Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Planning deficits in Huntington’s disease: A brain structural correlation by voxel-based morphometry

  • Jesus Calderon-Villalon,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Laboratorio de Neurociencias Cognitivas y Desarrollo, Escuela de Psicología, Universidad Panamericana, Ciudad de México, México

  • Gabriel Ramirez-Garcia,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Laboratorio de Neuropsicología, Departamento de Fisiología, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Ciudad de México, México

  • Juan Fernandez-Ruiz,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Laboratorio de Neuropsicología, Departamento de Fisiología, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Ciudad de México, México, Instituto de Neuroetología, Universidad Veracruzana, Ciudad de México, México

  • Fernanda Sangri-Gil,

    Roles Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Laboratorio de Neurociencias Cognitivas y Desarrollo, Escuela de Psicología, Universidad Panamericana, Ciudad de México, México

  • Aurelio Campos-Romo,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Unidad Periférica de Neurociencias, Facultad de Medicina, Instituto Nacional de Neurología y Neurocirugía “MVS”, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Ciudad de México, México

  • Victor Galvez

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    vgalvez@up.edu.mx

    Affiliations Laboratorio de Neurociencias Cognitivas y Desarrollo, Escuela de Psicología, Universidad Panamericana, Ciudad de México, México, Unidad Periférica de Neurociencias, Facultad de Medicina, Instituto Nacional de Neurología y Neurocirugía “MVS”, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Ciudad de México, México

Abstract

Introduction

Early Huntington’s disease (HD) patients begin to show planning deficits even before motor alterations start to manifest. Generally, planning ability is associated with the functioning of anterior brain areas such as the medial prefrontal cortex. However, early HD neuropathology involves significant atrophy in the occipital and parietal cortex, suggesting that more posterior regions could also be involved in these planning deficits.

Objective

To identify brain regions associated with planning deficits in HD patients at an early clinical stage.

Materials and methods

Twenty-two HD-subjects genetically confirmed with incipient clinical manifestation and twenty healthy subjects were recruited. All participants underwent MRI T1 image acquisition as well as testing in the Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) task to measure planning ability. First, group comparison of SOC measures were performed. Then, correlation voxel-based morphometry analyses were done between gray matter degeneration and SOC performance in the HD group.

Results

Accuracy and efficiency planning scores correlated with gray matter density in right lingual gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, anterior cingulate gyrus, and paracingulate gyrus.

Conclusions

Our results suggest that planning deficits exhibited by early HD-subjects are related to occipital and temporal cortical degeneration in addition to the frontal areas deterioration.

Introduction

Huntington’s Disease (HD) subjects is a neurodegenerative disease characterized by motor, behavioral, and cognitive deficits observed usually during middle adulthood (35–44 years old). These changes are associated with an abnormal expansion of the CAG trinucleotide repeat sequence in the gene 4p 1.3 [1]. Early changes that may precede clinical manifestation include executive function deficits on flexibility and planning that reduce their goal-directed behaviors in daily life [2]. Planning ability is defined as the identification and organization of the elements needed to carry out an intention or to achieve a goal [3]. This ability does not depend on a single brain region but has been related to a set of regions associated with the fronto-cingulo-limbic-parietal network [4].

The Tower of London (TOL) task has been used as a reliable measure of planning ability [510]. Functional imaging analyses have found that the basal ganglia, the premotor cortex, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex are the main regions activated during its execution [57]. However, previous studies with patients have revealed that subjects with posterior cortical damage also show impairments in its global execution [8, 9]. Additionally, it has been shown that the functional activity of the dorsal ("where") and ventral ("what") visual pathways correlate with the execution but no with the complexity of the task, suggesting that these posterior areas may have a sensory involvement during the visuospatial planning process, in contrast to the more anterior structures [9, 10].

Early HD neurodegeneration pattern also involve a decrease of gray matter (GM) in occipital, parietal, and motor cortices, in addition to the prefrontal cortex atrophy [11], suggesting that regions associated with planning abilities are broadly affected in this illness. This is supported by reports describing planning alterations in manifest HD-subjects, as a consequence of functional decoupling of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and the left premotor area [2, 12].

