Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Does contingent biofeedback improve cardiac interoception? A preregistered replication of Meyerholz, Irzinger, Withöft, Gerlach, and Pohl (2019) using the heartbeat discrimination task in a randomised control trial

  • Christian Rominger ,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    christian.rominger@uni-graz.at

    Affiliations Institute of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria, Otto Loewi Research Center, Section of Physiology, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria

  • Thilo Michael Graßmann,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria

  • Bernhard Weber,

    Roles Software

    Affiliation Institute of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria

  • Andreas R. Schwerdtfeger

    Roles Formal analysis, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria

Abstract

Meyerholz, Irzinger, Withöft, Gerlach, and Pohl (2019) reported on a comparably large effect (d = 1.21) of a contingent biofeedback procedure on cardiac accuracy as assessed by the heartbeat tracking task. However, this task has recently been criticized as a measure of interoceptive accuracy. We aimed to replicate this finding by using the well-validated heartbeat discrimination task and to compare the biofeedback with a deep breathing and a control condition (viewing a film clip). The trial was preregistered at open science framework (https://osf.io/9fxn6). Overall, 93 participants were randomized to one of the three conditions and the heartbeat discrimination task was presented prior and after the 20-minutes training sessions. The study had a power of .86 to detect a medium-sized effect in the biofeedback group and a power of .96 to detect a medium-sized interaction of intervention group and time. A general tendency for improvement in heartbeat detection accuracy was found across intervention groups (d = 0.19, p = .08); however, groups did not differ significantly. In particular, there was no significant interaction of intervention group and time (f = .00, p = .98) and no reliable effect for the biofeedback group (d = 0.15, p = .42). One limitation is that a different, but well-validated task was used to quantify interoceptive accuracy. This study suggests that biofeedback might not improve interoceptive accuracy in the cardiac domain, but effects seem to depend on the specific task applied.

1. Introduction

In a previously published report, Meyerholz, Irzinger, Withöft, Gerlach, and Pohl [1] found that a brief 20-minutes contingent biofeedback procedure resulted in a large-scaled improvement of interoceptive accuracy as assessed with the heartbeat tracking task, which measures interoceptive accuracy by means of comparing the perceived heartbeats with actual heartbeats of a predefined time period (e.g., 25 sec, 45sec, etc. [2, 3]). Specifically, the biofeedback training was based on an animated heart symbol presented 200ms after R-wave detection and participants were instructed to press a button after a pre-defined number of heartbeats (2, 3 or 4 heartbeats). In the later training phases, the heart symbol was not presented. The authors conclude that cardiac biofeedback could improve interoceptive accuracy in the cardiac domain.

As the authors discuss themselves, the heartbeat tracking task has been criticized, because (implicit) knowledge about the own heartbeat could lead to better task performance [4, 5] and participants might achieve high accuracy without heartbeat perception, but accurate knowledge of heart rate [6]. Indeed, recent research seriously questions the validity of this task [79]. An alternative approach involves discrimination between true and false sensory feedback of individual heartbeats [10]. Although discrimination tasks have been criticized, because they may not solely warrant allocation of attention on internal and organismic cues [11], the integration of external and internal signals is a part of interoception [6] and fundamental for self-consciousness [12]. Heartbeat discrimination may be considered more valid [13, 14], since it is more robust against changes of (implicit) knowledge [4, 6, 10], and was suggested to be a prerequisite for heartbeat tracking [6]. Furthermore, some authors recommended the application of signal detection theory to study interoceptive accuracy [1, 7], which is implemented in discrimination tasks and allows to assess perceptual sensitivity separately from other non-perceptual factors [10]. Of note, interoceptive accuracy has been differentiated from self-evaluated assessments of subjective interoception and metacognition–that is the ability to discriminate correct from incorrect perceptual decisions [15, 16].

