Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Compounds from plantar foot sweat, nesting material, and urine show strain patterns associated with agonistic and affiliative behaviors in group housed male mice, Mus musculus

  • Amanda J. Barabas ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft

    abarabas@purdue.edu

    Affiliation Department of Animal Science, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, United States of America

  • Helena A. Soini,

    Roles Data curation, Methodology, Resources, Software, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Chemistry and Institute for Pheromone Research, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America

  • Milos V. Novotny,

    Roles Data curation, Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Chemistry and Institute for Pheromone Research, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America

  • David R. Williams,

    Roles Data curation, Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Chemistry and Institute for Pheromone Research, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America

  • Jacob A. Desmond,

    Roles Data curation, Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Chemistry and Institute for Pheromone Research, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America

  • Jeffrey R. Lucas,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Biological Science, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, United States of America

  • Marisa A. Erasmus,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Animal Science, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, United States of America

  • Heng-Wei Cheng,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation USDA-ARS, Livestock Behavior Research Unit, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, United States of America

  • Brianna N. Gaskill

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Animal Science, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, United States of America

Abstract

Excessive home cage aggression often results in severe injury and subsequent premature euthanasia of male laboratory mice. Aggression can be reduced by transferring used nesting material during cage cleaning, which is thought to contain aggression appeasing odors from the plantar sweat glands. However, neither the composition of plantar sweat nor the deposits on used nesting material have been evaluated. The aims of this study were to (1) identify and quantify volatile compounds deposited in the nest site and (2) determine if nest and sweat compounds correlate with social behavior. Home cage aggression and affiliative behavior were evaluated in 3 strains: SJL, C57BL/6N, and A/J. Individual social rank was assessed via the tube test, because ranking may influence compound levels. Sweat and urine from the dominant and subordinate mouse in each cage, plus cage level nest samples were analyzed for volatile compound content using gas chromatography-mass spectrometry. Behavior data and odors from the nest, sweat, and urine were statistically analyzed with separate principal component analyses (PCA). Significant components, from each sample analysis, and strain were run in mixed models to test if odors were associated with behavior. Aggressive and affiliative behaviors were primarily impacted by strain. However, compound PCs were also impacted by strain, showing that strain accounts for any relationship between odors and behavior. C57BL/6N cages displayed the most allo-grooming behavior and had high scores on sweat PC1. SJL cages displayed the most aggression, with high scores on urine PC2 and low scores on nest PC1. These data show that certain compounds in nesting material, urine, and sweat display strain specific patterns which match strain specific behavior patterns. These results provide preliminary information about the connection between home cage compounds and behavior. Salient compounds will be candidates for future controlled studies to determine their direct effect on mouse social behavior.

Introduction

Aggression among group housed male mice is one of the most common reasons for premature euthanasia and reduces preclinical research data validity and reproducibility [13]. Individual housing appears to be a simple solution, but it comes with its own welfare concerns [4]. Mice form complex social structures in the wild [5, 6], which is why group housing for laboratory mice is recommended [7]. Enrichment is commonly suggested to reduce home cage aggression, but results are often inconsistent [2]. Nonetheless, nesting material is one of the most reliable and recommended types of enrichment, particularly for reducing aggression after cage cleaning [2, 8]. Routine cage cleaning is a known trigger of escalated aggression in males [9] with time periods of social unrest peaking approximately 15 to 45 minutes afterward [10, 11]. However, this aggression is reduced when a portion of the existing nest is transferred to the new cage [12]. Accordingly, nest transfer has become a widely used practice, but there is no empirical evidence to explain how it decreases aggression.

Although there are minimal data, the prevalent theory explaining these effects focuses on scent cue preservation. The familiar odors within the nesting material may include pheromones, which are commonly produced as volatile organic compounds (VOCs) and play a prominent role in regulating mammalian social interactions [13]. While pheromones are the most recognized odor signal, odors must meet strict criteria to be considered a pheromone: physiologically relevant concentrations must produce reliable effects in a bioassay [14, 15]. Individualized scent profiles can also relay information, and mice rely heavily on both pheromones and individual scent cue mixtures for communication and conspecific recognition [1518]. The disruption of these scent cues can in turn lead to aggressive interactions [19].

While odor signals relay a variety of messages, most of the literature on male, intra-sex, signaling focuses on urine borne signals that are connected to territory marking in wild mice and ultimately promote aggression in the laboratory [6, 2027]. In contrast, little is known about odor signals that may reduce aggression or promote affiliative behaviors among male mice. In pigs, synthetic androstenone and maternal mammary pheromones effectively reduce aggression in newly mixed groups of prepubescents [28, 29], but, to the best of our knowledge, compounds with similar effects in mice have not been identified. Affiliative behaviors, for example, are performed to strengthen social bonds between conspecifics, and examples in mice include allo-grooming and group sleep [21]. While aggression and affiliative behavior patterns do not always oppose each other [30], it has been proposed that they can be different context dependent strategies used for resource control. Affiliative behaviors are deemed more beneficial when resources are abundant, such as in a captive enclosure with free food and water access [31]. However, almost all work on domestic murine social behavior focuses on encounters with unfamiliar mice in a testing arena. Affiliative patterns between adult males in the home cage have been largely unexplored and will be examined here.

Despite the lack of explicit evidence, it has been suggested that nesting material contains an aggression appeasing odor signal [12]. Specifically, the nest site appears to act as a depository for secretions from the plantar sweat glands which are believed to appease aggression [8, 12]. However, there is little empirical data describing the properties of plantar sweat. Laboratory mice only have one type of sweat gland, eccrine glands, which are found on their food pads [5]. These glands produce an oily substance that is associated with maintaining traction during mobility, marking territory boundaries, and colony member recognition [21, 32, 33]. However, the only study to specifically link plantar sweat to a behavioral response demonstrated that the presence of sweat increases locomotion in stranger mice [34].

To date, there are no published studies that explore the mechanism behind the reduction in aggression observed in response to used nest material or whether odors exist that can promote affiliative behaviors in mice. Providing nesting material is becoming standard practice for laboratory mice and its transfer during cage cleaning helps reduce aggression although it does not completely eliminate it. In order to understand what in the nest is specifically effective at altering mouse behavior, we must have better insight into the chemical signals deposited there and where they come from. Once these specific signals have been identified, further research can examine methods to develop compounds that could then be added to mice environments to help reduce aggression. Additionally, there are no reports that quantitatively analyze the VOC contents of murine plantar sweat, which has historically been suggested as the source of nesting material odor deposits. Therefore, the first aim of this experiment was to quantify compounds deposited within the nests of mouse strains known to exhibit different aggression levels and link them to plausible sources. Our working hypothesis was that the compounds present on the nests would exhibit strain specific properties. We predicted that chemical analyses of the nests from historically peaceful mice would contain VOCs in different proportions than those from the nests of historically aggressive males; in particular, they would contain higher levels of VOCs originating in plantar sweat and lower levels of VOCs originating in urine. To do this, we used three strains known for varying aggression levels: SJL (high aggression), C57BL/6 (moderate aggression), and AJ (low aggression). Our second aim was to determine whether these VOC profiles are related to mouse social behavior. Our working hypothesis was that VOC profiles from the nest and sweat correlate with social behavior in group housed males, with the assumption that behavior is affected similarly across strains. We predicted that these odor profiles would be associated with lower rates of aggressive behavior and/or higher rates of affiliative behavior. In contrast, profiles from urine would be associated with higher rates of aggression. Social behavior was taken as a cage level measure, while odor profiles were taken from individuals based on dominance rank in the tube test [35].

This study served as the first step in a series of projects that aim to identify and validate whether the VOCs identified are true murine pheromones, based on criteria summarized by Wyatt [14, 15]. The goal of the current study was solely to compare profiles across experimental groups and identify molecules that align with quantified behavioral measures.

Results

Cages containing five male mice of SJL/JOrlIcoCrl (SJL), C57BL/6NCrl (B6), or A/JCr (AJ) strain were kept for one week (n = 8 cages per strain; N = 24 total cages). At the end of the week, samples of used nesting material were taken from each cage. Samples of sweat, saliva, and urine were also collected from each cage’s dominant and subordinate mouse as determined by the tube test. All samples were analyzed using gas chromatography- mass spectrometry (GC-MS) and proportions of each sample’s VOCs were evaluated. However, saliva samples were only sufficient enough for qualitative assessments. One AJ nest sample was excluded from analyses due to a flooded cage during the study (leaving N = 23); two sweat samples (one B6 and one AJ) were excluded due to missing labels (leaving N = 46); and six SJL mice did not urinate when stimulated (leaving N = 42). See Methods for further details.

Video data from days 1, 2, and 7 were collected and analyzed for social interactions (mediated aggression, escalated aggression, social investigation, allo-grooming, and group sleep) and nesting behaviors (paw nesting and oral nesting). Full behavioral descriptions can be found in the methods. Ultimately, we calculated the proportion of time that each behavior was observed. Unless otherwise indicated, behavior proportions represent values for all three days observed. An overview of the sample size used in each analysis is provided (S1 Table).

