Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Mothers’ neural response to valenced infant interactions predicts postpartum depression and anxiety

  • Megan Kate Finnegan ,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    mkfinne2@illinois.edu

    Affiliations Psychology Department, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, United States of America, Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, United States of America

  • Stephanie Kane,

    Roles Data curation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychological Science, University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, AR, United States of America

  • Wendy Heller,

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Psychology Department, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, United States of America, Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, United States of America

  • Heidemarie Laurent

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Psychology Department, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, United States of America, Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, United States of America

Abstract

It is currently unknown whether differences in neural responsiveness to infant cues observed in postpartum affective disturbance are specific to depression/anxiety or are better attributed to a common component of internalizing distress. It is also unknown whether differences in mothers’ brain response can be accounted for by effects of past episodes, or if current neural processing of her child may serve as a risk factor for development of future symptoms. Twenty-four mothers from a community-based sample participated in an fMRI session viewing their 3-month- old infant during tasks evoking positive or negative emotion. They were tracked across the ensuing 15 months to monitor changes in affective symptoms. Past and current episodes of depression and anxiety, as well as future symptoms, were used to predict differences in mothers’ hemodynamic response to their infant in positive compared to negative emotion contexts. Lower relative activation in largely overlapping brain regions involving frontal lobe structures to own infant positive vs. negative emotion was associated with concurrent (3-month) depression diagnosis and prospective (3–18 month) depression and anxiety symptoms. There was little evidence for impacts of past psychopathology (more limited effect of past anxiety and nonsignificant effect of past depression). Results suggest biased maternal processing of infant emotions during postpartum depression and anxiety is largely accounted for by a shared source of variance (internalizing distress). Furthermore, differential maternal responsiveness to her infant’s emotional cues is specifically associated with the perpetuation of postpartum symptoms, as opposed to more general phenotypic or scarring effects of past psychopathology.

1. Introduction

The transition to motherhood represents a vulnerable time marked by increasing prevalence of affective symptomatology [13]. Though demographic and contextual risk factors for peripartum depression are known, the proximal processes driving symptoms remain unclear. In particular, how current responding to affective cues in the social environment relates to future depression or anxiety remains largely unexplored. This study seeks to understand how differences in mothers’ neural responsiveness to their infants relate to postpartum affective symptoms, and whether such differences represent (a) effects of past psychopathology versus propensity to symptoms going forward, and (b) effects specific to depression or anxiety versus a common internalizing distress component that gives rise to both of these syndromes.

Affective neuroscience has largely based conclusions about brain bases of psychopathology on responses to stimuli with standardization and clearly defined valence (i.e., [46]), yet whose ecological validity is limited. While these may provide insight into the fundamental mechanisms underlying emotion, the context of personally meaningful images and sounds can elucidate how psychological distress manifests in ways relevant for daily function. That is, to understand how a new mother navigates more positively or negatively valenced situations and how that relates to affective psychopathology, it makes sense to incorporate stimuli closer to her everyday experience. Guided by this consideration, the present study examines neural response to salient affective stimuli in the mother’s social environment—i.e., her own infant in emotion-eliciting situations. Acknowledging that such neural responses both feed into and result from affective symptoms, we investigate both potential phenotypic/scarring effects of past psychopathology and prospective relations between early postpartum brain function and future symptoms.

Previous work on normative maternal brain response has identified several regions exhibiting preferential response to a mother’s own child across emotional contexts. A meta-analysis by Rigo and colleagues [7] revealed preferential activity for their own compared to other infants subcortically in the basal ganglia, thalamus, and amygdala; and cortically in the insula and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). These were predominately left lateralized, which the authors suggested reflects the socially rewarding experience of one’s own child. Indeed, reward-inducing positive emotions may be a critical component of innate mammalian parenting behaviors [8]. These same regions show reduced activity during reward processing when individuals are depressed [9], suggesting a potential biological mechanism for the caregiving behavioral deficits observed with peripartum depression (such as impaired sensitivity to their infant’s emotional cues; [10]). A systematic review of neurofunctional changes of postpartum depression observed substantial overlap with major depressive disorder (MDD) [11], strengthening the idea that depression may stem from and/or perpetuate a disruption in the rewarding aspects of mother-child interactions. This could manifest as blunted processing of her infant’s cues in emotionally positive relative to negative contexts, a possibility supported by previous findings of reduced activation to own infant joy relative to distress faces associated with concurrent depressive symptoms [12]. However, the cross-sectional design of that study cannot answer whether biased processing of infant emotions prospectively predicts postpartum psychopathology. It is also unclear how altered neural response may relate to history of affective disturbance. Behaviorally, previous episodes of affective disorders have been identified as an important predictor of postpartum depression [13] and anxiety [14], and there is evidence for both overlapping and distinct associations between past/current/future depression and affect regulation strategies [15]. However, research disentangling contributions of past vs. current psychopathology to maternal brain response is lacking.

Another important question is whether brain differences attributed to postpartum depression are syndrome-specific or represent a general internalizing distress shared with anxiety. In fact, comorbidity with anxiety appears to be more frequent than unipolar depression alone [16]. The small body of research investigating peripartum anxiety has yielded fragmentary evidence, with one study showing no state anxiety-related difference in brain response to their own compared to other infants’ negative emotions [17], and another study finding reduced amygdala response to own and unfamiliar infant positive emotion with increasing trait anxiety only among mothers who were not depressed [18]. Underlining the potential overlap [19], documented an association between IFG and insula activation to negative emotion faces and both anxiety and depression symptoms at different postpartum times. These findings are consistent with previous anxiety research in non-peripartum populations showing differences in processing valenced stimuli in some of the same regions implicated in both normative maternal response and depression such as the insula, prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate, and amygdala [2022]. However, it remains unclear how maternal brain response to own infant across differing emotion contexts may uniquely contribute to anxiety in the postpartum.

The current study aims to elucidate how maternal brain responses to their infants’ socio-emotional cues relate to past, current, and future depression and anxiety. We address the following questions: (1) How does maternal neural response to their infant in positive vs. negative emotion situations relate to depression and/or anxiety symptoms in the ensuing 15 months? (2) Are there unique effects associated with current vs. past depression and/or anxiety diagnoses? and (3) Are there unique effects associated with postpartum depression vs. anxiety symptoms? Based on the available literature, we predicted relatively greater neural activation to positive compared to negative infant emotion situations would predict lower levels of postpartum affective symptoms. Lacking prior work probing temporal (past/current) and syndromal (depression/anxiety) specificity in this population, the latter questions were approached in a more exploratory manner. These questions were tested in a community sample of mothers assessed longitudinally. This dataset was originally described in [23] where it was used to explore the association of dispositional mindfulness with preferential neural activity to infant affect. Maternal brain response to videos of their infant during arm-restraint and peek-a-boo interactions was measured at the initial (3-month) assessment, as were past and current affective disorder diagnoses. Self-reported symptoms at each assessment provided an estimate of overall depression and anxiety symptom severity across 3–18 months postpartum.

