Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Khat and neurobehavioral functions: A systematic review

  • Ayan Ahmed ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ayan.ahmed@surrey.ac.uk

    Affiliation Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, United Kingdom

  • Manuel J. Ruiz,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of Extremadura, Badajoz, Badajoz, Spain

  • Kathrin Cohen Kadosh,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, United Kingdom

  • Robert Patton,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, United Kingdom

  • Davinia M. Resurrección

    Roles Data curation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Universidad Loyola Andalucía, Dos Hermanas, Sevilla, Spain

Abstract

Background

Khat is a plant that is used for its amphetamine-like stimulant properties. However, although khat is very popular in Eastern Africa, Arabian Peninsula, and the Middle East, there is still a lack of studies researching the possible neurobehavioral impairment derived from khat use.

Methods

A systematic review was conducted to identify studies that assessed the effects of khat use on neurobehavioral functions. MedLine, Scopus, Cochrane, Web of Science and Open Grey literature were searched for relevant publications from inception to December 2020. Search terms included (a) khat and (b) several cognitive domains. References from relevant publications and grey literature were also reviewed to identify additional citations for inclusion.

Results

A total of 142 articles were reviewed, 14 of which met the inclusion criteria (nine human and five rodent studies). Available human studies suggest that long term khat use is associated with significant deficits in several cognitive domains, including learning, motor speed/coordination, set-shifting/response inhibition functions, cognitive flexibility, short term/working memory, and conflict resolution. In addition, rodent studies indicated daily administration of khat extract resulted in dose-related impairments in behavior such as motor hyperactivity and decreased cognition, mainly learning and memory.

Conclusions

The findings presented in this review indicates that long-term khat use may be contributing to an impairment of neurobehavioral functions. However, gaps in literature were detected that future studies could potentially address to better understand the health consequences of khat use.

Introduction

Khat refers to the shoots and leaves of the plant Catha Edulis Forsk, which is endemic in the countries around the Red Sea and Eastern Africa, and that has been used for centuries for its stimulant properties [1,2]. Young shoots and leaves are used to alleviate fatigue, enhance work capacity, stay alert, reduce hunger, and induce euphoria and self-esteem [3,4]. To date, no study has effectively calculated the global prevalence of khat use [5]; it is estimated that approximately 5–20 million people worldwide consume khat [6,7]. Although little is known about the neuroendocrine, neurophysiological, and neurochemical effects of khat use in humans [8], daily khat use has been associated with multiple social, medical and mental health problems, including psychosis, depression and self-harm [2,911].

Cathine and cathinone are the main active constituents of khat and, in terms of structure and effects, are comparable to amphetamines [12,13]. Cathinone accounts for most stimulant effects on the Central Nervous System (CNS), increasing concentrations of stimulant neurotransmitters such as dopamine, serotonin and/or noradrenaline in specific brain regions, in the striatum [1416]. As a result, the structural and pharmacological parallels between khat cathinone and amphetamines offer a model for understanding the long-term effects of khat use, which could be comparable to cognitive deficits such as learning and cognitive flexibility associated with prolonged amphetamine use [17, see 18 for a review]. While empirical research addressing the effects of chronic khat administration are still scarce, there is growing scientific evidence in human and animal studies that demonstrate cognitive deficits. Several studies have shown that khat is an addictive neurotoxic substance with an effect neurobehavioral functions [12,13,19,20]. In a rodent study, both subchronic and chronic exposure to khat extract was found to cause deficits in short-term memory [21]. Similarly, dose-dependent neurobehavioral effects were reported in another study, whereby repeated exposure to doses of khat extract (100-400mg/kg) was observed to enhance locomotor activity and impair cognitive performance [22].

Despite being a developing issue of global health significance, comprehensive reviews and research examining the impact of khat use on executive function are still lacking. To date, there have only been three reviews published on the neurobehavioral consequences of khat [17,23,24]. Prior to the first paper by Hoffman and al’Absi [17], there were no published studies on the effect of khat use on cognition; therefore, this narrative paper provided a brief literature review and directions for future neurobehavioral studies. This review primarily included broader evidence on cognitive deficit from previous studies on stimulant drugs such as amphetamines [25] and methamphetamine [26,27] to understand the potential behavioral and cognitive effects of khat in humans. In another review, Berihu and Asfeha [24] assessed the level of evidence for the impact of khat on neurobehavioral functions. Results from this meta-analysis revealed a significant association between daily khat use and cognitive flexibility, working memory, learning memory, and motor activities. Finally, the most recent systematic reviews and meta-analysis were explicitly focused on the relationship between khat and memory dysfunctions [23]. The pooled results from this review suggested that chronic khat use was associated with a short-term memory discrepancy. However, these earlier meta-analysis reviews [23,24] did not capture all publications on the subject under review in this current paper; specifically, key executive control components: inhibition, updating and shifting [28,29] that have previously been linked to drug addiction [30] In light of current debates concerning the increase of khat use in East Africa [31] and its contributing factor in cognitive deficits development, a new and updated review was warranted.

This systematic review aimed to clarify the relationship between khat, behavioral and cognitive dysfunction by synthesizing studies investigating the effects of khat use on both human and rodents’ neurobehavioral functions. Therefore, this review’s findings could help to better understand (1) the current pieces of evidence on cognitive and behavioral consequences and (2) to identify potential areas to investigate in future khat research.