Sample VOC profiles

To address aim 1, we identified or tentatively identified 32 compounds across all sample types (Table 1). Among those, 53% were found in at least 2 sample types; 6% were unique to nest samples; 22% were unique to sweat; 16% were unique to saliva; and 3% were unique to urine (Fig 1). Subsequent analyses excluded saliva samples due to low sample volumes (see Gas Chromatography- Mass Spectrometry Analysis in Methods). As indicated in Table 1, nesting material and urine samples shared many previously identified mouse urinary compounds. In turn, sweat samples showed several cyclic ketone compounds also found in the nesting material, which were not detected in urine samples.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Venn diagram of the number of volatile organic compounds detected in each sample type.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.g001

thumbnail
Table 1. List of identified compounds across sample type in order of ascending run time.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.t001

Strain and VOC profiles

Visual examination of sample profiles using non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS) showed sample separation based on strain for nesting material, sweat, and urine VOC profiles (Fig 2). Social (dominant versus subordinate) ranking effects were not distinguishable in the sweat and urine samples (Fig 2B and 2C).

thumbnail
Fig 2.

Volatile organic compound profiles of (A) nesting material, (B) plantar sweat, and (C) urine showed strain specific patterns. Non-metric multidimensional scaling using Bray-Curtis dissimilarity matrices for (A) used nesting material (stress = 0.095, N = 23), (B) plantar sweat (stress = 0.113, N = 46), and (C) urine (stress = 0.162, N = 42) showed sample separation corresponding to strain. Multivariate analyses using the Adonis test showed a significant difference in profiles between strains for all sample types: nest, p = 0.006; sweat, p = 0.001; and urine, p = 0.001. In contrast, (B) sweat and (C) urine samples did not show separation based on social ranking and Adonis tests did not show significant profile differences.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.g002

Analyses using the Adonis test showed that strain significantly impacted VOC proportions in nesting material, sweat, and urine (p values<0.01; S2 Table). Social rank did not significantly influence VOC proportions in sweat or urine (p values> 0.05; S2 Table).

Additionally, proportions of two urinary pheromones (β-farnesene and 2-sec-butyl-thiazoline (SBT)) were analyzed based on strain and social rank. Higher quantities of both pheromones have been reported in dominant compared to subordinate urine [36], so we used Restricted Maximum Likelihood mixed models to confirm rankings from the tube test. Here, proportions of neither of these pheromones differed by social rank (p values > 0.05; S3 Table), although AJ and B6 mice produced more SBT than SJL mice (Tukey: p< 0.05). Even though we assigned a “dominant” or “subordinate” label to the sampled mice, dominance rank was based solely on the tube test, and may not reflect in-cage behavior. Since dominance rank was not a significant source of variation between sweat and urine samples, the two samples from each cage were averaged together to give single cage mean values for subsequent analyses. However, in cases where only one sample was collected from a cage (see Methods for additional information), that sample alone was used for analysis (S1 Table).

VOC profiles and social interactions

Separate principal component analyses (PCA) were run for each sample type and the behavior data. Strong PC loadings (absolute value ≥ 0.300) considered important are indicated in gray highlighted cells and bold black numbers in Table 2. Influential PCA components from each data set were kept for mixed models. Aggressive behaviors and social investigation had high positive loadings on PC1 while allo-grooming had a strong negative loading. On PC2, group sleep and allo-grooming behaviors had high loadings. Loading values for all influential sweat, nest, urine, and behavior PCs are listed in Table 2.

thumbnail
Table 2. Loading values for all Principal Components (PCs) retained for mixed models.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.t002

To address aim 2, two mixed models were run and p values were corrected using the sequential Bonferroni procedure [37]: one for each behavior PC. All significant VOC PCs and strain were included as independent variables, as well as two covariate measures: average cage nest complexity score, and dominance linearity as measured by Landau’s H. Please refer to the methods for further description. All non-significant fixed effects were dropped from the final models for parsimony.

The only significant effect on behavior PC1 was strain (Table 3). Tukey tests showed that SJL mice had the highest scores, followed by AJ, and then B6 mice (Table 3). Strain also significantly impacted behavior PC2 (Table 3): AJ mice had lower scores than B6 and SJL mice (Table 3). Urine PC3 had a positive effect on behavior PC2 (F1,18.55 = 5.73, padj = 0.027; η2 = 0.278). Compounds with high loading on urine PC3 were β-farnesene, 5-ethylthiazolidine (tentative identification), hexadecanol, and 2-isopropylthiazole related to the inter-male aggression promoting pheromone SBT [25] (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 3. Strain patterns on VOC profile and behavior based on mixed models.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.t003

Since the behavior PCs were primarily impacted by strain, historical social behavior patterns were confirmed in the featured strains to determine if they vary across study days. As expected, AJ, B6, and SJL mice displayed different levels of each social behavior: escalated aggression; mediated aggression; social investigation; allo-grooming; and group sleep (Table 4). Study day only impacted escalated aggression while the day*strain interaction was not significant for any behavior category.

thumbnail
Table 4. Effects of strain and day on behaviors of interest based on mixed models.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.t004

After correcting for multiple comparisons, post-hoc custom tests showed that there was less escalated aggression on day 7 than 1 (GLIM: χ(1) = 5.88, p = 0.015). SJL mice displayed more escalated aggression than AJ mice (GLIM: χ(1) = 7.95, p<0.005), while post hoc Tukey tests showed SJL displayed the most mediated aggression (Tukey: p<0.05) and social investigation (Tukey: p<0.05). B6 and AJ mice displayed similar levels of all three behaviors (p values>0.05). B6 mice displayed the highest level of allo-grooming (Tukey: p<0.05) while SJL and AJ mice displayed similar levels (p>0.05). B6 also displayed more group sleep than AJ mice (Tukey: p<0.05), but SJL mice were similar to both strains (p values>0.05). All strain patterns are depicted in Fig 3.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Aggressive and affiliative behavior patterns varied according to strain.

SJL mice had (A) the highest rate of escalated aggressive behaviors (occurrences per day; p = 0.018). They also spent the highest percent of active time performing (B) mediated aggression (p<0.001) and (C) social investigation (p<0.001) behaviors. B6 mice spent the highest percent of active time (D) allo-grooming (p<0.001) and highest percent of observed time in (E) group sleep (p = 0.006). All data are presented as strain LSM +/- SE with the scatter of the individual data points’ residual differences from the LSM (N = 72, 3 observations from 24 cages). Y axes are shown on a square root back transformed scale in B and D, and on a log10 back transformed scale in C.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.g003

Because strain had such an overwhelming effect on behavior, each of the tested VOC PCs and covariates were run in a mixed model to determine the impact of strain. VOC and genetic effects can both influence behavior, either independently or in conjunction with one another, which is why mixed models were used to examine whether strain influenced VOC PCs. Overall, strain had a significant effect on sweat PC1, nest PC1, urine PC1, and urine PC2 (Table 3). Post hoc Tukey tests showed SJL cages had lower nest PC1 scores and higher urine PC2 scores than B6 and AJ. B6 cages had higher scores on sweat PC1 and lower scores on urine PC2 than SJL and AJ. They also had higher scores on urine PC1 than AJ mice. AJ cages had lower scores on urine PC1 than B6, similar scores to B6 on nest PC1, and similar scores to SJL on sweat PC1. On urine PC2, AJ mice had higher scores than B6 and lower scores than SJL (Table 3). Strain did not affect urine PC3 or nest PC2. In terms of covariate measures, strain significantly impacted average nest complexity score, but did not impact Landau’s H. (Table 3). AJ mice built the most complex nests, followed by B6, and SJL (Tukey: p<0.05).

In this study, the strain pattern of sweat PC1 matches that of allo-grooming, while patterns of nest PC1 and urine PC2 match that of aggression. Therefore, VOCs with high loading on these PCs were chosen for further analysis. Scores on sweat PC1 were positively correlated with both allo-grooming (Pearson’s r = 0.66, 95% CI: 0.35–0.84, p<0.001) and group sleep (Pearson’s r = 0.52, 95% CI: 0.15–0.76, p = 0.011). The following compounds had high positive loading on sweat PC1: 3,4-dimethyl-1,2-cyclopentanedione, ethylcyclopentenolone, and a newly identified compound, 3,5-diethyl- 2-hydroxycyclopent-2-en-1-one (Table 2 and Figs 4C–4E and S1). A verified structure (Fig 4E) is related to ethylcyclopentenolone (Fig 4D). Of these, 3,4-dimethyl-1,2-cyclopentanedione and 3,5-diethyl- 2-hydroxycyclopent-2-en-1-one varied by strain and were correlated with allo-grooming; 3,5-diethyl- 2-hydroxycyclopent-2-en-1-one only was correlated with group sleep (Table 5).

thumbnail
Fig 4. High loading compounds on sweat PC1 and their mass spectra (EI 70 eV).