To delve into the neural bases of the planning deficits in HD, here we investigated the possible relation between the neurodegeneration of specific brain areas and planning deficits observed in early HD-patients. For this purpose, GM from whole-brain structural T1 magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was correlated with the performance on the Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) planning task. We hypothesized that planning deficits exhibited by early HD-subjects are related to the cortical degeneration in both posterior and frontal brain regions.

Materials and methods

Participants

Twenty-two HD gene-mutation carriers at the early clinical stage and twenty healthy subjects matched for age, sex, and level of education participated in this study (Table 1). The HD-subjects were recruited at the Instituto Nacional de Neurología y Neurocirugía (INNN). After the positive molecular diagnosis, the patients were invited to participate in the study. Subsequently, the cognitive assessment was performed at the INNN and the brain imaging acquisition was performed at the Instituto Nacional de Psiquiatría. The inclusion criteria for the HD-subjects were a positive molecular genetic diagnosis for HD and a Total Functional Capacity score (TFC) higher than 7 points. The exclusion criteria was the presence of neuropathological findings in the MRI non-HD related. The control group subjects self-reported no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders. They were recruited at the same period of evaluation as the HD-subject group by an open invitation to the general public. All the procedures were performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki [13], approved by the health and ethics committees of the INNN and the Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México (UNAM) (N° DIC/419/14 and N° 41/14). All participants signed a written informed consent before their inclusion in this study.

Clinical and neuropsychological testing

MoCA [14] was used to evaluate the global cognitive status for all subjects. The TFC [15] and the motor subscales from Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale (UHDRS) [16] were used to measure the clinical status of the HD group. Particularly, the symptomatic HD patients were defined by the scores obtained in the TFC scale, where a lower score reflects a reduced functional capacity. The TFC range is from 0 to 13 points, consequently, in our study we defined as early symptomatic HD to the subjects with scores higher than 7, considering the scale criteria [15].

Planning performance task with SOC.

Planning performance in all participants was evaluated using the SOC test, a digital task from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) software version Eclipse®. It requires spatial abilities and strategic planning and is aimed to give a measure of frontal lobe function [17, 18]. Participants performed the task using a computer tablet; the touch screen showed two panels. The upper panel showed the reference pattern that the subject had to match in the lower panel. The objective was to move three colored balls, placed in different positions within three stockings to the reference position showed in the upper panel. The participants could only move the balls one at a time by selecting the required ball, then selecting the position to which it should be moved. The balls were arranged in different patterns in each problem, and participants were instructed to make as few moves as possible to solve it. The level of difficulty increased as the number of minimum moves needed to complete the task was risen. Twelve problems were evaluated. The first six problems could be solved in a minimum of two movements (and four at the most). The last six problems were considered “difficult” because the number of minimum moves needed to solve them was five (and twelve at the most). Two types of measurements were collected: the accuracy (the sum of the number of solved problems in the minimum of movements allowed, being 12 for all problems and 6 for the “difficult” problems), and efficiency (the sum of the number of movements made in the respective set of problems: all and “difficult” problems) [19]. It should be noted that in contrast to accuracy where a higher score is better, in efficiency a higher count indicates worse performance.

Image acquisition

All images were acquired using a 3T MRI scanner (Philips Medical Systems, Eindhoven, The Netherlands). The high-resolution anatomical acquisition consisted of a T1-3D Fast Field-Echo sequence with the following parameters: TR/TE: 8/3.7 ms; FOV: 256 × 256 mm2; Flip angle = 8°; acquisition and reconstruction matrix: 256 × 256; and isometric resolution: 1 × 1 × 1 mm3.

Voxel-based morphometry (VBM).

GM measurement was performed using VBM as implemented in FSL software (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl) following the standard procedure reported previously [20]. Then, to test if there was an association between GM density and SOC measures in HD-subjects, a one-sample t-test was performed in a voxel-wise analysis through the GLM, including GM and accuracy and efficiency scores in all problems (12 evaluated problems) and "difficult" problems (last 6 evaluated problems). The significance level was set at p < .01, and the Family Wise Error correction for multiple comparisons was done using the random permutation method (n = 10,000) using Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement (TFCE), suggested for general linear model inference analysis [21]. For all the analyses, the disease burden score (calculated using the formula: age (years) × [CAG repeat length − 35.5]) [22] was included as a nuisance variable. Only clusters with a minimum cluster size of 30 voxels were reported. Coordinates were reported in the MNI standard-space and anatomical labels were obtained from the Talairach Daemon labels, MNI cerebellum, and Harvard-Oxford Cortical Structural and Subcortical Structural Atlases.

