Figures
Abstract
Objective
To examine the main and interactive effects of the amount of daily television exposure and frequency of parent conversation during shared television viewing on parent ratings of curiosity at kindergarten, and to test for moderation by socioeconomic status (SES).
Study design
Sample included 5100 children from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort. Hours of daily television exposure and frequency of parent screen-time conversation were assessed from a parent interview at preschool, and the outcome of early childhood curiosity was derived from a child behavior questionnaire at kindergarten. Multivariate linear regression examined the main and interactive effects of television exposure and parent screen-time conversation on kindergarten curiosity and tested for moderation by SES.
Results
In adjusted models, greater number of hours of daily television viewing at preschool was associated with lower curiosity at kindergarten (B = -0.14, p = .008). More frequent parent conversation during shared screen-time was associated with higher parent-reported curiosity at kindergarten with evidence of moderation by SES. The magnitude of association between frequency of parent conversation during television viewing and curiosity was greater for children from low SES environments, compared to children from high SES environments: (SES ≤ median): B = 0.29, p < .001; (SES > median): B = 0.11, p < .001.
Conclusions
Higher curiosity at kindergarten was associated with greater frequency of parent conversation during shared television viewing, with a greater magnitude of association in low-SES families. While the study could not include measures of television program content, digital media use and non-screen time conversation, our results suggest the importance of parent conversation to promote early childhood curiosity, especially for children with socioeconomic disadvantage.
Citation: Shah PE, Hirsh-Pasek K, Kashdan TB, Harrison K, Rosenblum K, Weeks HM, et al. (2021) Daily television exposure, parent conversation during shared television viewing and socioeconomic status: Associations with curiosity at kindergarten. PLoS ONE 16(10): e0258572. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572
Editor: Susan Engel, Williams College, UNITED STATES
Received: January 31, 2020; Accepted: September 30, 2021; Published: October 28, 2021
Copyright: © 2021 Shah et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: Data cannot be shared publicly because the data were obtained from a restricted use dataset. Data are available from the Institute of Educational Statistics, from the US Department of Education’s National Center for Education Statistics (NCES) (contact via https://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/licenses.asp) for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data. To elaborate, this manuscript utilizes restricted data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B). The ECLS-B is a nationally representative, population-based longitudinal study sponsored by the Institute of Educational Statistics, from the US Department of Education’s National Center for Education Statistics (NCES). While the ECLS-B is a publicly available dataset, the data used for this analysis comes from the restricted ECLS-B dataset, which requires special access and permission from NCES, prior to accessing the data. The PI (senior author (PES)) had to enter into a data-use agreement with the Institute for Educational Statistics / NCES prior to receiving access for the restricted use data. Per the requirements of the NCES, the data cannot be freely shared with other investigators, and interested investigators must enter into a data use agreement with the NCES prior to accessing the restricted-use data from the Institution for Educational Statistics. Due to NCES's confidentiality legislation, ECLS-B case-level data are available only to qualified researchers who are granted a restricted-use data license from NCES. Information regarding how to obtain a restricted-data license for the ECLS-B can be found at https://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/licenses.asp.
Funding: This work was supported by grants from the University of Michigan, the Zero to Three Academy of Fellows, and NICHD (K08HD078506) to PES, and from the Zero to Three Academy of Fellows to KR. Study sponsors had no role in the study design; the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; the writing of the report; or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Curiosity, an important foundation for scientific innovation [1], is characterized by the drive to seek out new information [2], desire to explore [3], and joy in learning [4, 5]. Higher curiosity has been associated with numerous adaptive outcomes in childhood including more robust word acquisition [6], enhanced learning and exploration [7] and higher academic achievement [8, 9], highlighting the potential importance of fostering curiosity from an early age. Our previous work found a positive association between higher curiosity and higher academic achievement, with a greater magnitude of benefit for children with socioeconomic disadvantage [10], raising the possibility that promoting curiosity in young children may be one way to mitigate the achievement gap associated with poverty [11]. To foster curiosity in early childhood, it is necessary to consider the modifiable contexts that may promote or inhibit its expression.
One potential modifiable factor associated with differences in early childhood outcomes is the amount of daily television exposure. While there is an increasing interest in the role of digital media exposure on child development, televisions are in 98% of all homes, and television viewing remains the dominant screen activity of young children, accounting for 72% of all screen time [12], making television exposure a relevant developmental context in young children. Children are exposed to an average of 1–4 hours of television per day [13, 14], with higher exposure in children who are economically disadvantaged [15, 16]. In previous screen-time research with infants, toddlers and preschoolers, more television exposure has been associated with impaired self- regulation [17, 18], lower language outcomes [19, 20], and lower cognitive development [21, 22], however, association with curiosity has not been examined, and is a gap in the literature. Screen media exposure, including television, can displace exploratory activities such as play and parent-child interactions [23] that are thought to be necessary for the cultivation of curiosity [24]. We therefore sought to test the hypothesis that higher daily television exposure would be associated with lower curiosity (Hypothesis 1). We also considered that the association between the amount of television exposure on early childhood curiosity may be attenuated in children with higher SES, who may have other resources to foster curiosity, compared with low SES children. Therefore, we sought to test whether the association between higher daily television exposure and early childhood curiosity was moderated by SES, with a greater magnitude of effect seen in low SES /under-resourced families (Hypothesis 2).
