40 factors for faces processing and 39 factors for targets processing, were subjected to a robust ANOVA [8486] to determine which components were significantly modulated by experimental conditions. As recommended by Dien and colleagues (2005), a visual inspection of the waveforms associated with each combination of factors was used to select those that corresponded the most to ERP components relevant to the paradigm used. Accordingly, and following the literature on the electrophysiological correlates of AB in anxious children [3335], we considered as being representative for the P100, a maximal relative positivity recorded at occipital electrodes at ∼100 to 150 ms after stimulus onset. We will consider for the N170, a negative temporal deflection occurring and for the N200, a fronto-central negativity, peaking between 150 and 230 ms and 200 and 300 ms following stimulus onset, respectively. The P3a would be associated to a positivity located at fronto-central sites and peaking at ∼300 to 400 ms after stimulus onset; and for the P3b, a positivity located at parieto-occipital sites and peaking at ∼300 to 400 ms after stimulus onset.

2.5 Statistical analyses

Comparisons between groups were performed on anxiety (social, trait and state anxiety) characteristics using: (i) Wilcoxon rank-sum tests or independent t-tests for quantitative variables when the distributions of these variables were significantly different from normal or not, respectively; (ii) Pearson chi-square tests for qualitative variables. The normality of the distribution of quantitative parameters was assessed with Shapiro–Wilk tests. For RTs, General Linear Model analyses were conducted with 2-Emotion (Disgust and Neutral) X 2-Validity (Valid, Invalid) X 2-Visual Field (Left, Right) as within-subject factors and Groups (HSA, LSA) as the between-subjects factor. All the analyses have been performed using the software IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Version 21, Armonk, NY, IBM Corp.). The significance level was set at p < .05 (two-tailed) throughout the analyses.

Regarding ERP data, to avoid the biasing effects of non-normality, (co)variance heterogeneity between groups, and to reduce Type I errors [87], robust analyses of variance (ANOVA) were conducted on the selected temporal-spatial factors using the ERP PCA Toolkit [76, 85]. The seed for the number generation was set at 1000, and the number of iterations used for bootstrapping was 4,999 [77, 88]. Given potential variability in p values using this approach [84], simulations were run 11 times, with median p values reported, and only results in which the median p value plus 2 standard deviations remained below 0.05 were considered significant [84]. Robust ANOVA tests are indicated by “TWJt/c,” and the interpretation of this statistic and resulting p values are identical to a conventional ANOVA. Further information about robust ANOVA tests can be found at Dien (2010) [76].

A 2-Emotion (Disgust and Neutral) X 2-Visual Field (Left and Right) X 2-Group (HSA and LSA) analysis was conducted on temporal-spatial factors associated with faces processing. A 2-Emotion (Disgust and Neutral) X 2-Visual Field (Left and Right) X 2-Validity (Valid and Invalid) X 2-Groups (HSA and LSA) analysis was conducted on the temporal-spatial factors associated with targets processing. Only temporospatial factors for which statistically significant results were found are presented in the results section.

3. Results

3.1 Demographic and psychopathological data

Data are presented in Table 1. Groups did not significantly differ on sex ratio [U = 120; p = .201], age [t(33) = 1.22; p = .233] or state anxiety [U = 123.5; p = .370]. Social anxiety [t(33) = -8.32; p < .001] and trait anxiety [t(32) = -2.05; p = .049] was higher in the HSA group than in the LSA group.

3.2 Behavioral data

Mean RTs obtained by each group in each experimental condition are presented in Fig 1. ANOVA conducted on RTs only revealed a significant interaction between Emotion and Validity [F (1,33) = 28.28; p < .001]. However, paired sample T tests conducted on this interaction failed to demonstrate significant differences between conditions [p-values > .05].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Mean reaction times (in milliseconds) obtained by each group of children in each experimental condition of the emotional spatial-cueing task.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261172.g001

3.3 ERP data

3.3.1 Faces processing.

The original grand average waveforms for each face type and visual field are presented in Fig 1A. No significant effect was found on the factors corresponding to the N170 and the P3b components.

3.3.1.1 P1. The two factors (TF1/SF2, TF1/SF3) representing the P1 component peaked at 154 milliseconds at parieto-occipital recording sites (Table 2 and Fig 2B). For the TF1/SF2 factor, the main effect of Emotion was significant [TWJT/C (1.0, 30.0) = 5.41; p = .029], showing a higher amplitude for neutral faces [M = 1.93] than for disgusted faces [M = 0.91]. The main effect of EFE was also significant for the TF1/SF3 factor [TWJT/C (1.0, 20.1) = 6.12; p = .025], again showing an increased amplitude for neutral [M = 2.64] compared to disgusted faces [M = 2.15].

thumbnail
Fig 2. ERPs associated with faces processing.