Method

PRISMA guidelines for reporting systematic reviews were followed (S1 PRISMA Checklist) [32] and the protocol was registered in PROSPERO on January 17, 2020 (registration No.: CRD 42020159580). Comprehensive literature searches of PubMed Medline, Scopus, Cochrane Database, Web of Science, and Open Grey Repository databases were conducted, from inception to January 19, 2020, and last updated on December 2020. Databases were searched separately by two reviewers (DMR and MJR). The search strategy (see S2 Table) incorporated combinations of two different concepts: (a) khat; and (b) cognitive domains. Searches were piloted in PubMed and then adapted to run across the other databases. To identify any additional articles, the reference lists of the included studies and recent reviews in the field were checked. In addition, expert authors in the field were contacted.

Eligibility criteria

The rationale for our inclusion criteria was to have an extensive assessment of the relationship between khat use and different cognitive domains. According to previous reviews on khat and models of stimulant drugs administration, neurobehavioral functions included were: inhibition, mental flexibility, working memory, response conflict, problem-solving, memory, visual and verbal abilities, learning, speed of processing, and social cognition [17,3338].

Based on previous studies investigating khat, we focused on adults (between 18 to 60 years) because they are the majority of khat users [39], as well as rodents, as it is a well-accepted animal model to test neurobehavioral functions [40]. We focused on studies that assessed executive functions in habitual or chronic khat users, as well as khat-administered rodents by means of cognitive or neuropsychological tasks/batteries in case-control studies, including at least a khat-free control group to avoid cofounding factors of studies using non-khat free control groups or other non-controlled settings (e.g. community or educational) where extraneous variables are not controlled.

Selection of studies

Study selection was done in duplicate (AA and MJR), and a third reviewer participated in cases of disagreement (RP). First, duplicate studies were deleted. Second, based on the screening of the title and abstract a selection of potentially relevant articles was made. Finally, after reading the full text, a final selection was made. The Kappa inter-agreement statistic was moderate (κ: 0.606; 95% CI: 0.297–0.915). The studies included met specific inclusion and exclusion criteria (see Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Inclusion and exclusion criteria for the studies included in the review.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252900.t001

Data extraction

A data extraction sheet was developed, pilot tested and refined it accordingly. The main characteristics of these studies were rigorously extracted by AA and verified by a second reviewer (MJR). For each study, information was collected about the authors, year of publication, study country, sample size, age, sex, intervention and comparators, cognitive domain and tasks or batteries, and main results.

Risk of bias in individual studies

Quality assessment was performed independently in duplicate (DMR and MJR), and a third reviewer participated in cases of disagreement (AA). The quality of animal studies was assessed with the SYRCLE tool [41]. The quality of control case studies was assessed with the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) [42]. The SYRCLE tool assesses six categories within ten domains, assigning a judgment of low, high or unclear risk of bias to each domain. The NOS awards stars for three categories: selection, comparability, and exposure. The maximum number of stars that can be achieved in a study with the NOS is nine, which indicates a complete absence of bias.

Results

Search results

The search strategy produced 142 potentially relevant studies (see Fig 1 PRISMA flow diagram). Further 12 articles were identified from the references of the articles selected. Of these, 53 were duplicates. Of those remaining, 84 were excluded after reviewing the title and abstract. After reviewing the full text of the remaining articles, three was excluded for the following main reasons: (1) did not test a cognitive domain or (2) no appropriate control group. Finally, 14 articles were selected, nine case-control human studies and five rodent studies (including four mice and one rat models) (Tables 2 and 3).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Flow chart of articles included and excluded after the systematic review.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252900.g001

thumbnail
Table 2. Description of the human studies included in the present systematic review.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252900.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Description of the animal studies included in the present systematic review.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252900.t003

Study quality

The results of the quality assessment of the included studies are presented in S3 and S4 Tables.

The total mean NOS was 5.7 (SD = 1.5; range 4–8). Of the nine studies, four provided a case definition adequate, and only one study has representativeness of the cases. The analysis of comparability revealed that five studies controlled for age and other substance consumption. Only in one study, the outcome was measured through a blinded assistant. Finally, all the studies employed the same method of ascertainment for cases and control groups.

The SYRCLE tool was employed in five articles. All of them reported baseline characteristics, other sources of bias and incomplete outcome data. However, none of the studies provides sequence generation or allocation concealment. Only one study reported a blind performance and the random housing of the animals included in the study.

Human studies

Overall data from 774 subjects (481 users and/or concurrent users; 293 non-users (controls); 31% female) was included to review evidence for khat’s effect on human cognition (see Table 2). The included articles reported that khat use was associated with cognitive impairments in different domains, including attention, cognitive flexibility, conflict resolution, decision-making, information processing speed, inhibitory control, learning, motor speed/coordination, short-term memory/working memory, and visual memory [4351].

Executive function.

Regarding executive function, three studies examined changes specifically in response to conflict and inhibitory control associated with acute and chronic khat use [4446]. Colzato et al. [45] examined whether there was a performance difference between long-term khat users and khat free control on the Simon effect test. The Simon effect test is a behavioral measure of interference/conflict resolution in the face of congruent/incongruent stimulus-response trials. The study revealed that chronic khat users were significantly slower than khat-free controls (48 vs. 31ms, p < .05) in responding to incongruent stimulus-response trials, suggesting long-term khat use impairs cognitive control and the ability to resolve response conflict. In a later study, Colzato et al. [46], assessing acute khat use, they found that the khat group showed a significantly reduced Simon effect in the second task block, performing better than the controls (38 vs 59ms, p < .05). In sum, the authors found that chronic khat use was associated with negative effects on interference control. In contrast, acute consumption was related to an enhancement in the ability to inhibit behavioral responses. Moreover, individuals who use khat reported to display deficits in inhibiting and executing responses [44]. Using the stop-signal paradigm, authors found khat users to have significantly longer stop-signal reaction time (236ms) compared with khat-free controls (192ms) (p < .001).

