Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The relative effects of self-reported noise and odour annoyance on psychological distress: Different effects across sociodemographic groups?

  • Eline Berkers ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Eline Berkers, Ioana Pop

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    e.c.m.berkers@tilburguniversity.edu

    Affiliations Department of Sociology, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands, Tranzo, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands

  • Ioana Pop ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Eline Berkers, Ioana Pop

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Sociology, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands

  • Mariëlle Cloïn ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Tranzo, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands

  • Antje Eugster ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliations Tranzo, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands, GGD Brabant-Zuidoost, Eindhoven, The Netherlands

  • Hans van Oers

    Roles Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliations Tranzo, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands, Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sports, The Hague, The Netherlands

Abstract

In earlier research, both higher levels of noise and odour annoyance have been associated with decreased mental health. Presumably, these perceptions can trigger feelings of threat and stress reactions and in turn evoke psychological distress. There are two important lacunas in the research on this topic: most studies only consider either noise or odour annoyance and not their relative effect on psychological distress and there is scarce evidence about whether different sociodemographic groups experience more psychological distress due to noise and odour annoyance. Starting from the diversity in the available coping resources and in their daily life patterns, we distinguish gender, age and educational level as relevant sociodemographic variables. Using data from the Health Monitor (n = 25236) in Noord-Brabant, we found using Ordinary Least Squares Regression that individuals that reported higher levels of noise and odour annoyance reported higher levels of psychological distress. Furthermore, the effect of noise annoyance was relatively stronger compared to that of odour annoyance. Regarding the interaction effects, we found that younger adults’ psychological distress was more strongly affected by noise annoyance compared to older adults, but not by odour annoyance. The psychological distress of individuals with no or primary education was more strongly affected by both noise and odour annoyance compared those with tertiary education, but not when compared to those who completed lower or higher secondary education. Contrary to our expectations, we did not find different effects between men and women. Though the evidence for the interactions was mixed, classic health inequalities along age and education lines are reinforced when considering the relationship between noise and odour annoyance and psychological distress.

Introduction

Over the years, the mental health disadvantages of living in an unruly environment filled with environmental stressors have become clear [15]. Two of these stressors that negatively affect mental health are noise and odour, e.g., the World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe [6] estimates that every year more than 1 million healthy years are lost due to traffic noise only. In addition, odour pollution is associated with many health complaints and decreased quality of life [7]. If we add to this the fact that mental disorders, and especially mood disorders, pose a very high burden on the population of Europe and are expected to become even more prominent for population health in the future [8, 9], it is clear that a better understanding of the relationship between odour and noise and mental health is needed in order to mitigate these noxious pathways.

Noise and odour can be quantified by using decibels and odour standards (for example, ammonia thresholds) and are thought to affect individual’s mental health in two different ways: a direct pathway, i.e., a negative effect on the nervous system through physical arousal and an activation of stress hormones, and an indirect pathway through triggering negative emotional reactions such as anger or stress [1012]. However, people are differently affected by noise or odour exposure: what is annoying to some may not be to others depending on personal, social or general characteristics, such as personality, sensitivity to noise or odour, relationship with the source, ability to ‘escape’ and history of disturbance due to noise or odour [1315]. Subsequently, the relationship between noise exposure and noise annoyance is not that clear-cut and only around ⅓ of the variance in noise annoyance is explained by exposure to noise [13]. Similar sources are not available for odour but it is reasonable to assume a similar association.

In general, subjective perceptions of noise and odour more closely reflect the actual impact of daily life environmental circumstances than more objective ones simply because individuals are not fully aware of the objective circumstances in their living environment [16] given that they rely on their senses for perceiving them [17]. Furthermore, subjective perceptions are the pathway between objective stressors and the resulting emotional states (i.e., psychological distress) [18, 19]. Moreover, noise and odour form the top two environmental complaints for European citizens and therefore is an important issue for policy [7]. For policy makers at a municipality or province, sophisticated exposure data is hard to interpret and translate into policy. Therefore, studying the relationship between noise and odour annoyance and psychological distress is important, both from a scientific and policy perspective. Subsequently, in this contribution we focus on the noise and odour annoyance experienced by individuals and their impact on psychological distress.

Previous evidence from cross-sectional studies relates noise annoyance stemming from different sources (e.g. industry, traffic or neighbours) to increased levels of psychological distress among adults, both in urban and rural areas, and in different countries [18, 2025]. Furthermore, a recent longitudinal study by Beutel, Brähler [26] found that even after controlling for current levels of noise annoyance, the experience of past noise annoyance, as far as five years ago, was linked to a higher probability of anxiety and depressive symptoms. Turning to odour annoyance, previous studies report that higher levels of odour annoyance due to agricultural activities are associated with higher levels of psychological distress among adults living in rural areas [11, 2729].

This said, our first contribution to the literature is our focus on the relationship between both noise and odour annoyance and psychological distress as opposed to focusing on each factor separately. In this way, we account for the fact that noise and odour annoyance can occur at the same time (i.e., they can originate from the same source, e.g., industrial activity or traffic). By taking into account both noise and odour annoyance, this study will provide more insight into their relative contribution to psychological distress [23].

Second, the present study responds to calls in the literature to more closely examine for whom the daily living conditions (e.g., noise and odour annoyance) have a stronger effect on psychological distress [30, 31]. Life is full of low-impact factors (daily hassles, like noise and odour annoyance) that add up and use the coping resources available for people and in this way have a substantial impact on individuals [19]. Different sociodemographic groups have access to different coping resources and have different daily living patterns, which influence their capacity to cope with environmental stressors. As a result, certain groups might experience higher levels of psychological distress due to perceived noise and odour annoyance. Answering the question whether different groups are impacted differently by these stressors helps to understand the causes of health inequalities across different groups. However, so far, the scientific evidence supporting this assumption has been scarce, as we will detail later in the theoretical section.

