Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Effects of early life adversity on immediate early gene expression: Systematic review and 3-level meta-analysis of rodent studies

  • Heike Schuler ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Heike Schuler, Valeria Bonapersona

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Translational Neuroscience, University Medical Center Utrecht Brain Center, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands, Integrated Program in Neuroscience, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada

  • Valeria Bonapersona ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Heike Schuler, Valeria Bonapersona

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    v.bonapersona-2@umcutrecht.nl

    Affiliation Department of Translational Neuroscience, University Medical Center Utrecht Brain Center, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands

  • Marian Joëls ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ MJ and RAS also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliations Department of Translational Neuroscience, University Medical Center Utrecht Brain Center, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands

  • R. Angela Sarabdjitsingh

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ MJ and RAS also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Department of Translational Neuroscience, University Medical Center Utrecht Brain Center, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands

Abstract

Early-life adversity (ELA) causes long-lasting structural and functional changes to the brain, rendering affected individuals vulnerable to the development of psychopathologies later in life. Immediate-early genes (IEGs) provide a potential marker for the observed alterations, bridging the gap between activity-regulated transcription and long-lasting effects on brain structure and function. Several heterogeneous studies have used IEGs to identify differences in cellular activity after ELA; systematically investigating the literature is therefore crucial for comprehensive conclusions. Here, we performed a systematic review on 39 pre-clinical studies in rodents to study the effects of ELA (alteration of maternal care) on IEG expression. Females and IEGs other than cFos were investigated in only a handful of publications. We meta-analyzed publications investigating specifically cFos expression. ELA increased cFos expression after an acute stressor only if the animals (control and ELA) had experienced additional hits. At rest, ELA increased cFos expression irrespective of other life events, suggesting that ELA creates a phenotype similar to naïve, acutely stressed animals. We present a conceptual theoretical framework to interpret the unexpected results. Overall, ELA likely alters IEG expression across the brain, especially in interaction with other negative life events. The present review highlights current knowledge gaps and provides guidance to aid the design of future studies.

Introduction

Synaptic connections in the brain are continuously altered, including via gene expression, to accommodate experiences, thereby preparing the organism to deal with future events [13]. This potential for adaptation, called neuronal or synaptic plasticity, is prominently present during critical periods early in life [4]. For this reason, adverse experiences throughout childhood–such as physical, sexual or emotional abuse–have far-reaching effects on an individual’s brain function and structure, and consequently on cognition and behavior [57]. It is therefore not surprising that early-life adversity (ELA) is consistently associated with an increased risk for psychopathologies later in life, including major depressive disorder (MDD), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and schizophrenia [8, 9].

To investigate the mechanisms underlying the effects of ELA on brain and behavior, several models of alteration of maternal care in rodents have been developed [10, 11]. These models consistently show that ELA leads to fundamental remodeling of stress-sensitive brain regions, which in turn may be linked to altered function [12, 13]. For example, ELA has been reported to modify the regulatory response of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, an essential part of the organism’s stress response system [14, 15]. Furthermore, rodents exposed to ELA display a robust behavioral phenotype characterized by enhanced anxiety-like behavior, changes in memory formation, and decreased social behavior [1619]. Overall, this evidence highlights that ELA leads to structural, functional and behavioral alterations in the rodent brain, yet the events giving rise to the said alterations remain unclear.

Immediate-early genes (IEGs), such as cFos (alias Fos), Egr1 (alias Zif-268, NGFI-A, Krox-24) and Arc (alias Arg3.1), provide a potential link between experience-induced cellular activity in the brain and the resulting long-term changes in neurons and synapses. IEGs are immediately and transiently expressed in response to extracellular calcium influx, as occurs when an action potential is fired [20]. Among the IEGs, cFos is most often studied; it forms the activator protein-1 (AP1) by dimerization with a Jun-family transcription factor [21]. The AP1 complex initiates the transcription of other late genes, which result in long-lasting changes of cellular physiology. Consequently, a strong relationship between IEG expression and neuronal activity is observed, with increases in neuronal activity being accompanied by increased IEG expression [20]. For decades, IEGs have been a prominent tool for mapping neuronal activity in rodents by means of immunohistochemistry (IHC) and in-situ hybridization (ISH) due to their brain-wide expression. More recently, IEGs have been increasingly investigated for their protein properties, in particular with respect to synaptic plasticity [22].

Whereas the downstream products of IEGs are diverse (e.g., transcription factors, postsynaptic proteins, secretory factors), their functions are surprisingly homogeneous and can mostly be related to cellular processes, such as dendrite and spine development; synapse formation, strength and elimination; and regulation of the excitatory/inhibitory balance ([3]; Fig 1). In line with this functional similarity, knockouts (Kos) of several different IEGs affect behavior and synaptic plasticity in a similar manner. More specifically, system-wide Arc-KO and Egr1-KO, as well as central nervous system-specific cFos-KO mice all display behavioral impairments in learning and memory as well as deficits in long-term potentiation or depression, underscoring the necessity of IEGs for memory formation and retention [2325]. In addition, many neuropsychiatric disorders characterized by memory impairments, such as major depressive disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder and schizophrenia, have also been shown to feature a dysregulation of activity-dependent transcription [26]. Interestingly, the risk to develop any of these disorders is increased by exposure to ELA, further indicating a potential causal interaction between ELA, IEGs and mental health [8, 9].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Mechanisms of IEG activation.