(a) Grand average ERPS prior to PCA analyses for the two types of faces (disgust and neutral) and the two visual fields (RVF = right visual field: LVF = left visual field) obtained at Fz, Cz, and POz; (b)Topographic maps for the temporal-spatial factors associated with faces processing.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261172.g002

thumbnail
Table 2. PCA factor combinations selected for further analysis on faces and targets processing.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261172.t002

3.3.1.2 P3a. The TF3/SF3 factor was associated with the temporal window of the P3a, peaking at 382 milliseconds at frontal recording sites (Table 2 and Fig 2B). The main effect of Group was significant [TWJT/C (1.0, 27.5) = 4.63; p = .002] with a greater amplitude in the LSA group [M = 0.75] than in the HSA group [M = -0.43].

3.3.2 Targets processing.

The original grand average waveforms for each face type and visual field are presented in Fig 3A.

thumbnail
Fig 3. ERPs associated with targets processing.

(a) Grand average ERPS prior to PCA analyses for the two types of faces (disgust and neutral) and the two validity conditions (valid and invalid), and the two visual fields (RVF = right visual field: LVF = left visual field) obtained at Fz, Cz, and POz; (b)Topographic maps for the temporal-spatial factors associated with targets processing.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261172.g003

3.3.2.1 P1. The two factors associated with the P1 component (TF2/SF1, TF2/SF2) peaked at 154 milliseconds at parietal and parieto-occipital recording sites (Table 2 and Fig 3B). The interaction between Emotion and Validity was significant for the TF2/SF1 factor [TWJT/C (1.0, 24.9) = 5.45; p = .030]. This interaction could be decomposed in a significant effect of Validity for targets following neutral faces only [TWJT/C (1.0, 25.5) = 6.17; p = .023], showing an increased P1 amplitude for invalid-neutral [M = 9.54] compared to the valid-neutral targets [M = 8.91]. For the TF2/SF2 factor, there was a significant main effect of Emotion [TWJT/C (1.0, 18.7) = 8.22; p = .010] with higher amplitudes for targets following neutral faces [M = 1.43] compared to disgusted faces [M = 1]. A main effect of Validity was also found on this factor [TWJT/C (1.0, 29.8) = 29.11; p < .001], reflecting a greater P1 amplitude for targets in the valid [M = 1.51] than in the invalid condition [M = 0.93]. The interaction between Emotion and Validity was also significant for the TF2/SF2 factor [TWJT/C (1.0, 24.9) = 15.66; p = .004]: amplitude was higher for targets in the disgust-valid [M = 1.05] condition compared to the neutral-valid condition [M = 0.90; TWJT/C (1.0, 19.3] = 15.09; p = .004]. A significant interaction between Emotion and the Visual Field was found on the TF2/SF2 factor [TWJT/C (1.0, 27.8) = 16.79; p < .001] highlighting that, when the target was presented on the left side of the screen, the P1 amplitude was increased for neutral [M = 0.12] compared to disgusted faces [M = -0.84; TWJT/C (1.0, 21.7) = 18.61; p < .001].

3.3.2.2 N2. The two factors associated with the N2 were negative components peaking at 288 milliseconds at fronto-parietal (TF4/SF2) and parieto-occipital (TF4/SF4) recording sites (Table 2 and Fig 3B). For the TF4/SF2 factor, there was a significant main effect of Emotion [TWJT/C (1.0, 26.9) = 10.98; p = .006] showing more negative amplitudes for targets following neutral faces [M = 0.53] than for targets following disgusted faces [M = -0.26]. The interaction between Emotion and Validity was significant for the TF4/SF2 factor [TWJT/C (1.0, 29.5) = 39.99; p < .001], reflecting, in the valid condition, an increased amplitude for targets following disgusted faces [M = -0.35] in comparison to targets following neutral faces [M = 1.80; TWJT/c (1.0, 24.9) = 8.91; p = .005].

For the TF4/SF4 factor, a significant interaction between Emotion and Validity was found [TWJT/c (1.0, 28.7) = 10.65; p = .005] and indicated a wider N2 amplitude for disgusted faces [M = -0.12] than for neutral faces [M = 0.45] in the valid condition only [TWJT/c (1.0, 24.9) = 8.91; p = .005]. The interaction between Emotion and Visual Field was significant [TWJT/c (1.0, 27.4) = 4.60; p = .038] and revealed that, for targets presented on the left side of the screen, N2 amplitude was bigger for those following disgusted faces [M = -0.90] than for neutral faces [M = -0.42]. Finally, there was a significant interaction between Group, Emotion and Validity for the TF4/SF4 factor [TWJT/c (1.0, 28.7) = 21.18; p < .001]. For the children with HSA, the interaction between Emotion and Validity [TWJT/c (1.0, 13.0) = 28.68; p < .001] was significant, showing a more negative amplitude for targets in the disgust-valid [M = -0.54] compared to the disgust-invalid condition [M = 0.17; TWJT/c (1.0, 13.0) = 13.96; p = .006]. The opposite pattern was found for targets preceded by neutral faces, with a more negative amplitude in the neutral-invalid [M = -0.55] in comparison to the neutral-valid condition [M = -0.30; TWJT/c (1.0, 13.0) = 14.37; p = .019]. The interaction between Emotion and Validity was not significant for the LSA group [TWJT/c (1.0, 17.0) = 0.97; p = .33].