To sum up, the research questions in this study are: how are noise and odour annoyance related to psychological distress? What is the relative contribution of noise and odour annoyance to psychological distress? Is this relationship different for groups defined by gender, age and educational level? We address these questions based on data of adult respondents (n = 25236) collected in a province in the South of the Netherlands (Noord-Brabant). In this part of the Netherlands, livestock farming and industry are important sources of nuisance [32] and both the population density and livestock density in this province rank very high [33, 34]. Next, the province is home to one commercial and a few military airports. The variation of these contextual stressors between different living areas makes this an adequate area to study this subject.

Relationship between noise and odour annoyance and psychological distress

To understand why noise and odour annoyance could increase psychological distress, we draw from two related theoretical models, i.e., the transactional stress model proposed by Lazarus and Folkman [19] and the Conservation of Resources theory [35, 36]. Lazarus and Folkman [19] proposed that the subjective perception of a contextual stressor determines the actual strength of the individual stress reaction. Implicitly, individuals rely on their senses to appraise the conditions in their living environment [17]. If individuals appraise their living conditions as harmful to their wellbeing now or in the future, this creates a disbalance between the individual and its living environment. This in turn can lead to psychological distress [19, 37]. For the case of noise and odour, potential health risks are an increased probability of developing concentration or sleeping issues (for noise) or irritation on the eyes, nose and throat or respiratory issues (for odour) [27, 38, 39]. Subsequently, the appraisal of these potential risks in the environment can lead to feelings of anger, anxiety and stress, or in more general terms, to psychological distress.

The description of this process details how individuals’ perception of an acute stressor influences their levels of psychological distress. When the stressor is environmental, individuals are often confronted with it over longer periods of time, because individuals have relatively stable living arrangements [40]. These repeated confrontations with noise and odour annoyance will undermine the basic need of individuals to live in a clean and predictable living environment and instead paint a picture of a living environment that is chaotic and full of threats to individuals’ health [4].

According to the Conservation of Resources theory, in order to deal with these stressful conditions, individuals need to activate their coping resources, such as socioeconomic and health resources [4, 35, 41]. Coping takes effort (e.g., by taking political action or minimizing exposure by closing windows or doors) and if individuals have to persistently use resources to deal with noise and odour annoyance, they are at a greater risk of diminished ability to deal with future stressors [36, 42]. In addition, if their coping effort is unsuccessful, individuals are at risk to lose further coping resources [35]. This mechanism implies that individuals that are confronted with noise and odour annoyance will report higher levels of psychological distress.

Moreover, noise and odour annoyance can lead to psychological distress through the persistent disturbance of daily activities, such as relaxation (e.g., reading or watching television) or restoration (e.g., sleeping) which are essential for replenishing cognitive and physical resources that are used throughout the day [43]. Individuals that are unable to replenish these resources will have more difficulties in coping with stressors, which will lead to increased psychological distress [44].

Based on the above arguments we expect that individuals that report higher levels of noise annoyance (hypothesis 1a) and odour annoyance (hypothesis 1b) report higher levels of psychological distress.

Secondly, regarding the relative impact of noise and odour annoyance on psychological distress, we expect that noise annoyance will have a stronger relative impact. For odour annoyance there is more variation in the levels of annoyance in comparison to noise as a result of seasonal influences [23]. Noise, while depending to some extent on the wind direction, has a more constant presence in the environment, which results in a relatively larger burden for psychological distress compared to odour annoyance. Thus, our hypothesis is that noise annoyance will have a stronger effect on psychological distress compared to odour annoyance (hypothesis 2).

Different relationships across sociodemographic groups

As noted in the introduction, the differential effect of noise and odour annoyance on psychological distress for different sociodemographic groups is scarce [45]. However, the same process of coping can be used as a starting point to elaborate on these effects. Not surprising, the classic social inequality markers of gender, age and education are undisputable main dimensions of inequalities in coping resources. If individuals rely on coping resources in order to mitigate the effect of noise and odour annoyance on psychological distress, social groups with higher levels of such resources will fare better.

We identify personal control, financial resources and cognitive or physical health capacities as relevant buffers in between noise and odour annoyance and psychological distress. First, a lack of personal control, defined as ‘the belief that one can master, control and shape one’s own life’ [46] increases the perceived threat posed by the environmental stressors because individuals feel unable to avoid their negative consequences [5, 46]. As a result, individuals with lower levels of personal control, i.e. women, older adults and less educated [4, 47] are unlikely to act to change their situation and they develop negative emotions [48].

Second, individuals with more financial resources can better protect themselves from the negative effects of noise and odour annoyance because they can afford better housing (with better isolation) and are more likely to have political influence to prevent negative environmental changes around their home [41]. In general, women and older adults report lower levels of financial resources, e.g., they make fewer working hours due to part-time employment or retirement [49, 50]. Furthermore, a higher education level increases the opportunities for good jobs with a higher income [47].

And third, the experience of decreasing cognitive and physical health already puts individuals at a risk for increased psychological distress [35], adding to the negative effects of the precarious environmental conditions. Furthermore, individuals experiencing cognitive or physical health decline engage their financial and social resources, as well as their time and energy, to cope with their health situation [35], leaving less resources available to deal with noise and odour annoyance. The later middle-age and the older age are the phases where physical and cognitive health issues are more likely to arise, thus this argument could mostly apply to these age groups [51].

Next to differences in coping resources, sociodemographic groups defined by gender, age and education also have different daily living patterns [52], which can contribute to the differential susceptibility of being affected by noise and odour annoyance. One particular difference is the amount of time spent at home. This is an important indicator of noise and odour annoyance [53, 54]. It is reasonable to expect that the more people are exposed to noise and odour annoyance in an environment that they cannot easily escape, such as their own homes, the more likely it is that this annoyance will increase the psychological distress because it undermines the basic need for a peaceful living environment [44]. Women, older adults and less educated spend more time at home than men, younger adults and higher educated [55].