Motor/Information processing speed and set-shifting (cognitive flexibility).

Khat chronic users evidence difficulties related to motor speed and information processing, as a result, perform poorly on tasks that combine such skills with selecting relevant responses [45], the retrieval of information in short-term/working memory [43] and motor inhibition [44]. Ismail and colleagues [49] examined the effects of khat use on the finger-tapping test’s performance. They found that people who use khat displayed impairment in motor speed and coordination function. On the non-dominant hand, khat using individuals had a fewer number of taps than the control subjects. In the same group of participants, Ismail et al. [48] administered Trail Making B test (TMT-B) and found that chronic khat users exhibited significantly slower completion times on the TMT-B test compared to controls (192.4 vs 169.4ms, p < .05). In line with this, Colzato, Ruiz, van den Wildenberg, and Hommel [43] reported evidence that khat users showed deficits in a task-switching paradigm that specifically examined set-shifting between mental sets and task (known as cognitive flexibility). Khat users were found to have a significant reaction time difference between repetition and alternate trials than healthy controls. These results indicated khat users took longer (greater switching cost) (87 vs 37ms, p < .05) to redirect attention to respond to different tasks compared to khat-free controls.

Working memory.

Our literature search identified three studies that examined the effects of khat use on working memory [43,47,51]. Employing the N-back task, Colzato, Ruiz, van den Wildenberg, and Hommel [43] found that khat users showed deficits in working memory associated with updating information when comparing to khat free control group. This impairment was reflected in error rates because khat users were found to commit significantly more errors in both 1-back and 2-back conditions. However, there was no significant difference in reaction times between the groups. Another study found that the effects of concurrent use of khat and tobacco influences working memory and attention [51]. Concurrent users demonstrated poorer performance on working memory, with lower correct responses on a mental arithmetic test compared with khat-only users and healthy controls. In addition, these concurrent users were found to make fewer attempts on the task to generate correct responses than khat only users and displayed a significantly lower accuracy rate than controls.

Three studies have investigated the impact of khat on verbal learning and memory function [4749]. Concerning learning, Ismail et al. [49] found that individuals that chewed khat demonstrated deficits associated with learning. Compared to non-users, users were found to have significantly lower scores on the Serial Digit Learning test. In an earlier study targeting learning and memory, Hoffman and al’Absi [48] compared the performance of concurrent khat and tobacco users, khat only users and controls on the Arabic version of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT). Authors found that concurrent users demonstrated significantly greater difficulties in recalling words on trials 2–5 and on delayed recall measures of previously learned words. However, the authors did not find differences between chronic users and controls across all the verbal learning and memory recall measures. Thus, the results suggested that concurrent users had impairments in verbal learning and in the ability to retrieve information (after a short delay) from short-term memory. Consistent with this, Hoffman and al’Absi [47] found that chronic khat users compared to controls performed significantly worse in the backward Digit Span Test, in which a sequence of numbers had to be repeated in the reverse order.

Visual memory and decision-making process.

Finally, only one study assessed visual memory and decision making in khat users Khattab and Amer [50] reported deficits in perceptual visual memory and decision-making process in a population of regular and social khat users. Authors found a significant difference in all four memory subtests of the Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests [58], with both khat groups regular and social khat users scoring lower than controls on all the subtests. Additionally, they found differences among khat users, with regular users scoring lower than social users on all the tests.

Effect of frequency, duration of khat use on cognitive function.

Colzato et al. [46] found a significant positive correlation between the hours spent chewing khat and the Simon effect. Similarly, duration and frequency of khat use along with tobacco consumption and nicotine dependence were found to be important modulators of cognitive performance [50,51]. In addition, it has been suggested that consumption over a longer period could potentially be long-lasting and deleterious to cognitive functions [43,44,51].

Rodent studies

Overall data from 163 rodents were included to review evidence for the effect of khat on behavior (see Table 3).

Effect of khat use on motor behavior.

One study explored the effect of daily khat administration on locomotor activity in mice [55]. Authors found that mice treated with khat extract showed significantly enhanced exploration activity compared with the controls, which suggests acute khat exposure alters motor activity in rodents.

Learning and memory.

All five studies included in the present systematic review have examined the effects of acute khat administration on learning and memory [5357]. Kimani and Nyongesa [56] found that mice treated with moderate and high khat extract took significantly longer to locate the escape platform than mice treated with low khat extract and controls. In addition, a high dose of khat extract (360mg/kg bw) was found to improve memory performance function significantly, while moderate and low doses impaired accuracy for spatial memory of the platform location.

However, in another study [54] the authors observed increments in latencies and errors (on days 2 and 14) in mice treated with high khat dosage (200 mg/kg bw, daily), which suggests pronounced impairments in both learning and memory. In a second experiment in the same study, all treatment groups had a significantly higher frequency of repeated arms entries compared with the control group in the Y-maze test. Similarly, Geresu et al. [55] found a pronounced spatial working memory deficit in mice treated with higher khat doses (300 mg/kg bw, daily). However, both studies reported that khat treated mice had a lower percentage of alterations in the pattern of arm visits compared to controls, thus exhibiting poorer reward-seeking behavior [54,55].