3.3.2.3 P3a. The TF3/SF2 factor was associated with the P3a component and peaked at 340 milliseconds at frontal recording sites (Table 2 and Fig 3B). Moreover, the TF3/SF3 factor, a negative deflection that peaked at 340 milliseconds at parieto-occipital sites, could be topographically considered as a negative counterpart of the P3a component.

There was a significant main effect of Emotion for the TF3/SF2 [TWJT/c (1.0, 26.8) = 31.5; p < .001] indicating wider amplitudes for targets appearing after disgusted faces [M = 0.02] than for targets appearing after neutral faces [M = -0.82; TWJT/c (1.0, 26.8) = 31.50; p < .001]. The main effect of Validity was also significant, with an enhanced amplitude for targets appearing in the invalid condition [M = 0.24] compared to the valid condition [M = -1.03; TWJT/c (1.0, 23.2) = 46.68; p < .001]. There was a significant interaction between Emotion and Validity [TWJT/c (1.0, 28.5) = 41.53; p < .001] that showed larger amplitudes for targets in the neutral-valid [M = -2.02] than in the disgust-valid condition [M = -0.04; TWJT/c (1.0, 28.0) = 61.41; p < .001]. No such effect was observed for targets in the invalid condition [TWJT/c (1.0, 26.6) = 1.91; p = .017].

The interaction between Emotion and Visual Field was significant [TWJT/c (1.0, 30.0) = 39.08; p < .001], indicating that, for targets presented on the left side of the screen, P3a amplitude was larger for targets following disgusted faces [M = 4.27] than neutral faces [M = 2.37; TWJT/c (1.0, 25.8) = 51.03; p < .001]. Finally, the interaction between Emotion, Visual Field and Validity was also significant [TWJT/c (1.0, 29.1) = 73.59; p < .001]. This interaction could be decomposed in a significant interaction between Emotion and Validity when targets are presented on the left side of the screen [TWJT/c (1.0, 29.8) = 88.68; p < .001] which indicates larger P3a amplitude for targets in the disgust-valid [M = 3.70] than in the neutral-valid condition [M = -0.58; TWJT/c (1.0, 27.4) = 93.84; p < .001]. No such effect was observed for targets in the invalid condition [TWJT/c (1.0, 29.7) = 3.18; p = .084].

Analyses on the TF3/SF3 factor showed a significant interaction between Group, Emotion and Validity [TWJT/c (1.0, 27.7) = 4.89; p = .036]. For children with HSA, there was a significant interaction between Emotion and Validity [TWJT/c (1.0, 13.0) = 7.17; p = .021] showing lower amplitudes for targets in the disgust-valid [M = -2.53] than in the disgust-invalid condition [M = -2.07; TWJT/c (1.0, 13.0) = 6.43, p = .021]. We found no significant effect of Validity for neutral faces [p = .07]. No significant effect was observed for disgusted and for neutral faces for LSA [p-values > .05].

4. Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the mechanisms underlying the attentional bias towards disgusted faces in socially anxious children. To this end, 15 children with HSA and 20 children with LSA, all aged between 8 and 12, performed an emotional spatial-cueing task requiring them to detect a target following either neutral or disgusted faces while their electroencephalograms were recorded.

In line with our expectations, no group differences were found on RTs which confirmed some studies previously conducted in children with various anxiety disorders [8991]. However, some significant differences appeared between experimental groups on various ERP components. We found increased N2 amplitudes for targets following valid disgusted faces and increased P3a amplitudes for targets following invalid disgusted faces in HSA children and in comparison to neutral faces. Taken together, these results support the idea of preferential neural processing of threatening stimuli in children with HSA. More precisely, they suggest that the attention of socially anxious children would have been captured by the presentation of the disgusted faces. Therefore, when asked to process a following target, HSA children would have experienced difficulties and needed more effort to disengage their attention from the faces to detect and evaluate a target appearing on the opposite side of the screen correctly [9, 92]. We also found lower frontal P3a amplitudes in HSA children in comparison to LSA children that can be related to the less effective attentional control frequently reported in anxious children [19, 93, 94] that would lead them to devote less neural resources to the detection and evaluation of faces which are task-irrelevant [33].

Several other significant effects appeared on ERP components, and these were conflicting with our initial hypotheses. Indeed, we documented in all children increased P1 amplitudes for the processing of neutral faces, higher P1 amplitudes for targets following neutral faces in the valid conditions, and enhanced N2/P3a amplitudes for targets following neutral faces in the invalid conditions and in comparison with disgusted faces. The results of our study tend to show that 8 to 12-year-old children have an enhanced and sustained attentional engagement towards neutral faces and that the processing of neutral faces would have required an increased cognitive control [46] and conflict monitoring abilities [24, 47]. Specifically, here, the conflict arose from the fact that participants had to inhibit the cognitive process of the cue to detect the target appearing on the opposite side of the screen correctly. Consequently, results suggest that children’s attentional focus would have been maintained on the spot previously occupied by the neutral faces and that they experienced subsequent attentional disengagement difficulties from these. The observation of increased ERP amplitudes in response to neutral faces was somewhat contradictory to our hypotheses and quite surprising given the documented AB towards negative expressions in pediatric populations (e.g. [95]). However, an increased processing of neutral faces has already been demonstrated in both socially anxious adults [96100] and children [101]. We may interpret these effects within the framework of the attentional control theory [19], which initially states that anxious individuals would display global attentional control impairments that would be inflated during the confrontation to negative facial expressions. As explained by the theory, these impairments would lead to an imbalance between a hyperactive bottom-up attentional system and a hypoactive top-down system. Therefore, we hypothesize that the hyperactivation of the amygdala would have led children to interpret neutral faces as conveying a threatening load [100, 102]. Furthermore, and in line with the works by Kagan et al. [103, 104], one may hypothesize that the arousal level conveyed by the ambiguity of neutral faces may be considered as threatening by all children because of their uncertainty. If neutral faces can be interpreted as threatening, our observations question the relevance of opposing neutral and negative faces to assess attentional biases towards threat.