In addition, because development unfolds through reciprocal interactions between children and their parents, the quality of early dyadic experiences may also play a role in fostering curiosity. Previous work has demonstrated the benefits of parent conversation during shared television viewing on language development, with more frequent conversation moderating the adverse impact of heavy television exposure [25]. Previous research has also demonstrated that parent-child conversation facilitates children’s thinking, learning and exploration (i.e., behavioral indicators of curiosity) through pedagogical exchanges [26]. As such, we hypothesized that more frequent parent conversation during shared television viewing may be associated with higher curiosity (Hypothesis 3a) and may moderate the association between higher television exposure and curiosity (Hypothesis 3b). Furthermore, because the amount and quality of language that young children hear also varies by socioeconomic status [27], (e.g., the 30-million-word-gap) [28, 29], we theorized that there may be a similar “curiosity gap” among low income children who are exposed to less conversation. We hypothesized that, consistent with a cumulative risk model [30], socioeconomic disadvantage in combination with less frequent parental conversation may confer an added risk for lower curiosity, with greatest effects seen in children from low SES families (Hypothesis 4). The overarching aim of this work was to identify modifiable factors in the early caregiving environment (e.g., amount of early television viewing, frequency of parent conversation) which may be important for the promotion of early childhood curiosity, and to examine whether these factors were associated with differential effects in children from under-resourced families. Results from this work will help inform anticipatory guidance to promote early childhood curiosity in at risk populations.
Materials and methods
Study design and sample
Data were drawn from the restricted data of the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), a nationally representative, population-based longitudinal study sponsored by the US Department of Education’s National Center for Education Statistics (NCES). The ECLS-B is based on a nationally representative probability sample of children born in the United States in 2001. Data were collected from children and their parents at age 9 months, 24-months, preschool and kindergarten timepoints, and included parent interviews, and direct and indirect child assessments across multiple settings [31]. Our sample excluded children with congenital and chromosomal abnormalities, and included children born at 22–41 weeks gestation who had kindergarten behavioral data from which we could derive a measure of curiosity. Our study utilized data from birth, 24-months, preschool and kindergarten timepoints, with a final sample of 5100 children. This study was considered exempt by the Institutional Review Board because it involved the use of a publicly available dataset with de-identified participants who could not be linked to the data.
Measures
Outcomes.
Curiosity. Because the ECLS-B did not have a measure to examine curiosity, we derived a measure of curiosity from an existing assessment of child behavior available in the dataset, which included questions from the Preschool and Kindergarten Behavioral Scales Second Edition (PKBS-2) and Social Skills Rating System (SSRS). While we were limited by the questions that were available the parent PKBS-2 questionnaire at the kindergarten timepoint, we drew from previous theoretical work and behavioral descriptions of curiosity in young children [32–38] to select question items that most closely aligned with characteristics of curiosity. While there is no single definition of curiosity [33], there are certain behavioral characteristics of curiosity that are widely accepted, including, (1) the thirst for knowledge, and the drive to understand what one does not know [34]; (2) an exploratory drive to seek novelty [35]; (3) an openness to new experiences [36]; and, in young children, (4) innovation in exploratory play [37, 38]. Four question items from the PKBS-2 which aligned with these characteristics of curiosity were chosen for our “curiosity factor.” The individual question items included (1) shows eagerness to learn new things (i.e., thirst for knowledge); (2) likes to try new things (i.e., drive for novelty); (3) easily adjusts to a new situation (i.e., openness to new experiences); and (4) shows imagination in work and play (i.e., innovation in exploratory play). At the kindergarten timepoint, parents were asked to report the frequency of behaviors observed in the previous 3 months on a 5-point Likert scale (1, never to 5, very often). Items were reverse coded as appropriate such that higher scores indicated more positive behaviors. A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to assure reliability and to calculate the appropriate loading values for deriving our curiosity factor. Standardized scoring of the curiosity factor was conducted, and good internal consistency was demonstrated (α = 0.70, M = 0.07, SD = 1.2) [10]. Individual question items, loading coefficients, and model fit indices for our curiosity factor are shown in S3 Appendix.
Predictors.
Hours of television viewing. Hours of television viewing at preschool were determined from a parent questionnaire at the preschool timepoint. Parents were asked “…about how many hours of television does [your child] watch at home per day,” with responses ranging from 0–24 hours. Respondents who answered “N/A” to this question were not included in the analysis. Because most children (96%) reportedly watched 6 hours or fewer of television per day, the hours of daily television exposure were capped at 6+ hours, reducing the influence of a few statistical outliers.