Statistical analysis

SOC performance comparison between groups was conducted using the nonparametric Mann-Whitney U-test. Accuracy and efficiency measures were calculated for the 12 problems together, and for the 6 problems considered as “difficult”. SOC scores were correlated with the patients’ GM density. Spearman’s rho correlation analysis was performed using the average GM density from each VBM significant cluster. Finally, only significant correlations corrected by the Bonferroni method were selected (.01/number of significant clusters obtained by VBM), setting a significance level for all problems at p < .0016 (.01/6), and for “difficult” problems at p < .0025 (.01/4). All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS software (SPSS version 23, Chicago, Illinois, USA).

Results

Clinical testing

The TFC scale mean () and (±) standard deviation score from HD-subjects was = 11.9 ± 1.8, detecting eighteen patients (82%) in Stage I (scores from 11 to 13) and four patients (18%) in Stage II (scores from 7 to 10). The mean UHDRS score was = 17.1 ± 13.6 out of a maximum of 124. Five patients (22.7%) showed abnormal movements, with the highest score of 40 points in the most affected patient. Seventeen patients (77.3%) scored less than 17 points, and two patients (9.5%) scored 0 points. The above data confirmed their early clinical status according to the functional and motor decline [15, 23]. MoCA global score comparison showed significant group differences between the control ( = 27.5 ± 2.2) and HD ( = 24.6 ± 2.9) groups (U = 109.500, p = 0.002).

SOC scores analyses also showed significant accuracy and efficiency differences between groups (Fig 1). The control group ( = 8.8 ± 1.7) showed better accuracy than the HD group while solving all the problems ( = 6.9 ± 2.4) (U = 117.5, p = .009). Likewise, the control group ( = 3.4 ± 1.4) showed better accuracy than the HD group while solving the “difficult” problems ( = 2.5 ± 1.4) (U = 139, p = .038). Regarding the efficiency to solve all problems, the control group showed better performance ( = 115.5 ± 17.7) than the HD group ( = 145.1 ± 41.2) (U = 101.5, p = .003). Similarly, the control group ( = 75.6 ± 15) showed better efficiency solving the “difficult” problems than the HD group ( = 93.6 ± 24.2) (U = 116.5, p = .009).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Differences in SOC scores analyses between groups.

A) Accuracy to solve all problems: Number of problems solved in the minimum number of movements. B) Accuracy to solve "difficult" problems: Number of problems solved in the minimum number of movements. C) Efficiency to solve all problems: Sum of movements made to solve each of all problems. D) Efficiency to solve "difficult" problems: Sum of movements made to solve each “difficult” problem. *p < .05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249144.g001

Relationship of SOC scores and GM density in early manifest HD-subjects

VBM analyses showed a positive correlation between the accuracy to solve all problems with volume preservation in the right middle temporal gyrus (RMTG) (posterior division), right lingual gyrus (RLG), right paracingulate gyrus (RPCG), left putamen, left central opercular cortex and left insular cortex. A similar analysis found a negative correlation between the efficiency to solve “difficult” problems with volume preservation in the right cerebellum posterior lobe, left cerebellar nodule, RMTG (anterior division), and right anterior cingulate gyrus (RACG) (Fig 2). No significant correlations were found in the efficiency to solve all problems nor the accuracy to solve “difficult” problems.

thumbnail
Fig 2. VBM analysis association between GM and SOC scores.

Significant association between GM with accuracy to solve all problems (green scatterplot); and efficiency to solve “difficult” problems (blue scatterplot). Color maps indicate t-value level corrected by TFCE (p < .01). Scatter plots show the Spearman’s rho correlation between the average GM density from VBM significative clusters with the corresponding SOC score. Statistical parametric maps are shown onto the three-dimensionally rendered MNI template brain. RACG: right anterior cingulate gyrus; RMTG: right middle temporal gyrus; RLG: right lingual gyrus; RPCG: Right paracingulate gyrus.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249144.g002

Discussion

Here we tested if planning deficits in HD-subjects could be related to cortical degeneration in frontal and posterior brain regions. Our analyses of the VBM suggest that besides the contribution of the frontal lobe deterioration to these deficits, posterior degeneration also correlates with the HD group’s impaired performance in the planning task. Following is a discussion of these results.