2. Methods and materials

2.1. Participants and procedures

Results reported here are based on a subset of women from a larger study examining mother-infant stress regulation. Women were recruited from community agencies serving low-income women in the Pacific Northwest. Twenty-five mothers agreed to participate in the optional neuroimaging component, with 24 included in the final analysis (see Table 1). One mother was dropped from the analysis due to excessive head movement. Mothers were included if they could speak English, had an infant younger than 12 weeks-old, did not present with psychotic symptoms on clinical interview, and planned to remain in the region until the infant was 18 months old. Inclusion in the neuroimaging portion was based on absence of MRI contraindications; all women meeting criteria (See S1 File for more information on changes made in the initial recruitment strategies) were offered participation. Mothers were scanned at the initial 3-month postpartum assessment (T1), and returned for assessments at 6, 12, and 18-months postpartum (T2, T3, and T4). Of the 25 original mothers, all returned for the 6-month assessment, 19 returned at 12-months and 16 returned at 18-months. This dataset is also described in [23] and [24]. None of the mothers reported using psychotropic medication either during pregnancy or at the time of the scan (T1 assessment). One mother reported taking antidepressant medication at T2 only. Mothers who completed all assessments did not significantly differ from those who dropped out in demographic or psychological measures tested (See S1 File). The study was approved by the University of Oregon Institutional Review Board, and participants gave written informed consent.

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic information for mothers included in final analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250487.t001

2.2. Psychological data

2.2.1. Postpartum symptoms.

Participants completed the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CESD) [25] and the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) [26] at every assessment. One participant did not complete the T3, but returned for the T4 assessment (See S1 File for demographic comparisons). The CESD captures general symptoms of depression with an emphasis on affective components [25], and the BAI measures both somatic and cognitive symptoms of anxiety [27]. Hierarchical linear modeling was used to estimate mothers’ postpartum symptom trajectories in the presence of missing data (See Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Psychopathology data for mothers included in final analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250487.t002

2.2.2. Past/current diagnoses.

The presence of past (prior to T1) and current (at T1) depression and anxiety disorders were coded using the research version of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) [28]. Six met criteria for major depressive disorder (2 current, 4 past) and six met criteria for an anxiety disorder (4 current, 2 past). Although exact age of onset for past depression or anxiety was not gathered, all mothers meeting criteria for past depressive episodes and/or anxiety disorders reported their worst episode occurring prior to pregnancy. Depression history (Dep-Hx), depression current diagnosis (Dep-Dx), anxiety history (Anx-Hx), and anxiety current diagnosis (Anx-Dx) were used as covariates in subsequent regression analyses.

2.3. fMRI task

Participants engaged in two separate block-designed paradigms repeated for two runs each. The first involved viewing infants during emotion-eliciting tasks, and the second (not reported here) involved viewing and/or labeling emotionally expressive faces.

Before scanning, mothers were guided in arm restraint (designed to elicit negative emotion; [29]) and peekaboo (designed to elicit positive emotion; [30]) interactions during a video recorded home visit. Recordings were framed to center the infant’s face and zoomed to show no body parts below the neck. Videos contained both audio and visual streams. The 15s segments displaying maximal positive (peekaboo) and negative (arm restraint) emotions were selected for presentation through reference to the Baby FACS [31] emotional expression coding framework.

Mothers were verbally instructed to “watch and respond as you would naturally” to videos of their own or unknown infants. The unknown infant (a pilot drawn from the same overall study sample) displayed to all participants was matched for age and completed the same arm-restraint and peekaboo tasks. The stranger’s infant was not matched for any other demographic features such as ethnicity or gender. Each 7.5-min run comprised 6 blocks with five 15s trial conditions: four infant videos (own/other infant positive/negative tasks, abbreviated InfOwnPos, InfOwnNeg, InfOtherPos, InfOtherNeg) and a black screen (Rest). Trials were fully randomized within blocks, with each block including one instance of each trial type (See Fig 1). Presentation® software [32] was used to display stimuli.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Overview of infant viewing task.

Note that the mother’s infant and a stranger’s age-matched infant were used in each of the videos represented in this diagram as infant caricatures.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250487.g001

2.4. Valence of infant videos

As originally reported in Laurent, Wright, and Finnegan [23], immediately after scanning mothers re-watched videos and provided valence ratings from -100 (maximum negative) to +100 (maximum positive) with 0 as the neutral point, and intensity ratings from 0 to +100 (maximum intensity).

Although arm-restraint did not on average evoke strong negatively valenced emotions in mothers (μ = 22.39, 95% CI = [0.46, 44.33]) or perceptions of negative infant emotions (μ = 3.18, 95% CI = [-21.51, 27.87]), peekaboo videos evoked significantly positive valence maternal emotions (μ = 85.86, 95% CI = [74.40, 97.35]) and perceptions of positive infant emotions (μ = 69.42, 95% CI = [53.62, 85.21]). There was a clear difference in relative valence between conditions: mothers t(22) = 5.44, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 1.57 (95% CI .90–2.24); infants t(21) = 4.20, p < .001, d = 1.41 (95% CI 0.74–2.07). Thus, contrasts represent difference in emotional valence processing, with one task relatively more positive and the other more negative. Comparisons across mother/infant intensity ratings showed no significant differences between stimuli.

2.5. Image acquisition

All imaging occurred during a single session at the University of Oregon Robert and Beverly Lewis Center for Neuroimaging using a 3T Siemens Allegra 3 magnet and standard 32-channel phase array birdcage coil. Following a shimming routine to optimize signal-to-noise ratio, a fast localizer scan (FISP), Siemens Autoalign routine, field mapping, functional runs, and anatomical scan were completed.

2.5.1. Functional.

T2*-weighted gradient echo sequence, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, 32 contiguous slices acquired ascending and interleaved, thickness = 4 mm, 64 × 64 voxel matrix; 226 volumes per run.