To sum up, based on the difference in coping resources and daily living patterns between the three sociodemographic groups, we can derive the following hypotheses. First, we expect the psychological distress of women to be affected more strongly by noise annoyance (hypothesis 3a) and odour annoyance (hypothesis 3b) than men. Next, we expect that older adults’ psychological distress is affected more strongly by noise annoyance (hypothesis 4a) and odour annoyance (hypothesis 4b) compared to middle-aged and younger adults. Finally, individuals with lower levels of education will experience less psychological distress when confronted with noise annoyance (hypothesis 5a) and odour annoyance (hypothesis 5b) compared to individuals with higher levels of education.

Data and method

We used data from the Health Monitor collected by the Dutch Regional Health Services (GGD), Statistics Netherlands (CBS) and the National Institute for Public Health and the Environment (RIVM), collected between September and December in 2016. The data is collected every four years with the purpose of gathering relevant information about the public health situation in the Netherlands on different spatial levels (e.g., regional, national, local). In our main analysis, we use a sample that consists of adults (aged 19–64, n = 29647) from the province of Noord-Brabant (divided in three subregions: West, Middle and South-East Brabant). We replicate the main models on a larger sample which includes older adults aged 65+ (n = 34838, of which 25326 adults and 9512 older adults) because we expect older adults’ psychological distress to be more strongly affected by noise and odour annoyance. This sample was not used in the main analyses because it was drawn from the same subregion (South-East Brabant) and the respondents answered a subset of the items in the noise annoyance scales (three out of nine). Approval was obtained for the publication of the content by the Ethics Review Board of the school of Social and Behavioral Sciences at Tilburg University.

The sampling was done by Statistics Netherlands based on the Municipal Personal Records Database. Respondents were invited to participate in a web survey, which included topics such as health, lifestyle, perceived neighbourhood quality and social contacts (for more specific information about the questionnaire: visit the website of the National Institute for Public Health and the Environment [56]). Respondents were offered a paper questionnaire as an alternative to the web survey. In our sample, 74.4% of the adults filled in the web survey and the rest the paper survey. The overall response rate for Noord-Brabant was 32% for adults, which is slightly lower than the overall response rate for the entire survey (40%). After deleting the cases with missing values (n = 4411, about 15% of the full sample) on the main variables, the final included sample of adult respondents was 25236. Using Little’s MCAR test, we found that the missing values in our analysis were not completely missing at random (as the p-value was significant at an alpha of 0.05). To evaluate if there were differences in the missing values on psychological distress across the different sociodemographic groups, we used a cross tabulation to calculate the chi-square statistic. There were no significant differences in missing values among men and women, but those with lower levels of education and younger individuals were more likely to have missing values compared to those with higher educational levels and older individuals. We did not consider imputation methods for our missing values, because this is an estimation method which depends on the assumptions of the model that is estimated. Since we had a reasonable amount of missing values, we chose to rely on listwise deletion.

In our sample, 54.5 percent was female and the majority of the sample was aged between 45 and 64 (48.7 percent), whereas younger adults were slightly underrepresented (21.1 percent) compared to the other age groups.

Psychological distress

The survey included the Kessler-10 scale, which is commonly used to measure nonspecific psychological distress (e.g., symptoms of depression and anxiety disorders) [57, 58]. The participant was asked to think about how he or she felt in the past 4 weeks. Examples of questions are: ‘how often did you feel tired without a reason?’, ‘how often did you feel hopeless?’ or ‘how often did you feel so restless that you could not sit still?’ Respondents could answer between 1) ‘never’ and 5) ‘all of the time’. All items loaded on one factor and the scale had a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.91. Respondents that did not answer all questions were filtered out of the data (n = 3421). We computed a sum score, but to ease the interpretation of the results we rescaled the scale to range between 0 and 100, where a higher score represented higher levels of psychological distress. Subsequently, we can interpret the coefficients from our regression models as changes in percentage points. As part of our sensitivity analyses, we re-estimated the main models using a dichotomous dependent variable. We followed the recommendation by Donker, Comijs [57] and we used a cut-off point of 25 out of 100 to differentiate between participants at risk and those not at risk of clinical depression and anxiety disorder.

Noise annoyance

Respondents indicated to what extent they were hindered or annoyed by noise from different sources in the last twelve months: a) roads below < 50 km, b) roads above > 50 km, c) railways, d) aircraft, e) scooters, f) neighbours, g) industry or companies, h) (re)construction sites or, i) restaurants, cafes or bars. The measurement scale ranged from 0 (not annoyed) to 10 (highly annoyed). The items loaded on one factor (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.76). The survey included a separate category for individuals that could not hear the noise at home. Respondents that gave this answer were given a score of 0 as if they indicated being able to hear the noise and were not disturbed by it. Like previous studies that took into account multiple sources of noise [22, 59], a mean score was computed based on a minimum of eight out of the nine items (n = 352 individuals answered only eight items).

For our analyses, we used the noise annoyance scale as a continuous variable ranging from 0 “no odour annoyance” to 10 “high odour annoyance” (we present the distribution of scores in Fig 1 in the S1 Appendix). For the older adult sample, only three out of the nine items were available to compute the average noise annoyance score, namely noise annoyance due to industry or companies, (re)construction sites or restaurants, cafes and bars.