In rats, Alfadly et al. [53] found that khat increased the speed on entering the arms of the Radial Arm Maze. Specifically, khat fed rat groups were significantly slower in completing the experiment compared to both the control and MPD (methylphenidate) groups. With respect to working memory, authors reported khat fed rats (500 mg/kg bw) were more likely to commit working memory correct errors by repeated entries to a baited arm that no longer had food; and working memory incorrect errors, as evidenced by the animal’s tendency to re-enter the same unbaited arm when compared with MPD (3 mg/kg bw, daily) and control [53].

Kimani and Nyongesa [56] reported that administration of khat has differential dose effects on learning and memory performance in the Morris Water Maze test. This study found that khat administration reduced the swim speed of mice treated with moderate and high doses during the baseline (acquisition) phase compared with controls. After the removal of the escape platform from the maze (day 5), khat exposure was found to not interfere with long-term (reference) memory. However, performance during the reversal learning phase, showed dose-dependent impairments in learning, as indicated by escape latency and swim distance. For instance, mice treated with low (40 mg/kg bw, daily) khat extract displayed longer escape latency (during the first 2 days) and significantly longer swim distance (in day 2) compared to baseline performances. In contrast, high dose (360 mg/kg bw, daily) of khat significantly increased escape latency across the reversal learning trials than baseline performance and control animals. During the post reversal learning probe trial (day 9), mice treated with low and high doses of khat displayed a stronger bias for the former target quadrant compared to the new target, whereas mice treated with moderate khat dose were successful in switching their behavior to learn the new location.

Discussion

The objective of this systematic review was to provide an updated review of the effects of khat on neurobehavioral performance in both humans and animals. While this review’s conclusions and that of Berihu et al. [23] on the effects of khat use on learning and memory are similar, this paper has identified additional studies not included in the previous reviews. In line with other psychostimulants (cocaine and amphetamine) drugs studies [59,60] acute administration of khat has been observed in one study [46] to improve conflict resolution performance in humans (e.g. resolving stimulus-induced response conflict) as a result of the potential natural effect of khat use [13]. Furthermore, long-term khat use was reported to induce significant deficits in several cognitive domains: learning, motor speed/coordination, short-term/working memory, conflict resolution, decision-making, and visual memory [43,44,4751]. Specifically, this review contains publications on key executive control domains associated with long-term psychostimulant drugs such as set-shifting/response inhibition functions and cognitive flexibility [4351]. These findings support previous studies which have demonstrated long-term use of psychostimulant drugs such as cocaine, methamphetamine and amphetamine are associated with detrimental effects on executive functions [27,59,6163]. Chronic exposure to amphetamine and methamphetamine has been broadly implicated in producing alterations at the neuromodulatory and cortical level [6265]. Given the chemical resemblance between cathinone and amphetamine, khat is classified as a psychostimulant that could potentially disrupt the dopaminergic pathways within the frontostriatal and limbic regions of the brain, which are responsible for executive (higher-order) functions implicated in the control of addiction [63,66].

Additionally, khat’s neurobiological and behavioral consequences are further complicated with the interactions of other substances such as alcohol and smoking cigarettes (nicotine) [67]. Several studies have reported that many khat users also smoked cigarettes [20,51,68] and that these concurrent users display impairments in verbal learning, memory recall and working memory [48,51]. One explanation is that cathinone has been found to act as a presynaptic release and uptake inhibitor of dopamine [69] leading to depletion of serotonin [70] in brain areas involved in spatial learning and memory [71]. Previous literature suggests an association between nicotine and cognitive deficits in learning and memory [72]. Despite this, it remains unclear the extent to which the observed deficits preceded substance use arises because of concurrent use or might reflect the effect of either khat or nicotine alone. Moreover, it has been suggested that factors such as time spent chewing khat, duration of use, amount (dosage), frequency of use are related to the general slowing degree of impairment in cognition [45,47,48,50,51].

In animals, daily administration of khat extract resulted in dose-related impairments in behavior such as motor hyperactivity and decreased spatial learning, memory as reward-seeking behavior [53,55]. It has been reported that changes such as the escalated behavioral and motor-stimulant responses associated with the repeated daily administration of khat in rodents are typical of behavioral sensitization [53]. Repeated exposure to khat sensitizes stimulant effects and leads to pairing khat to environmental cues that elicit conditioned activity.

Findings from animal studies suggest that khat dosage is the primary determinant of the neurobehavioral effects observed in rodents [53]. Converging evidence from methamphetamine and amphetamine studies reported repeated dosing enhanced behavioral abnormalities responses in a dose-dependent manner [73,74]. Similarly, khat was found to selectively enhance spatial learning and impair working memory [53,56,57]. These differential patterns of learning and memory deficits suggest a loss of cognitive flexibility related to differences in dose and time [57]. More recent findings reported khat extract administered on acute and subacute induced short-term memory discrepancy but had no effect on long-term memory across the treatment regimens [21,75].

Strengths and limitations

A major strength of this systematic review is that the synthesis of the available literature provided insight into cognitive deficits that were not examined in previous reviews [17,23,24]. However, the studies identified in this review have several limitations that should be addressed in future research. One of the drawbacks of this review is that it is focused on a comprehensive literature search that provides an outline of current findings. Unlike a meta-analysis, this review does not provide statistical estimates of absolute effects nor report causation and effect but rather indicates possible associations between khat use and cognitive impairments.