The observation of such an attentional bias towards neutral in both LSA and HAS group can be understood from a developmental point of view. Indeed, attentional control has been shown to rely on the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [23, 105], which undergoes one of the longest periods of development of any brain region [21, 106] and developmental variabilities have been evidenced in studies involving attentional control functions until the age of 12 [107, 108]. We may posit that the function would still be immature in our selected age range (8–12 years old). Therefore, all the children of our study would have presented immature attentional control abilities, resulting in hypervigilance and facilitated detection of neutrality and in disengagement difficulties from what they may have considered as threatening. However, unlike socially anxious children, with age, we postulate that LSA children will be progressively more able to inhibit and control the automatic processing of threat [109, 110], which stresses the need for longitudinal studies on these issues.

Our findings should be considered cautiously regarding a number of limitations. The first limitation concerns the relatively small sample size and the use of robust ANOVAs that may limit the statistical power of analyses, but which outlined the need for future studies on a larger population. The second concerns the age of the children investigated. Children from our sample were aged between 8 and 12 years old, which prevented us from investigating the modulation of effects with age and with the maturation of attentional control. Studies found developmental variabilities in tasks involving attentional control until the age of 12 [107, 108]. It would thus be interesting to replicate this study with older children. Thirdly, we investigated a sub-clinical sample and not children with clinical levels of SAD. Therefore, we could assume that more pronounced or additional effects could be revealed in clinical samples. Furthermore, the effects–or the lack of effect–on the early visual component such as the P1 may arise from differences in the low-level properties between stimuli. Therefore, it could be useful, in future research, to assess these low-level properties or to control them beforehand. Finally, the presentation duration of our stimuli (500 milliseconds) prevented us from evaluating the components associated with the later-stage of attentional processing such as the late positive potential (LPP); for which interesting results for anxious children have already been demonstrated (for a review, see [39]).

5. Conclusion

This study is the first to investigate the neural responses associated with threat processing in children with high social anxiety levels. Results showed that high levels of social anxiety in children was not associated with behavioral manifestation of AB which confirms one of the main assumptions of the attentional control theory [19, 20] stating that the effects of anxiety are higher on processing efficiency than on performance effectiveness. However, ERP data showed that high socially anxious children experience difficulties to disengage their attention from disgusted faces. Social anxiety appears to act as a filter on the later stages of the (still early) attentional processing of threatening social stimuli. Our data confirmed the presence of attentional control deficits in HSA children, as suggested by the lower P3a amplitudes in response to all emotional faces in this group. They also demonstrated that HSA children appears to commit more neural resources to obtain behavioral performances similar to the ones of LSA children, as suggested by the increased N2 and P3a amplitudes for targets associated with disgusted faces. This suggests the applicability of the distinction between the notions of performance effectiveness and processing efficiency made by the attentional control theory [19, 20]. Our research also allows to assume that the N2 and the P3a components should be considered in further studies as biomarkers of high levels of social anxiety, notably when investigating the efficacy of attentional training sessions designed to reduced anxiety levels. Finally, we hypothesized that the ambiguity conveyed by neutral faces leads children to devote more neural resources to perceive them and perhaps interpret them as threatening probably due to the immaturity of the cerebral regions implicated in attentional control abilities in this age range. Further studies should investigate AB towards neutral social stimuli.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Alyssa Picone and Juliette Giacobbe (University of Mons) for their help in the recruitment of participants and for data collection.