(A) Total ion chromatogram (TIC); (B) Post-run extracted m/z 126 single ion current chromatogram (SIC); (C) Compound 1, 3,4-dimethyl-1,2-cyclopentadione from SIC at retention time 16.1 min; (D) Compound 2, ethylcyclopentenolone from SIC at retention time 22.02 min; (E) 3,5-diethyl- 2-hydroxycyclopent-2-en-1-one from SIC at retention time 29.69 min.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.g004

thumbnail
Table 5. Relationship between behavior, strain, and high loading VOCs from sweat PC1, nest PC1, and urine PC2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.t005

Scores on nest PC1 were negatively correlated with both escalated (Pearson’s r = -0.56, 95% CI: -0.79- -0.20, p = 0.005) and mediated aggression (Pearson’s r = -0.49, 95% CI: -0.75- -0.10, p = 0.018). Compounds with high positive loading on nest PC1 were geranylacetone, 1,2- cyclopentadione, and another unknown compound (Table 2 and Fig 5). We will refer to this unknown compound as MW 152 based on its assumed molecular weight. Currently the identity of MW 152 has not been determined. Two dehydration products of 6-hydroxy-6-methyl-3-heptanone had high negative loading on nest PC1 (Table 2 and Fig 5). Since positively loading compounds would be associated with less aggression, only they were analyzed. Geranylacetone was negatively correlated with both mediated and escalated aggression and varied by strain (Table 5). MW 152 was negatively correlated with escalated aggression and was not impacted by strain (Table 5).

thumbnail
Fig 5. High loading compounds on nest PC1 and their mass spectra (EI 70 eV).

(A) Z-5,5-dimethyl-2-ethylidenetetrahydrofuran at retention time 7.98 min; (B) E-5,5-dimethyl-2- ethylidenetetrahydrofuran at retention time 9.38 min; (C) 1,2-cyclopentadione at retention time 10.85 min; (D) geranylacetone at retention time 43.93 min.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.g005

Scores on urine PC2 were positively correlated with both escalated (Pearson’s r = 0.63, 95% CI: 0.31–0.82, p<0.001) and mediated aggression (Pearson’s r = 0.59, 95% CI: 0.24–0.80, p = 0.002). Compounds with high positive loading on urine PC2 were hexadecenoic acid, octadecanoic acid, and aggression-related dehydrobrevicomin [22], while testosterone dependent 6-hydroxy-6-methyl-3-heptanone [24] had high negative loading (Table 2). Negatively loading compounds would be associated with less aggression, so only 6-hydroxy-6-methyl-3-heptanone was further analyzed. It was correlated with allo-grooming and varied by strain (Table 5).

Since urine PC3 significantly impacted behavior PC2, it was also compared to each behavior. Urine PC3 did not correlate with any individual behaviors or show strong strain variation (Table 3), so high loading compounds were not examined further.

In summary, SJL mice displayed substantially more aggressive behavior and social investigation. They also had the highest scores on urine PC2 and the lowest on nest PC1. B6 mice displayed the most allo-grooming, had the highest scores on sweat PC1, and the lowest on urine PC2. AJ mice displayed minimal social behavior, performed the most nesting behavior, and had the highest nest complexity scores (Fig 6).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Summary diagram of observed strain patterns.

Variables that were associated with aggressive behavior are listed in red, while those associated with an affiliative behavior are listed in blue.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.g006

Strain and nesting behavior

A side objective of this study was to explore how nest manipulation behaviors varied across the three strains, since the main focus examined secreted chemical contents on nesting material resulting from manipulation with the paws or mouth. Separate mixed models were run for manipulation performed with the paws and mouth. Behaviors performed with the paws were expected to influence compounds originating in the sweat while behaviors performed with the mouth would have more impact on compounds from the saliva. Nesting done with the paws and mouth were significantly influenced by strain (Table 4). Post hoc Tukey tests showed that AJ mice performed more nesting with their paws than B6 (Tukey: p<0.05), while SJL were similar to both (p values>0.05). AJ mice performed more nesting with their mouth than SJL (Tukey: p<0.05), while B6 were similar to both (p values>0.05).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this experiment is the first to report the VOC profiles of used nesting material and foot plantar gland sweat in male laboratory mice (aim 1). It is also the first to examine the relationship between these profiles and social behavior (aim 2). It has been shown that preserving used nesting material can reduce aggression at cage change [12], but the theory that nesting material holds aggression reducing plantar sweat has remained speculation until now.

Observed behavior

The behavior PCA, PC1 showed that mediated aggression, escalated aggression, and social investigation were strongly correlated across all cages. In contrast, allo-grooming was negatively associated with the latter three behaviors on PC1, and positively associated with group sleep behavior on PC2. However, all of these patterns were strongly explained by strain. On behavior PC1 (high aggression and low allo-grooming), SJL had the highest scores followed by AJ and then B6. This reflects the greater amount of aggression and social investigation performed by SJL and the greater amount of allo-grooming performed by B6. On the other hand, behavior PC2 scores (high allo-grooming and group sleep) reflect the higher amount of group sleep performed by B6 and SJL mice than AJs.

Several of these strain patterns were unexpected. First, SJL mice are known for excessive inter-male aggression [38], but they also displayed the most social investigation behavior. Our coding scheme was not detailed enough to make conclusions about the direct behavioral sequence, but anecdotally, social investigatory sniffing tended to precede aggressive interactions. Initially the ethogram did not include a separate category for social investigation, but it was added after observing this pattern after the first few cages. This calls into question the underlying motivations of sniffing behavior, as it is traditionally considered to be neutral or exploratory [3941]. However, these data make the actor mouse’s intentions less clear.

Second, B6 males are frequently the subject of caretaker complaints about aggression. Here they displayed minimal aggression which is consistent with previous work [42, 43], but we anticipated conflict in some cages in order to demonstrate a more linear relationship between VOCs and observed aggression. Thirdly, AJ cages displayed minimal social interactions, aside from group sleep. They are known for minimal levels of inter-male aggression [38], so we mistakenly presumed that this would equate to higher rates of affiliative behavior. Generally, aggressive and affiliative behaviors are performed more by species that are sociable, like mice [5]. However, AJ have previously demonstrated low sociability to stranger mice [44, 45], so these data extend this pattern to behavior towards familiar cage mates.

We purposefully designed this experiment to incorporate multiple inbred mouse strains in order to ensure that a wide range of specific behaviors were observed. However, we did not expect to find such limited variation within these strains. Thus, strain unfortunately acts as a confounding factor for subsequent interpretations.

VOC patterns that match behavior

Overall, we found that several VOCs in urine, sweat, and nesting material aligned with strain specific patterns of social behavior in the home cage. While VOCs did not directly account for a significant amount of variation in aggressive behavior, it is possible that they may be one of the many factors that contribute to inherent strain differences in behavior. Along with relatively high aggression levels, SJL mice displayed low scores on nest PC1, and high scores on urine PC2. Scores on each of these respective PCs were negatively and positively correlated with aggression. Therefore, VOCs with a positive loading on nest PC1 and a negative loading on urine PC2 showed potential for an aggression appeasement role.

Geranylacetone was the only VOC from the nest to be both negatively correlated with aggression and have a strain specific pattern. It was produced less in SJL mice than AJ, but quantities in B6 were similar to both other strains. It was also present in sweat and saliva samples and has previously been detected in hamster ventral glands [46]. This gland is typically used for territory marking [47] and there is some evidence that secretions are capable of changing in response to individual social interactions [46]. Perhaps proportions from the nest samples related to aggression due to a dilution effect from being in the environment. Odor signals are often effective at small concentrations, so the values seen here from pure body fluids may be too high to relate to behavior. Additionally, quantities of geranylacetone showed the same strain pattern as nest complexity score. Scores were lowest in SJL, which supports previous research showing that nest score decreases with the number of wounded mice in a cage [48]. As mice engage in more aggressive interactions that include rapid fighting or chasing, any existing nest structure is likely to be destroyed during escape attempts. As stress and pain levels rise in the cage, motivation to restructure or maintain a complex nest decreases [48].

MW 152 was negatively correlated with escalated aggression and was the only compound not detected in any of this study’s body fluid samples. Furthermore, it was not present in control (unused) nest samples, so it is possible that it originated from another body gland, fur oils, or fecal residues. Although precautions were taken to minimize contamination with fecal residue (e.g. cleaning the surface of the foot and the anesthesia chamber), it is possible that fecal odors could have contaminated the samples and future work could examine how the fecal VOC profile may impact behavior. At this time, a verified structure for MW 152 has not been determined.