A) Action potentials induced by glutamate signaling result in membrane depolarization, which in turn results in opening of L-type voltage calcium channels (LVCCs). The resulting Ca2+ influx induces calcium-dependent signaling pathways. These cascades further result in the recruitment of existing transcription factor, such as CREB, which in turn lead to the expression of IEGs. Once transcribed, IEGs act as B) transcription factors in the nucleus or C) regulators of synaptic plasticity at the synapse as, for example, post-synaptic proteins. B) The transcription factors of the Fos family bind to a transcription factor of the Jun family to form the AP1 complex, whereas Egr1 acts independently. Egr1 and cFos are transiently expressed, whereas ΔFosB accumulates over time in the nucleus. C) Arc acts at the post-synaptic density by reducing the number of surface AMPA receptors. Therefore, increased Arc expression results in reduced synaptic strength by AMPA receptor endocytosis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253406.g001

While numerous studies have used IEGs to identify differences in cellular activity after ELA, the study designs are heterogeneous, and findings are seemingly discrepant. Reviewing the available literature will provide a clearer picture of the effects of ELA on IEG expression and will aid future development of study designs by identifying sources of heterogeneity within and between experiments. To that end, we performed a systematic review to synthesize the available evidence and explore outcomes in a sex-, gene- and region-specific manner. A meta-analysis was then conducted on a subset of the data based on a priori determined thresholds. We hypothesized that ELA as alteration of maternal care leads to an exaggerated increase in IEG expression after an acute stress challenge, further amplified by exposure to additional hits in life, in line with the multiple-hit concept of vulnerability [27].

Methods

Search strategy, protocol and risk of bias assessment of the present review were performed in line with SYRCLE (Systematic Review Center for Laboratory animal Experimentation) guidelines [2830]. We adhered to the PRISMA checklist for reporting [31] (Supporting Information). The protocol (S1.1 in S1 File) and the PRISMA checklist are openly accessible at https://osf.io/qkyvd/.

Study selection and data extraction

We conducted a systematic literature search with the search engines PubMed and Embase on the 3rd of April 2019 to select experiments investigating differences in IEG expression between control and ELA exposed rodents. The terms ‘mice and rats’ and ‘postnatal ELA’ were used to construct the search string (S1.2 in S1 File). For the purpose of this review, ELA was defined as models altering maternal care. We included the ELA models of maternal separation and deprivation, isolation, limited bedding and nesting, as well as licking and grooming. Study selection was performed in Rayyan [32] in alphabetical order and any disagreements between investigators were resolved by discussion until unison was reached. An overview of the study selection procedure is displayed in Fig 2.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Flow-chart of study selection process.

^ = not included in pre-specified inclusion/exclusion criteria (S1.1 in S1 File).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253406.g002

A complete list of final inclusion and exclusion criteria can be found in the protocol (S1.1 in S1 File). First, titles and abstracts were screened by at least three blinded investigators (HS, VB, EK, DvN, LvM) for the following exclusion criteria: 1) not a primary experimental publication, 2) not using adult (>8 weeks) mice or rats which are younger than 1 year, 3) not using a postnatal model of ELA as specified in S1.1 in S1 File. Eligibility was then determined by full-text screening of the remaining studies by at least two blinded investigators (HS, EK, LvM), with a random subset screening performed by a fourth (VB), blinded investigator to confirm agreement. Publications were deemed non-eligible based on the following criteria: 1) not measuring an IEG product in the brain, 2) deviation from a priori determined criteria concerning the background of the animals, interventions, or outcomes, 3) control and experimental groups differed at more aspects than just ELA exposure. Lastly, reference sections of eligible publications were screened for articles missed by the search string, but none were added through this procedure.

Data from eligible studies were extracted into a combined dataset using a priori determined sets of variables to comprehensively capture experimental design, methods and results with minimal subjectivity (S1.3 in S1 File). Differently from the original protocol, we extracted also measurements without acute stress to have an appropriate control, baseline condition. Outcome data for each comparison (i.e. group-based mean and variance) were extracted in the following order of preference: 1) from numbers provided in the text or tables; 2) from graphs by using WebPlotDigitizer (v4.3 [33]; or 3) from statistical test results. A comparison is defined as the difference in expression of a specific IEG in a specific brain area at rest or after acute stress exposure in ELA-exposed animals and controls. To compare the results on a systematic review level, we performed an independent samples t-tests on the extracted summary statistics. The results were interpreted dichotomously as significant / not significant, with p<0.05 used as a criterion. We chose this approach to equalize the statistical method used for analysis across publications.

Meta-analysis

Data selection.

We performed a meta-analysis on outcomes that were assessed by at least three independent comparisons (i.e., at least one comparison from three independent publications). During analysis coding, the investigators were blinded to the outcome by randomly multiplying half of the effect sizes by -1.

To account for potential sex differences, we planned to perform separate meta-analyses for males and females. However, only few comparisons were reported for female rodents, and their study designs were strongly heterogeneous. We therefore restricted our quantitative synthesis to outcomes from male rodents, with female data being evaluated qualitatively only. Furthermore, only comparisons using either IHC, immunocytochemistry (ICC), or ISH to quantify IEG expression were included on the meta-analytic level. While both methods differ in the type of molecule being assessed, quantification and analysis procedures largely overlap. To confirm this, we investigated whether the choice of quantification method affects the outcome. PCR based methods and western blots were evaluated qualitatively only.

Based on the aforementioned threshold and restrictions, the meta-analysis was performed on comparisons of cFos expression in the amygdala, thalamus, hippocampal formation, hypothalamus, prefrontal cortex and midbrain at rest and after acute stress experiences. Smaller subregions were grouped into larger structures (S1.4 in S1 File) in line with the Allen Mouse Brain Atlas (©2004, Allen Institute for Brain Science) to allow for comparisons between studies.

Statistical analysis.

For comparisons included in the meta-analysis, we calculated the standardized mean difference Hedge’s g as a measure of effect size. If only the standard error of the mean (SEM) was reported, the standard deviation (SD) was calculated as SEM*√n, where n = the number of animals per group. If the total number of animals was reported, this was distributed equally across groups. If the number of animals was reported as a range (e.g.6-8 animals/group), we used the mean (e.g. 7 animals/group). If the same control group was used as control of multiple experimental groups (e.g. different ELA models), the sample size of the control group was divided by the number of experimental groups and the adjusted sample size was used for the calculation of the effect size [34]. Heterogeneity was assessed with Cochran’s Q-test [35]. Influential outliers were determined in accordance with Viechtbauer and Cheung [36] and removed from quantitative synthesis. Of such comparisons, we explored whether elements of the experimental design could explain the deviation of these comparisons from the mean.