References

  1. 1. Hofmann SG, Otto MW. Cognitive behavioral therapy for social anxiety disorder: Evidence-based and disorder specific treatment techniques: Routledge; 2017.
  2. 2. Kashani JH, Orvaschel H. A community study of anxiety in children and adolescents. The American journal of psychiatry. 1990;147(3):313. pmid:2309948
  3. 3. Fichter MM, Kohlboeck G, Quadflieg N, Wyschkon A, Esser G. From childhood to adult age: 18-year longitudinal results and prediction of the course of mental disorders in the community. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2009;44(9):792–803. pmid:19212695
  4. 4. Schneier FR. Social anxiety disorder. New England Journal of Medicine. 2006;355(10):1029–36.
  5. 5. Hearn CS, Donovan CL, Spence SH, March S, Holmes MC. What’s the worry with social anxiety? Comparing cognitive processes in children with Generalized Anxiety Disorder and Social Anxiety Disorder. Child Psychiatry & Human Development. 2017;48(5):786–95. pmid:27917455
  6. 6. Van Roy B, Kristensen H, Groholt B, Clench-Aas J. Prevalence and characteristics of significant social anxiety in children aged 8–13 years. Social psychiatry and psychiatric epidemiology. 2009;44(5):407–15. pmid:19015797
  7. 7. Bittner A, Egger HL, Erkanli A, Costello EJ, Foley DL, Angold A. What do childhood anxiety disorders predict? Journal of Child Psychology and psychiatry. 2007;48(12):1174–83. pmid:18093022
  8. 8. Davidson JR, Hughes D, George LK, Blazer DG. The epidemiology of social phobia: findings from the Duke Epidemiological Catchment Area Study. Psychological medicine. 1993;23(3):709–18. pmid:8234577
  9. 9. Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van Ijzendoorn MH. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological bulletin. 2007;133(1):1. pmid:17201568
  10. 10. Mathews A, MacLeod C. Induced processing biases have causal effects on anxiety. Cognition & Emotion. 2002;16(3):331–54.
  11. 11. Mogg K, Bradley BP. A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour research and therapy. 1998;36(9):809–48. pmid:9701859
  12. 12. Vasey MW, Daleiden EL, Williams LL, Brown LM. Biased attention in childhood anxiety disorders: A preliminary study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1995;23(2):267–79. pmid:7642837
  13. 13. Waters AM, Lipp OV. Visual search for emotional faces in children. Cognition and Emotion. 2008;22(7):1306–26.
  14. 14. Cisler JM, Olatunji BO. Components of attentional biases in contamination fear: Evidence for difficulty in disengagement. Behaviour research and therapy. 2010;48(1):74–8. pmid:19781691
  15. 15. Koster EH, Crombez G, Van Damme S, Verschuere B, De Houwer J. Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? Emotion. 2004;4(3):312–7. pmid:15456400
  16. 16. Schmidtendorf S, Wiedau S, Asbrand J, Tuschen-Caffier B, Heinrichs N. Attentional bias in children with social anxiety disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2018;42(3):273–88.
  17. 17. MacLeod C, Mathews A, Tata P. Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of abnormal psychology. 1986;95(1):15. pmid:3700842
  18. 18. Britton JC, Bar‐Haim Y, Carver FW, Holroyd T, Norcross MA, Detloff A, et al. Isolating neural components of threat bias in pediatric anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53(6):678–86. pmid:22136196
  19. 19. Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, Calvo MG. Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion. 2007;7(2):336. pmid:17516812
  20. 20. Derakshan N, Eysenck MW. Anxiety, processing efficiency, and cognitive performance: New developments from attentional control theory. European Psychologist. 2009;14(2):168–76.
  21. 21. Ladouceur CD, Silk JS, Dahl RE, Ostapenko L, Kronhaus DM, Phillips ML. Fearful faces influence attentional control processes in anxious youth and adults. Emotion. 2009;9(6):855. pmid:20001128
  22. 22. Reinholdt-Dunne ML, Mogg K, Bradley BP. Effects of anxiety and attention control on processing pictorial and linguistic emotional information. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2009;47(5):410–7. pmid:19246028
  23. 23. Bishop SJ. Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention. Nature neuroscience. 2009;12(1):92. pmid:19079249
  24. 24. Dennis TA, Chen C-C. Emotional face processing and attention performance in three domains: Neurophysiological mechanisms and moderating effects of trait anxiety. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2007;65(1):10–9. pmid:17383040
  25. 25. Friedman NP, Miyake A. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. Journal of experimental psychology: General. 2004;133(1):101. pmid:14979754
  26. 26. Puliafico AC, Kendall PC. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious youth: A review. Clinical child and family psychology review. 2006;9(3–4):162–80. pmid:17053961
  27. 27. Roy AK, Vasa RA, Bruck M, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Sweeney M, et al. Attention bias toward threat in pediatric anxiety disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008;47(10):1189–96. pmid:18698266
  28. 28. Waters AM, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Pine DS. Attention bias for angry faces in children with social phobia. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology. 2011;2(4):475–89.