Planning performance in early HD-subjects

Early HD-subjects exhibited planning deficits associated with accuracy and efficiency performance. HD patients made significantly more movements to solve the SOC problems, suggesting impaired planning efficiency compared with the control group. Additionally, HD-subjects were less accurate as suggested by the smaller number of solved problems using the fewest possible moves. These SOC results confirm what has been reported in other studies employing similar tasks to explore planning skills in HD-patients [2, 12, 24].

SOC performance association with early HD-subjects brain

Our results suggest that, in addition to frontal areas such as the cingulate gyrus, HD planning deficits are also associated with the degeneration in occipital and temporal cortices. Volume reductions in three cortical areas were correlated positively with SOC scores in the HD group: RMTG, RLG, and the cingulate gyrus (RACG and RPCG). It is important to note that VBM results showed a consistent GM density negative correlation between RMTG and cingulate gyrus with the efficiency to solve “difficult” problems, and with the accuracy to solve all SOC problems.

The possible explanation for the strong association between the accuracy to solve all problems and RMTG degeneration could be related to its role in visual processing, including visuospatial information processing [25, 26]. The hypothesis that planning deficits may involve visuospatial deterioration is further supported by the correlation between accuracy deficits with the degeneration of posterior brain areas like RLG. This is supported by previous reports showing significant activity changes in this area in Parkinson’s disease patients in comparison to control subjects while performing the TOL task [27]. This is consistent with the evidence that visual perceptual deficits could give rise to changes in cognitive performance, particularly when planning ability is involved [28].

Although planning ability in HD-subjects has been associated with mPFC functional connectivity [12], our GM structural analysis showed relative preservation of this area in our patient cohort. In contrast, our analysis found a significant correlation between the RACG with low efficiency planning, and RPCG with low accuracy planning; both limbic brain regions. This may be explained by considering a previous functional MRI study in healthy subjects during TOL performance [29]. That study describes the prefrontal-cingulate connectivity; however, it also shows more activity in the anterior cingulate cortex to solve low difficult problems but switching to more activity in prefrontal regions to solve the more difficult problems, suggesting that both areas are differentially activated depending on the degree of cognitive demand [29]. The above could help explain the differences in brain regions that correlated with planning deficits depending on the problem complexity in HD-subjects.

In addition, RACG is reported as part of the executive attention network related to error detection (self-monitoring) [30] and arousal increase during complex problems performance [9]. In this regard, the low efficiency of patients may also be part of an attentional domain deficit that matches with the idea that planning ability is related to the fronto-cingulo-limbic-parietal network where different brain regions converge to coordinate executive functions [4].

Finally, the role of the subcortical structures like the putamen in planning deficits in HD-subjects should also be taken into account. The striatum is part of a wide fronto-executive network involved in planning processing, which network includes the medial prefrontal areas and the dorsolateral prefrontal areas [2, 12]. Usually, cognitive impairments exhibited in HD patients are associated to caudate nucleus decrease due to the spiny neurons atrophy [31]. This decrease seems to affect particularly the caudate nucleus during the clinical debut, specially compromising the prefrontal cortico-striatal loop [2, 32, 33]. However, our study found that putamen deterioration in HD patients also contributes to the accuracy decrease to solve planning problems. This result suggests that, in addition to the caudate nucleus, the putamen degeneration could also compromise cognitive functions during the early clinical stage. This decrease seems to affect particularly the caudate nucleus during the clinical debut, specially compromising the prefrontal cortico-striatal loop [2, 32, 33]. However, our study found that putamen deterioration in HD patients also contributes to the accuracy decrease to solve planning problems. This result suggests that, in addition to the caudate nucleus, the putamen degeneration could also compromise cognitive functions during the early clinical stage.