A three-level mixed-effects model was built to capture variance not only between publications (Level 1), but also between experiments (Level 2) and outcomes (Level 3), thereby taking into account the statistical dependency of outcomes acquired from the same animals within the same publication [3739]. Moderators of the multilevel model were i) presence of an acute stress challenge, ii) presence of additional hits and iii) brain area.

We tested whether ELA effect sizes at rest or after acute stress challenges are significantly different from zero to understand the effects of ELA on cFos expression under each of these conditions. Subsequently, a subgroup analysis was performed to investigate whether the effects are moderated by the experience of multiple negative life experiences (additional hits). The presence of additional hits was classified with previously determined criteria [16]. Finally, we explored the effects of type of acute stressor (i.e. mild versus severe, S1.4 in S1 File), novelty of stress experience, and brain region using subgroup analyses.

Bias assessment and sensitivity analyses.

We followed SYRCLE guidelines on risk of bias assessment, with items not reported being coded as ‘unclear’ [30]. To detect publication bias, funnel plot asymmetries for each outcome variable were evaluated [30]. Due to the uneven frequency of the number of studies, we performed sensitivity analyses (rather than subgroup as specified in the protocol) on the type of ELA model, and difference between mRNA and protein. Since these analyses were not initially included, the results were only qualitatively assessed and were in line with the interpretation of the main results. All analyses can be found at our repository (https://osf.io/qkyvd/).

Software.

All analyses were performed in R (v3.6.1; [40]). The following R packages were used: etaphor (v 2.1.0; [41]), tidyverse (v1.2.1; [42]). Data are presented as the standardized mean difference Hedge’s g and standard error of the mean (g[±SEM]). The significance level α was set to 0.05. Multiple testing correction on the planned analysis was performed using the Holm-Bonferroni method [43]. The code for analysis is openly accessible at https://osf.io/qkyvd/.

Results

Study selection and characteristics

A total of 1019 animals reported in 39 publications were included in the review. The animals were predominantly male (72.5%); rats (76.3%) were used more often than mice; and protein (77.4%) rather than mRNA was more frequently assessed as outcome. The IEG cFos was investigated in the majority of studies (88.7%), and maternal separation was the most frequently used ELA model (90.6%). Fig 3 shows a graphical overview of the study characteristics.

thumbnail
Fig 3.

A) Study characteristics and B) Investigated brain areas reported as percentage of experiments. Fem = females.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253406.g003

Research synthesis

Systematic review of cFos and ELA.

A total of 31 publications reported cFos expression in control and ELA animals (Table 1). IEG expression was reported to be significantly affected by ELA in 72 (45.8%) comparisons, of which 33 (59.6%) displayed upregulation and 39 comparisons (54.2%) reported downregulation.

thumbnail
Table 1. Overview of study designs and findings of reviewed publications reporting on cFos expression in ELA and control animals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253406.t001

Overall, of the 322 comparisons within these studies, 140 comparisons (npub = 20) qualified for further meta-analysis in male rodents after removal of influential outliers (ncomp = 1); these are analyzed quantitatively in the following section. No element of the experimental design pointed towards a biological origin of the outlying value, nor was its publication published in a predatory journal [75]. Comparisons were excluded from quantitative review because of brain area (ncomp = 40), acute stressor type (ncomp = 49; S2.4.3 in S1 File) or unspecified or pooled sex (ncomp = 15). The excluded comparisons are subject to a qualitative review in the Supporting Information.

Given fundamental biological differences between males and females [76], we a priori chose to evaluate female cFos data separately from males’. However, only ten publications reported on cFos expression in female rodents (ncomp = 77). Given the limited number of studies, with variable designs, we had to abandon the separate meta-analytical evaluation of female rodents. Qualitatively, the majority of the studies with females found no significant differences between cFos levels of ELA versus controls at rest or after an acute stress challenge (ncomp = 55 [51, 54, 55, 57, 62]). A more detailed description is supplied in the Supporting Information.

Systematic review of ELA and other IEGs.

We here only summarize the main findings on IEGs other than cFos. In general, the number of studies on these IEGs compared to cFos was very limited. For a more elaborate description and discussion we refer to the Supporting Information.

Arc is a post-synaptic protein, which plays an essential role in regulating the homeostatic scaling of AMPA receptors, thereby directly modifying plasticity at the synapse [77]. Arc expression was investigated in only five publications under varying conditions in male and female mice and rats (see Table 2 and Supporting Information).

thumbnail
Table 2. Overview of study designs and findings of reviewed publications reporting on Arc expression in ELA and control animals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253406.t002

Early-growth response (Egr) proteins are a family of transcription factors with a zinc-finger motif, which allows all Egr factors to connect to identical DNA binding sites [82]. We identified only three studies investigating Egr expression after ELA exposure at rest (Table 3 and Supporting Information); specifically, one investigated Egr-1 [83], another investigated Egr-4 only [80], and one other investigated Egr-2 and Egr-4 [79].

thumbnail
Table 3. Overview of study designs and findings of reviewed publications reporting on expression of the Egr-family in ELA and control animals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253406.t003

FosB is an IEG of the Fos family, and—similarly to cFos—it binds to members of the Jun family to form the AP1 transcription factor [84]. Of particular interest in stress research is its isoform ΔFosB, whose extended half-life makes it an exceptional marker for chronic stress [84]. Three publications reporting on the expression of ΔFosB at rest in ELA and control animals were identified (see Table 4 and Supporting Information).

thumbnail
Table 4. Overview of study designs and findings of reviewed publications reporting on ΔFosB expression in ELA and control animals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253406.t004

Meta-analysis of cFos in male rodents.