  29. 29. Dudeney J, Sharpe L, Hunt C. Attentional bias towards threatening stimuli in children with anxiety: A meta-analysis. Clinical psychology review. 2015;40:66–75. pmid:26071667
  30. 30. Bar‐Haim Y, Morag I, Glickman S. Training anxious children to disengage attention from threat: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52(8):861–9. pmid:21250993
  31. 31. Clarke PJ, MacLeod C, Guastella AJ. Assessing the role of spatial engagement and disengagement of attention in anxiety-linked attentional bias: a critique of current paradigms and suggestions for future research directions. Anxiety, Stress & Coping. 2013;26(1):1–19. pmid:22136158
  32. 32. Chronaki G, Broyd SJ, Garner M, Benikos N, Thompson MJ, Sonuga-Barke EJ, et al. The moderating effect of self-reported state and trait anxiety on the late positive potential to emotional faces in 6–11-year-old children. Frontiers in psychology. 2018;9:125. pmid:29515476
  33. 33. Bechor M, Ramos ML, Crowley MJ, Silverman WK, Pettit JW, Reeb-Sutherland BC. Neural correlates of attentional processing of threat in youth with and without anxiety disorders. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 2019;47(1):119–29. pmid:29607460
  34. 34. Pollak SD, Tolley-Schell SA. Selective attention to facial emotion in physically abused children. Journal of abnormal psychology. 2003;112(3):323. pmid:12943012
  35. 35. Thai N, Taber-Thomas BC, Pérez-Edgar KE. Neural correlates of attention biases, behavioral inhibition, and social anxiety in children: An ERP study. Developmental cognitive neuroscience. 2016;19:200–10. pmid:27061248
  36. 36. Allison T, Puce A, Spencer DD, McCarthy G. Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. I: Potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex by face and non-face stimuli. Cerebral cortex. 1999;9(5):415–30. pmid:10450888
  37. 37. Sass SM, Heller W, Stewart JL, Silton RL, Edgar JC, Fisher JE, et al. Time course of attentional bias in anxiety: emotion and gender specificity. Psychophysiology. 2010;47(2):247–59. pmid:19863758
  38. 38. Csibra G, Kushnerenko E, Grossmann T. Electrophysiological methods in studying infant cognitive development. 2008.
  39. 39. Wauthia E, Rossignol M. Emotional processing and attention control impairments in children with anxiety: an integrative review of event-related potentials findings. Frontiers in psychology. 2016;7:562. pmid:27199802
  40. 40. Luck SJ, Woodman GF, Vogel EK. Event-related potential studies of attention. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2000;4(11):432–40. pmid:11058821
  41. 41. Bentin S, Allison T, Puce A, Perez E, McCarthy G. Electrophysiological Studies of Face Perception in Humans. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 1996;8(6):551–65. pmid:20740065
  42. 42. Rossion B, Gauthier I, Tarr M, Pirenne D, Debatisse D, Despland P. The N170 occipito-temporal component is delayed to inverted faces but not to inverted objects: Electrophysiological evidence of face-specific processes in the human brain. NEUROIMAGE. 1999;9:S864–S.
  43. 43. Eimer M. The face-specific N170 component reflects late stages in the structural encoding of faces. Neuroreport. 2000;11(10):2319–24. pmid:10923693
  44. 44. Banich MT, Milham MP, Jacobson BL, Webb A, Wszalek T, Cohen NJ, et al. Chapter 29 Attentional selection and the processing of task-irrelevant information: insights from fMRI examinations of the Stroop task. Progress in Brain Research. 134: Elsevier; 2001. p. 459–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0079-6123(01)34030-x pmid:11702561
  45. 45. Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, Van Den Wildenberg W, Ridderinkhof KR. Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: effects of response conflict and trial type frequency. Cognitive, affective, & behavioral neuroscience. 2003;3(1):17–26.
  46. 46. Van Veen V, Carter CS. The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies. Physiology & behavior. 2002;77(4–5):477–82. pmid:12526986
  47. 47. Dennis TA, Chen CC. Trait anxiety and conflict monitoring following threat: an ERP study. Psychophysiology. 2009;46(1):122–31. pmid:19055504
  48. 48. Halgren E, Marinkovic K, Chauvel P. Generators of the late cognitive potentials in auditory and visual oddball tasks. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. 1998;106(2):156–64. pmid:9741777
  49. 49. Polich J. Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical neurophysiology. 2007;118(10):2128–48. pmid:17573239
  50. 50. Delplanque S, Silvert L, Hot P, Sequeira H. Event-related P3a and P3b in response to unpredictable emotional stimuli. Biological psychology. 2005;68(2):107–20. pmid:15450691
  51. 51. Goldstein A, Spencer KM, Donchin E. The influence of stimulus deviance and novelty on the P300 and novelty P3. Psychophysiology. 2002;39(6):781–90. pmid:12462506
  52. 52. Polich J. Theoretical overview of P3a and P3b. Detection of change: Springer; 2003. p. 83–98.
  53. 53. Segalowitz SJ, Santesso DL, Jetha MK. Electrophysiological changes during adolescence: a review. Brain and cognition. 2010;72(1):86–100. pmid:19914761
  54. 54. Falkenstein M, Hoormann J, Hohnsbein J. ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. Acta psychologica. 1999;101(2–3):267–91. pmid:10344188