Some limitations in our study that should be considered for future research: 1) Because we had access to a relatively small number of patients, given the rarity of this disease, we recommend including the assessment of planning deficits in HD studies with a larger sample size. This could confirm the consistency of the brain gray matter areas associated with SOC deficits in HD reported in this study. 2) The cohort consisted of HD patients at an early symptomatic stage; it remains to be determined if prodromal patients start showing planning deficits before the clinical manifestation of the disease. 3) In this study we used two tests to analyze the cognitive impairment in general and the planning deficits in particular. It is recommended that for future assessments of the global cognitive status, and the planning ability, more neuropsychological batteries should be used, allowing a more comprehensive evaluation in these patients. 4) It would be interesting to explore if the correlation between the planning performance deficits and the loss of gray matter density could also be found with white matter deterioration, or changes in functional connectivity in HD-subjects. 5) Finally, even though we identified temporo-occipital degeneration associated with planning performance, we must be cautious with the interpretation of this result. The reported correlations passed the multiple comparison corrections, and each correlation showed different coefficients of determination. However, they could still have a collinearity problem, which is a main challenge to overcome in this type of study.

Acknowledgments

We thank the HD patients and healthy controls who participated in this study.

References

  1. 1. Bates GP, Dorsey R, Gusella JF, Hayden MR, Kay C, Leavitt BR, et al. Huntington disease. Nature Reviews Disease Primers. 2015; 1: 15005. pmid:27188817
  2. 2. Mörkl S, Müller NJ, Blesl C, Wilkinson L, Tmava A, Wurn W, et al. Problem solving, impulse control and planning in early- and late-stage Huntington’s disease. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2016; 266(7): 663–671. pmid:27372072
  3. 3. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012.
  4. 4. Bartley JE, Boeving ER, Riedel MC, Bottenhorn KL, Salo T, Eickhoff SB, et al. Meta-analytic evidence for a core problem solving network across multiple representational domains. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2018; 92: 318–337. pmid:29944961
  5. 5. Schall U, Johnston P, Lagopoulos J, Jüptner M, Jentzen W, Thienel R, et al. Functional brain maps of Tower of London performance: a positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage. 2003; 20(2): 1154–1161. pmid:14568484
  6. 6. Newman SD, Greco JA, Lee D. An fMRI study of the Tower of London: a look at problem structure differences. Brain research. 2009; 1286: 123–132. pmid:19540214
  7. 7. Vriend C, Wagenmakers MJ, Van den Heuvel OA, Van der Werf YD. Resting-state network topology and planning ability in healthy adults. Brain Structure and Function. 2020; 225(1): 365–374. pmid:31865409
  8. 8. Shallice T. Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society. 1982; 298: 199–209. pmid:6125971
  9. 9. Dagher A, Owen AM, Boecker H, Brooks DJ. Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task. Brain. 1999; 122: 1973–1987. pmid:10506098
  10. 10. Coppen EM, van der Grond J, Hart EP, Lakke EAJF, Roos RAC. The visual cortex and visual cognition in Huntington’s disease: an overview of current literature. Behavioral Brain Research. 2018; 351: 63–74. pmid:29792890
  11. 11. Ramirez-Garcia G, Galvez V, Diaz R, Bayliss L, Fernandez-Ruiz J, Campos-Romo A. Longitudinal atrophy characterization of cortical and subcortical gray matter in Huntington’s disease patients. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2019; 51: 1827–1843. pmid:31705594
  12. 12. Unschuld PG, Liu X, Shanahan M, Margolis RL, Bassett SS, Brandt J, et al. Prefrontal executive function associated coupling relates to Huntington’s disease stage. Cortex. 2013; 49(10): 2661–2673. pmid:23906595
  13. 13. Declaration H. Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. 2013. pmid:24141714