Odour annoyance

We measured odour annoyance using an item that asked the respondents if they experienced annoyance or hinder during the last twelve months in their home due to: a) roads, b) sewerage, c) fireplaces, d) agriculture, e) industry, f) stables, g) manure, h) animal feed, i) a digester or j) aircraft traffic. Answers ranged from 0 (not annoyed) to 10 (highly annoyed). Again, respondents that indicated not being able to smell the odour at home were scored 0 as if they were not annoyed by this odour, but could smell it. We performed an exploratory factor analysis that revealed two factors: one related to agriculture (items d, f, g and h) and one relating to other types of odour (items a, b, c, e, i and j). In total, 62% of the respondents indicated that they were not annoyed by odour by agriculture and 45% indicated the same referring to odour annoyance due to other sources. This makes sense given that agricultural odour annoyance will be more present in rural areas, whereas odour annoyance due to other sources will be omnipresent (e.g., due to fireplaces, roads, sewerage). We computed a mean score on a minimum of three out of four items for agricultural odour (n = 123 respondents with 3 items) and five out of six items for other odour annoyance (n = 234 respondents with 5 items). Both scales had good reliability, i.e., Cronbach’s alpha was 0.86 for agricultural odour annoyance scale and 0.67 for the scale measuring annoyance due to other sources. We used the two scales as continuous variables that ranged from 0 “no odour annoyance” to 10 “high odour annoyance” (information about the distribution of scores can be found in Figs 2 and 3 in the S1 Appendix).

Sociodemographic groups

Following the terminology by Pai and Kim [51] and Lachman [50] we divided the sample into three age groups: age between 19 and 35 (younger adults, ref.), between 36 and 50 (young middle-aged) and 51 and 64 (older middle-aged). In the analysis with the older adult sample, those older than 64 form a separate age group. For gender, men formed the reference category. For educational level, four educational levels were taken into account: no or primary (ref.), lower secondary, higher secondary and tertiary education.

Control variables

Given that individuals with a lower socioeconomic status are more exposed to noise because of their living situation (e.g., worse housing conditions) and are more at risk for psychological distress, we controlled for subjective economic wellbeing [39, 59]. For subjective economic wellbeing, respondents indicated whether they had experienced financial hardship in the last twelve months or not (ref. is no). In addition, we controlled for self-rated health, because on the one hand, worse health increases the vulnerability for environmental stressors [15] and on the other hand, is strongly related to mental health [18, 22, 60]. Self-rated health was measured by the following question: how do you perceive your health in general? [61]. Three answer options were used: bad, moderate and good health (ref.). We also took into account parenthood (measured by cohabitation with a child under 18, ref. is no). Parents are more likely to spend time around their home environment due to child-rearing duties [62] and children form an extra reason for concern about noise and odour annoyance [63]. Next, parents’ child-rearing duties and work-life balance take up time and energy and consequently lead to higher levels of psychological distress [55, 64, 65].

Because we do not have access (for data protection reasons) to detailed address data, we cannot model actual noise and odour exposure for respondents. Alternatively, to get an indication of noise and odour exposure around the home, we modelled indicator variables for all districts and self-reported noise and odour annoyance around the home. Following the study by van Deurzen, Rod [40], we used indicator variables to control for differences in the objective living conditions and exposure to noise and odour between the 306 districts in the sample (size between 42 and 272 respondents). The first in the list (Wijk en Aalburg) is used as the reference category. Finally, we controlled for the self-reported risks of noise and odour annoyance in the vicinity of the home (e.g., a busy street, wind turbine, agriculture or a gas station) by taking into account whether they are present or not (ref. is no).

Descriptive statistics for the variables in our analyses are summarized in Table 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of all variables (n = 25236).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258102.t001

Analytical strategy

In this study, we relied on bivariate analysis of mean scores and ordinary least squares regressions (as our dependent variable is a scale) using Stata 16.0. For our bivariate analysis, we showed the average psychological distress scores of those with different scores on the noise and odour annoyance scale to illustrate if there are differences between these groups. Moreover, the results of the regression models are presented in Tables 2 and 3. In Table 2, Model 1 we estimated the effect of the noise annoyance measure, controlling for the sociodemographic and environmental control variables. Model 2 included both scales of odour annoyance and Model 3 both noise annoyance and odour annoyance measures (controlling only for the sociodemographic and environmental control variables). In Table 3, Model 1 we added parenthood as a control variable. In Model 2, we replaced the parenthood measure with self-rated health and in Model 3 we included both parenthood and self-rated health. All interaction effects are based on Model 3. In Tables 4 and 5 we presented the significant effects of the estimated interactions of noise and odour annoyance measures with gender, age groups and educational levels. For comparison purposes of the strength of the effect, besides the unstandardized coefficients we also present the standardized ones (beta). We also present the analyses for the sample that includes the older adults in the S1 Appendix. As a sensitivity analysis, we estimated our models for a dependent variable that differentiated between individuals with and without a risk of clinical depression and anxiety disorder by using logistic regression.

thumbnail
Table 2. Selected effects of OLS regression for psychological distress.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258102.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Selected effects of OLS regression for psychological distress.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258102.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Interaction effects of noise annoyance for different age groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258102.t004

thumbnail
Table 5. Interaction effects of noise and odour annoyance for educational level.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258102.t005

Results

Simple bivariate analyses (Table A1 in the S1 Appendix) showed that those who were least annoyed by noise had the lowest average psychological distress score (score ranges from 0 to under 3; 14.9) as compared to those who were annoyed (score ranges from 3 to under 8; 21.1) and highly annoyed (score 8–10; 31.5). This categorization was based on a report of the Dutch National Institute for Public Health that used the same data [66]. The same pattern of higher average psychological distress was found for those who were highly annoyed by odour due to agriculture. However, for odour annoyance relating to other sources, those who were quite annoyed reported the highest levels of psychological distress (though the n of 13 individuals with high levels of this type of odour annoyance is small). In addition, before turning to our results from the regression models, we presented the correlations between noise and odour annoyance and psychological distress. The correlation between noise annoyance and psychological distress was 0.17, for odour due to agriculture it was 0.08 and for odour due to other sources it was 0.13.