Therefore, the heartbeat discrimination task in combination with signal detection theory seems to be a good choice to replicate the training effects of Meyerholz et al. [1], since it allows to measure interoceptive accuracy independently from heartbeat-related knowledge [6] and the quantification of interoceptive metacognition—indicating the knowledge about own interoceptive performance [16]. The study-results may verify the validity of the reported contingent training effects and could indicate potential transfer-effects. Specifically, if the biofeedback training indeed enhances cardiac interoceptive accuracy, performance increases in the discrimination task could be expected. We used the very same training and passive control condition as Meyerholz et al. [1].

In order to extend the study of Meyerholz et al. [1], a further training condition was realized. Based on recent findings of an optimization of blood flow in brain areas associated with interoception (e.g., insula) due to deep nasal breathing [17], we aimed to examine the efficacy of a coherent breathing intervention on cardiac interoceptive accuracy. Of note, breathing at about 0.1Hz (i.e., 6 breaths per minute) has been associated with beneficial effects on physiology and psychological functioning [18, 19]. Slow breathing may induce resonance, meaning that metabolism in different physiological systems is synchronized and optimized [20, 21]. Furthermore, breathing seems to be important for corporal awareness [22], might change the focus of attention on internal body processes, and seems to activate an interoceptive network including the insula [17, 23], thus specifically coherent breathing potentially benefits interoception.

Taken together, firstly, we expected an increase in performance during the biofeedback training. Secondly, we hypothesized that the biofeedback training and the breathing condition would lead to an increase of interoceptive accuracy, in contrast to the control condition. Thirdly, we hypothesized that the training may increase the metacognition of participants.

2. Methods

2.1 Participants

Based on the original study, which reported a large effect for the biofeedback group (d = 1.21) and a large effect for the between (group) x within (pre/post) interaction (ηp2 = .24, f = 0.56), the required sample size for a mixed ANOVA 2 x 3 design given a power of .95 and moderate correlation between measures was N = 21 (G*power 3.192; [24]). However, due to overly large and biased effect size estimates of many (unregistered) primary studies [25, 26], we based sample size calculation on the assumption of small to medium effects (d = .30, ηp2 = .05) with a power of .90. The resulting sample size was N = 90 (30 participants in each group). We strived to sample 93 individuals to account for dropouts. See Fig 1 for the flow of participants through the study. It should be noted that in the original study women (n = 74) were more prevalent than men (n = 26). Mean age was 23.95 years (SD = 4.18), which was slightly younger as compared to the original sample (M = 25.28, SD = 5.67). Due to technical problems the blood pressure of one participant and the heart rate of another participant were missing. Eligibility included a) being over 18 years and b) no regular yoga, meditation, mindfulness-related practice, or the use of heartbeat trackers. Ethics approval was granted by the Ethics Committee of the university of Graz (reference number: GZ.39/59/63 ex 2018/19), and the parallel randomised control trial was pre-registered in the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/9fxn6). All participants gave their written informed consent.

thumbnail
Fig 1. CONSORT flow chart illustrating the flow of participants through the study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248246.g001

2.2 Procedure and material

Participants were recruited at Graz from September to October 2019 via web, and flyer-based advertisements. They were randomly assigned to either of the three groups at a 1:1:1 ratio (see Fig 1). The second author generated the random allocation sequence by the Excel random number generation function, enrolled participants, and assigned them to the interventions, when they occurred at the laboratory. Although participants and the second author delivering the intervention could not be blinded to treatment assignment, the assessor conducting outcome assessments was blinded. Each condition lasted 20 minutes. Groups did not significantly differ on relevant variables (e.g., age, sex, education, pre/post heartrate, lifestyle variables). The only significant difference was found in the not-worrying subscale of the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA; [27]), where participants in the control condition showed slightly higher scores as compared to participants in the contingent biofeedback group (see Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Descriptive data of the three experimental groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248246.t001