  14. 14. Aguilar-Navarro SG, Mimenza-Alvarado AJ, Palacios-García AA, Samudio Cruz A, Gutiérrez-Gutiérrez LA, Ávila-Funes JA. Validez y confiabilidad del MoCA (Montreal Assessment Test) para el tamizaje del deterioro cognoscitivo en México. Asociación Colombiana de Psiquiatría. 2017; 47(4): 237–243. pmid:30286846
  15. 15. Shoulson I, Fahn S. Huntington’s disease: clinical care and evaluation. Neurology. 1979; 19: 1–3. pmid:154626
  16. 16. Huntington Study Group. Unified Huntington’s disease rating scale: reliability and consistency. Movement disorders. 1996; 11(2). pmid:8684382
  17. 17. Lawrence AD, Hodges JR, Rosser AE, Kershaw A, French-Constant C, Rubinsztein DC, et al. Evidence for specific cognitive deficits in preclinical Huntington’s disease. Brain: a journal of neurology. 1998; 121(7): 1329–1341. pmid:9679784
  18. 18. Torgersen J, Flaatten H, Engelsen BA, Gramstad A. Clinical validation of Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery in a Norwegian epilepsy population. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science. 2012; 2(1): 108–116.
  19. 19. Berg WK & Byrd D. The Tower of London Spatial Problem-Solving Task: Enhancing Clinical and Research Implementation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2002; 24(5): 586–604. pmid:12187443
  20. 20. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation of FSL. Neuroimage. 2004; 23: 208–219. pmid:15501092
  21. 21. Winkler AM, Ridgway GR, Webster MA, Smith SM, Nichols TE. Permutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage. 2014; 92: 381–397. pmid:24530839
  22. 22. Penney JB Jr, Vonsattel JP, Macdonald ME, Gusella JF, Myers RH. CAG repeat number governs the development rate of pathology in Huntington’s disease. Annals of Neurology: Official Journal of the American Neurological Association and the Child Neurology Society. 1997; 41(5): 689–692. pmid:9153534
  23. 23. Schobel SA, Palermo G, Auinger P, Long JD, Ma S, Khwaja OS, et al. Motor, cognitive, and functional declines contribute to a single progressive factor in early HD. Neurology. 2017; 89(24): 2495–2502. pmid:29142089
  24. 24. Watkins LHA, Rogers RD, Lawrence AD, Sahakian BJ, Rosser AE, Robbins TW. Impaired planning but intact decision making in early Huntington’s disease: implications for specific fronto-striatal pathology. Neuropsychologia. 2000; 38(8): 1112–1125. pmid:10838146
  25. 25. Dravida S, Saxe R, Bedny M. People can understand descriptions of motion without activating visual motion brain regions. Frontiers in psychology. 2013; 4(1): 1–14. pmid:24009592
  26. 26. Deng Y, Shi L, Lei Y, Liang P, Li K, Chu , et al. Mapping the “What” and “Where” Visual Cortices and Their Atrophy in Alzheimer’s Disease: Combined Activation Likelihood Estimation with Voxel-Based Morphometry. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2016; 10: 1–16. pmid:26858619
  27. 27. Dagher A, Owen AM, Boecker H, Brooks DJ. The role of the striatum and hippocampus in planning: a PET activation study in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2001; 124(5): 1020–1032. pmid:11335704
  28. 28. Owen AM, Doyon J, Petrides M, Evans AC. Planning and spatial working memory: a positron emission tomography study in humans. European Journal of Neurosciences. 1996; 8(2): 353–364. pmid:8714706
  29. 29. Cazalis F, Valabrègue R, Pélégrini-Issac M, Asloun S, Robbins TW, Granon S. Individual differences in prefrontal cortical activation on the Tower of London planning task: implication for effortful processing. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003; 17(10): 2219–2225. pmid:12786989
  30. 30. Posner MI, Petersen SE. The attention system of human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2012; 21(35): 73–89. pmid:22524787
  31. 31. Cepeda C, Wu N, Andre V, Cummings D, Levine M. The corticostriatal pathway in Huntington’s disease. 2007; 81(5–6), 253–271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.11.001
  32. 32. Johnson EB, Ziegler G, Penny W, Rees G, Tabrizi SJ, Scahill RI, et al. Dynamics of cortical degeneration over a decade in Huntington’s disease. Biological Psychiatry. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.11.009
  33. 33. Poudel GR, Stout JC, Domínguez D JF, Churchyard A, Chua P, Egan GF, et al. Longitudinal change in white matter microstructure in Huntington’s disease: The IMAGE-HD study. Neurobiology of Disease. 2015; 74, 406–412. pmid:25497085