Another limitation is that most of the studies in this paper have limited generalizability as they are based on relatively small sample sizes and have mainly studied male subjects. In addition, some of these previous studies have failed to account for the role of current or past use of other substances (e.g. nicotine, polysubstance abuse) on cognition. It is not unusual to recruit khat users who use other substance such as alcohol, nicotine, cannabis, and benzodiazepines [7679]. Additionally, the variability in the included studies’ experimental design and the lack of screening concerning the duration between khat consumption and cognitive testing make it difficult to ascertain more robust conclusions. For instance, only a subset of eligibility criteria appears to have been applied in human studies. The extent to which individuals with premorbid diagnosis such as Attention Deficit Hyperactive Disorder (ADHD) and/or polysubstance users were effectively prescreened and excluded remains uncertain. Thus, making it difficult to conclude if the reported dysfunctions in cognitive performance are exclusively due to khat exposure. For this reason, the observed neuropsychological impairment reported in khat users could potentially be a result of premorbid diagnosis or multiple substances consumption.

Moreover, most rodent studies were not blinded and increased the risk of performance and detection biases. Such biases could result in responding to the treatment group differently, potentially influencing the interpretation and accuracy of the result. Fourth, most of the studies included were conducted in countries where khat is imported, thus limiting the interpretation of our results. It is well documented that khat’s potency and subsequently neurocognitive and behavioral consequences heavily depend on the type, quality, and freshness that drastically decreased after harvesting [80,81]. Therefore, evidence could evolve as more studies are conducted in geographic regions where higher potent khat is readily available. A key issue about using many neuropsychological measures has small or non-representative normative data for non-western populations. Normative “cut-offs” provide fundamental information on what is a normal range to highlight any deficits or disease. Test performance can be influenced by many factors such as culture; therefore, having normative data adjusted to cognitive performance improves the objectivity of the measures. In addition, language and cultural biases can lead to mislabeling differences in cognitive function between users and controls as ‘impairments’ or ‘deficit’, and so further research is needed to confirm these findings.

Although animal models provide crucial information on a drug’s addictive properties, animal studies remain scarce and challenging to replicate in humans because of psychokinetic differences and the varying route and dosage administration. Therefore, it is harder to establish consistency to determine the translational value of these findings to human subjects with different environmental and social factors that could also influence the degree of neurobehavioral impairment [82]. Finally, these selected studies were restricted to laboratory settings and so lack ecological validity, which means the study’s conclusion must be interpreted with care.

Future studies

To overcome the limitations mentioned above and improve on the quality of both human and animal studies, several recommendations for future research are proposed. First, specific examinations of multiple factors that influence khat use outcomes are needed. That is, studies that looks at genetic, neurobiological, behavioral, environmental, social, cultural, sex differences, psychological factors (mental health), their interactions, and mediating characteristics related to khat use and their relationship with neurobehavioral functions. Other areas of investigation for future research include studies that examine the relationship between varying patterns of khat use and sociodemographic characteristics (e.g. age, education) on cognition and whether abstinence influences cognitive recovery. Specifically, cognitive domains that have been previously identified in amphetamine and methamphetamine studies addressing current neuropsychological gaps such as social cognition, visuoconstruction, and episodic memory that have yet to be fully understood in khat users is recommended. A more specific pre-screening of research participants will be needed in future clinical and rodent preclinical khat studies to account for confounding variables such as sample size, polydrug/concurrent use, premorbid diagnosis (e.g. ADHD) and sex. To improve validity and reliability of any future studies by implementing random allocation and good blinding in RCT studies and quality checklists to reduce potential biases related to performance, selection and detection. Also, incorporating assertion measure such as biochemical tests to verify drug use status and more structured methods to assess the history of mood and psychiatric disorders. Future animal studies need to devise more ecologically relevant models assessing cognition in habitual and social users with varying exposure and doses of khat. Overall, the findings highlight the need to develop further neuropsychological measures for studying substance misuse in diverse populations. Although one neuroimaging study was identified in this review, based on the analysis, it was concluded that the electroencephalogram (EEG) might not have been sensitive enough to detect cases of khat use. Lastly, more empirical neuroscientific studies aimed at improving our understanding of the neural correlates of khat are needed to inform prevention strategies and identify potential risk markers to shape clinical interventions [83].

Conclusions

The current systematic review has updated previous reviews on the impact of khat use on neurobehavioral functions implicated on everyday tasks’ performance. The findings suggest that khat is associated with deficits in a wide range of cognitive domains, mainly working memory, learning, motor speed/coordination, and set-shifting/response inhibition functions. Prospective studies and randomized control trials are required to determine the underlying neural mechanisms and the interrelationships between khat use and neuropsychological performances. Also, longitudinal multifactor studies on behavioral, environmental, mental and physical health consequences are needed to improve understanding of the long-term consequences of khat use.

Supporting information

S2 Table. Search strategy piloted for PubMed.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252900.s002

(DOC)

S3 Table. Risk of bias in the studies reviewed assess with the NOS-scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252900.s003

(DOC)

S4 Table. Risk of bias in the studies reviewed assess with the SYRCLE’s tool.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252900.s004

(DOC)