2.2.1 ECG.

The ECG was recorded with a Biopac MP150 amplifier system (1000Hz) running AcqKnowledge 4.3 (standard lead II configuration). The R-waves were identified with the Accusync® 72 ECG Trigger Monitor, which sent R-wave contingent triggers to the computer running the biofeedback training and the heartbeat discrimination task (PsychoPy; [28]). Auditory stimuli were presented via stereo loudspeakers approximately 2m in front of the participants, who sat in a separated quiet and light attenuated room in a comfortable chair (1m in front of a computer screen). The instruction conformity was monitored by the experimenter via two cameras. A pre/post resting ECG with 3 minutes duration was recorded (Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Procedure of the pre-registered replication study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248246.g002

2.2.2 Heartbeat discrimination task.

Interoceptive accuracy was measured by the heartbeat discrimination task. Auditory playback of the participants’ heartbeats were presented with either a minimal (230ms) or prolonged (540ms) delay [10, 29, 30]. The task was to decide, after 10 tones (50ms duration; [29]), if the feedback accurately represented (was synchronous with) the own heartbeats or not. Thereafter the participants rated the confidence in perceiving their own heartbeat on a visual analogue scale form “total guess” to “complete confidence” [15]. First, one training block with 20 trials was conducted. The assessment of participants’ interoceptive accuracy consisted of 40 trials (in two blocks) for the pre/post intervention separately [31]. Interoceptive accuracy was indexed by d-prime [10, 29] with d = zhit ratezfalse alarm rate (Z refers to the normal inverse cumulative distribution function).

Metacognition, that is knowing when making good or bad interoceptive decisions was determined by the area under a type II receiver operating characteristic curve [15, 16, 32]. We differentiated between positive and negative predictions and participants who thought to make good interoceptive decisions but did worse were indexed with a score lower than .5 (similar logic see [33]). With other words, participants who showed a negative association between subjective ratings and objective performance (systematically evaluating the interoceptive performance as good when showing poor accuracy) were indexed with scores lower than .5.

2.3 Experimental manipulation

2.3.1 Contingent biofeedback training.

The training was exactly conducted as described by Meyerholz et al. [1] and consisted of twelve initial trials to get familiar, followed by three blocks with 48 trials each. There was a short break of 15s in the middle of each block and a longer break between the blocks (max. 1min). In each trial, participants were asked to press a keyboard-button after 2, 3 or 4 consecutive heartbeats. Participant’s responses were classified as correct, if the button press fell within 200ms - 450ms after the final R-wave in the ECG. In the first 24 trials of each block, participants received a visual feedback (i.e., animated heart symbol) on their heartbeat (200ms delay) on the monitor. The intervals between the trials varied between one and four heartbeats. The number of heartbeats until button press and the intervals between the trials were pseudo-randomized. Participants received feedback after each trial in form of a checkmark (correct response) or a cross (false response) and after 24 trials by means of a percent-correct number.

2.3.2 Coherent nasal breathing.

Participants were instructed to breathe at their individual resonance frequency ([21], p. 23), which was determined during normal breathing at the baseline before the slow-paced nasal breathing intervention. A power spectral density analysis was applied to determine the individual resonance frequency. Specifically, the highest peak in the power spectrum within the HRV low frequency band (0.04–0.15Hz) was analyzed. If the highest peak fell above 0.12Hz or below 0.07Hz (19% of the subsample), respiratory frequency was set to 0.1Hz in order to comply with research favoring the benefits of slow breathing [18, 34]. The slow-paced nasal breathing consisted of seven blocks of two minutes each with one minute of rest in between (12 slow-paced training-breaths before intervention). Pacer stimuli were presented on a monitor: Inhalation was guided by an enlarging bar and exhalation by a lessening bar.

2.3.3 Control condition.

Similar to Meyerholz et al. [1], participants watched a nature documentary for 20 minutes.

2.4 Questionnaires

2.4.1 Interoceptive awareness.

The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA; [27]) is a self-report measures and was used to assess several factors of participants’ interoceptive awareness (German version, [35]). It is composed of 32 items, which are rated on a six-point Likert-Scale from 0 = never to 5 = always. The MAIA assesses eight concepts of interoceptive awareness (i.e., noticing, not-distracting, not-worrying, attention regulation, emotional awareness, self-regulation, body listening, trusting) with good psychometric properties (Cronbach’s α of subscales ranged from .66 to .87). In the present study, the subscales not-worrying (α = .44), not-distracting (α = .59), noticing (α = .61) showed low internal consistencies, while the other scales showed satisfactory Cronbach’s α of >.70.