Parent conversation during shared television viewing. Parent conversation during television viewing was determined from a parent questionnaire at the preschool timepoint. Parents were asked, “In a typical week, when your family watches TV together, how often do you or another family member talk with [your child] about the TV programs?” Responses were coded categorically as 1 = never, 2 = hardly ever, 3 = sometimes, or 4 = often. Parents were not asked to report on the amount of time a child watched television without adults, thus we were unable to control for the amount of time children watched television without adult co-viewing.
Relatedly, there was also no measure of overall (non-television) parental language for the entire sample. As such, we were unable to control for non-screen time parental language. To address this limitation, using a subsample of 500 parent-child dyads with available data on a structured reading task, we examined the association between parent conversation during TV viewing and parent conversation during the reading task. We found a positive association for the frequency of television-related parent conversation and elaborative parent language during the reading task characterized by use of open-ended questions (p = .008) and relating the book to the child’s experience (p = .03). Based on this subsample analysis, we considered that television-related parent conversation may also reflect the quality of the language environment in the home. For the purpose of this study, we considered the amount of parent conversation during shared television viewing to serve as a proxy for the amount of language in the caregiving environment.
Covariates.
In our primary analyses, we included sociodemographic variables that might be associated with the amount of television viewing and curiosity. Specifically, we controlled for maternal age, race/ethnicity, marital status (married/unmarried), maternal education (< high school; high school graduate; > high school), and poverty (< 185% federal poverty line; ≥ 185% federal poverty line). The latter two variables were integrated into a composite measure of household socioeconomic status (SES) at kindergarten [31]. We also controlled for child sex, child age, the type of childcare/preschool experience (no non-parental care; relative/nonrelative home-based care; center-based care), and average number of hours of childcare/center-based care per day. Because lower developmental skills [39] and inability to delay gratification [18] have been associated with increased television exposure, additional covariates included a measure of infant development at 24-months from the Bayley Short-Form Research Edition, and parent-report of delay of gratification at 24-months (“My child is able to wait” dichotomized as “no/yes”). Of note, there was no measure of the content of television programming available in the dataset (i.e., educational programming vs. entertainment), so we were not able to control for television content in our analyses.
Statistical analyses
All analyses were conducted using SAS 9.4 [40] (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC). Maternal and child characteristics were examined using descriptive statistics. Multivariate linear regression utilizing the SURVEYREG (SAS) procedure allowed for tests of associations between hours of daily television viewing, frequency of parent conversation during shared television viewing and kindergarten curiosity in linear and non-linear (quadratic) models, with minimal differences between the linear and quadratic models. We included covariates related to television viewing, parent conversation, and curiosity to adjust for theoretically justified confounds. For our primary analyses, in adjusted models, we tested the association between the hours of television viewing and curiosity at kindergarten (Hypothesis 1), and whether the association between hours of television viewing and curiosity was moderated by SES (Hypothesis 2). We examined whether the amount of parent conversation during shared television viewing at preschool was associated with early childhood curiosity (Hypothesis 3a), and whether the amount of parent conversation moderated the association between the amount of television viewing and early childhood curiosity (Hypothesis 3b). Finally, we examined whether the association between parent conversation during television viewing and curiosity at kindergarten was moderated by SES (i.e., our test of a cumulative risk hypothesis) (Hypothesis 4). In all our moderation analyses, we included the interaction term in the final step of the multivariate regression models. When the interaction was statistically significant (p < .05), we performed a stratified analysis of the association between the predictor and curiosity, adjusting for covariates. Because of the complex sample design, sample weights and the Jackknife method [41] were used to account for stratification, clustering and unit non-response, thereby allowing the weighted results to be generalized to the population of U.S. children born in 2001. In accord with the NCES requirements for ECLS-B data use, reported numbers were rounded to the nearest 50.
Results
Sample characteristics
Of the 6350 children who had behavioral (curiosity) data at kindergarten, 5100 children had television-viewing data at preschool and all covariates, which served as our analytic sample. The 5100 children in our final sample did not differ from the 1250 children who were excluded (due to missing data) on most demographic characteristics. However, children who were excluded were more likely to be non-White, have lower SES, have higher 24-month development, watched fewer hours/day of television, and attended childcare/preschool more hours per day. At the preschool timepoint, parents reported that children watched an average of 2.5 hours of television per day, and almost half of parents (49.8%) reported talking with their children “often” when viewing television together. After applying sample weights, the maternal and child characteristics were generalizable to the US population in 2001. The sample characteristics for the weighted sample are shown in Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the amount of television viewing and parent conversation during shared television viewing are shown in Table 2.