References

  1. 1. Balint EE, Falkay G, Balint GA. Khat—a controversial plant. Wien Klin Wochenschr. 2009;121: 604–614. pmid:19921126
  2. 2. Cox G, Rampes H. Adverse effects of khat: a review. Adv psychiatr treat. 2003;9: 456–463.
  3. 3. Brenneisen R, Fisch H, Koelbing U, Geisshusler S, Kalix P. Amphetamine-like effects in humans of the khat alkaloid cathinone. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 1990;30: 825–828. pmid:2288828
  4. 4. Kalix P. Catha Edulis, a plant that has amphetamine effects. Pharm World Sci. 1996;18: 69–73. pmid:8739260
  5. 5. Mateen FJ, Cascino GD. Khat chewing: a smokeless gun? Mayo Clin Proc. 2010;85: 971–973. pmid:21037041
  6. 6. Numan N. The green leaf: khat. World J Medical Sci. 2012;7: 210–223.
  7. 7. Corkery JM, Schifano F, Oyefeso A, Ghodse AH, Tonia T, Naidoo V, et al. Overview of literature and information on “khat-related” mortality: a call for recognition of the issue and further research. Ann Ist Super Sanita. 2011;47: 445–464. pmid:22194080
  8. 8. Ruiz M, Colzato L. Khat, Mephedrone and MDPV: Pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic parameters. The SAGE Handbook of Drug & Alcohol Studies. London: SAGE Publications Ltd; 2016. pp. 249–268. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781473922143.n15
  9. 9. Al-Habori M. The potential adverse effects of habitual use of Catha Edulis (khat). Expert Opin Drug Saf. 2005;4: 1145–1154. pmid:16255671
  10. 10. Bhui K, Craig T, Mohamud S, Warfa N, Stansfeld SA, Thornicroft G, et al. Mental disorders among Somali refugees: Developing culturally appropriate measures and assessing socio-cultural risk factors. Soc Psychiat Epidemiol. 2006;41: 400–408. pmid:16520881
  11. 11. Omar YS, Jenkins A, Altena M van R, Tuck H, Hynan C, Tohow A, et al. Khat Use: What Is the Problem and What Can Be Done? Biomed Res Int. 2015;2015: 1–7. pmid:26064915
  12. 12. Patel NB. “Natural amphetamine” Khat: A cultural tradition or a drug of abuse? Int Rev Neurobiol. 2015;120: 235–255. pmid:26070760
  13. 13. Patel NB. Khat (Catha Edulis Forsk)–And now there are three. Brain Res Bull. 2019;145: 92–96. pmid:30059706
  14. 14. Kalix P, Braenden O. Pharmacological aspects of the chewing of khat leaves. Pharmacol Rev. 1985;37: 149–164. pmid:2864707
  15. 15. Nyongesa A, Oduma J, Al’Absi M, Chirwa S. Immunohistochemical localization of anterior pituitary cell types of vervet monkey (Chlorocebus aethiops) following sub-chronic cathinone exposure. J Ethnopharmacol. 2015;174: 168–177. pmid:26277490
  16. 16. Zelger JL, Carlini EA. Influence of cathinone ([alpha]-aminopropiophenone) and cathine (phenylpropanolamine) on circling behavior and on the uptake and release of [3H]dopamine in striatal slices of rats. Neuropharmacology. 1981;20: 839–843. pmid:7290356
  17. 17. Hoffman R, al’Absi M. Khat use and neurobehavioral functions: Suggestions for future studies. J Ethnopharmacol. 2010;132(3):554–563. pmid:20553832
  18. 18. Wood S, Sage JR, Shuman T, Anagnostaras SG. Psychostimulants and cognition: a continuum of behavioral and cognitive activation. Pharmacol Rev. 2014;66: 193–221. pmid:24344115
  19. 19. Gowing LR, Ali RL, Allsop S, Marsden J, Turf EE, West R, et al. Global statistics on addictive behaviours: 2014 status report: Addiction global statistics. Addiction. 2015;110: 904–919. pmid:25963869
  20. 20. Kassim S, Dalsania A, Nordgren J, Klein A, Hulbert J. Before the ban—an exploratory study of a local khat market in East London, U.K. Harm Reduct J. 2015;12. pmid:26066043
  21. 21. Mohammed F, Gerbi A, Teffera A, Seyoum G, Nedi T, Engidawork E. Subchronic crude khat (Catha edulis F.) extract administration produces short-term memory impairment in behavioral tasks without morphological toxicity to the dentate gyrus in mice. Ethiop Pharm J. 2014;30: 77–94.
  22. 22. Bogale T, Engidawork E, Yisma E. Subchronic oral administration of crude khat extract (Catha edulis forsk) induces schizophernic-like symptoms in mice. BMC Complement Altern Med. 2016;16: 153. pmid:27245332
  23. 23. Berihu BA, Asfeha GG, Welderufael AL, Debeb YG, Zelelow YB, Beyene HA. Toxic effect of khat (Catha edulis) on memory: Systematic review and meta-analysis. J Neurosci Rural Pract. 2017;8: 30–37. pmid:28149078
  24. 24. Berihu BA, Asfeha GG. Effect of khat (Catha edulis Forsk) on neurobehavioral functions: systematic review and meta analysis. Int J Pharm Sci Res. 2015;6: 1369–1377.
  25. 25. McKetin R, Leung J, Stockings E, Huo Y, Foulds J, Lappin JM, et al. Mental health outcomes associated with the use of amphetamines: A systematic review and meta-analysis. EClinicalMedicine. 2019;16: 81–97. pmid:31832623
  26. 26. Simon SL, Domier C, Carnell J, Brethen P, Rawson R, Ling W. Cognitive impairment in individuals currently using methamphetamine. Am J Addict. 2000;9: 222–231. pmid:11000918
  27. 27. Simon SL, Domier CP, Sim T, Richardson K, Rawson RA, Ling W. Cognitive performance of current methamphetamine and cocaine abusers. J Addict Dis. 2001;21: 61–74. pmid:11831501
  28. 28. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41: 49–100. pmid:10945922
  29. 29. Gould TJ. Addiction and cognition. Addict Sci Clin Pract. 2010;5: 4. pmid:22002448