The only VOC with a high negative loading on urine PC2 was 6-hydroxy-6-methyl-3-heptanone, a MUP ligand that accelerates puberty in female mice [49]. Although it did not directly relate to aggression, it was positively correlated with allo-grooming and was produced more by B6 mice than SJL and AJ. This result was unexpected since male mouse pheromones from urine have been shown to promote aggression between males [25, 50]. However, to our knowledge, 6-hydroxy-6-methyl-3-heptanone has not been directly tested for effects in males. Based on this data, it may have a role promoting affiliative behavior.

Although this study aimed to find aggression reducing compounds, the relationship between sweat and social behavior was central to the study hypothesis. B6 mice had the highest scores on sweat PC1 and displayed the most allo-grooming. Of the VOCs with a high positive loading, 3,4-dimethyl-1,2-cyclopentanedione and 3,5-diethyl- 2-hydroxycyclopent-2-en-1-one both were correlated with allo-grooming and were produced in higher quantities by B6 mice than AJ and SJL. 3,5-diethyl- 2-hydroxycyclopent-2-en-1-one was also correlated with group sleep. To the best of our knowledge, 3,4-dimethyl-1,2-cyclopentanedione does not have a known behavioral role, but shows potential for improving mouse welfare. It would be a worthy candidate for future behavioral testing to explore its potential role in mouse communication, along with the newly discovered 3,5-diethyl- 2-hydroxycyclopent-2-en-1-one. To our knowledge, these kinds of cyclopentanone derivatives are unique to mouse plantar sweat and, based on our data, may play a role in promoting affiliative behaviors.

Dominance hierarchy

Surprisingly, dominance linearity in the tube test as measured by Landau’s H did not account for significant differences in behavior. This result is the opposite of what we had expected. A previous study showed that increasing values of Landau’s H correlated with lower levels of aggression, suggesting that certainty in social rank reduces escalated interactions [51]. One main difference between that study and this was that the former used outbred CD-1 mice, while inbred strains were used here. This may reflect a strain impact on the relationship between dominance linearity in the tube test and aggression. Additionally, the previous study measured aggression and linearity during multiple time periods and across changes in cage enrichment. Our study focused on a one-week time period and kept housing conditions stable. Even though mice were acclimated to the arena before testing, it has been argued that there is a learned component to tube testing, such that more than one tube testing session is required for mice to display valid rankings outside the home cage [52]. However, a previous assessment of stable male groups found the tube test produced inconsistent rankings over 3 weeks’ time, with the most stable relationships occurring between the second and third trials [53]. This finding was published while our experiment was in progress; consequently, the approach used here does not take these new findings into consideration. It was also suggested that competitive learning in the tube test may be specific to that arena and not reflect home cage behavior [53]. Considering both the contrasting relationship between dominance linearity and aggression, and the lack of variation in β-farnesene and SBT between dominant and subordinate urine, it is likely that the tube test, at least as it was carried out here, may not be a valid indicator of individual in-cage social rank. That being said, the lack of a relationship between rank stability in the tube arena and aggression in the cage may still be meaningful. Further research will be valuable in explaining differences between tube test social rank and in cage social rank.

Limitations and future research

In this study, we were concerned about obtaining a sufficient quantity of sweat for analysis and utilized pilocarpine injections to increase sample volume. We do acknowledge that using pilocarpine to induce plantar sweat secretion may have unknown effects on VOC ratios. Pilocarpine functions by stimulating M3 muscarinic receptors on exocrine glands, such as the sweat glands [54]. Currently, there is little evidence to determine how the increased gland activity impacts VOC content, but it is possible the compounds were diluted in the larger sample volume. Work in humans shows that sweat induced by pilocarpine is generally similar in content to sweat induced by exercise, although the latter contains more compounds indicative of a more demanding metabolic state [55]. However, mice do not produce sweat to thermoregulate, and to the best of our knowledge, there are no direct VOC comparisons of fluids collected without stimulation versus pilocarpine. Additionally, individual variation in responses to the pilocarpine treatment could have impacted the data. Pilocarpine is a common treatment for dry mouth in humans, but efficacy can depend on the individual [56]. At this time, factors that impact pilocarpine success have not been identified, and it was not possible to quantify the volume of collected sweat based on the sampling method.

A second limitation worth noting is that this study only focused on VOC profiles. It is possible that protein signals could have impacted these data. Urinary MUP20 (“darcin”) in particular is a pheromone that promotes aggression between males, but also is necessary for social learning to occur [26, 57]. Darcin is expressed more in mice of the C57 lineage [58], so it is possible that it caused B6 mice to become familiar with cage mates more quickly than other strains and as a result perform more affiliative behavior. That being said, production itself cannot predict aggression since AJ and SJL mice both produce low levels of darcin [59], but more complex compound interactions have yet to be explored. This study also did not address the effect on behavior of individual differences in odor perception. Many odor signals, especially pheromones, are detected by the vomeronasal organ (VNO) [60]. Gene expression in the VNO, particularly those encoding chemoreceptors, show great variation between strains and could be a major contributor to variability in behavior [61]. While strain specific expression was the focus of this study, we cannot assume that sensitivity follows the same pattern. For example, even though darcin is produced more by the C57 line, BALB/c males (Castle lineage) are still reactive and display the expected scent marking response when exposed to it [62].

Another point of consideration in this study was the amount of time mice in each cage spent performing nesting related behaviors, as this is likely to impact the relative amount of VOC deposits in the nest. AJ mice performed the most nesting done with the paws and mouth, but their scores on nest PC1 were similar to those of B6. Of the high loading compounds on nest PC1, geranylactone was detected in both sweat and saliva samples, and 1,2-cyclopentadione (tentative) was detected in saliva. However, all of the VOCs in the nest samples traced to sweat or saliva were also detected in urine. Since the VOCs in the nest deposits are produced in multiple body fluids, it is difficult to conclude how time spent nesting directly impacted the nest VOC profile. This is especially true since our saliva samples were not sufficient for quantitative analyses. Anecdotally, AJ mice produced the lowest volume of saliva, so the increased time spent nesting may be necessary for compounds levels on the nest to be similar to B6.

Conclusion

Overall, this study found that, in the home cage, odor profiles from sweat, nesting material, and urine, show strain specific patterns that align with affiliative and aggressive behavior. These findings warrant future studies that directly test the influence of compounds found in sweat, urine, and nesting material on expression of social behaviors, to hopefully put the field one step closer to promoting socio-positive behaviors and improving laboratory mouse welfare.

Methods

Ethics statement

All procedures were approved by Purdue University’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol #1707001598) and reporting adhered to the ARRIVE 2.0 guidelines [63]. The protocol was not previously registered before conducting the study.

Due to concern over heightened aggression in the cage, we established humane endpoint criteria in which any mouse with wounding greater than 1cm2 would be immediately euthanized. Animals were monitored daily for general activity and signs of pain/distress. If any animals developed minor wounding, they were monitored more frequently. No mice reached our endpoint criteria.

Animals

All mice in this study were acquired from Charles River and were free of common known pathogen agents at shipping. More information can be found in [64]. Eight cages each containing five male mice of the following strains were used: SJL/JOrlIcoCrl (SJL)- Wilmington, MA; C57BL/6NCrl (B6)- Kingston, NY; and A/JCr (AJ)–Frederick, MD (N = 24 cages; 120 mice). Per the ARRIVE 2.0 guidelines [63], we are declaring that no strain served as a traditional control due to the study’s exploratory nature. Sample size was determined using Mead’s resource equation. Due to spatial constraints, the twenty four cages were divided into four equal groups containing two cages per strain. B6 mice were used as they are the most commonly studied inbred mouse and have the widest practical application; SJL males were used as a known high-aggressive strain [38]; while AJ mice were used as a known low aggressive strain compared to B6 mice [65]. Mice arrived at approximately 8 weeks of age and were housed for one week in open top micro-isolator cages, 11.5” x 7.25” x 4.25” (Ancare, Bellmore, NY) with food (Envigo, Teklad 2018, Indianapolis, IN) and reverse osmosis water offered ad libitum. Each cage contained aspen wood chip bedding (NEPCO, Warrensburg, NY) and 8.5g of virgin kraft crinkle paper (Enviro-dri, Fibercore, Cleveland, Ohio) for nesting material. Cages were kept under a 12:12 light: dark cycle (lights on at 06:00) with relative humidity ranging between 28–76% and temperature ranging between 18.8–23.3°C. All mice were weighed at the beginning (mean weight 20.06 ±1.71 g) and end (mean weight: 21.73±1.86g) of the study and ear punched for identification. All animal handling was performed by female researchers and husbandry staff. Male scents can influence stress response in rodents and may alter baseline measurements [66].