Turning to the results from our ordinary least squares regression, in Model 1 from Table 2, we found a positive and significant effect of noise annoyance on psychological distress. Since the psychological distress scale ranges from 0 to 100, the effects can be interpreted as percentage point changes. To make our findings more intuitive, we compared the psychological distress scores of individuals that were not annoyed at all by noise (scoring 0) to those that were most annoyed (scoring 10), and we found a difference in the predicted psychological distress score of 13.3 percentage points. In Model 2, both types of odour annoyance showed a positive and significant effect on the level of psychological distress. Individuals that were most annoyed by odour from agriculture reported an increase of 4.1 percentage points in psychological distress compared to those who were least annoyed. For odour annoyance from other sources the difference was larger: those who were most annoyed reported an increase of 13.6 percentage points in psychological distress compared to those who were least annoyed. In Model 3, all three sources of annoyance showed a positive effect on psychological distress, independent of each other, supporting the idea that they each have a unique contribution in explaining psychological distress among our adult population.

After testing the direct effects of noise and odour annoyance, we added different control variables to test the robustness of the results, as described in the Analytical strategy section. When comparing model 1 from Table 3 to model 3 in Table 2, we concluded that the effects of noise and odour annoyance on psychological distress are robust after adding parenthood. After adding self-rated health to the model, the effects of noise and odour annoyance on psychological distress became smaller (except for odour annoyance due to agriculture, where the coefficient remained stable). However, all three effects remained significant. Finally, in Table 3 model 3 we added both parenthood and self-rated health and in this model we again found significant positive effects for noise and odour annoyance on psychological distress.

Overall, these results supported hypothesis 1a and 1b, which stated that higher levels of noise and odour annoyance are associated with higher levels of psychological distress. When comparing their relative contribution to the psychological distress of individuals, we found that noise annoyance had the largest standardized effect (beta = 0.11 compared to 0.02 for both sources of odour annoyance). This was in line with hypothesis 2, which stated that noise annoyance would have a stronger relative contribution to psychological distress than odour annoyance.

Different relationships across sociodemographic groups

For noise annoyance, we found two significant interactions which are presented in Tables 4 and 5. We found a negative interaction of noise annoyance for older middle-aged adults (and young middle-aged, but this result was not significant) (see Table 4). This implies that, contrary to what we expected, among younger adults (compared to older middle-aged) noise annoyance was more strongly related to psychological distress. For younger adults the increase in psychological distress between those who were not annoyed and those who were most annoyed was 15.7 percentage points, whereas this was 14.4 and 11.6 percentage points respectively for the younger middle-aged and older middle-aged. In Fig 1 we present the predicted level of psychological distress for the different age groups at different levels of noise annoyance. Overall, these findings go against our hypothesis 4a, which stated that the relationship between noise annoyance and psychological distress would be stronger among older adults compared to their younger counterparts.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Interaction plot for predicted psychological distress among different age groups by level of noise annoyance.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258102.g001

We found significant positive interaction effects for noise annoyance across different educational levels (as can be found in Table 5). The interactions showed differences in the psychological distress of those with no or primary education and those with tertiary education at similar levels of noise annoyance, but not between those with no or primary education and those with secondary education. This means that the psychological distress of those with no or primary education was higher when compared to those with tertiary education at similar levels of noise annoyance. This is also depicted graphically in Fig 2.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Interaction plot for predicted psychological distress among different educational levels by level of noise annoyance.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258102.g002

We found a similar result for odour annoyance. For both odour annoyance due to agriculture and to other sources, we found that those with no or primary education experience more psychological distress as a result of it compared to those with tertiary education. Figs 3 and 4 graphically present these results. All in all, we found support for hypothesis 5b, which stated that the psychological distress of less educated people would be stronger affected by odour annoyance than of better educated.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Interaction plot for predicted psychological distress among different educational levels by level of odour annoyance by agriculture.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258102.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Interaction plot for predicted psychological distress among different educational levels by level of noise annoyance by other sources.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258102.g004

Finally, we found non-significant interactions. For noise annoyance, the psychological distress of women was not more strongly affected by noise annoyance compared to men’s (hypothesis 3a) and for both types of odour annoyance we did not find significant interactions across gender or age groups. Hypotheses 3b and 4b did not receive thus support from our analyses.

Additional analysis on the sample including older adults

We re-estimated the model including the interaction with age on the sample that included older adults (65+) (Tables A2-A5 in the S1 Appendix). Like in the previous analyses, we found a significant negative interaction between noise annoyance for young adults when compared to older middle-aged and this interaction effect was also present for older adults (results also presented in Fig 4 in the S1 Appendix).

Sensitivity analyses

We re-estimated our results using logistic regression and a dichotomous dependent variable that differentiated between respondents at risk and not at risk of clinical depression and anxiety disorder. We found that higher levels of noise annoyance and odour annoyance due to other sources were related to a higher chance for clinical depression and anxiety disorder. We did not find a significant effect for odour annoyance due to agriculture after controlling for the other two sources of annoyance (but there was a significant positive effect in the model that did not include noise annoyance). However, different from the main analyses, the interaction terms with age groups or educational levels were not significant.

Conclusion and discussion

In line with previous studies linking noise and odour annoyance and mental health, we found that individuals who experienced higher levels of noise and odour annoyance (due to agriculture or other sources) in their home environment reported higher levels of psychological distress, even after controlling for regional variation and self-reported risks for exposure to noise and odour. When looking into the relative impact of noise and odour annoyance on psychological distress, and in line with a previous study [23], we found that the standardized effect of noise annoyance was the largest. Based on these findings, we encourage researchers and policy makers to pay closer attention to subjective perceptions of the environmental conditions (especially for noise annoyance), as these form a relevant indicator of the burden on health [45]. As such, the subjective evaluations of noise and odour provide a relevant starting point for public health strategies [25], e.g., problem areas within neighbourhoods can be identified or results of policy measures targeted at improving living conditions can be easily evaluated without the need of complex technology or prolonged data collection.