2.5.2. Anatomical.

T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE sequence, TI = 1100 ms, TR = 2500 ms, TE = 3.41 ms, flip angle = 7°, 176 sagittal slices 1.0 mm thick, 256 × 256 voxel matrix, FOV = 256 mm.

2.6. Preprocessing

Functional images were preprocessed using SPM12 (v.6906) [33] with MATLAB 2018a running on Windows 10. If not otherwise stated, default values or recommended values provided by the batch interface were used. First, slice-timing correction interpolated values to the midpoint collected slice. Realignment used a 2-pass procedure, calculating the mean image and realigning images to the mean. A 2mm/2° framewise displacement threshold was established with subjects discarded if > 40% of volumes in any run exceeded threshold. Images were unwarped to correct for image distortion caused by gradient inhomogeneities and a mean image calculated. Because subjects were allowed to move between scans, the field map was not used, and no phase correction was applied. The anatomical MPRAGE image was then coregistered to the mean image from unwarping. A set of warps calculated using the anatomical image was applied to functional images to map them into MNI space. Finally, an 8mm FWHM Gaussian smoothing kernel was applied to all images.

2.7. fMRI analysis

2.7.1. Main effects.

Functional data were analyzed in a 2-level process. For each subject a General Linear Model (GLM) was estimated at each voxel using restricted maximum likelihoods (ReML). Stimuli as specified above—InfOwnPos, InfOwnNeg, InfOtherPos, InfOtherNeg, and Rest—were modeled using the SPM12 canonical hemodynamic response function. Variations in hemodynamic responses were accounted for with time and spatial dispersion derivatives. Regressors of no interest included a global approximate AR(1) autocorrelation model to account for serial correlations, and scanner drift was fitted with a Discrete Cosine Transform basis (128s cut-off). Head motion was modeled using 6 movement estimation parameters from realignment, and high motion volumes deweighted with indicator variables for volumes exceeding 2mm/2° framewise displacement. All runs from both tasks were included in a single model with regressors from the emotional faces task treated as regressors of no interest.

Contrasts were created for each participant with regressor coefficients averaged across runs. Contrast images were passed to a 1-sample t-test to examine group-level whole-brain voxelwise effects. Statistical maps were thresholded with clusterwise inference using a cluster-defining threshold (CDT) of p < .001 (uncorrected) and FWE-corrected cluster threshold of p < .05, which has demonstrated reasonable control for false positives [34]. See S1 File for a list of all main effects tested and results. Clusters were labeled using the AAL atlas [35]. To address the aim of identifying psychopathology-related differences in the way a mother responds to her own infant in more positive versus negative contexts, the main effect of interest was (InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg).

2.7.2. Association with affective psychopathology.

To examine how (InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg) related to anxiety and depression, individual-level contrasts were submitted to ReML multiple regression analysis using the following sets of predictors, tested in separate models: 1. CESD[T1-T4], 2. BAI[T1-T4], 3. Dep-Hx, Dep-Dx, 4. Anx-Hx, Anx-Dx. Depression and anxiety terms were also included in joint models, and potential “dosage” effects explored by testing summed scores of past and current diagnoses. Results were consistent with the simplified models and are not reported in detail here (see S1 File). All models included an intercept term, and continuous psychological covariates were mean-centered. Contrasts tested significance of each model regression weight in both positive and negative directions. Again, images were thresholded with a CDT of p < .001 (uncorrected) and cluster threshold of p < .05 (FWE-corrected).

A post hoc exploratory analysis was conducted to examine how depression and anxiety relate to positive and negative contexts individually. Regression coefficients (β values) for InfOwnPos and InfOwnNeg were extracted for each subject from clusters determined by negative relationships of (InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg) with BAI[T1-4] and CESD[T1-4]. The Marsbar toolbox (v.0.44) [36] was used to extract average β values, producing a representative value for each subject-cluster. These were plotted against the psychological variable that produced the clusters to visually inspect direction of change. BAI[T1-T4] and CESD[T1-T4] were chosen for illustration because they showed extensive relations with maternal responsiveness.

3. Results

3.1. Main effects

The contrast of (InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg) did not demonstrate significant clusters after correction (see S1 File for all main effects).

3.2. Associations with depression

Mothers who later reported greater depression symptoms showed predominantly lower relative activation to their own infant in positive versus negative contexts (negative association of InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg with CESD[T1-T4]), with reductions most evident in the right superior frontal gyrus and some clusters overlapping the parietal lobe and caudal portions of the frontal lobe (Fig 2). See Table 3 for an abbreviated summary of cluster labels and S1 File for an expanded list.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Regions of reduced activation to positive versus negative infant emotion tasks with increasing markers of depression.

(i.e. Negative associations of (InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg) with CESD[T1-T4] & Dep-Dx. Anatomical labels added for reference are taken from the AAL atlas, but do not represent an exhaustive list of cluster extent. Activation overlaid on smoothed (8mm Gaussian kernel) average of subject anatomical images. Past episodes of depression did not relate to differential responding for this contrast.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250487.g002

thumbnail
Table 3. Model of estimated future postpartum depression.

Brain regions showing differences in maternal activation to own infant in positive versus negative emotion tasks (InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg) related to increasing postpartum depression symptoms (CESD[T1-4]–estimated postpartum average depression).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250487.t003

Current depression also related to widespread reductions in differential response (negative association of InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg with Dep-Dx). As shown in Table 4 and Fig 2, this expanded over the right (dorsolateral) superior, middle, and inferior frontal gyri, left inferior and middle temporal lobe, and bilateral angular gyri. A history of depressive episodes did not independently impact relative (InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg) neural response.

thumbnail
Table 4. Model of current and past depressive disorders.

Brain regions showing differences in maternal activation to own infant in positive versus negative emotion tasks (InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg) related to current diagnosis of depression.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250487.t004

3.3. Associations with anxiety

Greater subsequent or “future” anxiety also related to extensive decreases in relative neural activation to positive versus negative contexts (InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg). This included bilateral postcentral gyri and adjacent fronto-parietal areas, left middle temporal and right parahippocampal gyri, and right superior and middle frontal gyri (Fig 3). See Table 5 and S1 File for cluster labels.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Regions of reduced activation to positive versus negative infant emotion tasks with increasing markers of anxiety.

(i.e. Negative associations of (InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg) with BAI[T1-T4], Anx-Hx, & Anx-Dx). Anatomical labels added for reference are taken from the AAL atlas, but do not represent an exhaustive list of cluster extent. Activation overlaid on smoothed (8mm Gaussian kernel) average of subject anatomical images.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250487.g003

thumbnail
Table 5. Model of estimated future postpartum anxiety.