Tests of association between hours of daily television viewing and child curiosity at kindergarten (Hypothesis 1), moderation by SES (Hypothesis 2), and main and moderating effects of parent conversation during shared television viewing (Hypotheses 3a and 3b)
In adjusted models, higher daily television viewing at preschool was associated with lower curiosity at kindergarten (B = -0.14, p = .008) (Hypothesis 1, S1 Appendix). The association between the hours of daily television viewing at preschool and kindergarten curiosity was not moderated by SES (p = .22) (Hypothesis 2). In adjusted models, we also found that more frequent parent screen-time conversation was associated with higher curiosity at kindergarten (p < .001), (Hypothesis 3a, S1 Appendix), but that more frequent parent conversation did not moderate the association between the amount of television exposure and early childhood curiosity (p = .23) (Hypothesis 3b).
Tests of association between parent conversation during shared television viewing and child curiosity at kindergarten, and moderation by socioeconomic status (Hypothesis 4)
We then examined whether the association between the frequency of parent screen-time conversation at preschool and kindergarten curiosity was moderated by socioeconomic status (SES) (Hypothesis 4). We found evidence of moderation by SES, (S2 Appendix), and proceeded to examine this association further by stratifying by lower SES (≤ median) and higher SES (> median), adjusting for the a priori covariates. We found differences in parent-reported curiosity between families from high and low levels of SES, for each category of parent conversation (never, hardly ever, sometimes, often), with stronger association among families from under-resourced environments (i.e., low SES) (Table 3). To test and confirm the linear trend between parent conversation and curiosity, in this model only, we then tested the association with parent conversation coded as a continuous variable (1–4). The linear trend demonstrated that the effect of more frequent parent conversation on curiosity was stronger among low SES families (B = 0.29, p < .001) compared with high SES families (B = 0.11, p < .001) (Fig 1).
B coefficients are unstandardized betas. Bars indicate parent conversation during TV viewing examined as a categorical variable. Lines indicate parent conversation during TV viewing examined as a continuous variable, to test and confirm the linear trend. *: p < .05; **: p < .01. SOURCE: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort. Selected years 2001–2007.
Frequency of parent conversation during shared television viewing and associations with characteristics of childhood curiosity
To further examine the psychometrics of our measure of curiosity and consider the value of each question item, we conducted a post hoc analysis to determine if there were specific features of childhood curiosity that were susceptible to the effects of parent conversation. We ran four models, examining the association between the frequency of parent conversation (as a continuous variable), and each curiosity question item as our outcome, adjusting for a priori covariates. In these models, more frequent parent conversation was positively associated with each curiosity question item, with the greatest magnitude of association demonstrated by “shows imagination in work and play,” (B = 0.14, p < .001) (Table 4). The relatively similar findings across items suggests that our curiosity measure tends to act as a unified construct.
Discussion
This is the first study examining associations among amount of daily television exposure, frequency of parent conversation during shared television viewing at preschool, socioeconomic status, and parent-report of curiosity at kindergarten using a nationally representative sample. In adjusted analyses, we found that higher daily television viewing at preschool had a small but significant association with lower curiosity at kindergarten (Hypothesis 1), but that this association was not moderated by socioeconomic status (SES) (Hypothesis 2). We found that more frequent parent conversation during shared television viewing was associated with higher curiosity at kindergarten (Hypothesis 3a), but that more frequent parent conversation during shared television viewing did not moderate the association between the amount of television exposure and early childhood curiosity (Hypothesis 3b). While we found an association between higher television viewing at preschool and lower parent-report curiosity at kindergarten, we were not able to include measures of the content of the television programming. Because the opportunities for conversation and scaffolding may differ if dyads are watching educational TV versus other type of programming, our inability to include the content of the television programming in our analyses (due to the constraints of the dataset) limits the interpretability of the association between the amount of television viewing and kindergarten curiosity.
We found an association between the amount of parent conversation during shared television watching at preschool and early childhood curiosity (Hypothesis 3a) with evidence of moderation by SES (Hypothesis 4). In both high and low SES families, parents who reported higher amounts of conversation also rated their children as being more curious, with a greater magnitude of association in children from under-resourced families. We have several possible explanations to account for these findings. One interpretation is that parents who report engaging in more conversation may also be more attuned to children’s expression of curiosity (e.g., children’s asking of questions, and engagement in pedagogical exchanges in conversation), and thus they also report their children as having higher curiosity. However, while greater parental conversational exchanges have been associated with more question-asking from their children [42], this explanation does not explain why the magnitude of association between the frequency of parent conversation and curiosity would be greater in low SES children. One possible explanation is that some parents may engage in frequent conversation with their children in settings other than television, but allow their children watch television alone, which may explain why there is an attenuated association between conversations during shared television watching and curiosity for higher SES parents. An alternate explanation is that while the “cumulative risks” of socioeconomic disadvantage and less frequent parental conversation may confer an added risk for lower curiosity [30], the same children who are more vulnerable to suboptimal development (e.g. “lower curiosity”) may also be more susceptible to the effects of more stimulating caregiving environments (e.g. more frequent parent conversation) [43]. This suggests a potential “differential susceptibility” to the quality of the caregiving environment, whereby low-SES children may reap added benefits from language-promotive environments. Prior research has demonstrated how the quality of the linguistic environment in the home (e.g. quality and quantity of language stimulation) can mitigate the effects of socioeconomic disparities (i.e., poverty) on brain structure and later language and literacy outcomes [44, 45]. Our results similarly suggest that the quality of the early linguistic environment (characterized by more frequent parent conversation during shared TV viewing) while promotive of higher curiosity in all children, may be especially beneficial to foster curiosity in children with socioeconomic disadvantage.