Upon arrival, mice were randomly distributed into the cages (5 mice per cage) from the shipping containers using a numerical sequence from RANDOM.org. Cage placement on the two MetroRacks was initially randomized based on a RANDOM.org sequence, and subsequently balanced by strain across two shelves on each rack. Each shelf contained 2 cages and was enclosed by partitions of white foam board (Office Depot, Boca Raton, FL) to remove background noise for video monitoring (see Home Cage Observation below). Light intensity during the day was reduced from 430 lux, in the middle of the room, to an average of 67 lux at each cage location. Each cage was given its own numerical label from 1 to 24 that corresponded to its group and strain. Only the numerical label was present on the cage card to partially blind caregivers to cage identities during routine husbandry and research staff during sample collection/processing, behavior tests, and video coding.

VOC sample collection and processing

Nest.

Mice were left in their home cage for 7 days after arrival. At the end of the week, 25 strips of crinkle paper were collected for VOC analysis (see below for GC-MS procedure). Samples were taken from both the periphery and center of the nest since mice restructure their nests daily [67] and it is not known if they are in contact with one area more than another. Some cages did not contain a structured nest, so the area containing dispersed material was divided into quadrants and each quadrant was equally sampled. The weighed sample of crinkle paper was placed in a 10 mL head-space sample vial with a Teflon cap (Gerstel GmbH, Mülheim an der Ruhr, Germany). An acetone (Avantor, Center Valley, PA) washed, straightened, and dried metal paper clip was punched through the vial Teflon seal. A magnetic Gerstel stir bar was attached to the clip above the nest material, 5 μL of 7-tridecanone in methanol (Baker Analyzed, Mallinckrodt Baker Inc., Phillipsburg, NJ) (8 ng/5 μL) was added to the nest material and the vial cap was closed tight. The head-space VOCs were collected at room temperature for 1 hour.

Two exceptions occurred within the AJ strain during nest sample collection. One cage flooded at the end of the third study day. Nest material was soaked and unable to be collected. It was replaced and subsequently collected four days later. A second cage flooded on the sixth study day. The nest from this cage was collected since there was a short proximity to the planned sampling day and enough dry material could be collected for processing. The former data point produced unusual data and was excluded from analysis; however, the latter was included.

Sweat.

To analyze compounds from mouse sweat, the stir bar surface sampling method (previously used for human skin VOC analyses) was replicated [68, 69]. To collect secretions from the plantar sweat glands, mice were anesthetized with compressed isoflurane and each foot was cleaned with ethanol. After air drying, hindfeet and forefeet were given a subcutaneous injection of 50 μL and 20 μL of 1mg/1mL pilocarpine (Sigma- Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) respectively. Previous studies have shown that gland activity is highest approximately 10–20 minutes after injection [70, 71], so mice were kept under anesthesia for 20 minutes post injection. Sweat was collected on the surface of one forefoot and one hindfoot per mouse using Twister™ polydimethylsiloxane coated stir bars (Gerstel GmbH, Mülheim an der Ruhr, Germany) embedded previously with the internal standard, 7-tridecanone (Sigma- Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) as described previously [69]. Every five minutes post injection the stir bar was rolled across the surface of the hind and forefeet five times. All collections were performed in the mice’s housing room between the 7th and 9th hour of the light cycle. All mice were monitored throughout the procedure for signs of distress (uneven, shallow breaths; pale color of foot tissue).

Saliva.

Saliva was collected while the mice were anesthetized for sweat collection as the pilocarpine injections also stimulated saliva production. After the mice lost consciousness, the exposed chamber floor was quickly cleaned with ethanol. Saliva samples were collected via pipette from the acrylic chamber floor and transferred into a 1.5 mL centrifuge tube. Saliva samples (25–100 μL) were pipetted into 20 mL glass scintillation vials containing 5.0 mL water (OmniSolv™ LC-MS grade, EMD Millipore Corporation, Billerica, MA), 8 ng of 7-tridecanone as an internal standard and the Twister™ stir bar. The vial was placed in a water bath at 40°C for 2.5 hours for static aqueous stir bar extraction. This sampling method was modified from a previously reported study with human saliva [72].

Urine.

Since mice naturally urinate upon handling, each mouse was held over a fresh aluminum foil bowl to collect urine on day 5 of the study week, before behavior testing. Gentle abdominal massage was administered when needed to facilitate collection and samples were transferred via pipette to a 1.5mL centrifuge tube. However, when mice would not urinate during handling, the fluid was collected after the mice acclimated to the plexiglass tube test arena used for the behavioral assay (see Social Ranking section for description).

Urine samples (15–200 μL) were pipetted in a 20 mL glass scintillation vial with the metal foil cap containing 2.0 mL of water (OmniSolv™) [73], 8 ng of 7-tridecanone as an internal standard and a Twister™ stir bar. Stir bar extraction was performed for 60 min at room temperature at 850 rpm speed (15-place stir plate Variomag Multipoint HP15, H+P Labortechnic, Oberschleissheim, Germany).

After extraction, all stir bars were washed with OmniSolv™ water, dried with non-lint KimWipes tissue (Kimberly-Clark, Roswell, GA), and placed in a Thermal Desorption Autosampler and Cooled Injection System (TDSA-CIS 4 from Gerstel GmbH) connected to an Agilent 6890N gas chromatograph– 5973iMSD mass spectrometer (Agilent Technologies, Inc., Wilmington, DE).

Since the sampling unit was the cage, sweat and saliva samples were collected from each cage’s dominant and subordinate mouse based on results from the tube test (see Social Ranking section for test procedure) as social ranking has been reported to impact pheromone levels [20, 49, 74, 75]. Urine was collected from each mouse, but only samples belonging to each cage’s dominant and subordinate were analyzed. All samples were collected at Purdue University and transported to Indiana University for analysis. In total, 24 nest samples, 48 sweat samples, 48 saliva samples, and 42 urine samples were collected. Six mice, each from a different cage of the SJL strain, did not produce urine when stimulated. Additionally, two sweat samples originating from different cages lost their labels during transport and could not be processed, leaving 46 data points for sweat analysis.

Gas Chromatography-Mass Spectrometry (GC-MS) analysis

Splitless mode was used for thermal desorption sampling (TDS) with a temperature program of 20°C for 0.5 min, then a 60°C/min increase up to 280°C for 8 min. The transfer line temperature was set at 290°C and the cooled injection system (CIS) was cooled using liquid nitrogen to 0°C during the thermal desorption. For the sample introduction into the GC-MS, the CIS was heated at 12°C/s to 280°C and held for 10 min. Solvent vent mode was used for the CIS inlet with a vent pressure of 9.1 psi, a vent flow of 50 mL/min, and a purge flow of 50 mL/min. The gas chromatograph (GC) separation capillary was a DB-5MS (30 m x 0.25 mm, i.d., 0.25 μm film thickness) from Agilent, and the GC carrier gas (helium) head pressure was 9.1 psi at a constant 1.2 mL/min flow mode. The GC oven temperature program started at 40°C for 1 min, then increased at 2°C/ min to 180°C and immediately 10°C/ min to 230°C and held for 6 min (total GC run time 85 min). For the mass spectrometer (MS), positive electron ionization (EI) mode at 70eV was used with a scanning rate of 2.47 scans/s and mass range of 41–350 amu. The mass spectrometric detector (MSD) transfer line temperature was 300°C, the ion source temperature was 230°C, and the quadrupole temperature was set at 150°C.

Compounds were identified or tentatively identified by matching retention times and mass spectra with standard compounds when available (Sigma-Aldrich Chemical Co.) and with spectra through NIST Mass Spectral Search Program for the NIST/EPA/NIH Mass Spectral Library (Version 2.0 a, 2002). Additionally, in-house (Novotny Laboratory) synthesized mouse urinary pheromone compounds and the in-house spectral database were utilized for identifications.

All VOC data was used to calculate odor proportions by dividing each absolute peak value by the sample’s total peak area [76]. This was done to determine how behavior is affected by the relative VOC amount perceived by the mice. Due to the low volume of saliva that was collected, the GCMS analysis was unable to provide reliable quantitative values. The saliva VOC profile only served to make qualitative comparisons about nest compound origins. All VOC data was used to calculate odor proportions by dividing each absolute peak value by the sample’s total peak area [76]. This was done to determine how behavior is affected by the relative VOC amount perceived by the mice. Due to the low volume of saliva that was collected, the GCMS analysis was unable to provide reliable quantitative values. The saliva VOC profile only served to make qualitative comparisons about nest compound origins.

Behavioral measures

Home cage observations.

Cages were continuously recorded for one week from arrival to sample collection with closed circuit television cameras (Sony, Tokyo, Japan) and GeoVision monitoring software (Taipei, Taiwan). Dark cycle recordings used 2 infrared illuminators (Sodial, China) per cage. The following social behaviors were documented: escalated aggression, mediated aggression, allo-groom, group sleep, and social investigation (Table 6). Coders were partially blinded to strain due to the difference in coat color between B6 and AJ/SJL. All social interactions were scored using one-zero focal sampling for one minute every five minutes between 12:00AM- 12:00PM on days 1, 2, and 7 of the study.

thumbnail
Table 6. Ethogram of observed behavior categories.