For cFos, our survey yielded sufficient data to carry out a meta-analysis, next to the systematic review. In comparison to control animals, rodents with a history of ELA displayed significantly increased cFos levels at rest (g[SEM] = 0.421[±0.18], t = 2.35, padj = 0.041), but not after acute stress exposure (g[SEM] = 0.133[±0.166], t = 0.805, padj = 0.422; Fig 4A). To gain a deeper understanding of these findings, we performed subgroup analyses to investigate the experience of additional hits, i.e. an additional negative life event. Of note, the control and experimental groups always differed only in the presence/absence of ELA. Therefore, in the ‘additional hits’ comparisons, both control and ELA animals experienced multiple negative life events. This was important for cFos expression after acute stress, where the effects of ELA were pronounced only in synergy with additional hits (Fig 4B, acuteno hit: g[SEM] = -0.193[±0.135], z = -1.436, padj = 0.151; acutemult hits: g[SEM] = 0.442[±0.159], z = 2.784, p = 0.016; at restno hit: g[SEM] = 0.475 [±0.16], z = 2.976, p < .012; at restmult hits: g[SEM] = 0.344[±0.153], z = 2.253, p = 0.049; the analyses were conducted comparing the effect size between control and ELA animals against 0). Lastly, we performed an exploratory analysis to investigate potential interactions with acute stressor severity on the effect sizes. For the categorization of acute stressor severity, please see Supporting Information S1.4 in S1 File. Acute stressor severity was not a significant moderator (QM(3) = 4.35, p = 0.226). Of note, no publication investigated cFos expression after a mild acute stressor in animals that experienced additional hits (ncomp = 0). Of the 20 publications included in the meta-analysis, only two did not use maternal separation as an ELA model [49, 59]. Nevertheless, these studies adhere to the above findings with no significant differences found after acute stress in the areas meta-analytically investigated. The findings of our main analysis do not confirm our hypothesis that cFos expression is higher in ELA animals compared to control particularly after acute stress; rather, the results indicate that cFos expression is increased after ELA already at baseline, i.e. at rest. Moreover, the results highlight the relevance of including the presence of additional hits in the analysis.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Main and subgroup analyses.

A) Effects of ELA on cFos expression in male rodents at rest and after an acute stressor. B) Subgroup analysis for absence (No Additional Hits) or presence (Additional Hits) of additional negative life events. Of note, control and experimental animals always differed only in the presence/absence of ELA. Therefore, in the ‘Additional Hits’ comparison, also control animal experienced the additional negative life events. * p < 0.05.H.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253406.g004

Next, we tested whether the effects of ELA on cFos expression differed across brain regions important for the stress reaction (Fig 5), when only considering those datasets with sufficient observations (npublications>3). Brain region was not a significant moderator (QM(12) = 13.908, p = 0.307) of the effects of ELA on cFos expression. Exploratory subgroup analysis suggests that at rest all brain areas show a comparable increase in cFos expression. After an acute stress challenge, the effects appeared more variable across brain areas than at rest. We then performed an additional exploratory analysis to investigate whether brain areas after acute stress differed after ELA with / without the experience of additional hits. The results of this analysis suggested that the prefrontal cortex may be specifically affected; however, since this effect was supported by those studies unevenly represented in the funnel plot, these results may not be reliable due to presumed publication bias.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Effects of ELA on cFos expression across brain areas at rest and after an acute stressor.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253406.g005

Despite significant contribution of the moderators (QM(23) = 40.089, p = 0.015), residual heterogeneity between studies remained significant (QE(117) = 167.95, p = 0.001). Study of the distribution of variance showed that remaining variance is mainly attributable to differences between experiments (Level 2) and not to differences within experiments (Level 3). Concerning potential bias, while reporting risk of bias was incomplete in all publications (S2.1a in S1 File), 46% of studies reported adequate randomization and blinding procedures (npub = 10). Visual assessment of the funnel plot for the studies qualifying for quantitative synthesis suggests the presence of publication bias (S2.1b in S1 File), which was also supported by a significant Eggers’ test (z = 4.6903, p < .0001). We identified two studies [52, 69] which were mainly responsible for the bias.

Discussion

In this review, we synthesized the evidence of 39 publications investigating the effects of ELA on IEG expression in mice and rats. Due to low number of animals used in preclinical research, studies are commonly underpowered [88], rendering results of individual studies vulnerable to confounding effects of the chosen study design. In order to circumvent this limitation, we systematically reviewed the available literature on several IEGs in males and females. We meta-analyzed a subset of our male data to quantify cFos expression following ELA exposure and to identify potential moderators of the observed effects. Using a three-level mixed effects model, we observed an increase in cFos expression after an acute stress exposure due to ELA only in combination with one or more other negative life events. This suggests that ELA creates a vulnerable phenotype that is manifested only when sufficiently triggered. If rodents had ‘only’ experienced ELA, we report–contrary to our expectations–an increase in cFos expression already at rest, suggesting that the situation normally seen (in naïve rodents) after acute stress is already visible at rest when the animals have been exposed to early life adversity. These findings led us to propose a new model as outlined in Fig 6.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Summary interpretation of the results.

Cartoon image of how to interpret effect sizes in absolute terms (y-axis, cFos expression, e.g. number of cFos+ cells). Significance levels identify the difference between control and ELA groups that we identified in our analysis (Fig 3). Of note, cFos expression levels are expected to be higher after acute stress than at rest, although this cannot be studied in the current meta-analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253406.g006

At rest, ELA animals compared to controls show increased IEG expression. Since raw values of IEG expression are either not reported or of incomparable scales, we could only investigate effect sizes and not absolute values of IEG expression. This has a direct effect on the interpretation of the results. Specifically, if IEG levels in control animals were low, effect sizes could be inflated. If IEG levels in control animals were high, the results should be interpreted not as “rest” but rather as “mildly aroused”, since IEG levels are expected to be minimal for control, naïve animals. Nonetheless, we observed a consistent, positive standardized mean difference in cFos expression after ELA across five out of the six quantitatively investigated brain regions. This suggests a small, but stable brain-wide effect. Previous studies showed that IEG expression matches the transcriptional activity from early environment and experiences [89]. In control animals, this is likely to result in a minimal IEG expression. However, in ELA animals, the expression observed may be the result of long-lasting ELA effects on brain structure and chemistry [90]. Indeed, the transcriptional activity of ELA mice at rest is comparable to that of acutely-stressed control mice [91]. Increased activity-regulated transcription at rest after ELA could be indicative of an overall synaptic alteration, in accordance with increased anxiety-like behavior and reduced memory performance under neutral conditions [16]. Functionally, increased IEG expression at rest could reflect a differential, less adaptive way of processing previous experiences and could potentially hint towards an overall increased transcriptional activity as a result of synaptic sensitization. Intuitively, considering the relationship between IEGs and synaptic plasticity, we would suspect that ELA results in increased synaptic plasticity. In line with this idea, it has been shown that ELA leads to increased LTP in freely-behaving adult, male rats as compared to controls [92].Taken together, this evidence suggests that differences we report in IEG expression after ELA at rest may underlie long-lasting effects on transcriptional activity, pushing the system towards an “activated” state similar to acute stress.