These findings have implications for the anticipatory guidance provided to parents. There is some evidence suggesting that children with low curiosity fail engage with their environments in ways that foster motivation, achievement, and more specifically, academic development [46]. Building on our previous work which suggested that higher curiosity can help narrow the achievement gap associated with poverty [10], our results suggest that one potential way to foster curiosity is through facilitating conversational exchanges between children and their parents around moments of shared activity, especially for children from low socioeconomic environments. This aligns with previous language-related research which demonstrates that socioeconomically disadvantaged children preferentially benefit from greater child-directed speech and conversational exchanges [27, 45, 47, 48]. Our findings also highlight the importance of parental scaffolding for child engagement and learning. In the same way that parental engagement with children around shared play with toys facilitates children’s learning and exploration [49], we found that parent conversation (as measured around shared television viewing) could be similarly scaffolding, associated with higher expressions of child curiosity.
Prior research has demonstrated that children learn best in environments that are interactive, encouraging turn-taking, dialogic exchanges and intrinsically motivated questions [47, 50]. Our results similarly attest to this, but with an important consideration for children with socioeconomic disadvantage. While incremental increases in the frequency of parent conversation were associated with higher curiosity for all children, for children from under-resourced (i.e., low SES) environments, only parents who often engaged in conversation around shared television viewing had children whose curiosity scores were above the mean. Conversely, children from more-resourced environments (i.e., high SES) had curiosity scores above the mean even if parents hardly ever conversed when viewing television together. The “curiosity gap” between higher and lower SES children was greatest when parents “never” or “hardly ever” engaged in television-related conversation but was not observed when conversational exchanges occurred “often.” This suggests that for children from under-resourced environments, more frequent parent conversation may help enable the expression of curiosity. One implication is that parents from low SES environments might benefit from anticipatory guidance regarding the importance of dialogic (back and forth) conversation to promote inquisitiveness and learning. Such guidance may include interventions similar to “parent coaching” to facilitate conversational exchanges to promote early language development [51]. At present, because the dominant screen activity of low-income children involves television viewing [52], and because television viewing is essentially non-conversational and non-interactive [53], fostering opportunities for conversational exchanges around television viewing (in addition to other shared activities) may be one potential naturalistic intervention [47].
Our results also indicate that more frequent parent conversation was associated with parent reports of higher imagination at kindergarten. The topics eliciting a child’s curiosity are often related to a child’s idiosyncratic interests [54], and are revealed in the context of responsive, interactive exchanges [55]. Because we hypothesize that conversation around shared television viewing likely included pedagogical exchanges, (e.g., “What do you think is going on? Why do you think that happened?”), our results suggest the possibility that more frequent conversation (in all contexts, not just television viewing) can promote imaginative expression (one of the underpinnings of curiosity [56]) at kindergarten. Interventions to promote dialogic exchanges and language-rich caregiver-child interactions have been shown to be beneficial for early imagination and learning, and may be similarly promotive for early childhood curiosity [57, 58].
Our study had several strengths and limitations. Strengths include the use of a nationally representative sample which included a child behavior questionnaire from which we could derive a measure of curiosity, whose results are generalizable to the population. One limitation is that our study used parent self-reports to measure the amount of television viewing and parent conversation, and our curiosity factor was derived from a single parent-report behavioral measure at the kindergarten timepoint. As such, we acknowledge the potential bias and shared method variance associated with parent report measures. In addition, although a subsample analysis indicated that parents who engaged in more frequent television-related conversation were more likely to use elaborative language, there was no independent measure of non-television parent-child conversation for the entire sample, so we were unable to control for non-screen time language. Although there was a teacher-report of child behavior at kindergarten, it did not include all the “curiosity” items, so we could not examine curiosity across reporters. In addition, the dataset did not contain information regarding the content of the television programs watched, which is a potential confounder which we were not able to include in our analyses. We also acknowledge that while we found significant associations between the hours of television viewing, frequency of parent conversation and parent reports of curiosity, our effect sizes were small. Finally, while the ECLS-B is a rich dataset and among the only longitudinal cohorts from the United States, the data are older, and did not include measures of smartphones and other portable technologies on which television programming may be watched, along with more conversational media such as video-chatting, which is an additional limitation. Future research should consider examining these associations in relation to use of conversational and non-conversational digital media across screen platforms. Future research should also examine other features of curiosity that might help mitigate the poverty achievement gap [59], and consider other adaptive outcomes associated with early childhood curiosity [60]. Despite these limitations, we believe that our results have some important implications for caregivers and pediatricians.