Brain regions showing differences in maternal activation to own infant in positive versus negative emotion tasks (InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg) related to increasing postpartum anxiety symptoms (BAI[T1-4]–estimated postpartum average anxiety).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250487.t005

Consistent with the above, both current and past anxiety disorders were associated with an overall reduction in mothers’ relative neural response (Table 6, Fig 3). Past anxiety predicted reductions in the right frontal areas with superior, middle and inferior frontal gyri implicated. Current anxiety exhibited more widespread effects than anxiety history and was further associated with reduced differential responding in parietal and temporal regions, along with left inferior frontal and bilateral middle frontal gyri.

thumbnail
Table 6. Model of current and past anxiety disorders.

Brain regions showing differences in maternal activation to own infant in positive versus negative emotion tasks (InfOwnPos > InfOwnNeg) related to current diagnosis or history of anxiety disorders.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250487.t006

3.4. Syndromal specificity

Extensive overlap between regions of differential activation associated with depression and anxiety suggests effects are best attributed to a common internalizing distress component, rather than distinct syndromal components. To further probe specificity, a model including both prospective depression and anxiety symptoms was estimated (see S1 File). Lower differential activation was associated with BAI[T1-4] only, suggesting effects related to postpartum depression were driven by shared variance with anxiety. The reduction (over 50%) in cluster extent further suggests a substantial proportion of postpartum anxiety effects were driven by shared variance with depression. Given the collinearity of these predictors (Fig 4) and absence of regularization, results should be interpreted with caution. Future work either employing factor analytic models dissociating the shared and unique variance of depression/anxiety symptoms or construction of clear group delineations of anxiety-only and depression-only may help clarify this. In addition, regression models with mean subject-level contrast values (InfOwnPos>InfOwnNeg) from the clusters identified in the above analysis tested whether T1 maternal brain response predicted T4 depression or anxiety over and above T1-T4 maternal stress (life events and parenting stress). Maternal brain response measures continued to significantly predict later symptoms with no meaningful reduction in effect size when controlling for stress, supporting independent prospective effects of infant-directed brain response on maternal psychopathology.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Associations between markers of psychopathology: Past, present, and future depression and anxiety.

Dep Hx = history of depressive disorder based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID); Dep Dx = current depressive disorder; Anx Hx = anxiety disorder history; Anx Dx = current anxiety disorder; CESD[T1-4] = estimated mean CESD score across all assessment time points (i.e. Estimated Future Depression Symptom Severity); BAI[T1-4] = estimated mean BAI score (i.e. Estimated Future Anxiety Severity). Significance delineated at uncorrected thresholds. (A) Denotes Pearson’s correlation between a continuous-continuous pair. All other associations reported (categorical-categorical or categorical-continuous pairs) are reported using Spearman’s rank correlation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250487.g004

3.5. Direction of relationship with anxiety and depression

To probe the source of mothers’ reduced relative neural activation with increasing affective psychopathology, exploratory plots of the mean InfOwnPos and InfOwnNeg β values for each cluster against BAI[T1-T4] and CESD[T1-T4] were created. These revealed general upward trends of both coefficients with increasing symptoms across most clusters, but this was especially pronounced for the negative situation. Fig 5 provides a representative plot.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Plots of the subject-level average regression coefficient predicting maternal neural response to own infant positive and negative emotion tasks.

One subject with an elevated CESD[T1-T4] and two with elevated BAI[T1-T4] scores appeared to deviate from the general trend of their less symptomatic peers. The plots are displayed with these potential outliers removed. A.) shows these values plotted against participant anxiety scores (BAI). This cluster was defined from the regression against BAI and is a large area covering primarily parietal lobe structures. The upper plot is the difference between these two mean coefficients for each subject and is reflective of the general decreases observed in the whole-brain analysis. B.) shows a similar trend when coefficients are plotted against depression scores (CESD). This cluster was defined from the regression against CESD and is located mostly within the right dorsolateral superior frontal gyrus.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250487.g005

4. Discussion

The current findings point to a relation between maternal brain response–specifically, decreasing relative activation to positive compared to negative infant emotion situations–and a shared internalizing component of postpartum symptoms that may fuel ongoing distress. This was related in varying degrees to past, current, and future affective psychopathology; the strongest effects involved prospective associations with anxiety symptoms and concurrent associations with diagnosed depression, alongside more limited effects of past anxiety and nonsignificant effects of past depression. This presents a picture in which postpartum depression plays a role in the mother’s ability to differentially process the emotional cues of her child, with anxiety having a more prominent bidirectional longitudinal relationship to brain response. Despite some regions of unique covariance, largely overlapping clusters support the idea that biased brain responding is involved in a common internalizing symptom dimension. At the same time, the preponderance of concurrent/prospective psychopathology effects implicate disruptions in maternal response to their infants specifically in the perpetuation of postpartum symptoms, as opposed to more general phenotypic or scarring effects. However, a more careful examination of unique components of anxiety and depression may yield evidence for differential associations with outcomes and patterns of brain activity (see [37]).

Exploratory plots of regression coefficients within significant clusters suggested differences were due to steeper increases in response to the negative situations with increasing affective symptoms. It appears framing maternal postpartum depression (or anxiety) as primarily a deficit in reward responsiveness is overly simplistic, and preferential sensitization to negative emotional contexts with their infants may better capture the nature of response imbalance. The current analysis does not allow us to discern whether this represents a homogenization (response to positive and negative context becoming more alike) or an exaggeration of differential response. Further investigation to clarify the nature of this response bias, as well as when it emerges and how it may relate to mother/infant cognitions and behaviors, is needed. For example, a maternal response bias toward more negative/stressful emotion situations with her infant may heighten mutual physiological dysregulation [38, 39] while impairing attunement to infant emotional needs and behavioral synchrony that would help repair mother/infant wellbeing [40]. Over time, repeated failures in behavioral synchrony in turn shape the child’s risk for emotion dysregulation and disorder, in part through changes in the child’s neural processing of social stress/distress [41, 42]. As noted by other researchers, such shifts in the mother’s neural processing—perhaps based on her own history of nonoptimal caregiving—may contribute to disruptions in maternal reflective functioning and mother-infant attachment that put the child at further risk of psychological difficulties later in life [43, 44]. Post hoc tests exploring the role of neural responsivity to her own infant compared to another infant when controlling for valence (See S1 File sections 5.D. and 5.E) revealed associations with internalizing distress in similar regions, supporting the idea that ongoing postpartum depression and anxiety are associated with specific deficits in maternal response bias to her own child (as opposed to general disfunction in socio-emotional processes). Notably, these follow-up analyses also contained clusters not found in the contrasts isolating differences in valance functioning, warranting future work disentangling the relationship between affective distress and specific components of the maternal response pattern. Thus, the currently detected associations should be used to guide further exploration of upstream (maternal response bias) mechanisms that may contribute to a broader chain of dyadic dysregulation and psychopathology transmission.