The model proposed in Fig 6 relies primarily on the quantitative and qualitative analysis of cFos data, as there are only few publications investigating the effects of ELA on the expression of the IEGs Arc, ΔFosB, and IEGs of the Egr-family. Nonetheless, the available evidence suggests a sensitization effect of ELA on IEG expression (and, more generally, synaptic plasticity) at rest. Although IEGs overlap in function and overall expression pattern, they have specific and independent roles [3, 20, 2325, 93]. cFos and Egr-family members are transcription factors, while Arc is a post-synaptic protein modifying dendritic AMPA receptors, and ΔFosB is a less transient marker of neuronal activity [84, 94, 95]. With technological advances in the field of immunohistochemistry and bioinformatics it becomes increasingly feasible to investigate and interpret multiple IEGs within one animal, thereby also allowing for the investigation of interactions between IEGs and their downstream effects. In the future, the study of different IEGs could be used as a proxy to more thoroughly understand ELA-induced changes in gene-regulated synaptic plasticity [96].

On a systematic review level, effects in females appear more limited than in males. Whether this is a true biological effect is unclear. For example, it could be due to the comparatively low number of female publications, or to a male-biased experimental methodology [16, 76]. While it has been shown that acute stress exposure increases IEG expression in both sexes in the hippocampus [97], it is possible that effects of ELA on IEG expression will be more subtle in females than in males due to model characteristics. Of note, among the 39 publications included in this review, only 5 investigated males and females under the same experimental conditions.

Lastly, given the expected heterogeneity in study designs, we restricted our meta-analysis to adult animals only, and–at this stage–it cannot be generalized to other age groups. It is possible that different patterns of IEG expression associated to ELA exposure may emerge in juvenile or adolescent animals. Future experiments investigating the longitudinal effects of ELA on IEG expression over the course of development can shed light on the interaction between ELA, development and IEG-related brain activity.

To conclude, we systematically provided a general overview on the relationship between ELA and IEG expression and highlighted current knowledge gaps. Despite subject-specific and methodological limitations, the outcomes of the meta-analysis were robust and suggest a sensitization of activity-regulated transcription in ELA rodents at rest while changes after acute stress only became apparent in combination with additional hits. Recent advances in the fields of immunostaining, live cell imaging and bioinformatics may help close the described voids, yielding a more comprehensive picture on the complex relationship between IEGs, ELA and psychopathologies.

Supporting information

S1 File. Supplementary methods, results and references.

S1.1) SYRCLE’s study protocol; S1.2) Search string; S1.3) Extracted variables; S1.4) Grouping of variables into functional units; S2.1) Bias assessment; S2.2) Sensitivity analysis species; S2.3) Forest plot; S2.4) systematic review; S3) supplementary references.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253406.s001

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Eline Kraaijenvanger, Dennis van Nuijs, Lieke van Mourik for their help with articles’ selection, and Judith van Luijk for reviewing the study protocol.