  55. 55. Jonkman L, Lansbergen M, Stauder J. Developmental differences in behavioral and event‐related brain responses associated with response preparation and inhibition in a go/nogo task. psychophysiology. 2003;40(5):752–61. pmid:14696728
  56. 56. Ciesielski KT, Harris RJ, Cofer LF. Posterior brain ERP patterns related to the go/no‐go task in children. Psychophysiology. 2004;41(6):882–92. pmid:15563341
  57. 57. Okazaki S, Hosokawa M, Kawakubo Y, Ozaki H, Maekawa H, Futakami S. Developmental change of neurocognitive motor behavior in a continuous performance test with different interstimulus intervals. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2004;115(5):1104–13. pmid:15066536
  58. 58. Kujawa A, Weinberg A, Hajcak G, Klein DN. Differentiating event‐related potential components sensitive to emotion in middle childhood: Evidence from temporal–spatial PCA. Developmental Psychobiology. 2013;55(5):539–50. pmid:22692816
  59. 59. Foti D, Hajcak G, Dien J. Differentiating neural responses to emotional pictures: Evidence from temporal‐spatial PCA. Psychophysiology. 2009;46(3):521–30. pmid:19496228
  60. 60. Weinberg A, Hajcak G. Electrocortical evidence for vigilance‐avoidance in Generalized Anxiety Disorder. Psychophysiology. 2011;48(6):842–51. pmid:21073479
  61. 61. Arbel Y, Donchin E. When a child errs: the ERN and the Pe complex in children. Psychophysiology. 2011;48(1):55–63. pmid:20525012
  62. 62. Rossignol M, Philippot P, Bissot C, Rigoulot S, Campanella S. Electrophysiological correlates of enhanced perceptual processes and attentional capture by emotional faces in social anxiety. Brain Research. 2012;1460:50–62. pmid:22592075
  63. 63. Amir N, Beard C, Bower E. Interpretation bias and social anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2005;29(4):433–43.
  64. 64. Buckner JD, Maner JK, Schmidt NB. Difficulty disengaging attention from social threat in social anxiety. Cognitive therapy and research. 2010;34(1):99–105. pmid:20182655
  65. 65. Charash M, McKay D. Attention bias for disgust. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2002;16(5):529–41. pmid:12396210
  66. 66. Yuan J, Zhang Q, Cui L. Social anxiety and attention to dynamic disgust facial expression: No more attention captured from evidence of N2pc. Neuroscience Letters. 2020;736:135269. pmid:32712349
  67. 67. Mao N, Xia L, Zhang Q, Li C, Cui L. Mechanisms of cross-modal selective attentional bias for negative faces of anger and disgust in high-trait anxiety individuals. NeuroReport. 2020;31(12):879–84. pmid:32427804
  68. 68. Chavira DA, Stein MB, Malcarne VL. Scrutinizing the relationship between shyness and social phobia. Journal of anxiety disorders. 2002;16(6):585–98. pmid:12405519
  69. 69. Beidel DC, Turner SM, Morris TL. A new inventory to assess childhood social anxiety and phobia: The Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory for Children. Psychological assessment. 1995;7(1):73.
  70. 70. Spielberger CD, Edwards C, Lushene R, Montuori J, Platzek D. STAIC preliminary manual. Palo Alto, Calif: Consulting Psychologists Press, t973. 1973.
  71. 71. Achenbach TM, Vermont VDoPUo, Edelbrock CS. Manual for the child behavior checklist and revised child behavior profile: Department of Psychiatry of the University of Vermont; 1983.
  72. 72. Fombonne E, Chedan F, Carradec A, Achard S. Le Child Behavior Checklist: un instrument pour la recherche en psychiatrie de l’enfant. Psychiatrie & psychobiologie. 1988.
  73. 73. Langner O, Dotsch R, Bijlstra G, Wigboldus DH, Hawk ST, Van Knippenberg A. Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database. Cognition and emotion. 2010;24(8):1377–88.
  74. 74. Jung TP, Makeig S, Humphries C, Lee TW, Mckeown MJ, Iragui V, et al. Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology. 2000;37(2):163–78. pmid:10731767
  75. 75. Makeig S, Jung T-P, Bell AJ, Ghahremani D, Sejnowski TJ. Blind separation of auditory event-related brain responses into independent components. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1997;94(20):10979–84. pmid:9380745
  76. 76. Dien J. The ERP PCA Toolkit: An open source program for advanced statistical analysis of event-related potential data. Journal of neuroscience methods. 2010;187(1):138–45. pmid:20035787
  77. 77. Dien J. Evaluating two‐step PCA of ERP data with geomin, infomax, oblimin, promax, and varimax rotations. Psychophysiology. 2010;47(1):170–83. pmid:19761521
  78. 78. Dien J, Khoe W, Mangun GR. Evaluation of PCA and ICA of simulated ERPs: Promax vs. Infomax rotations. Human brain mapping. 2007;28(8):742–63. pmid:17133395
  79. 79. Dien J, Frishkoff G. Principal components analysis of event-related potential datasets. Event-related potentials: A methods handbook. 2005:189–208.
  80. 80. Horn JL. A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika. 1965;30(2):179–85. pmid:14306381
  81. 81. Cattell RB. The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate behavioral research. 1966;1(2):245–76. pmid:26828106
  82. 82. Dien J. Addressing Misallocation of Variance in Principal Components Analysis of Event-Related Potentials. Brain Topography. 1998;11(1):43–55. pmid:9758391
  83. 83. Dien J, Beal DJ, Berg P. Optimizing principal components analysis of event-related potentials: matrix type, factor loading weighting, extraction, and rotations. Clinical neurophysiology. 2005;116(8):1808–25. pmid:15996897