Regions of lower relative activation in distressed mothers comprised a number of prefrontal areas, notably the left medial and right medial orbital sections of the superior frontal gyrus (SFG). This area is hypothesized to coordinate task set switching [45], a cognitive function known to be impaired in unipolar depression [46] and thought to reflect inability to inhibit processing of prior information [47]. The medial SFG has also been observed to coordinate activity with other frontal lobe areas and limbic structures during successful emotion regulation [48], and the left medial prefrontal cortex in particular shows preferential activity toward sad videos in depressed compared to non-depressed participants [49]. Our paradigm required mothers to switch between viewing positive and negative emotional contexts of infant interaction; as such, the disrupted medial SFG function observed could represent a difficulty in shifting the response repertoire needed to successfully meet her infant’s needs in each context and/or a larger breakdown in emotional regulation of response to her child’s emotions. Future work examining coordinated functional activity may help resolve the role of the SFG in the brain response of a mother to her child.

We also observed reduced relative response in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) related to past, current, and future anxiety and current and future depression. Increased IFG response to negatively valenced facial expressions has been associated with early (within 48 hours of birth) postpartum anxiety symptoms and later (4–6 weeks) postpartum depression symptoms in prior region-of-interest analyses [19]. The explanation offered by these researchers that exaggerated IFG engagement represents an exacerbation of normative postpartum increases in emotional reactivity and/or inability to scale back inhibitory control demands when confronting negatively valenced stimuli may apply to the current results. This could potentially provide a neural basis for the behavioral patterns observed in depressed and anxious mothers interacting with their infants. For depressed mothers, interactions are often characterized by less maternal engagement and emotional availability, or conversely by increased intrusiveness with the infant (with a subset of depressed mothers displaying normative interactions) [50]. Similarly, anxious mothers exhibit increased intrusiveness [51] and impaired sensitivity in responding to infant needs [52]. Which behaviors predominate could be a result of the particular balance of increasing maternal neural response to negative and/or decreasing response to positive cues during infant interactions. Subsequent reactions of the infant (e.g. avoidance of eye contact, reduced emotional expressiveness, less contingent responding [50, 53]) could serve to exacerbate already biased neural processing of infant cues with implications for chronicity of symptoms. This would suggest more far-reaching links between this neural response pattern and both prior and future affective psychopathology. Further work is needed to elucidate the relationship between maternal brain response, mother-infant interactions, and the development of psychopathology in both mother and child. Such avenues could provide a rich source of information to allow pediatricians and other health providers to identify and provide additional resources to promote resilience in dyads that may be at risk for development of psychological disorders.

Consistent with some [18], but not all [12], prior work involving mothers, we did not find associations between anxiety or depression and differential response in the amygdala or insula. We also did not find evidence that affective distress involved differences in neural activity in the thalamus, and minimal evidence for involvement of the caudate and putamen—areas that, together with the amygdala and insula, were implicated in the literature as reward processing regions with preference for one’s own infant that may be impacted by depression [7, 9, 11]. If postpartum depression and/or anxiety primarily represented a deficit in a mother’s ability to process the reward-inducing positive emotions of her infant’s socio-emotional cues as originally hypothesized, we would have expected more prominent changes in differential response to positive vs negative infant cues in these regions. This was not supported by the current results and further strengthens the conclusion that postpartum affective distress is not simply a reduced responsiveness to reward.

It is notable that several regions implicated in this analysis did not fully align with our original hypotheses which identified the basal ganglia, thalamus, amygdala, insula, and inferior frontal gyrus as potential regions impacted. Although some regions, such as the IFG were consistent with prior work, others like the SFG were unexpected. Much of the preexisting literature examining peripartum affective disorders has focused on functional masking or region-of-interest analyses to target hypothesized brain structures (for example, see the work of [5458]). While this has the advantage of increased statistical power, it also means that many of the potential changes in neural activity across the postpartum brain have been left unexamined. This could have driven some of the divergence between the hypothesized brain regions and our results. More work examining whole-brain response may help to elucidate these discrepancies.

Limitations to the current study include a modest sample size of research participants, the somewhat mild of intensity of the stimuli, the low prevalence of clinically significant depression and anxiety (with symptom-level severity predominantly in subclinical ranges), and the potential existence of explanatory confounds (for example, trauma exposure). Although our sample size is typical of this research domain (our sample size is either greater than or comparable to those reported in [12, 5457, 59, 60]), it is well known that the standard error of regression coefficients increases with decreasing sample size. Thus, test statistics derived from small sample designs may exhibit more variability upon replication. This study included several strengths known to increase metrics of reliability, such as the use of a naturalistic stimuli [61], collecting data over two runs (to minimize participant fatigue) [62], and block design with target vs non-target contrasts [61]. While the lower bounds of reliability for task-based fMRI has been purported to be low [63], the degree of improvement provided by design and analysis considerations is still an active area of debate [64] with potential for achieving high levels of reliability. The results of this work represent the first attempt to characterize the relationship between neural response to infants and the developmental course of postpartum depression and anxiety, but future replication will be essential for establishing the most robust predictors of symptom trajectories. We address the implications of the other identified limitations in the discussion that follows.

The current null findings may also be attributable to the relatively mild nature of the emotional tasks with which mothers were presented or the subclinical levels of maternal distress in the majority of cases. Future stimuli could involve a prescreening process for emotional intensity and valence similar to that employed by [65] to ensure only the highest rated samples are chosen. Employing probe measures during the task may facilitate greater engagement with the stimuli. Future investigation could also assess the role of habituation to the stimuli as a source of decreased responsivity through more detailed time series analysis. Clearly, more work involving a range of infant emotional stimuli and maternal clinical presentations will be needed.