Based on the scarce literature on differential vulnerability to noise and odour annoyance among different sociodemographic groups [45, 60], we expected to find that these environmental conditions would be more harmful for women, individuals with lower levels of education and for older individuals. Lacking an established theoretical framework for such differences, we built our argumentation borrowing from theories and findings that regard differences in coping resources and daily living patterns in the population. Our reasoning was that these elements are important explanations of why noise and odour annoyance would impact psychological distress and their systematic unequal distribution among specific groups would translate into stronger or weaker effects.

Not all our results were in line with this reasoning. For example, we found that among older middle-aged adults noise annoyance was less strongly associated with psychological distress compared to their younger counterparts. Although seemingly counter intuitive, this finding is in line with Pai and Kim [51] who found that for older adults the relationship between neighbourhood perception of physical disorder (e.g., litter or air quality) and psychological distress is less strong compared to their younger counterparts. Their explanation was that older adults have more life experience and a greater psychological strength, which enables them to deal better with a disorderly environment. Moreover, because older adults more often experience hearing loss, they might be less affected, especially by noise annoyance [67].

Contrary to the study by Dratva, Zemp [45], we did not find gender differences in the effects of noise and odour annoyance on psychological distress. We argued that women have lower levels of coping resources such as personal control or financial resources compared to men, but this might not hold in a relatively gender equal society such as the Netherlands (ranked 4 out of 162 countries based on the 2019 Gender Inequality Index [68]). If so, when confronted with unpleasant environmental situations that result in noise and odour annoyance, Dutch women could feel that they can act in a way that would address the problem just as much as Dutch men. Moreover, we argued that women spend more time at home and this would translate in a stronger effect of their environment on their psychological distress. This argument might not hold because the Netherlands has experienced an increase in the labour market participation of women over the years [69]. Finally, and in line with our expectations, we found that educational level was an important factor, i.e., the psychological distress of those with no or primary education was more strongly affected by noise annoyance and odour annoyance compared to those with a tertiary education.

These results present a scattered image of how noise and odour annoyance differently impact psychological distress. Due to data limitations, we were unable to examine the underlying mechanisms that we proposed and in the light of the mixed findings we encourage future research along these lines. In addition, future studies could take other elements of the neighbourhood into account next to noise and odour annoyance, for instance the presence of green space or neighbourhood facilities. This would provide a more in-depth picture into how different aspects of the living environment together influence the mental health of individuals [2]. Finally, as stated in the introduction, there are many variables that influence both noise and odour annoyance on the one hand and psychological distress on the other hand. We cannot rule out that underlying personality traits (like sensitivity to environmental factors) influence both noise annoyance and noise sensitivity [15]. Future studies should investigate this relationship. In addition, a longitudinal design could provide more clarity on the causal relationship between noise and odour annoyance and psychological distress.