All descriptions were taken from mousebehavior.org.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.t006

Since we were interested in compounds deposited on the nest, we were also interested in how mice interacted with the nest. Thus, oral nest manipulation and paw nest manipulation (Table 6) were scored using one-zero sampling for one minute every half hour between 12:00AM- 12:00PM on days 1, 2, and 7 of the study.

The 12:00AM -12:00PM time frame was chosen because it allows for equal observation across light and dark conditions and the mice experienced the least amount of disturbance during this time frame. Day 1 was monitored to include behaviors that occurred while the mice adjusted to their new cage, before the hierarchy is established; day 2 reflects interactions that occur as the hierarchy is beginning to form; and day 7 reflects the last 24 hours of the study in which the hierarchy is established [77]. Day 7 is also a common day for mice to undergo cage cleaning, so the maximum level of secretions in the nesting material represents the amount that many mice are exposed to before their nests are replaced. Ultimately the proportion of active time in which each behavior category occurred was determined for each cage, with the exception of group sleep for which the proportion of all observed time was calculated.

Nest scores.

Daily nest scores were taken around the ninth hour of the light cycle based on Hess et al. [78]. This time was used as it is when nest scores are typically highest [67]. This scale was used as it provides the most variability for mice that are good nest builders and has been shown to reflect changes based on aggression [48]. Briefly, the nest is divided into a square region and each quarter is given a score from 1–5 based on its complexity with higher scores corresponding to more complex structures. The four quarter scores are then averaged for the overall nest score of a cage. In situations where more than 1 nest was present in a single cage, the scores from both nests were averaged. Daily values from each cage were used to determine the average score for the study week.

Social ranking.

On days 5 and 6 of the study, the tube test was run to determine the linearity of each cage’s social hierarchy based on Howerton et al. [51]. Previously, lower linearity has been reported with higher aggression levels [51]. The tube test was run over 2 days due to the time consuming nature of the pairwise tests for all mice within the cage. When conducting the test, strain was blocked by time of day to counteract systematic test order bias. That is, we tested one cage of every strain in each time period (morning (06:30–12:30) and afternoon (13:00–17:30)).

In brief, the test is conducted using a PVC tube (approx. 2.5cm diameter) connected to two plexiglass containers (approx. 19 cm x 19 cm x 21.5 cm). To acclimate the mice, 24 hours before the trials each mouse was placed in the test arena and given at least five, but no more than ten minutes to acclimate which was defined by the mouse comfortably exploring the areas on each side of the tube. Testing began by placing two mice from the same cage on opposite sides of the tube. They typically entered the tube immediately. The first mouse to place both hindfeet on the floor outside the tube was considered the loser. In a cage of five mice, there were ten different pairwise trials to test. All trials were repeated four times to give forty total trials per cage. The test arena was cleaned with ethanol and allowed to air dry between each trial. Trials were given a cutoff time of two minutes. Each mouse received a dominance score (Vij) determined by the number of trials won by mouse i when competing against mouse j. Vij scores were used to calculate the hierarchy linearity of the cage based on Landau’s h [79].

Where N = the number of mice per cage and Vi is the summation of Vij for each mouse i on its opponent mouse j. Scores near 1 correspond to a near complete hierarchy while scores near 0 signify the lack of a hierarchy. Each mouse’s rank was also calculated by determining the number of trials won over all trials in which he participated. These scores were used to determine the dominant and subordinate mice used for sweat and saliva sampling.

Statistical analysis

Sample VOC profiles.

Before formal analysis, all VOC data were visualized using a Venn diagram to summarize similar and unique compounds across sample types. R Studio (version 3.4.3) and the VennDiagram package were used to create the visualization.

Strain and VOC profiles.

Individual nest (N = 23), sweat (N = 46), and urine (N = 42) samples were separately visualized in two dimensions using non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS) to examine similarity based on VOC proportions across strain. Sweat and urine data were also examined for similarity between two levels of social rank. Factor differences were tested using the Adonis test since the datasets did not meet multivariate normality. Beta dispersion assumption was checked post hoc. Since cages were run in four groups over time, the batch number was also included as a blocking factor. NMDS, Adonis test, and assumption check were run in R Studio (version 3.4.3) using vegan, tidyverse, ggplot2, and mvnormtest packages.

Additionally, since mice were sampled based on ranking in the tube test, we wanted to confirm differences in two known urinary pheromones, β-farnesene and 2-sec-butyl-thiazoline (SBT) between social rank. Both pheromones have been previously reported to vary based on social rank [36]. Proportions of β-farnesene and SBT were analyzed using restricted maximum likelihood mixed models with strain, rank, and their interaction as fixed effects, and batch number as a random factor. Cage nested within strain was also included as a random factor to account for repeated sampling from the same cage. The models were run in JMP Pro (version 14.0.0), and assumptions were checked post hoc.

VOC profiles and social interactions.

Cage level proportion data for each sample type (nest, sweat, urine) and social behavior were run in separate Principal Component Analyses (PCA) with values scaled to a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1. The broken stick model (BSM) was used for principle component (PC) retention with the following exception: for the behavior PCA, BSM showed that only PC1 was significant. However, behavior PC2 explained a large portion of the variance, 29.67%, and had an eigenvalue of 1.48, therefore it was kept for further analysis. The following numbers in parentheses represent the number of retained PCs for each dataset: nest (2), sweat (1), urine (3), and behavior (2). Varimax rotation was used on the nest, sweat, and urine PCAs to maximize variable separation across PCs.

Mixed models were used to determine how nest, sweat, and urine odors affect behavior. Strain, and PCs from the nest, sweat, and urine data were used as independent variables, while PCs from the behavior data were tested separately as dependent variables. The cage average weekly nest score and Landau’s H were included as covariates. Batch number was used a random factor. Non-significant variables were manually excluded from the models and those with the lowest AIC value were kept for interpretation. Since this study used two models to assess whether VOCs impact behavior, p values were adjusted using the sequential Bonferroni procedure to correct for multiple comparisons [37]. All further analyses examining strain effects on VOC PCs and individual VOCs were also run as mixed models. Individual VOC models had compound specific hypotheses and therefore a multiple comparisons correction was not performed. Individual VOCs were only tested in a mixed model if their PC of origin showed strong correlation with behavior based on Pearson’s r. Normality and homogeneity of variance were tested post hoc by visually examining the residual Q-Q plot and spread of the residual by predicted plots for each model [80]. PCAs were run in R Studio (version 3.4.3) using FactoMineR, factoextra, and tidyverse packages. JMP Pro (version 14.0.0) was used for the mixed models and assumption check [80].

Data from one AJ nest was excluded due to flooding, making group sizes for the nest dataset unbalanced for NMDS, Adonis test, and PCA (AJ: n = 7, B6 and SJL: n = 8).

Behavior across study days.

To validate historical differences in strain social behavior and explore differences in strain nesting behavior, cage level behavior proportions from each day of observation (1, 2, 7) were tested in a series of REML mixed models with strain, day and the interaction as fixed effects, and batch number and cage nested within strain as random factors (N = 72, 3 observations from 24 cages). Post hoc Tukey tests were used to assess factor level differences. Assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance were tested by visual examination of the residual Q-Q plot using JMP Pro (version 14.0.0) and Levene’s test using R Studio (version 3.4.3) respectively. A log10 transformation was used on social investigation data, and square root transformations were used on the mediated aggression and allo-groom data. Data for escalated aggression was extremely skewed and transformation was unsuccessful to meet model assumptions. Therefore, count data per day were calculated and analyzed using a generalized linear mixed model (GLIM) with a negative binomial distribution. Custom tests corrected for multiple comparisons were used to identify specific factor differences.

Supporting information

S1 File. Methods for 3,5-diethyl- 2-hydroxycyclopent-2-en-1-one preparation and synthesis are submitted.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.s001

(DOCX)

S1 Fig.

(A) Total ion chromatogram (TIC); peak elutes at 31.562 minutes. (B) Full mass spectrum of 31.562 minute peak. (C) Molecular ion region. The boxes represent the theoretical distribution.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.s002

(TIF)

S1 Table. Total number of data points used in each analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.s003

(XLSX)

S2 Table. Effects of strain, social rank, and batch number on raw VOC proportions based on the Adonis test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.s004

(XLSX)

S3 Table. Fixed effects on known urinary pheromones based on mixed models.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.s005

(XLSX)

S5 Table. Calculated proportion nest GC-MS data (raw values/ total sample peak area).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.s007

(XLSX)

S7 Table. Calculated proportion sweat GC-MS data (raw values/ total sample peak area).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.s009

(XLSX)

S9 Table. Calculated proportion urine GC-MS data (raw values/ total sample peak area).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.s011

(XLSX)

S10 Table. Non-normalized saliva GC-MS data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.s012

(XLSX)

S11 Table. Legend matching raw data files in the Agilent ChemStation format to each respective cage or mouse sample ID.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.s013

(XLSX)

S12 Table. Behavior data across all study days.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.s014

(XLSX)

S13 Table. Behavior proportions from individual study days.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251416.s015

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

All GC-MS data were acquired through the Institute for Pheromone Research at Indiana University in the Department of Chemistry. Data to verify 3,5-diethyl- 2-hydroxycyclopent-2-en-1-one structure were collected by Jonathan Karty using an Agilent 7890B/G7250 GC-QTOF-MS in the Indiana University Mass Spectrometry Facility. We’d like to thank our undergraduate assistants, Katie Bachert and Nicole Brockway, for the many hours spent coding video as well as Mikayla Burrell at the Institute for Pheromone Research for assisting with VOC sample analyses.