References

  1. 1. Burns SB, Szyszkowicz JK, Luheshi GN, et al. Plasticity of the epigenome during early-life stress. Semin Cell Dev Biol 2018; 77: 115–132. pmid:29017800
  2. 2. Citri A, Malenka RC. Synaptic plasticity: Multiple forms, functions, and mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008; 33: 18–41. pmid:17728696
  3. 3. Yap EL, Greenberg ME. Activity-Regulated Transcription: Bridging the Gap between Neural Activity and Behavior. Neuron 2018; 100: 330–348. pmid:30359600
  4. 4. Hübener M, Bonhoeffer T. Neuronal plasticity: Beyond the critical period. Cell 2014; 159: 727–737. pmid:25417151
  5. 5. Lang J, Kerr DM, Petri-Romão P, et al. The hallmarks of childhood abuse and neglect: A systematic review. PLoS One 2020; 15: e0243639. pmid:33290423
  6. 6. Lippard ETC, Nemeroff CB. The devastating clinical consequences of child abuse and neglect: Increased disease vulnerability and poor treatment response in mood disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2020; 177: 20–36. pmid:31537091
  7. 7. Nemeroff CB. Paradise Lost: The Neurobiological and Clinical Consequences of Child Abuse and Neglect. Neuron 2016; 89: 892–909. pmid:26938439
  8. 8. Herzog JI, Schmahl C. Adverse Childhood Experiences and the Consequences on Neurobiological, Psychosocial, and Somatic Conditions Across the Lifespan. Front Psychiatry 2018; 9: 420. pmid:30233435
  9. 9. Merrick MT, Ports KA, Ford DC, et al. Unpacking the impact of adverse childhood experiences on adult mental health. Child Abus Negl 2017; 69: 10–19. pmid:28419887
  10. 10. Liu D, Diorio J, Tannenbaum B, et al. Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science (80-) 1997; 277: 1659–1662.
  11. 11. Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. Early, postnatal experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content and stress-induced release in adult rats. Mol Brain Res 1993; 18: 195–200. pmid:8497182
  12. 12. McEwen BS, Nasca C, Gray JD, et al. Stress Effects on Neuronal Structure: Hippocampus, Amygdala, and Prefrontal Cortex. Neuropsychopharmacol Rev 2016; 41: 3–23.
  13. 13. Molet J, Maras PM, Kinney-Lang E, et al. MRI uncovers disrupted hippocampal microstructure that underlies memory impairments after early-life adversity. Hippocampus 2016; 26: 1618–1632. pmid:27657911
  14. 14. Chen Y, Baram TZ. Toward understanding how early-life stress reprograms cognitive and emotional brain networks. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016; 41: 197–206. pmid:26105143
  15. 15. Van Bodegom M, Homberg JR, Henckens MJAG. Modulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis by early life stress exposure. Front Cell Neurosci 2017; 11: 87. pmid:28469557
  16. 16. Bonapersona V, Kentrop J, Van Lissa CJ, et al. The behavioral phenotype of early life adversity: A 3-level meta-analysis of rodent studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2019; 102: 299–307. pmid:31047892
  17. 17. Sandi C, Haller J. Stress and the social brain: behavioural effects and neurobiological mechanisms. Nat Rev Neurosci 2015 165 2015; 16: 290–304. pmid:25891510
  18. 18. Molet J, Maras PM, Avishai-Eliner S, et al. Naturalistic rodent models of chronic early-life stress. Dev Psychobiol 2014; 56: 1675–88. pmid:24910169
  19. 19. Rocha M, Wang D, Avila-Quintero V, et al. Deficits in hippocampal-dependent memory across different rodent models of early life stress: systematic review and meta-analysis. Transl Psychiatry 2021 111 2021; 11: 1–12. pmid:33879774
  20. 20. Regulation Okuno H. and function of immediate-early genes in the brain: Beyond neuronal activity markers. Neurosci Res 2011; 69: 175–186. pmid:21163309
  21. 21. Kovács KJ. c-Fos as a transcription factor: A stressful (re)view from a functional map. Neurochem Int 1998; 33: 287–297. pmid:9840219
  22. 22. Yap EL, Pettit NL, Davis CP, et al. Bidirectional perisomatic inhibitory plasticity of a Fos neuronal network. Nature 2021; 590: 115–121. pmid:33299180
  23. 23. Fleischmann A, Hvalby O, Jensen V, et al. Impaired long-term memory and NR2A-type NMDA receptor-dependent synaptic plasticity in mice lacking c-fos in the CNS. J Neurosci 2003; 23: 9116–9122. pmid:14534245
  24. 24. Plath N, Ohana O, Dammermann B, et al. Arc/Arg3.1 Is Essential for the Consolidation of Synaptic Plasticity and Memories. Neuron 2006; 52: 437–444. pmid:17088210
  25. 25. Jones MW, Errington ML, French PJ, et al. A requirement for the immediate early gene Zif268 in the expression of late LTP and long-term memories. Nat Neurosci 2001; 4: 289–296. pmid:11224546
  26. 26. Gallo FT, Katche C, Morici JF, et al. Immediate Early Genes, Memory and Psychiatric Disorders: Focus on c-Fos, Egr1 and Arc. Front Behav Neurosci 2018; 12: 79. pmid:29755331
  27. 27. Daskalakis NP, Bagot RC, Parker KJ, et al. The three-hit concept of vulnerability and resilience: Toward understanding adaptation to early-life adversity outcome. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013; 38: 1858–1873. pmid:23838101
  28. 28. de Vries RBM, Hooijmans CR, Langendam MW, et al. A protocol format for the preparation, registration and publication of systematic reviews of animal intervention studies. Evidence-based Preclin Med 2015; 2: e00007.
  29. 29. Leenaars M, Hooijmans CR, van Veggel N, et al. A step-by-step guide to systematically identify all relevant animal studies. Lab Anim 2012; 46: 24–31. pmid:22037056
  30. 30. Hooijmans CR, Rovers MR, de Vries RBM, et al. SYRCLE’s risk of bias tool for animal studies. BMC Med Res Methodol; 14. Epub ahead of print 26 March 2014. pmid:24667063
  31. 31. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Int J Surg; 88. Epub ahead of print 1 April 2021. pmid:33789826
  32. 32. Ouzzani M, Hammady H, Fedorowicz Z, et al. Rayyan-a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Syst Rev 2016; 5: 210. pmid:27919275
  33. 33. Rohatgi A. WebPlotDigitizer, https://automeris.io/WebPlotDigitizer (2020).
  34. 34. Vesterinen HM, Sena ES, Egan KJ, et al. Meta-analysis of data from animal studies: A practical guide. J Neurosci Methods 2014; 221: 92–102. pmid:24099992
  35. 35. Chochran WG. The comparison of percentages in matched samples. Biometrika 1950; 37: 256–266. pmid:14801052
  36. 36. Viechtbauer W, Cheung MWL. Outlier and influence diagnostics for meta‐analysis. Res Synth Methods2 2010; 1: 112–125. pmid:26061377