To sum up, we provided evidence that both subjective perceptions of noise and odour annoyance are important for psychological distress, but when comparing their relative influence, noise annoyance had a stronger relative contribution to psychological distress. Evidence for stronger effects of noise and odour annoyance on the psychological distress across sociodemographic groups was mixed. In general, our results suggest that classic dimensions of social inequality along age and education lines are reinforced when focusing on the relationship between noise and odour annoyance.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Theo Kuunders, Leonard Vanbrabant and Joyce de Goede for their input in developing the research topic and their feedback on the manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Gruebner O, Rapp MA, Adli M, Kluge U, Galea S, Heinz A. Cities and Mental Health. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2017;114(8):121–7. pmid:28302261.
  2. 2. Honold J, Beyer R, Lakes T, van der Meer E. Multiple environmental burdens and neighborhood-related health of city residents. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2012;32(4):305–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.05.002.
  3. 3. Rautio N, Filatova S, Lehtiniemi H, Miettunen J. Living environment and its relationship to depressive mood: A systematic review. International Journal of Social Psychiatry. 2018;64(1):92–103. pmid:29212385.
  4. 4. Ross CE, Mirowsky J. Neighborhood Disorder, Subjective Alienation, and Distress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2009;50(1):49–64. pmid:19413134.
  5. 5. Schafer MH, Upenieks L. Environmental disorder and functional decline among older adults: A layered context approach. Social Science & Medicine. 2015;124:152–61. pmid:25461872
  6. 6. World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe. Burden of disease from environmental noise: quantification of healthy life years lost in Europe. Copenhagen: World Health Organization. Regional Office for Europe; 2011 2011.
  7. 7. D-NOSES consortium. Odour Pollution—A growing societal concern 2019 June 28 2021]. Available from: https://dnoses.eu/wp-content/uploads/2019/03/Policy-Brief_-Digital-A4-Europe_EN.pdf.
  8. 8. OECD, European Union. Health at a Glance: Europe 2020. 2020. Available from: https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/content/publication/82129230-en.
  9. 9. World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe. The European Mental Health Action Plan 2013–2020. 2015.
  10. 10. Clark C, Paunovic K. WHO Environmental Noise Guidelines for the European Region: A Systematic Review on Environmental Noise and Quality of Life, Wellbeing and Mental Health. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2018;15(11). pmid:30380665
  11. 11. Schiffman SS, Williams CM. Science of Odor as a Potential Health Issue. Journal of Environmental Quality. 2005;34:129–38. pmid:15647542
  12. 12. van den Bosch M, Meyer-Lindenberg A. Environmental Exposures and Depression: Biological Mechanisms and Epidemiological Evidence. Annual Review of Public Health. 2019;40(1):239–59. pmid:30633709
  13. 13. Birk M, Ivina O, von Klot S, Babisch W, Heinrich J. Road traffic noise: self-reported noise annoyance versus GIS modelled road traffic noise exposure. Journal of Environmental Monitoring. 2011;13(11):3237–45. pmid:21952421
  14. 14. Miedema HME, Vos H. Noise sensitivity and reactions to noise and other environmental conditions. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2003;113(3):1492–504. pmid:12656384
  15. 15. Schreckenberg D, Griefahn B, Meis M. The associations between noise sensitivity, reported physical and mental health, perceived environmental quality, and noise annoyance. Noise and Health. 2010;12(46):7–16. pmid:20160386
  16. 16. Weden MM, Carpiano RM, Robert SA. Subjective and objective neighborhood characteristics and adult health. Social Science & Medicine. 2008;66(6):1256–70. pmid:18248865
  17. 17. Aglioti SM, Pazzaglia M. Sounds and scents in (social) action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15(2):47–55. pmid:21239212
  18. 18. Hammersen F, Niemann H, Hoebel J. Environmental Noise Annoyance and Mental Health in Adults: Findings from the Cross-Sectional German Health Update (GEDA) Study 2012. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2016;13(10). pmid:27681736
  19. 19. Lazarus RS, Folkman S. Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York: Springer Publishing Company; 1984.
  20. 20. Beutel ME, Jünger C, Klein EM, Wild P, Lackner K, Blettner M, et al. Noise Annoyance Is Associated with Depression and Anxiety in the General Population- The Contribution of Aircraft Noise. PLOS ONE. 2016;11(5):e0155357. pmid:27195894
  21. 21. Dzhambov AM, Tilov B, Markevych I, Dimitrova D. Residential road traffic noise and general mental health in youth: The role of noise annoyance, neighborhood restorative quality, physical activity, and social cohesion as potential mediators. Environment International. 2017;109:1–9. pmid:28917129
  22. 22. Jensen HAR, Rasmussen B, Ekholm O. Neighbour and traffic noise annoyance: a nationwide study of associated mental health and perceived stress. European Journal of Public Health. 2018;28(6):1050–5. pmid:29846583
  23. 23. Oiamo TH, Luginaah IN, Baxter J. Cumulative effects of noise and odour annoyances on environmental and health related quality of life. Social Science & Medicine. 2015;146:191–203. pmid:26519604
  24. 24. Putrik P, de Vries NK, Mujakovic S, van Amelsvoort L, Kant I, Kunst AE, et al. Living Environment Matters: Relationships Between Neighborhood Characteristics and Health of the Residents in a Dutch Municipality. Journal of Community Health. 2015;40(1):47–56. pmid:24917124
  25. 25. Shepherd D, Dirks K, Welch D, McBride D, Landon J. The Covariance between Air Pollution Annoyance and Noise Annoyance, and Its Relationship with Health-Related Quality of Life. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2016;13(8). pmid:27509512
  26. 26. Beutel ME, Brähler E, Ernst M, Klein E, Reiner I, Wiltink J, et al. Noise annoyance predicts symptoms of depression, anxiety and sleep disturbance 5 years later. Findings from the Gutenberg Health Study. European Journal of Public Health. 2020. pmid:32034913
  27. 27. Hooiveld M, van Dijk CE, van der Sman-de Beer F, Smit LA, Vogelaar M, Wouters IM, et al. Odour annoyance in the neighbourhood of livestock farming–perceived health and health care seeking behaviour. Annals of Agricultural and Environmental Medicine. 2015;22(1):55–61. pmid:25780829
  28. 28. Lowman A, McDonald MA, Wing S, Muhammad N. Land application of treated sewage sludge: community health and environmental justice. Environ Health Perspect. 2013;121(5):537–42. Epub 2013/04/09. pmid:23562940
  29. 29. Radon K, Peters A, Praml G, Ehrenstein V, Schulze A, Hehl O, et al. Livestock odours and quality of life of neighbouring residents. Annals of Agricultural and Environmental Medicine. 2004;11(1):59–62. pmid:15236499
  30. 30. Barnett A, Zhang CJP, Johnston JM, Cerin E. Relationships between the neighborhood environment and depression in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. International Psychogeriatrics. 2018;30(8):1153–76. Epub 2017/12/10. pmid:29223174
  31. 31. Sharkey P, Faber JW. Where, When, Why, and For Whom Do Residential Contexts Matter? Moving Away from the Dichotomous Understanding of Neighborhood Effects. Annual Review of Sociology. 2014;40(1):559–79.
  32. 32. Netherlands Environmental Assessment Agency. Environmental Balance 2009—Summary. 2009.