Taken in the context of relations among psychopathology indicators in this sample, these findings may point to a particular risk profile in women. In particular, mothers’ concurrent (3-month) depression diagnosis was most strongly associated with prospective (3–18 month) anxiety symptoms, and to a lesser extent with past anxiety diagnosis (See Fig 4). Certain women may show a profile of sensitivity to interpersonal stress or challenge that gives rise to problems with anxiety prior to parenthood, leading to a depressive episode in the early postpartum as the mother struggles to adjust to infant emotional demands, and continuing as a pervasive sense of stress/distress going forward as the child develops. It is important to note that elevated symptoms did not correlate with difference in maternal self-report of their emotional response or assessment of their infant’s emotions. That is, mothers did not necessarily think their baby was more negative in these situations, but rather, their brains responded differently to viewing their babies. This dissociation from conscious appraisal of their infant’s or their own emotions highlights the importance of considering brain measures to detect underlying response biases that may help drive postpartum psychopathology.

It should be noted that this work restricted analysis to the constructs of depression and anxiety, which may overlap with other unexplored sources of the observed bias in neural response. In particular, the role of trauma exposure and trauma symptoms has not been fully separated from neural responses related to disordered affective processing. Traumatic stress exposure is known to affect maternal behavior in both humans and rodent models [6669] and post-traumatic stress disorders are highly comorbid with depression and anxiety [70] (although this was not evident in our sample; see S1 File). Previous work by [71] found mothers with PTSD displayed greater differential response in the entorhinal cortex and caudate to videos of their own toddlers during separation compared to free play. Reduced difference was also noted in the superior frontal gyrus and bilateral superior parietal lobes, both regions in this study that displayed an altered magnitude of differential response (although in the opposite direction) with greater severity of future depression or anxiety. It is possible that trauma exposure may provide a common source of covariance, potentially mediated by physiological changes to homeostatic set points such as changes to HPA stress reactivity [72]. Teasing apart the potential causal mechanisms for our reported observations will be essential for understanding how brain response may influence, or be influenced by, affective symptom trajectories. Future work will also be needed for replication and validation of these findings; however, such neural markers of differences in maternal response to their infants may be useful as additional evidence to aid in identifying mothers at risk for worsening depression and anxiety. Combined with information on other risk factors for postpartum depression, it may be possible to construct a “risk score” that pinpoints mothers most in need of additional support services. Risk scores could in turn allow health providers to maximize resource utilization by targeting early interventions to mothers that are particularly vulnerable.

Our study focused on a small non-clinical cohort with a relatively low incidence of past anxiety and current depression (each comprising about 8% of the sample), and future work should assess whether these observations apply to higher levels of psychological distress. Ideally such work would be complemented with behavioral observations, which may help to interpret the disconnect between maternal self-reported emotions and changes in brain response with depression or anxiety. It will also be important to determine how early identification and intervention on these patterns could impact postpartum symptom trajectories. Although we did not find strong evidence for an association of maternal brain response with history of psychopathology, future work in larger and more diagnostically varied samples could help make finer-grained distinctions regarding effects that depend on the recency, severity, or number of earlier episodes.

Based on the results highlighted here, intervention approaches that help mothers shift attention toward pleasant/potentially rewarding interactions with their infant while lowering the level of perceived threat and/or effortful regulation of response to more difficult/potentially distressing interactions should be explored. Mother-infant interventions that aim to increase maternal savoring of positive emotional experiences and offer tools to regulate negative emotions, such as mindfulness-based childbirth and parenting programs, may prove particularly beneficial (See [73] and [74] for a review of evidence-based interventions for mothers with young children). We provide here some of the first observations that alterations in mothers’ relative brain response to valenced infant interactions predict ongoing affective psychopathology in the postpartum. These findings further suggest biased brain responding represents a prospective risk for postpartum psychopathology (as opposed to a more stable phenotypic pattern) that can best be characterized as internalizing distress (as opposed to depression or anxiety). Such insights may allow us to better identify and remediate psychopathology-perpetuating patterns for the good of both parent and child.

Supporting information

S1 File. Detailed results and models not reported in main manuscript.

Contains full results of main contrasts and results of regression against psychological variables reported in the main manuscript. This also contains additional participant demographics, analysis of post-scan ratings of infant videos, and models not reported in the main manuscript.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250487.s001

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank the families who participated in this study, as well as the Lewis Center for Neuroimaging at the University of Oregon for providing the facilities to conduct this research. We also extend gratitude to Katie Haigler and Saumya Agrawal who’s contributions creating a de-identified and BIDS compliant version of this dataset were indispensible.