Conclusions
Our results suggest that more frequent parent-child conversations around television viewing (which may be a proxy for other conversational exchanges) are associated with higher curiosity, especially in children with socioeconomic disadvantage. This highlights the importance of parents engaging in reciprocal conversations around topics and experiences of mutual interest [47], and suggests the importance of finding opportunities foster conversational exchanges in the context of daily routines (e.g., even when watching television). Aligning with the American Academy of Pediatrics’ recommendations on media [61, 62], parents can be counseled on the value of parental instructive dialogue during television viewing (e.g. “active mediation”) [63], as an opportunity to promote inquiry [64]. Parent-child conversations that are guided by active mediation have been associated with more adaptive social-emotional development in young children, with a greater magnitude of effect in children from low-income families [65]. Our work extends this line of research and highlights the benefits of active mediation on early childhood curiosity. Because parent conversation around television viewing is likely related to parent conversation in the home, our results also suggest the importance of fostering opportunities for dialogic exchanges around all topics (not just television), especially for children from environments of socioeconomic disadvantage [27].
Supporting information
S1 Appendix. Adjusted associations of daily television viewing, parent conversation during shared television viewing and curiosity (Step 1 –main effects).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572.s001
(DOCX)
S2 Appendix. Adjusted associations of daily television viewing, parent conversation during shared television viewing and curiosity, moderated by SES (Step 2- moderation results, prior to median split).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572.s002
(DOCX)
S3 Appendix. Derived curiosity factor from the ECLS-B.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572.s003
(DOCX)
References
- 1.
Pennock RTM, J. D., editor Virtues of U.S. scientists guiding scientific practice. Symposia Presented at the AAAS Annual Meeting; 2016; Washington, D.C.
- 2. Loewenstein G. The psychology of curiosity: A review and reinterpretation. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;116(1):75–98.
- 3. Jirout J, Klahr D. Children’s scientific curiosity: In search of an operational definition of an elusive concept. Developmental Review. 2012;32(2):125–60.
- 4.
Nakamura J, Csikszentmihalyi M. The concept of flow. In. Snyder CR & Lopez SJ (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 89–105). New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2002.
- 5.
McCombs BL. The learner-centered psychological principles: A framework for balancing academic achievement and social-emotional learning outcomes. In: Zins JE, Weissberg RP, Wang MC, Wallberg HJ, editors. Building academic success on social and emotional learning: What does the research say. 23. New York: Teachers College Press; 2004.
- 6. Ackermann L, Hepach R, Mani N. Children learn words easier when they are interested in the category to which the word belongs. Developmental science. 2019:e12915. pmid:31618505
- 7. Stahl AE, Feigenson L. Observing the unexpected enhances infants’ learning and exploration. Science. 2015;348(6230):91. pmid:25838378
- 8. Lepper MR, Corpus JH, Iyengar SS. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations in the classroom: age differences and academic correlates. Journal of educational psychology. 2005;97(2):184.
- 9. Kashdan TB, Yuen M. Whether highly curious students thrive academically depends on perceptions about the school learning environment: A study of Hong Kong adolescents. Motivation and Emotion. 2007;31(4):260–70.
- 10. Shah PE, Weeks HM, Richards B, Kaciroti N. Early childhood curiosity and kindergarten reading and math academic achievement. Pediatric research. 2018. pmid:29884846
- 11. Hassinger-Das B, Hirsh-Pasek K. Appetite for knowledge: curiosity and children’s academic achievement. Pediatric research. 2018. pmid:29967534
- 12. Rideout V. The Common Sense census: Media use by kids age zero to eight.2017 August 16, 2019.