  33. 33. Brabantse Milieufederatie. Volksgezondheid en veehouderij: alles op een rij 2017.
  34. 34. National Institute for Public Health and the Environment. Veehouderij en gezondheid omwonenden. 2016.
  35. 35. Hobfoll SE, Jackson AP. Conservation of resources in community intervention. American Journal of Community Psychology. 1991;19(1):111–21. pmid:1867144
  36. 36. Riedel N, Köckler H, Scheiner J, Berger K. Objective exposure to road traffic noise, noise annoyance and self-rated poor health–framing the relationship between noise and health as a matter of multiple stressors and resources in urban neighbourhoods. Journal of Environmental Planning and Management. 2015;58(2):336–56.
  37. 37. Kroesen M, Molin EJE, van Wee B. Testing a theory of aircraft noise annoyance: A structural equation analysis. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2008;123(6):4250–60. pmid:18537376
  38. 38. Casey JA, Kim BF, Larsen J, Price LB, Nachman KE. Industrial Food Animal Production and Community Health. Current Environmental Health Reports. 2015;2(3):259–71. pmid:26231503
  39. 39. van Kamp I, Davies H. Noise and health in vulnerable groups: A review. Noise and Health. 2013;15(64):153–9. pmid:23689296
  40. 40. van Deurzen I, Rod NH, Christensen U, Hansen ÅM, Lund R, Dich N. Neighborhood perceptions and allostatic load: Evidence from Denmark. Health & Place. 2016;40:1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2016.04.010.
  41. 41. Dreger S, Schüle SA, Hilz LK, Bolte G. Social Inequalities in Environmental Noise Exposure: A Review of Evidence in the WHO European Region. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2019;16(6). pmid:30897765
  42. 42. Wallenius MA. The interaction of noise stress and personal project stress on subjective health. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2004;24(2):167–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2003.12.002.
  43. 43. Öhrström E, Skånberg A, Svensson H, Gidlöf-Gunnarsson A. Effects of road traffic noise and the benefit of access to quietness. Journal of Sound and Vibration. 2006;295(1):40–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsv.2005.11.034.
  44. 44. von Lindern E, Hartig T, Lercher P. Traffic-related exposures, constrained restoration, and health in the residential context. Health & Place. 2016;39:92–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2015.12.003.
  45. 45. Dratva J, Zemp E, Dietrich DF, Bridevaux P-O, Rochat T, Schindler C, et al. Impact of road traffic noise annoyance on health-related quality of life: results from a population-based study. Quality of Life Research. 2010;19(1):37–46. pmid:20044782
  46. 46. Ross CE, Mirowsky J, Pribesh S. Powerlessness and the Amplification of Threat: Neighborhood Disadvantage, Disorder, and Mistrust. American Sociological Review. 2001;66(4):568–91.
  47. 47. Ross CE, Mirowsky J. Social structure and psychological functioning. In: Delamater J, editor. Handbook of Social Psychology. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers; 2006. p. 411–47.
  48. 48. Carver CS, Scheier MF, Weintraub JK. Assessing coping strategies: a theoretically based approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989;56(2):267–83. Epub 1989/02/01. pmid:2926629.
  49. 49. Grow A, Van Bavel J. The Gender Cliff in the Relative Contribution to the Household Income: Insights from Modelling Marriage Markets in 27 European Countries. European Journal of Population. 2020;36(4):711–33. pmid:32994759
  50. 50. Lachman ME. Development in Midlife. Annual Review of Psychology. 2004;55(1):305–31. pmid:14744218
  51. 51. Pai M, Kim J. Neighborhood Physical Disorder and Psychological Distress: Does the Risk Increase With Age? The International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 2016;84(4):378–402. pmid:27913760
  52. 52. Hoogerbrugge MM, Burger MJ. Neighborhood-Based social capital and life satisfaction: the case of Rotterdam, The Netherlands. Urban Geography. 2018;39(10):1484–509.
  53. 53. Blanes-Vidal V, Suh H, Nadimi ES, Løfstrøm P, Ellermann T, Andersen HV, et al. Residential exposure to outdoor air pollution from livestock operations and perceived annoyance among citizens. Environment International. 2012;40:44–50. pmid:22280927
  54. 54. Paunović K, Jakovljević B, Belojević G. Predictors of noise annoyance in noisy and quiet urban streets. Science of The Total Environment. 2009;407(12):3707–11. pmid:19327817
  55. 55. Umberson D, Pudrovska T, Reczek C. Parenthood, Childlessness, and Well-Being: A Life Course Perspective. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2010;72(3):612–29. pmid:21869847
  56. 56. National Institute for Public Health and the Environment. Gezondheidsmonitor Volwassenen en Ouderen 2020 [cited 2020 July 17]. Available from: https://www.monitorgezondheid.nl/gezondheidsmonitor-volwassenen-en-ouderen.
  57. 57. Donker T, Comijs H, Cuijpers P, Terluin B, Nolen W, Zitman F, et al. The validity of the Dutch K10 and extended K10 screening scales for depressive and anxiety disorders. Psychiatry Research. 2010;176(1):45–50. pmid:20071036
  58. 58. Kessler R, Mroczek D. Final versions of our non-specific psychological distress scale. Ann Arbor, MI: Survey Research Center of the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan. 1994.
  59. 59. Dzhambov AM, Markevych I, Tilov B, Arabadzhiev Z, Stoyanov D, Gatseva P, et al. Pathways linking residential noise and air pollution to mental ill-health in young adults. Environmental Research. 2018;166:458–65. pmid:29940479
  60. 60. Boardman JD, Downey L, Jackson JS, Merrill JB, Saint Onge JM, Williams DR. Proximate industrial activity and psychological distress. Population and Environment. 2008;30(1):3–25. pmid:19444334
  61. 61. Idler EL, Benyamini Y. Self-rated health and mortality: a review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of health and social behavior. 1997:21–37. pmid:9097506
  62. 62. Schüle SA, Nanninga S, Dreger S, Bolte G. Relations between Objective and Perceived Built Environments and the Modifying Role of Individual Socioeconomic Position. A Cross-Sectional Study on Traffic Noise and Urban Green Space in a Large German City. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2018;15(8):1562. pmid:30042308
  63. 63. Thomas GO, Fisher R, Whitmarsh L, Milfont TL, Poortinga W. The impact of parenthood on environmental attitudes and behaviour: a longitudinal investigation of the legacy hypothesis. Population and Environment. 2018;39(3):261–76. pmid:29568145
  64. 64. Nomaguchi KM, Milkie MA. Costs and Rewards of Children: The Effects of Becoming a Parent on Adults’ Lives. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2003;65(2):356–74. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2003.00356.x.
  65. 65. Simon RW, Caputo J. The Costs and Benefits of Parenthood for Mental and Physical Health in the United States: The Importance of Parenting Stage. Society and Mental Health. 2018;9(3):296–315.
  66. 66. Slob M, van Ballegooij M, Breugelmans O, Esser P, Groenewold A, Janssen I, et al. GGD-richtlijn medische milieukunde: omgevingsgeluid en gezondheid. 2019.
  67. 67. Van Gerven PWM, Vos H, Van Boxtel MPJ, Janssen SA, Miedema HME. Annoyance from environmental noise across the lifespan. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2009;126(1):187–94. pmid:19603876
  68. 68. United Nations Development Program. Human Development Report 2020. The Next Frontier: Human Development and the Antropocene 2020.
  69. 69. Khoudja Y, Fleischmann F. Gender Ideology and Women’s Labor Market Transitions Within Couples in the Netherlands. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2018;80(5):1087–106.