References

  1. 1. Ahmed A, Feng C, Bown A, Muhajarine N. Latent trajectory groups of perinatal depressive and anxiety symptoms from pregnancy to early postpartum and their antenatal risk factors. Archives of Women’s Mental Health. 2018; 21(6): 689–698. pmid:29654413
  2. 2. Uguz F, Yakut E, Aydogan S, Bayman M, Gezginc K. Prevalence of mood and anxiety disorders during pregnancy: A case-control study with a large sample size. Psychiatry Res. 2019; 272: 316–318. pmid:30597383
  3. 3. Vesga-Lopez O, Blanco C, Keyes K, Olfson M, Grant B, Hasin D. Psychiatric disorders in pregnant and postpartum women in the United States. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008; 65(7): 805–815. pmid:18606953
  4. 4. Bradley M, Lang J. The international affective digitized sounds (IADS-2): Affective ratings of sounds and instruction manual. Gainesville, FL:; 2007.
  5. 5. Lang P, Bradley M, Cuthbert B. International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Gainesville, FL:; 1997.
  6. 6. Tottenham N, Tanaka J, Leon A, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare T, et al. The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 2009; 168(3): 242–249. pmid:19564050
  7. 7. Rigo P, Kim P, Esposito G, Putnick D, Venuti P, Bornstein M. Specific maternal brain responses to their own child’s face: An fMRI meta-analysis. Developmental Review. 2019; 51: 58–69. pmid:30872887
  8. 8. Kim P, Strathearn L, Swain J. The maternal brain and its plasticity in humans. Horm Behav. 2016; 77: 113–123. pmid:26268151
  9. 9. Zhang W, Chang S, Guo L, Zhang K, Wang L. The neural correlates of reward-related processing in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. J Affect Disord. 2013; 151(2): 531–539. pmid:23856280
  10. 10. Goodman J. Perinatal depression and infant mental health. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing. 2019; 33(3): 217–224. pmid:31227073
  11. 11. Fiorelli M, Aceti F, Marini I, Giacchetti N, Macci E, Tinelli E, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging studies of postpartum depression: An overview. Behavioural neurology. 2015;: 1–7. pmid:26347585
  12. 12. Laurent H, Ablow J. A face a mother could love: Depression-related maternal neural responses to infant emotion faces. Social neuroscience. 2013; 8(3): 228–239. pmid:23330663
  13. 13. Lancaster C, Gold K, Flynn H, Yoo H, Marcus S, Davis M. Risk factors for depressive symptoms during pregnancy: a systematic review. American journal of obstetrics and gynecology. 2010; 202(1): 5–14. pmid:20096252
  14. 14. Dennis C, Falah-Hassani K, Brown H, Vigod S. Identifying women at risk for postpartum anxiety: a prospective population-based study. Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2016; 134(6): 485–493. pmid:27639034
  15. 15. Nelis S, Holmes E, Raes F. Response Styles to Positive Affect and Depression: Concurrent. Cogn Ther Res. 2015; 39: 480–491. pmid:26229213
  16. 16. Wisner K, Sit D, McShea M, Rizzo D, Zoretich R, Hughes C, et al. Onset timing, thoughts of self-harm, and diagnoses in postpartum women with screen-positive depression findings. JAMA psychiatry. 2013; 70(5): 490–498. pmid:23487258
  17. 17. Guo C, Moses-Kolko E, Phillips M, Swain J, Hipwell A. Severity of anxiety moderates the association between neural circuits and maternal behaviors in the postpartum period. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2018; 18(3): 426–436.
  18. 18. Wonch K, de Medeiros C, Barret J, Dudin A, Cunningham W, Hall G, et al. Postpartum depression and brain response to infants: Differential amygdala response and connectivity. Social neuroscience. 2016; 11(6): 600–617. pmid:26680151
  19. 19. Gingnell M, Bannbers E, Moes H, Engman J, Sylvén S, Skalkidou A, et al. Emotion reactivity is increased 4–6 weeks postpartum in healthy women: A longitudinal fMRI study. PloS ONE. 2015; 10(6). pmid:26061879
  20. 20. Bishop S, Duncan J, Lawrence A. State anxiety modulation of the amygdala response to unattended threat-related stimuli. J Neurosci. 2004; 24(46): 10364–10368. pmid:15548650
  21. 21. Fonzo G, Etkin A. Affective neuroimaging in generalized anxiety disorder: An integrated review. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience. 2017; 19(2): 169–179. pmid:28867941
  22. 22. Mochcovitch M, da Rocha Freire R, Garcia R, Nardi A. A systematic review of fMRI studies in generalized anxiety disorder: Evaluating its neural and cognitive basis. J Affect Disord. 2014; 167: 336–342. pmid:25020268
  23. 23. Laurent H, Wright D, Finnegan M. Mindfulness-related differences in neural response to own infant negative versus positive emotion contexts. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2018; 30: 70–76. pmid:29331659
  24. 24. Laurent H. Early calibration of the HPA axis by maternal psychopathology. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017; 78: 177–184. pmid:28212519
  25. 25. Radloff L. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977; 1(3): 385–401.
  26. 26. Beck A, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer R. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988; 56(6): 893–897. pmid:3204199
  27. 27. Nitschke J, Heller W, Imig J, McDonald R, Miller G. Distinguishing dimensions of anxiety and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2001; 25(1): 1–22.
  28. 28. First M, Spitzer R, Gibbon M, Williams J. Structured clinical interview for Axis I DSM-IV disorders. New York:; 1994.
  29. 29. Moscardino U, Axia G. Infants’ responses to arm restraint at 2 and 6 months: A longitudinal study. Infant Behavior and Development. 2006; 29(1): 59–69. pmid:17138262
  30. 30. Montague D, Walker-Andrews A. Peekaboo: A new look at infants’ perception of emotion expressions. Dev Psychol. 2001; 37(6): 826–838. pmid:11699756
  31. 31. Oster H, Rosenstein D. Baby FACS: Analyzing facial movements in infants. 1993.
  32. 32. Neurobehavioral Systems. Presentation, software. 2014.
  33. 33. Ashburner J, Barnes G, Chen C, Daunizeau J, Flandin G, Friston K, et al. SPM12 Manual. 2016.
  34. 34. Eklund A, Nichols T, Knutsson H. Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016; 113(28): 7900–7905. pmid:27357684
  35. 35. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002; 15(1): 273–289. pmid:11771995
  36. 36. Brett M, Anton J, Valabregue R, Poline J. Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox. In 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain; 2002; Sendai, Japan. p. 497.
  37. 37. Sharp P, Miller G, Heller W. Transdiagnostic dimensions of anxiety: Neural mechanisms, executive functions, and new directions. Int J Psychophysiol. 2015 November; 98(2): 365–377.
  38. 38. Atkinson L, Jamieson B, Khoury J, Ludmer J, Gonzalez A. Stress Physiology in Infancy and Early Childhood: Cortisol Flexibility. Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 2016; 28(8). pmid:27344031
  39. 39. Laurent H, Ablow J, Measelle J. Risky shifts: How the timing and course of mothers’ depressive. Dev Psychopathol. 2011; 23: 521–538. pmid:23786693
  40. 40. Busuito A, Quigley K, Moore G, Voegtline K, DiPietro J. In Sync: Physiological Correlates of Behavioral Synchrony in Infants and Mothers. Dev Psychol. 2019. pmid:30742469
  41. 41. Levy J, Goldstein A, Feldman R. The neural development of empathy is sensitive to caregiving and early trauma. Nat Commun. 2019; 10. pmid:31015471