  84. 84. Dien J. Best practices for repeated measures ANOVAs of ERP data: Reference, regional channels, and robust ANOVAs. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2017;111:42–56. pmid:27622381
  85. 85. Keselman H, Wilcox RR, Lix LM. A generally robust approach to hypothesis testing in independent and correlated groups designs. Psychophysiology. 2003;40(4):586–96. pmid:14570166
  86. 86. Pegg S, Dickey L, Mumper E, Kessel E, Klein DN, Kujawa A. Stability and change in emotional processing across development: A 6‐year longitudinal investigation using event‐related potentials. Psychophysiology. 2019;56(11):e13438. pmid:31376164
  87. 87. Dien J, Santuzzi A. In Event-related potentials. A methods handbook. 2005.
  88. 88. Dien J, Franklin MS, May CJ. Is “Blank” a suitable neutral prime for event-related potential experiments? Brain and Language. 2006;97(1):91–101. pmid:16165200
  89. 89. Bechor M, Ramos ML, Crowley MJ, Silverman WK, Pettit JW, Reeb-Sutherland BC. Neural correlates of attentional processing of threat in youth with and without anxiety disorders. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 2018:1–11. pmid:29199360
  90. 90. Benoit KE, McNally RJ, Rapee RM, Gamble AL, Wiseman AL. Processing of emotional faces in children and adolescents with anxiety disorders. Behaviour Change. 2007;24(4):183–94.
  91. 91. Price RB, Siegle GJ, Silk JS, Ladouceur C, McFarland A, Dahl RE, et al. Sustained neural alterations in anxious youth performing an attentional bias task: A pupilometry study. Depression and anxiety. 2013;30(1):22–30. pmid:22700457
  92. 92. Waters AM, Henry J, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Pine DS. Attentional bias towards angry faces in childhood anxiety disorders. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2010;41(2):158–64. pmid:20060097
  93. 93. Susa G, Pitică I, Benga O, Miclea M. The self regulatory effect of attentional control in modulating the relationship between attentional biases toward threat and anxiety symptoms in children. Cognition & emotion. 2012;26(6):1069–83. pmid:22404477
  94. 94. Éismont E, Lutsyuk N, Pavlenko V. Reflection of anxiety in the characteristics of evoked EEG potentials in 10-to 11-year-old children. Neurophysiology. 2009;41(6):435–42.
  95. 95. Dudeney J, Sharpe L, Sicouri G, Lorimer S, Dear BF, Jaffe A, et al. Attentional bias in children with asthma with and without anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2017;45(8):1635–46. pmid:28066857
  96. 96. Wieser MJ, Moscovitch DA. The effect of affective context on visuocortical processing of neutral faces in social anxiety. Frontiers in psychology. 2015;6:1824. pmid:26648889
  97. 97. Birbaumer N, Grodd W, Diedrich O, Klose U, Erb M, Lotze M, et al. fMRI reveals amygdala activation to human faces in social phobics. Neuroreport. 1998;9(6):1223–6. pmid:9601698
  98. 98. Cooney RE, Atlas LY, Joormann J, Eugène F, Gotlib IH. Amygdala activation in the processing of neutral faces in social anxiety disorder: is neutral really neutral? Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 2006;148(1):55–9. pmid:17030117
  99. 99. Gentili C, Gobbini MI, Ricciardi E, Vanello N, Pietrini P, Haxby JV, et al. Differential modulation of neural activity throughout the distributed neural system for face perception in patients with Social Phobia and healthy subjects. Brain Research Bulletin. 2008;77(5):286–92. pmid:18771714
  100. 100. Yoon KL, Zinbarg RE. Threat is in the eye of the beholder: Social anxiety and the interpretation of ambiguous facial expressions. Behaviour research and therapy. 2007;45(4):839–47. pmid:16797485
  101. 101. Melfsen S, Florin I. Do socially anxious children show deficits in classifying facial expressions of emotions? Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 2002;26(2):109–26.
  102. 102. Yoon KL, Zinbarg RE. Interpreting neutral faces as threatening is a default mode for socially anxious individuals. Journal of abnormal psychology. 2008;117(3):680. pmid:18729619
  103. 103. Kagan J. Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity. Child development. 1997;68(1):139–43. pmid:9084130
  104. 104. Kagan J, Snidman N. The long shadow of temperament. Cambridge/Londen: Belknap. Harvard Press; 2004.
  105. 105. Pessoa L, Padmala S, Morland T. Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation. Neuroimage. 2005;28(1):249–55. pmid:15993624
  106. 106. Diamond A. Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. Principles of frontal lobe function. 2002:466–503.
  107. 107. Ruff H, Rothbart M. Attention in development. Oxford University Press, Oxford Google Scholar; 1996.
  108. 108. Booth JR, Burman DD, Meyer JR, Lei Z, Trommer BL, Davenport ND, et al. Neural development of selective attention and response inhibition. Neuroimage. 2003;20(2):737–51. pmid:14568448
  109. 109. Kindt M, Bierman D, Brosschot JF. Cognitive bias in spider fear and control children: Assessment of emotional interference by a card format and a single-trial format of the Stroop task. Journal of experimental child psychology. 1997;66(2):163–79. pmid:9245474
  110. 110. Kindt M, Brosschot JF, Everaerd W. Cognitive processing bias of children in a real life stress situation and a neutral situation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1997;64(1):79–97. pmid:9126629