  30. 30. Hester R, Lubman DI, Yücel M. The role of executive control in human drug addiction. In: Self DW, Staley Gottschalk JK, editors. Behavioral neuroscience of drug addiction. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer; 2010. pp. 301–318. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2009_28 pmid:21161758
  31. 31. Cochrane L O’Regan D. Legal harvest and illegal trade: Trends, challenges, and options in khat production in Ethiopia. Int J Drug Policy. 2016;30: 27–34. pmid:26949190
  32. 32. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Group TP. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6: e1000097. pmid:19621072
  33. 33. Dean AC, Groman SM, Morales AM, London ED. An evaluation of the evidence that methamphetamine abuse causes cognitive decline in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38: 259–274. pmid:22948978
  34. 34. Ersche KD, Clark L, London M, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Profile of executive and memory function associated with amphetamine and opiate dependence. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31: 1036–1047. pmid:16160707
  35. 35. Moratalla R, Khairnar A, Simola N, Granado N, García-Montes JR, Porceddu PF, et al. Amphetamine-related drugs neurotoxicity in humans and in experimental animals: Main mechanisms. Prog Neurobiol. 2017;155: 149–170. pmid:26455459
  36. 36. Panenka WJ, Procyshyn RM, Lecomte T, MacEwan GW, Flynn SW, Honer WG, et al. Methamphetamine use: a comprehensive review of molecular, preclinical and clinical findings. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;129: 167–179. pmid:23273775
  37. 37. Potvin S, Stavro K, Rizkallah É, Pelletier J. Cocaine and cognition: a systematic quantitative review. J Addict Med. 2014;8: 368–376. pmid:25187977
  38. 38. Rusyniak DE. Neurologic manifestations of chronic methamphetamine abuse. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2013;36: 261–275. pmid:23688691
  39. 39. Wondemagegn AT, Cheme MC, Kibret KT. Perceived psychological, economic, and social impact of khat chewing among adolescents and adults in Nekemte Town, East Welega Zone, West Ethiopia. Biomed Res Int. 2017;2017: 1–9. pmid:28265577
  40. 40. Ellenbroek B, Youn J. Rodent models in neuroscience research: is it a rat race? Dis Model Mech. 2016;9: 1079–1087. pmid:27736744
  41. 41. Hooijmans CR, Rovers MM, de Vries RB, Leenaars M, Ritskes-Hoitinga M, Langendam MW. SYRCLE’s risk of bias tool for animal studies. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2014;14: 43. pmid:24667063
  42. 42. Wells G, Shea B, O’conell D, Peterson J, Welch V, Losos M, et al. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomised studies in meta-analyses. 2011. Available: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp.
  43. 43. Colzato LS, Ruiz MJ, van den Wildenberg W, Hommel B. Khat Use Is Associated with Impaired Working Memory and Cognitive Flexibility. PLoS One. 2011;6: e20602. pmid:21698275
  44. 44. Colzato LS, Ruiz MJ, van den Wildenberg W, Bajo MT, Hommel B. Long-term effects of chronic khat use: impaired inhibitory control. Front Psychology. 2011;1: 219. pmid:21833274
  45. 45. Colzato L, Ruiz MJ, van den Wildenberg W, Hommel B. Khat use is associated with increased response conflict in humans. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2012;27: 315–321. pmid:22585591
  46. 46. Colzato LS, Sellaro R, Ruiz M, Sikora K, Hommel B. Acute khat use reduces response conflict in habitual users. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7. pmid:23801952
  47. 47. Hoffman R, al’Absi M. Working memory and speed of information processing in chronic khat users: preliminary findings. Eur Addict Res. 2013;19: 1–6. pmid:22948202
  48. 48. Hoffman R, al’Absi M. Concurrent use of khat and tobacco is associated with verbal learning and delayed recall deficits. Addiction. 2013;108: 1855–1862. pmid:23714286
  49. 49. Ismail AA, El Sanosy RM, Rohlman DS, El-Setouhy M. Neuropsychological functioning among chronic Khat users in Jazan Region, Saudi Arabia. Subst Abus. 2014;35: 235–244. pmid:24965057
  50. 50. Khattab NY, Amer G. Undetected neuropsychophysiological sequelae of khat chewing in standard aviation medical examination. Aviat Space Environ Med. 1995;66: 739–744. pmid:7487806
  51. 51. Nakajima M, Hoffman R, Al’Absi M. Poor working memory and reduced blood pressure levels in concurrent users of khat and tobacco. Nicotine Tob Res. 2014;16: 279–287. pmid:24078758
  52. 52. Salthouse TA. Aging and measures of processing speed. Biol Psychol. 2000;54: 35–54. pmid:11035219
  53. 53. Alfadly SO, Batarfi AM, Veetil PK. Catha edulis deteriorates spatial working memory in rats, but spares reference memory. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 2014;58: 239–249. pmid:25906607
  54. 54. Bedada W, Engidawork E. The neuropsychopharmacological effects of Catha edulis in mice offspring born to mothers exposed during pregnancy and lactation. Phytother Res. 2010;24: 268–276. pmid:19585482
  55. 55. Geresu B, Onaivi E, Engidawork E. Behavioral evidence for the interaction between cannabinoids and Catha edulis F. (Khat) in mice. Brain Res. 2016;1648: 333–338. pmid:27502029