  42. 42. Priel A, Djalovski A, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. Maternal depression impacts child psychopathology across the first decade of life: Oxytocin and synchrony as markers of resilience. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2019; 60(1): 30–42. pmid:29484656
  43. 43. Enlow MB, Egeland B, Carlson E, Blood E, Wright RJ. Mother-Infant Attachment and the Intergenerational Transmission of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Developmental Psychopathology. 2015 February; 26(1): 41–65.
  44. 44. Moser DA, Suardi F, Rossignol AS, Vital M, Manini A, Serpa SR, et al. Parental Reflective Functioning correlates to brain activation in response to video-stimuli of mother–child dyads: Links to maternal trauma history and PTSD. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 2019 November; 293: 110985. pmid:31627112
  45. 45. Rushworth M, Walton M, Kennerley S, Bannerman D. Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004; 8(9): 410–417. pmid:15350242
  46. 46. Meiran N, Diamond G, Toder D, Nemets B. Cognitive rigidity in unipolar depression and obsessive compulsive disorder: Examination of task switching, Stroop, working memory updating and post-conflict adaptation. Psychiatry Res. 2011; 185(1–2): 149–156. pmid:20580836
  47. 47. Whitmer A, Banich M. Inhibition versus switching deficits in different forms of rumination. Psychological Science. 2007; 18(6): 546–553. pmid:17576269
  48. 48. Morwitz C, Bode S, Baudewig J, Heekeren H. Effective amygdala-prefrontal connectivity predicts individual differences in successful emotion regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2017; 12(4): 569–585. pmid:27998996
  49. 49. Beauregard M, Leroux J, Bergman S, Arzoumanian Y, Beaudoin G, Bourgouin P, et al. The functional neuroanatomy of major depression: An fMRI study using an emotional activation paradigm. Neuroreport. 1998; 9(14): 3253–3258. pmid:9831460
  50. 50. Reck C, Hunt A, Fuchs T, Weiss R, Noon A, Moehler E, et al. Interactive Regulation of Affect in Postpartum Depressed Mothers and Their Infants: An Overview. Psychopathology. 2004 May; 37(6): 272–280. pmid:15539778
  51. 51. Hakanen H, Flykt M, Sinervä E, Nolvi S, Kataja EL, Pelto J, et al. How maternal pre- and postnatal symptoms of depression and anxiety affect early mother-infant interaction? Journal of Affective Disorders. 2019 October; 257(1): 83–90. pmid:31299408
  52. 52. Nicol-Harper R, Harvey AG, Stein A. Interactions between mothers and infants: Impact of maternal anxiety. Infant Behavior and Develoment. 2007 February; 30(1): 161–167. pmid:17292789
  53. 53. Hoffman Y, Drotar D. The impact of postpartum depressed mood on mother-infant interactions: Like mother like baby? Infant Mental Health. 1991 March; 12(1): 65–80.
  54. 54. Moses-Kolko EL, Perlman SB, Wisner KL, James J, Saul AT, Phillips ML. Abnormally reduced dorsomedial prefrontal cortical activity and effective connectivity with amygdala in response to negative emotional faces in postpartum depression. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2010; 167(11): 1337–1380. pmid:20843875
  55. 55. Moses-Kolko EL, Fraser D, Wisner KL, James JA, Saul AT, Phillips ML. Rapid habituation of ventral striatal response to reward receipt in postpartum depression. Biological Psychiatry. 2011; 70(4): 395–399. pmid:21507385
  56. 56. Silverman ME, Loudon H, Liu X, Mauro C, Leiter G, Goldstein MA. The nerual processing of negative emotion postpartum: A preliminary study of amygdala function in postpartum depression. Archives of Women’s Mental Health. 2011; 14(4): 355–359. pmid:21713456
  57. 57. Silverman ME, Loudon H, Safier M, Protopopescu X, Leiter G, Goldstein MA. Neural dysfunction in postpartum depression: an fMRI pilot study. CNS Specturms. 2007; 12(11): 583.
  58. 58. Swain ME, Ho SS, Rosenblum KL, Morelen D, Dayton CJ, Muzik M. Parent-child intervention decreases stress and increases maternal brain activity and connectivity during own baby-cry: An Exploratory Study. Development and Psychopathology. 2017; 29(2): 535. pmid:28401845
  59. 59. Ho SS, Swain JE. Depression alters maternal extended amygdala response and functional connectitivity during distress signals in attachment relationship. Behavioural brain research. 2017;: 290–296. pmid:28263829
  60. 60. Laurent HK, Ablow JC. A cry in the dark: Depressed mothers show reduced neural activation to their own infant’s cry. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2012; 7(2): 125–134. pmid:21208990
  61. 61. Noble S, Scheinost D, Constable RT. A decade of test-retest reliability of functional connectivity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroimage. 2019; 116157: 203. pmid:31494250
  62. 62. Bennet CM, Miller MB. How reliable are the results from functional magnetic resonance imaging? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010; 1191: 133–155. pmid:20392279
  63. 63. Elliott ML, Knodt AR, Ireland D, Morris ML, Poulton R, Ramrakha S, et al. What is the test-retest reliability of common task-functional MRI measures? New empirical evidence and a meta-analysis. Psychological Science. 2020; 31(7): 792–806. pmid:32489141
  64. 64. Kragel PA, Han X, Kraynak TE, Gianaros PJ, Wager TD. fMRI can be highly reliable, but it depends on what you measure. PsyArXiv. 2020.
  65. 65. Kim P, Feldman R, Mayes L, Eicher V, Thompson N, Leckman J, et al. Breastfeeding, brain activation to own infant cry, and maternal sensitivity. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry. 2011; 52(8): 907–915. pmid:21501165
  66. 66. Ivy A, Brunson K, Sandman C, Baram T. Dysfunctional nurturing behavior in rat dams with limited access to nesting material: A clinically relevant model for early-life stress. Neuroscience. 2008 June; 154(3): 1132–1142. pmid:18501521
  67. 67. Patin V, Lordi B, Vincent A, Thoumas J, Vaudry H, Caston J. Effects of prenatal stress on maternal behavior in the rat. Developmental Brain Research. 2002 November; 139(1): 1–8. pmid:12414088
  68. 68. Murgatroyd CA, Nephew BC. Effects of early life social stress on maternal behavior and neuroendocrinology. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013 February; 38(2): 219–228. pmid:22770862
  69. 69. van Ee E, Kleber Rj, Jongmans MJ. Relational Patterns Between Caregivers with PTSD and their Nonexposed Children: A Review. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse. 2016; 17(2): 186–203. pmid:25964276
  70. 70. Spinhoven P, Penninx B, van Hemert AM, de Rooij M, Elzinga BM. Comorbidity of PTSD in anxiety and depressive disorders: Prevalence and shared risk factors. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2014 August; 38(8): 1320–1330. pmid:24629482
  71. 71. Schechter DS, Moser DA, Wang Z, Marsh R, Hao X, Duan Y, et al. An fMRI study of the brain response of traumatized mothers to viewing their toddlers during separation and play. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2012 October; 7(8): 969–979. pmid:22021653
  72. 72. Kern S, Laurent HK. Childhood abuse predicts affective symptoms via HPA reactivity during mother-infant stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2019 May; 107: 19–25. pmid:31071498
  73. 73. Goodman S, Garber J. Evidence-Based Interventions for Depressed Mothers and Their Young. Child Development. 2017 April; 88(2): 368–377. pmid:28160275
  74. 74. Letourneau NL, Dennis CL, Cosic N, Linder J. The effect of perinatal depression treatment for mothers on. Depression and Anxiety. 2017 August; 34(10): 928–966. pmid:28962068