References

  1. 1. Poole T. Happy animals make good science. Lab Anim. 1997;31: 116–24. pmid:9175008
  2. 2. Weber EM, Dallaire JA, Gaskill BN, Pritchett-Corning KR, Garner JP. Aggression in group-housed laboratory mice: why can’t we solve the problem? Lab Anim (NY). 2017;46: 157–161. pmid:28328884
  3. 3. Kappel S, Hawkins P, Mendl MT. To group or not to group? Good practice for housing male laboratory mice. Animals. 2017;7: 1–25. pmid:29186765
  4. 4. Bartolomucci A, Palanza P, Sacerdote P, Ceresini G, Chirieleison A, Panerai AE, et al. Individual housing induces altered immuno-endocrine responses to psychological stress in male mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2003;28: 540–558. pmid:12689611
  5. 5. Crowcroft P. Mice all over. London [UK]: Foulis; 1966.
  6. 6. Latham N, Mason G. From house mouse to mouse house: the behavioural biology of free-living Mus musculus and its implications in the laboratory. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2004; 261–289.
  7. 7. National Reseach Council. Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals. 8th ed. Washington DC: The National Academies Press; 2011. https://doi.org/10.17226/12910
  8. 8. Van Loo PLP, Van Zutphen LFM, Baumans V. Male management: coping with aggression problems in male laboratory mice. Lab Anim. 2003;37: 300–313. pmid:14599305
  9. 9. Jennings M, Batchelor GR, Brain PF, Dick A, Elliott H, Francis RJ, et al. Refining rodent husbandry: the mouse. Report of the Rodent Refinement Working Party. Lab Anim. 1998;32: 233–259. pmid:9718472
  10. 10. Gray S, Hurst JL. The effects of cage cleaning on aggression within groups of male laboratory mice. Anim Behav. 1995;49: 821–826.
  11. 11. Ambrose N, Morton DB. The Use of Cage Enrichment to Reduce Male Mouse Aggression. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 2000;3: 117–125.
  12. 12. Van Loo PL., Kruitwagen CLJJ, Van Zutphen LFM, Koolhaas JM, Baumans V. MODULATION OF AGGRESSION IN MALE MICE: INFLUENCE OF CAGE CLEANING REGIME AND SCENT. Anim Welf. 2000;9: 281–295.
  13. 13. Apps PJ, Weldon PJ, Kramer M. Chemical signals in terrestrial vertebrates: search for design features. Nat Prod Rep. 2015;32: 1131–1153. pmid:26100000
  14. 14. Wyatt TD. Fifty years of pheromones. Nature. 2009;457: 262–263. pmid:19148082
  15. 15. Wyatt T. Semiochemicals: Pheromones, Signature Mixtures and Behaviour. In: Nielsen BL, editor. Olfaction in Animal Behaviour and Welfare. Boston, MA: CABI; 2017. pp. 36–38.
  16. 16. Arakawa H, Blanchard DC, Arakawa K, Dunlap C, Blanchard RJ. Scent marking behavior as an odorant communication in mice. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32: 1236–1248. pmid:18565582
  17. 17. Novotny M V. Pheromones, binding proteins and receptor responses in rodents. Biochem Soc Trans. 2003;31: 117–122. pmid:12546667
  18. 18. Liberles SD. Mammalian Pheromones. Annu Rev Physiol. 2014;76: 151–175. pmid:23988175
  19. 19. Hurst JL, Fang J, Barnard CJ. The role of substrate odours in maintaining social tolerance between male house mice, Mus musculus domesticus. Anim Behav. 1993;45: 997–1006.
  20. 20. Lee W, Khan A, Curley JP. Major urinary protein levels are associated with social status and context in mouse social hierarchies. Proc R Soc B. 2017;284. pmid:28931741
  21. 21. Brown RE. The Rodents II: suborder Myomorpha. In: Brown RE, Macdonald DW, editors. Social odours in mammals. Oxford [UK]: Claredon Press, Oxford University Press; 1985. pp. 345–428. https://doi.org/10.1016/0376-6357(86)90097-5
  22. 22. Touhara K, Vosshall LB. Sensing Odorants and Pheromones with Chemosensory Receptors. Annu Rev Physiol. 2009;71: 307–332. pmid:19575682
  23. 23. Stoddart DM. Dispersion and social integration. The Ecology of Vertebrate Olfaction. London: Chapman and Hall Ltd; 1980. pp. 141–160.
  24. 24. Novotny M V, Schwende FJ, Wiesler D, Jorgenson JW, Carmack M. Identificiation of a testosterone dependent unique volatile constituent of male mouse urine: 7-exo-ethyl-5-methyl-6,8-dioxabicyclo[3,2,1]-3-octene. Experientia. 1984;40: 217–219. pmid:6698181
  25. 25. Novotny M, Harvey S, Jemiolo B, Alberts J. Synthetic pheromones that promote inter-male aggression in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1985;82: 2059–2061. pmid:3856883
  26. 26. Chamero P, Marton TF, Logan DW, Flanagan K, Cruz JR, Saghatelian A, et al. Identification of protein pheromones that promote aggressive behaviour. Nature. 2007;450: 899–902. pmid:18064011
  27. 27. Wyatt TD. Pheromones and Animal Behavior: Chemical Signals and Signatures. Second. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2014.
  28. 28. McGlone JJ, Morrow JL. Reduction of pig agonistic behavior by androstenone. J Anim Sci. 1988;66: 880–884. pmid:3378947
  29. 29. Guy JH, Burns SE, Barker JM, Edwards SA. Reducing post-mixing aggression and skin lesions in weaned pigs by application of a synthetic maternal pheromone. Anim Welf. 2009;18: 249–255.
  30. 30. De Waal FBM. Primates—A natural heritage of conflict resolution. Science (80-). 2000;289: 586–590. pmid:10915614
  31. 31. Pellegrini AD. The roles of aggressive and affiliative behaviors in resource control: A behavioral ecological perspective. Dev Rev. 2008;28: 461–487.
  32. 32. Ropartz P. Chemical signals in agonistic and social behavior of rodents. In: Muller-Schwarze D, Mozell MM, editors. Chemical Signals in Vertebrates. New York: Plenum Press; 1977. pp. 169–184.
  33. 33. Taylor DK, Bubier JA, Silva KA, Sundberg JP. Development, Structure, and Keratin Expression in C57BL/6J Mouse Eccrine Glands. Vet Pathol. 2012;49: 146–154. pmid:22135020
  34. 34. Ropartz P. Mise en evidence du role d’une secretion odorante des glandes sudoripares dans la regulation de l’activite locomotrice chez la Souris. CR Acad Sc Paris. 1966;263: 525–528.
  35. 35. Lindzey G, Winston H, Manosevitz M. Social Dominance in Inbred Mouse Strains. Nature. 1961;191: 474–476. pmid:13762409
  36. 36. Harvey S, Jemiolo B, Novotny M. Pattern of Volatile Compounds in Dominant and Subordinate Male Mouse Urine. J Chem Ecol. 1989;15: 2061–2072. pmid:24272296
  37. 37. Eichstaedt KE, Kovatch K, Maroof DA. A less conservative method to adjust for familywise error rate in neuropsychological research: The Holm’s sequential Bonferroni procedure. NeuroRehabilitation. 2013;32: 693–696. pmid:23648625
  38. 38. Festing M. Inbred Strains of Mice [Internet]. 1998. Available: http://www.informatics.jax.org/inbred_strains/mouse/STRAINS.shtml
  39. 39. Mouse Ethogram [Internet]. Available: mousebehavior.org
  40. 40. Grant EC, Mackintosh JH. A comparison of the social postures of some common laboratory rodents. Behaviour. 1963;21: 246–259.
  41. 41. van Abeelen JHF. Mouse mutants studied by means of ethological methods. Genetica. 1963;34: 79–94.
  42. 42. Lidster K, Owen K, Browne WJ, Prescott MJ. Cage aggression in group-housed laboratory male mice: an international data crowdsourcing project. Sci Rep. 2019;9: 1–12. pmid:30626917
  43. 43. Bisazza A. Social organization and territorial behaviour in three strains of mice. Bolletino di Zool. 1981;48: 157–167.
  44. 44. Moy SS, Nadler JJ, Perez A, Barbaro RP, Johns JM, Magnuson TR, et al. Sociability and preference for social novelty in five inbred strains: An approach to assess autistic-like behavior in mice. Genes, Brain Behav. 2004;3: 287–302. pmid:15344922