- 13. Tandon PS, Zhou C, Lozano P, Christakis DA. Preschoolers’ total daily screen time at home and by type of child care. The Journal of pediatrics. 2011;158(2):297–300. pmid:20980020
- 14. Mendelsohn AL, Berkule SB, Tomopoulos S, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Huberman HS, Alvir J, et al. Infant television and video exposure associated with limited parent-child verbal interactions in low socioeconomic status households. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2008;162(5):411–7. pmid:18458186
- 15. Lapierre MA, Piotrowski JT, Linebarger DL. Background television in the homes of US children. Pediatrics. 2012;130(5):839–46. pmid:23027166
- 16. Yang-Huang J, van Grieken A, Moll HA, Jaddoe VWV, Wijtzes AI, Raat H. Socioeconomic differences in children’s television viewing trajectory: A population-based prospective cohort study. PloS one. 2017;12(12):e0188363. pmid:29211770
- 17. Munzer TG, Miller AL, Peterson KE, Brophy-Herb HE, Horodynski MA, Contreras D, et al. Media Exposure in Low-Income Preschool-Aged Children Is Associated with Multiple Measures of Self-Regulatory Behavior. Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics: JDBP. 2018;39(4):303–9. pmid:29538186
- 18. Radesky JS, Silverstein M, Zuckerman B, Christakis DA. Infant Self-Regulation and Early Childhood Media Exposure. Pediatrics. 2014;133(5):e1172. pmid:24733868
- 19. Tomopoulos S, Dreyer BP, Berkule S, Fierman AH, Brockmeyer C, Mendelsohn AL. Infant media exposure and toddler development. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2010;164(12):1105–11. pmid:21135338
- 20. Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN. Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years. The Journal of pediatrics. 2007;151(4):364–8. pmid:17889070
- 21. Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Mori C, Tough S. Association Between Screen Time and Children’s Performance on a Developmental Screening Test. JAMA pediatrics. 2019. pmid:30688984
- 22. Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA. Children’s television viewing and cognitive outcomes: a longitudinal analysis of national data. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2005;159(7):619–25. pmid:15996993
- 23. Kirkorian HL, Pempek TA, Murphy LA, Schmidt ME, Anderson DR. The impact of background television on parent-child interaction. Child Dev. 2009;80(5):1350–9. pmid:19765004
- 24. Yogman M, Garner A, Hutchinson J, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. The Power of Play: A Pediatric Role in Enhancing Development in Young Children. Pediatrics. 2018;142(3). pmid:30126932
- 25. Mendelsohn AL, Brockmeyer CA, Dreyer BP, Fierman AH, Berkule-Silberman SB, Tomopoulos S. Do Verbal Interactions with Infants During Electronic Media Exposure Mitigate Adverse Impacts on their Language Development as Toddlers? Infant Child Dev. 2010;19(6):577–93. pmid:21593996
- 26. Yu Y, Bonawitz E, Shafto P. Pedagogical Questions in Parent-Child Conversations. Child Dev. 2019;90(1):147–61. pmid:28617972
- 27. Hirsh-Pasek K, Adamson LB, Bakeman R, Owen MT, Golinkoff RM, Pace A, et al. The Contribution of Early Communication Quality to Low-Income Children’s Language Success. Psychological science. 2015;26(7):1071–83. pmid:26048887
- 28.
Hart B, Risley TR. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children: Paul H Brookes Publishing; 1995.
- 29. Golinkoff RM, Hoff E, Rowe ML, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Hirsh-Pasek K. Language Matters: Denying the Existence of the 30-Million-Word Gap Has Serious Consequences. Child Dev. 2019;90(3):985–92. pmid:30102419
- 30. Atkinson L, Beitchman J, Gonzalez A, Young A, Wilson B, Escobar M, et al. Cumulative risk, cumulative outcome: a 20-year longitudinal study. PloS one. 2015;10(6):e0127650. pmid:26030616
- 31.
Snow K, Derecho A, Wheeless S, Lennon J, Rosen J, Rogers J, et al. Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B) Kindergarten 2006 and 2007 Data File User’s Manual (2010–010). In: Statistics NCfE, editor. Washington, D.C.: Institute of Educational Sciences, U.S. Department of Education; 2009.
- 32. Piotrowski JT, Litman JA, Valkenburg P. Measuring epistemic curiosity in young children. Infant and Child Development. 2014;23(5):542–53.
- 33. Kidd C, Hayden Benjamin Y. The Psychology and Neuroscience of Curiosity. Neuron. 2015;88(3):449–60. pmid:26539887
- 34. Hall GS, Smith TL. Curiosity and Interest. Pedag Sem. 1903;X:315–58.
- 35. Berlyne DE. A theory of human curiosity. British Journal of Psychology General Section. 1954;45(3):180–91. pmid:13190171
- 36. Maw W, Maw E. The measurement of curiosity in elementary school children. A summary Cooperative Research Project. 1964(801).
- 37. Cook C, Goodman ND, Schulz LE. Where science starts: Spontaneous experiments in preschoolers’ exploratory play. Cognition. 2011;120(3):341–9. pmid:21561605
- 38. Schulz LE, Bonawitz EB. Serious fun: preschoolers engage in more exploratory play when evidence is confounded. Developmental psychology. 2007;43(4):1045. pmid:17605535
- 39. Lin L-Y, Cherng R-J, Chen Y-J, Chen Y-J, Yang H-M. Effects of television exposure on developmental skills among young children. Infant Behavior and Development. 2015;38:20–6. pmid:25544743
- 40.
Inc. SI. Base SAS(R) 9.4 Procedures Guide: Statistical Procedures. In: Inc. SI, editor. Cary, NC 2014.
- 41.
Wicklin R. Statistical programming with SAS/IML software: SAS Institute; 2010.