  56. 56. Kimani ST, Nyongesa AW. Effects of single daily khat (Catha edulis) extract on spatial learning and memory in CBA mice. Behav Brain Res. 2008;195: 192–197. pmid:18588917
  57. 57. Kimani ST, Patel NB, Kioy PG. Memory deficits associated with khat (Catha edulis) use in rodents. Metab Brain Dis. 2016;31: 45–52. pmid:26423676
  58. 58. Ekstrom RB, French JW, Harman HH, Dermen D. Manual for Kit of Factor-referenced Cognitive Tests, 1976. Pricenton, New Jersey: Educational Testing Service; 1976.
  59. 59. Fillmore MT, Kelly TH, Martin CA. Effects of d-amphetamine in human models of information processing and inhibitory control. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;77: 151–159. pmid:15664716
  60. 60. Garavan H, Kaufman JN, Hester R. Acute effects of cocaine on the neurobiology of cognitive control. Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci. 2008;363: 3267–3276. pmid:18640911
  61. 61. Gonzalez R, Bechara A, Martin EM. Executive functions among individuals with methamphetamine or alcohol as drugs of choice: Preliminary observations. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007;29: 155–159. pmid:17365250
  62. 62. Rogers RD, Everitt BJ, Baldacchino A, Blackshaw AJ, Swainson R, Wynne K, et al. Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;20: 322–339. pmid:10088133
  63. 63. Salo R, Nordahl TE, Possin K, Leamon M, Gibson DR, Galloway GP, et al. Preliminary evidence of reduced cognitive inhibition in methamphetamine-dependent individuals. Psychiatry Research. 2002;111: 65–74. pmid:12140121
  64. 64. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ. Imaging studies on the role of dopamine in cocaine reinforcement and addiction in humans. J Psychopharmacol (Oxford). 1999;13: 337–345. pmid:10667609
  65. 65. Gonzalez R, Rippeth JD, Carey CL, Heaton RK, Moore DJ, Schweinsburg BC, et al. Neurocognitive performance of methamphetamine users discordant for history of marijuana exposure. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2004;76: 181–190. pmid:15488342
  66. 66. Scott JC, Woods SP, Matt GE, Meyer RA, Heaton RK, Atkinson JH, et al. Neurocognitive effects of methamphetamine: a critical review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev. 2007;17: 275–297. pmid:17694436
  67. 67. al’Absi MN, Grabowski J. Concurrent use of tobacco and khat: Added burden on chronic disease epidemic. Addiction. 2012;107: 451–452. pmid:22248142
  68. 68. Griffiths P. Qat use in London. A study of qat use among a sample of Somalis living in London. London: Home Office Drug Prevention Initiative; 1998.
  69. 69. Nencini P, Ahmed AM. Khat consumption: A pharmacological review. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1989;23: 19–29. pmid:2537717
  70. 70. Banjaw MY, Miczek K, Schmidt WJ. Repeated Catha edulis oral administration enhances the baseline aggressive behavior in isolated rats. J Neural Transm. 2006;113: 543–556. pmid:16082505
  71. 71. Volkow ND, Chang L, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Franceschi D, Sedler MJ, et al. Higher cortical and lower subcortical metabolism in detoxified methamphetamine abusers. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158: 383–389. pmid:11229978
  72. 72. Connor JD, Rostom A, Makonnen E. Comparison of effects of khat extract and amphetamine on motor behaviors in mice. J Ethnopharmacol. 2002;81: 65–71. pmid:12020929
  73. 73. Connor JD, Rostom A, Makonnen E. Comparison of effects of khat extract and amphetamine on motor behaviors in mice. J Ethnopharmacol. 2002;81: 65–71. pmid:12020929
  74. 74. Izawa J, Yamanashi K, Asakura T, Misu Y, Goshima Y. Differential effects of methamphetamine and cocaine on behavior and extracellular levels of dopamine and 3,4-dihydroxyphenylalanine in the nucleus accumbens of conscious rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 2006;549: 84–90. pmid:16979160
  75. 75. Mohammed F, Engidawork E, Nedi T. The effect of acute and subchronic administration of crude Khat extract (Catha Edulis F.) on weight in mice. American Scientific Research Journal for Engineering, Technology, and Sciences (ASRJETS). 2015;14: 132–141.
  76. 76. Selassie SG, Gebre A. Rapid assessment of drug abuse in Ethiopia. Bull Narc. 1996;48: 53–63. pmid:9839035
  77. 77. Zein ZA. Polydrug abuse among Ethiopian university students with particular reference to khat (Catha edulis). J Trop Med Hyg. 1988;91: 71–75. pmid:2898021
  78. 78. Omolo OE, Dhadphale M. Alcohol Use Among Khat (Catha) Chewers in Kenya. Br J Addict. 1987;82: 97–99. pmid:2881571
  79. 79. Odenwald M, Lingenfelder B, Schauer M, Neuner F, Rockstroh B, Hinkel H, et al. Screening for posttraumatic stress disorder among Somali ex-combatants: a validation study. Confl Health. 2007;1: 10. pmid:17822562
  80. 80. Nabben T, Korf DJ. Consequences of criminalisation: the Dutch khat market before and after the ban. Drugs. 2017;24: 332–339.
  81. 81. Wabe NT. Chemistry, Pharmacology, and Toxicology of Khat (Catha edulis Forsk): A Review. Addict Health. 2011;3: 137–149. pmid:24494129
  82. 82. Spanagel R. Animal models of addiction. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2017;19: 247–258. pmid:29302222
  83. 83. Yasaminshirazi M, Ahmadlou M. Neuroimaging Findings in Methamphetamine Abusers. Addict Res Ther. 2016.