  45. 45. Moy SS, Nadler JJ, Young NB, Perez A, Holloway LP, Barbaro RP, et al. Mouse behavioral tasks relevant to autism: Phenotypes of 10 inbred strains. Behav Brain Res. 2007;176: 4–20. pmid:16971002
  46. 46. Rendon NM, Soini HA, Scotti MAL, Weigel ER, Novotny M V., Demas GE. Photoperiod and aggression induce changes in ventral gland compounds exclusively in male Siberian hamsters. Horm Behav. 2016;81: 1–11. pmid:26944610
  47. 47. Wynne-Edwards KE, Surov AV, Telitzina AY. Field studies of chemical signalling: direct observations of dwarf hamsters (Phodopus) in soviet Asia. Chemical signals in vertebrates. 6th ed. Springer; 1992. pp. 485–491.
  48. 48. Gaskill BN, Karas AZ, Garner JP, Pritchett-Corning KR. Nest building as an indicator of health and welfare in laboratory mice. J Vis Exp. 2013;180: 51012. pmid:24429701
  49. 49. Novotny M V, Jemiolo B, Wiesler D, Ma W, Harvey S, Xu F, et al. A unique urinary constituent, 6-hydroxy-6-methyl-3-heptanone, is a pheromone that accelerates puberty in female mice. Chem Biol. 1999;6: 377–383. pmid:10375540
  50. 50. Novotny M, Harvey S, Jemiolo B. Chemistry of male dominance in the house mouse, Mus domesticus. Experientia. 1990;46: 109–113. pmid:2298278
  51. 51. Howerton CL, Garner JP, Mench JA. Effects of a running wheel-igloo enrichment on aggression, hierarchy linearity, and stereotypy in group-housed male CD-1 (ICR) mice. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;115: 90–103.
  52. 52. Wang F, Kessels HW, Hu H. The mouse that roared: Neural mechanisms of social hierarchy The mouse that roared: neural mechanisms of social hierarchy. 2014;37. pmid:25160682
  53. 53. Varholick JA, Bailoo JD, Palme R, Würbel H. Phenotypic variability between Social Dominance Ranks in laboratory mice. Sci Rep. 2018;8: 1–11. pmid:29311619
  54. 54. Landis SC. Development of muscarinic receptors and regulation of secretory responsiveness in rodent sweat glands. 1999;64: 381–385.
  55. 55. Delgado-Povedano MM, Calderón-Santiago M, Luque de Castro MD, Priego-Capote F. Metabolomics analysis of human sweat collected after moderate exercise. Talanta. 2018;177: 47–65. pmid:29108583
  56. 56. Fox PC, Atkinson JC, Macynski AA, Wolff A, Kung DS, Valdez IH, et al. Pilocarpine Treatment of Salivary Gland Hypofunction and Dry Mouth (Xerostomia). Arch Intern Med. 1991;151: 1149–1152. pmid:2043017
  57. 57. Roberts SA, Davidson AJ, Simpson DM, Robertson DH, Armstrong SD, McLean L, et al. Darcin: a male pheromone that stimulates female memory and sexual attraction to an individual male’s odour. BMC Biol. 2010;8: 75. pmid:20525243
  58. 58. Kwak J, Grigsby CC, Rizki MM, Preti G, Köksal M, Josue J, et al. Differential binding between volatile ligands and major urinary proteins due to genetic variation in mice. Physiol Behav. 2012;107: 112–120. pmid:22728785
  59. 59. Cheetham SA, Smith AL, Armstrong SD, Beynon RJ, Hurst JL. Limited variation in the major urinary proteins of laboratory mice. Physiol Behav. 2009;96: 253–261. pmid:18973768
  60. 60. Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL. Principles of Animal Communication. 2nd ed. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates; 2011.
  61. 61. Duyck K, DuTell V, Ma L, Paulson A, Yu CR. Pronounced strain-specific chemosensory receptor gene expression in the mouse vomeronasal organ. BMC Genomics. 2017;18: 1–16. pmid:28049423
  62. 62. Kaur AW, Spehr M, Kuo T-H, Dey S, Cichy A, Logan DW, et al. Murine Pheromone Proteins Constitute a Context-Dependent Combinatorial Code Governing Multiple Social Behaviors. Cell. 2014;157: 676–688. pmid:24766811
  63. 63. du Sert NP, Hurst V, Ahluwalia A, Alam S, Avey MT, Baker M, et al. The arrive guidelines 2.0: Updated guidelines for reporting animal research. PLoS Biol. 2020;18: 1–12. pmid:32663219
  64. 64. North American Health Reports by Strain [Internet]. Available: https://www.criver.com/products-services/research-models-services/health-reports/north-america/north-american-health-reports-strain?region=3611
  65. 65. Southwick CH, Clark LH. Interstrain differences in aggressive behavior and exploratory activity of inbred mice. Commun Behav Biol Part A. 1968;1: 49–59.
  66. 66. Sorge RE, Martin LJ, Isbester KA, Sotocinal SG, Rosen S, Tuttle AH, et al. Olfactory exposure to males, including men, causes stress and related analgesia in rodents. Nat Methods. 2014;11. pmid:24524129
  67. 67. Jirkof P, Fleischmann T, Cesarovic N, Rettich A, Vogel J, Arras M. Assessment of postsurgical distress and pain in laboratory mice by nest complexity scoring. Lab Anim. 2013;47: 153–161. pmid:23563122
  68. 68. Soini HA, Bruce K, Klouckova I, Brereton RG, Penn DJ, Novotny M V. In-situ surface sampling of biological objects and preconcentration of their volatiles for chromatographic analysis. Anal Chem. 2006;78: 7161–7168. pmid:17037916
  69. 69. Penn DJ, Oberzaucher E, Grammer K, Fischer G, Soini HA, Wiesler D, et al. Individual and gender fingerprints in human body odour. J R Soc/Interface. 2007;4: 331–340. pmid:17251141
  70. 70. Klar J, Hisatsune C, Baig SM, Tariq M, Johansson ACV, Rasool M, et al. Abolished InsP3R2 function inhibits sweat secretion in both humans and mice. J Clin Invest. 2014;124: 4773–4780. pmid:25329695
  71. 71. Vilches JJ, Ceballos D, Verdú E, Navarro X. Changes in mouse sudomotor function and sweat gland innervation with ageing. Auton Neurosci Basic Clin. 2002;95: 80–87. pmid:11871787
  72. 72. Soini HA, Klouckova I, Wiesler D, Oberzaucher E, Grammer K, Dixon SJ, et al. Analysis of volatile organic compounds in human saliva by a static sorptive extraction method and gas chromatography-mass spectrometry. J Chem Ecol. 2010;36: 1035–1042. pmid:20809147
  73. 73. Soini HA, Wiesler D, Koyama S, Feron C, Baudoin C, Novotny M V. Comparison of urinary scents of two related mouse species, Mus spicilegus and Mus domesticus. J Chem Ecol. 2009;35: 580–589. pmid:19390894
  74. 74. Nelson AC, Cunningham CB, Ruff JS, Potts WK. Protein pheromone expression levels predict and respond to the formation of social dominance networks. J Evol Biol. 2015;28: 1213–1224. pmid:25867293
  75. 75. Jemiolo B, Alberts J, Sochinski-Wiggins S, Harvey S, Novotny M. Behavioural and endocrine responses of female mice to synthetic analogues of volatile compounds in male urine. Anim Behav. 1985;33: 1114–1118.
  76. 76. Whittaker DJ, Rosvall KA, Slowinski SP, Soini HA, Novotny M V., Ketterson ED. Songbird chemical signals reflect uropygial gland androgen sensitivity and predict aggression: implications for the role of the periphery in chemosignaling. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sensory, Neural, Behav Physiol. 2018;204: 5–15. pmid:29063285
  77. 77. Tallent BR, Law LM, Rowe RK, Lifshitz J. Partial cage division significantly reduces aggressive behavior in male laboratory mice. Lab Anim. 2018; pmid:29409371
  78. 78. Hess SE, Rohr S, Dufour BD, Gaskill BN, Pajor EA, Garner JP. Home improvement: C57BL/6J mice given more naturalistic nesting materials build better nests. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci. 2008;47: 25–31. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2687128&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract pmid:19049249
  79. 79. Landau HG. On dominance relations and the structure of animal societies. Bull Math Biophys. 1951;13: 1–19.
  80. 80. Gosselin RD. Guidelines on statistics for researchers using laboratory animals: the essentials. Lab Anim. 2019;53: 28–42. pmid:29954248