  37. 37. Assink M, Wibbelink CJM. Fitting three-level meta-analytic models in R: A step-by-step tutorial. Quant Methods Psychol 2016; 12: 154–174.
  38. 38. Aarts E, Verhage M, Veenvliet J V., et al. A solution to dependency: Using multilevel analysis to accommodate nested data. Nat Neurosci 2014; 17: 491–496. pmid:24671065
  39. 39. Cheung MWL. Modeling dependent effect sizes with three-level meta-analyses: A structural equation modeling approach. Psychol Methods 2014; 19: 211–229. pmid:23834422
  40. 40. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistica, https://www.r-project.org (2019).
  41. 41. Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in {R} with the {metafor} package. J Stat Softw 2010; 36: 1–48.
  42. 42. Wickham H. tidyverse: Easily Install and Load the ‘Tidyverse’, https://cran.r-project.org/package=tidyverse (2017).
  43. 43. Holm S. A Simple Sequentially Rejective Multiple Test Procedure. Scand J Stat 1979; 6: 65–70.
  44. 44. Auth CS, Weidner MT, Popp S, et al. Differential anxiety-related behaviours and brain activation in Tph2-deficient female mice exposed to adverse early environment. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2018; 28: 1270–1283. pmid:30146458
  45. 45. Banqueri M, Méndez M, Arias JL. Why are maternally separated females inflexible? Brain activity pattern of COx and c-Fos. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2018; 155: 30–41. pmid:29908971
  46. 46. Benner S, Endo T, Endo N, et al. Early deprivation induces competitive subordinance in C57BL/6 male mice. Physiol Behav 2014; 137: 42–52. pmid:25089814
  47. 47. Chung EKY, Zhang XJ, Li Z, et al. Neonatal maternal separation enhances central sensitivity to noxious colorectal distention in rat. Brain Res 2007; 1153: 68–77. pmid:17434464
  48. 48. Clarke M, Cai G, Saleh S, et al. Being suckled in a large litter mitigates the effects of early-life stress on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function in the male rat. J Neuroendocrinol 2013; 25: 792–802. pmid:23763285
  49. 49. Cohen MM, Jing D, Yang RR, et al. Early-life stress has persistent effects on amygdala function and development in mice and humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2013; 110: 18274–18278. pmid:24145410
  50. 50. Daskalakis NP, Diamantopoulou A, Claessens SEF, et al. Early experience of a novel-environment in isolation primes a fearful phenotype characterized by persistent amygdala activation. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2014; 39: 39–57. pmid:24275003
  51. 51. Desbonnet L, Garrett L, Daly E, et al. Sexually dimorphic effects of maternal separation stress on corticotrophin-releasing factor and vasopressin systems in the adult rat brain. Int J Dev Neurosci 2008; 26: 259–268. pmid:18367364
  52. 52. Felice VD, Gibney SM, Gosselin RD, et al. Differential activation of the prefrontal cortex and amygdala following psychological stress and colorectal distension in the maternally separated rat. Neuroscience 2014; 267: 252–262. pmid:24513388
  53. 53. Gardner KL, Thrivikraman K V, Lightman SL, et al. Early life experience alters behavior during social defeat: Focus on serotonergic systems. Neuroscience 2005; 136: 181–191. pmid:16182451
  54. 54. Gaszner B, Jensen KO, Farkas J, et al. Effects of maternal separation on dynamics of urocortin 1 and brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the rat non-preganglionic Edinger-Westphal nucleus. Int J Dev Neurosci 2009; 27: 439–451. pmid:19460425
  55. 55. Genest SE, Gulemetova R, Laforest S, et al. Neonatal maternal separation and sex-specific plasticity of the hypoxic ventilatory response in awake rat. J Physiol 2004; 554: 543–557. pmid:14634199
  56. 56. Hidaka C, Kashio T, Uchigaki D, et al. Vulnerability or resilience of motopsin knockout mice to maternal separation stress depending on adulthood behaviors. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2018; 14: 2255–2268. pmid:30233183
  57. 57. James MH, Campbell EJ, Walker FR, et al. Exercise reverses the effects of early life stress on orexin cell reactivity in male but not female rats. Front Behav Neurosci 2014; 8: 244. pmid:25100956
  58. 58. Loi M, Sarabdjitsingh RA, Tsouli A, et al. Transient prepubertal mifepristone treatment normalizes deficits in contextual memory and neuronal activity of adult male rats exposed to maternal deprivation. eNeuro; 4. Epub ahead of print 2017. pmid:29098176
  59. 59. Menard JL, Champagne DL, Meaney MJP. Variations of maternal care differentially influence ‘fear’ reactivity and regional patterns of cFOS immunoreactivity in response to the shock-probe burying test. Neuroscience 2004; 129: 297–308. pmid:15501588
  60. 60. O’Leary OF, Felice D, Galimberti S, et al. GABAB(1) receptor subunit isoforms differentially regulate stress resilience. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2014; 111: 15232–15237. pmid:25288769
  61. 61. Ren TH, Wu J, Yew D, et al. Effects of neonatal maternal separation on neurochemical and sensory response to colonic distension in a rat model of irritable bowel syndrome. Am J Physiol—Gastrointest Liver Physiol 2007; 292: 849–856.
  62. 62. Renard GM, Rivarola MA, Suárez MM. Gender-dependent effects of early maternal separation and variable chronic stress on vasopressinergic activity and glucocorticoid receptor expression in adult rats. Dev Neurosci 2010; 32: 71–80. pmid:20389078
  63. 63. Rincel M, Lépinay AL, Delage P, et al. Maternal high-fat diet prevents developmental programming by early-life stress. Transl Psychiatry 2016; 6: e966–e966. pmid:27898075
  64. 64. Rivarola MA, Dalmasso C, Valdez DJ, et al. Early maternal separation and chronic variable stress as adults differentially affect fos expression in the anterodorsal thalami nuclei. Int J Neurosci 2008; 118: 735–748. pmid:18446587
  65. 65. Rivarola MA, Suárez MM. Early maternal separation and chronic variable stress in adulthood changes the neural activity and the expression of glucocorticoid receptor in limbic structures. Int J Dev Neurosci 2009; 27: 567–574. pmid:19563881
  66. 66. Shin S, Pribiag H, Lilascharoen V, et al. Drd3 Signaling in the Lateral Septum Mediates Early Life Stress-Induced Social Dysfunction. Neuron 2018; 97: 195–208. pmid:29276054