- 42. Verdine BN, Zimmermann L, Foster L, Marzouk MA, Golinkoff RM, Hirsh-Pasek K, et al. Effects of geometric toy design on parent–child interactions and spatial language. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2019;46:126–41. pmid:30555211
- 43. Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135(6):885. pmid:19883141
- 44. Merz EC, Maskus EA, Melvin SA, He X, Noble KG. Socioeconomic disparities in language input are associated with children’s language‐related brain structure and reading skills. Child development. 2020;91(3):846–60. pmid:30919945
- 45. Pace A, Luo R, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language development. Annual Review of Linguistics. 2017;3(1):285–308.
- 46. Maw WH, Maw EW. Self-Concepts of High- and Low-Curiosity Boys. Child Development. 1970;41(1):123–9. pmid:5490675
- 47. Zimmerman FJ, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, Christakis DA, Xu D, Gray S, et al. Teaching by listening: The importance of adult-child conversations to language development. Pediatrics. 2009;124(1):342–9. pmid:19564318
- 48. Vernon-Feagans L, Bratsch-Hines ME, Investigators FLPK. Caregiver–child verbal interactions in child care: A buffer against poor language outcomes when maternal language input is less. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2013;28(4):858–73. pmid:24634566
- 49. Bigelow AE, MacLean K, Proctor J. The role of joint attention in the development of infants’ play with objects. Developmental science. 2004;7(5):518–26. pmid:15603284
- 50. Hirsh-Pasek K, Zosh JM, Golinkoff RM, Gray JH, Robb MB, Kaufman J. Putting education in “educational” apps: Lessons from the science of learning. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2015;16(1):3–34.
- 51. Ferjan Ramirez N, Lytle SR, Fish M, Kuhl PK. Parent coaching at 6 and 10 months improves language outcomes at 14 months: A randomized controlled trial. Developmental science. 2019;22(3):e12762. pmid:30318708
- 52. Fletcher EN, Whitaker RC, Marino AJ, Anderson SE. Screen Time at Home and School among Low-Income Children Attending Head Start. Child Indic Res. 2014;7(2):421–36. pmid:24891924
- 53. Christakis DA, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, Zimmerman FJ, Garrison MM, Xu D, et al. Audible Television and Decreased Adult Words, Infant Vocalizations, and Conversational Turns: A Population-Based Study. JAMA pediatrics. 2009;163(6):554–8.
- 54. Alvarez AL, Booth AE. Motivated by Meaning: Testing the Effect of Knowledge-Infused Rewards on Preschoolers’ Persistence. Child Development. 2014;85(2):783–91. pmid:24033222
- 55.
Dore RA, Zosh JM, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Chapter 4—Plugging Into Word Learning: The Role of Electronic Toys and Digital Media in Language Development. In: Blumberg FC, Brooks PJ, editors. Cognitive Development in Digital Contexts. San Diego: Academic Press; 2017. p. 75–91.
- 56. Arasteh JD. CREATIVITY AND RELATED PROCESSES IN THE YOUNG CHILD: A REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE. The Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human Development. 1968;112(1):77–108.
- 57. Hassinger-Das B, Bustamante AS, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Learning landscapes: Playing the way to learning and engagement in public spaces. Education Sciences. 2018;8(2):74.
- 58.
Hadani HS, Winthrop R, Hirsh-Pasek K. Playful Learning Landscapes: Convergence of Education and City Planning. Powering a Learning Society During an Age of Disruption: Springer, Singapore; 2021. p. 151–64. https://doi.org/10.5603/PJNNS.a2021.0017 pmid:33559873
- 59. Kashdan TB, Stiksma MC, Disabato DD, McKnight PE, Bekier J, Kaji J, et al. The five-dimensional curiosity scale: Capturing the bandwidth of curiosity and identifying four unique subgroups of curious people. Journal of Research in Personality. 2018;73:130–49.
- 60. Kashdan TB, Sherman RA, Yarbro J, Funder DC. How Are Curious People Viewed and How Do They Behave in Social Situations? From the Perspectives of Self, Friends, Parents, and Unacquainted Observers. Journal of Personality. 2013;81(2):142–54. pmid:22583101
- 61. American Academy of Pediatrics. Children, Adolescents, and the Media. Pediatrics. 2013;132(5):958. pmid:28448255
- 62. American Academy of Pediatrics. Media and Young Minds. Pediatrics. 2016;138(5):e20162591. pmid:27940793
- 63. Harrison K, Liechty JM, The SKP. US Preschoolers’ Media Exposure and Dietary Habits: The primacy of television and the limits of parental mediation. Journal of Children and Media. 2012;6(1):18–36.
- 64. Coyne SM, Radesky J, Collier KM, Gentile DA, Linder JR, Nathanson AI, et al. Parenting and Digital Media. Pediatrics. 2017;140(Supplement 2):S112. pmid:29093044
- 65. Rasmussen EE, Shafer A, Colwell MJ, White S, Punyanunt-Carter N, Densley RL, et al. Relation between active mediation, exposure to Daniel Tiger’s Neighborhood, and US preschoolers’ social and emotional development. Journal of Children and Media. 2016;10(4):443–61.