  67. 67. Tenorio-Lopes L, Henry MS, Marques D, et al. Neonatal maternal separation opposes the facilitatory effect of castration on the respiratory response to hypercapnia of the adult male rat: Evidence for the involvement of the medial amygdala. J Neuroendocrinol 2017; 29: e12550. pmid:29063642
  68. 68. Troakes C, Ingram CD. Anxiety behaviour of the male rat on the elevated plus maze: Associated regional increase in c-fos mRNA expression and modulation by early maternal separation. Stress 2009; 12: 362–369. pmid:19051121
  69. 69. Trujillo V, Durando PE, Suárez MM. Maternal separation in early life modifies anxious behavior and Fos and glucocorticoid receptor expression in limbic neurons after chronic stress in rats: Effects of tianeptine. Stress 2016; 19: 91–103. pmid:26452320
  70. 70. van Hasselt FN, de Visser L, Tieskens JM, et al. Individual variations in maternal care early in life correlate with later life decision-making and c-Fos expression in prefrontal subregions of rats. PLoS One 2012; 7: e37820. pmid:22693577
  71. 71. Vivinetto AL, Suárez MM, Rivarola MA. Neurobiological effects of neonatal maternal separation and post-weaning environmental enrichment. Behav Brain Res 2013; 240: 110–118. pmid:23195113
  72. 72. Yajima H, Haijima A, Khairinisa MA, et al. Early-life stress induces cognitive disorder in middle-aged mice. Neurobiol Aging 2018; 64: 139–146. pmid:29458841
  73. 73. Zhang X-J, Li Z, Chung EKY, et al. Activation of Extracellular Signal-Regulated Protein Kinase is Associated with Colorectal Distension-Induced Spinal and Supraspinal Neuronal Response and Neonatal Maternal Separation-Induced Visceral Hyperalgesia in Rats. J Mol Neurosci 2009; 37: 274–287. pmid:18649147
  74. 74. Zhao XP, Yu L, Chen ZY, et al. Effect of early-life stress on chronic functional visceral pain and CRH R1 expression in the paraventricular nucleus in adult rats. World Chinese J Dig 2013; 21: 3344–3355.
  75. 75. Grudniewicz A, Moher D, Cobey KD, et al. Predatory journals: no definition, no defence. Nature 2019; 576: 210–212. pmid:31827288
  76. 76. Shansky RM, Murphy AZ. Considering sex as a biological variable will require a global shift in science culture. Nat Neurosci 2021; 24: 457–464. pmid:33649507
  77. 77. Bramham CR, Worley PF, Moore MJ, et al. The Immediate Early Gene Arc/Arg3.1: Regulation, Mechanisms, and Function. J Neurosci 2008; 28: 11760–11767. pmid:19005037
  78. 78. Benekareddy M, Goodfellow NM, Lambe EK, et al. Enhanced function of prefrontal serotonin 5-HT2 receptors in a rat model of psychiatric vulnerability. J Neurosci 2010; 30: 12138–12150. pmid:20826676
  79. 79. McGregor NW, Dimatelis JJ, Van Zyl PJ, et al. A translational approach to the genetics of anxiety disorders. Behav Brain Res 2018; 341: 91–97. pmid:29288745
  80. 80. Rincel M, Aubert P, Chevalier J, et al. Multi-hit early life adversity affects gut microbiota, brain and behavior in a sex-dependent manner. Brain Behav Immun 2019; 80: 179–192. pmid:30872090
  81. 81. Solas M, Aisa B, Mugueta MC, et al. Interactions between age, stress and insulin on cognition: Implications for alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010; 35: 1664–1673. pmid:20182419
  82. 82. Beckmann AM, Wilce PA. Egr transcription factors in the nervous system. Neurochem Int 1997; 31: 477–510. pmid:9307998
  83. 83. Navailles S, Zimnisky R, Schmauss C. Expression of glucocorticoid receptor and early growth response gene 1 during postnatal development of two inbred strains of mice exposed to early life stress. Dev Neurosci 2010; 32: 139–148. pmid:20453467
  84. 84. Nestler EJ. δfosB: A transcriptional regulator of stress and antidepressant responses. Eur J Pharmacol 2015; 753: 66–72. pmid:25446562
  85. 85. Kim JY, Lee JH, Kim D, et al. Beneficial effects of highly palatable food on the behavioral and neural adversities induced by early life stress experience in female rats. Int J Biol Sci 2015; 11: 1150–1159. pmid:26327809
  86. 86. Lippmann M, Bress A, Nemeroff CB, et al. Long-term behavioural and molecular alterations associated with maternal separation in rats. Eur J Neurosci 2007; 25: 3091–3098. pmid:17561822
  87. 87. Wang H, Tao X, Huang ST, et al. Chronic stress is associated with pain precipitation and elevation in DeltaFosB expression. Front Pharmacol 2016; 7: 138. pmid:27303299
  88. 88. Bonapersona V, Hoijtink H, Abbinck M, et al. Increasing the statistical power of animal experiments with historical control data. Nat Neurosci 2021; 24: 470–477. pmid:33603229
  89. 89. Marrone DF, Schaner MJ, McNaughton BL, et al. Immediate-early gene expression at rest recapitulates recent experience. J Neurosci 2008; 28: 1030–1033. pmid:18234881
  90. 90. De Kloet ER, Joëls M, Holsboer F. Stress and the brain: From adaptation to disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2005; 6: 463–475. pmid:15891777
  91. 91. Marrocco J, Gray JD, Kogan JF, et al. Early life stress restricts translational reactivity in CA3 neurons associated with altered stress responses in adulthood. Front Behav Neurosci 2019; 13: 157. pmid:31354448
  92. 92. Blaise JH, Koranda JL, Chow U, et al. Neonatal isolation stress alters bidirectional long-term synaptic plasticity in amygdalo-hippocampal synapses in freely behaving adult rats. Brain Res 2008; 1193: 25–33. pmid:18178177
  93. 93. Guzowski JF, Setlow B, Wagner EK, et al. Experience-dependent gene expression in the rat hippocampus after spatial learning: A comparison of the immediate-early genes Arc, c-fos, and zif268. J Neurosci 2001; 21: 5089–5098. pmid:11438584
  94. 94. Vialou V, Bagot RC, Cahill ME, et al. Prefrontal Cortical Circuit for Depression- and Anxiety-Related Behaviors Mediated by Cholecystokinin: Role of ΔFosB. J Neurosci 2014; 34: 3878 LP– 3887. pmid:24623766
  95. 95. Vialou V, Robison AJ, LaPlant QC, et al. ΔFosB in brain reward circuits mediates resilience to stress and antidepressant responses. Nat Neurosci 2010; 13: 745–752. pmid:20473292
  96. 96. Li Y, Pehrson AL, Waller JA, et al. A critical evaluation of the activity-regulated cytoskeleton-associated protein (Arc/Arg3.1) ‘s putative role in regulating dendritic plasticity, cognitive processes, and mood in animal models of depression. Front Neurosci 2015; 9: 279. pmid:26321903
  97. 97. Marrocco J, Petty GH, Ríos MB, et al. A sexually dimorphic pre-stressed translational signature in CA3 pyramidal neurons of BDNF Val66Met mice. Nat Commun 2017; 8: 808. pmid:28993643