2.4.2 STADI.

The State Trait Anxiety Depression Inventory (STADI; German version, [36]; based on [37]) was used to asses participants’ trait level of anxiety and depression by means of 20 items. All items were rated on a four-point Likert scale from 1 “nearly never” to 4 “nearly ever”. For the purpose of the present study we calculated a total score of the STADI (indexing negative affectivity) to control for differences between the intervention groups. The Cronbach’s α of this total score was .91.

2.5 Data analysis

The within-training effect was analyzed with an ANOVA for repeated measures and the factors block (block1/block2/block3) and phase (visual/no feedback). A mixed 3 (contingent biofeedback/coherent nasal breathing/control) x 2 (pre/post) ANOVA with the factors group and time was calculated to examine group specific effects on performance. Moreover, separate t-tests were performed in order to analyze the effect of interventions for each group separately. The alpha level was fixed at p < .05 (two-tailed).

3. Results

3.1 Manipulation check of biofeedback training

Comparison of the percent correct responses in the training blocks showed a significant main effects of block, (F(1.505,43.649) = 5.77, p = .011, ηp2 = 0.17) and a main effect of phase (F(1,29) = 120.91, p < .001, ηp2 = 0.81), but no interaction effect (F(2,58) = 0.925, p = .402, ηp2 = 0.03). Similar to Meyerholz et al. [1], Bonferroni corrected pairwise comparisons indicated that the percentage of correct reactions improved significantly from block 1 (M = 55.37%, SD = 19.62) to block 3 (M = 65.90%, SD = 17.36; p = .027), which indicates the expected training effects. There were relatively more correct reactions in the phase with visual feedback (M = 76.45%, SD = 16.94) than without (M = 45.14%, SD = 14.93).

3.2 Effects of biofeedback training and slow breathing on cardiac interoception

Although the main effect of time was not significant (F(1,90) = 3.129, Wilks Ʌ = .966, p = .080, ηp2 = .034), on a descriptive level performance increased from pre (M = 0.24, SD = 0.66) to post intervention (M = 0.39, SD = 0.71). Importantly, no significant interaction of group by time was found (F(2,90) = 0.020, Wilks Ʌ = 1.000, p = .980, ηp2 = .000) as well as no main effect of group (F(2,90) = 1.717, p = .185, ηp2 = .04). Separate t-tests for the two intervention conditions were non-significant (all ps>.293; see Table 2). The observed biofeedback training effect of d = 0.15 seems to be over 20 million times more consistent with the null hypothesis than with a large effect of d = 1.21, reported by Meyerholz et al. [1]. (The likelihood to observe a d = 0.15 when dtrue = 0 is 0.277 and the likelihood to observe d = 0.15 when dtrue = 1.21 is 1.372*10−8. The resulting likelihood ratio is therefore 20,189,504; see S1 Fig for the likelihood ratio distribution of observed data under an alternative vs. under the null hypothesis). In line with this, the Bayes factor of 0.26 strongly supports the null hypothesis for the observed biofeedback training effect [38].

thumbnail
Table 2. Observed means and standard deviations at pre-intervention and post-intervention for the intervention groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248246.t002

Although the metacognitive performance revealed no interaction effect (F(2,90) = 1.348, Wilks Ʌ = .971, p = .265, ηp2 = .03), it significantly increased from pre- to post-intervention (F(1,90) = 9.938, Wilks Ʌ = .901, p = .002, ηp2 = .10; see Fig 3). The main effect group was not significant (F(2,90) = 0.128, p = .880, ηp2 = .00).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Interoceptive accuracy assessed with the heartbeat discrimination task from pre- to post-intervention (top row), separately for all the three groups (contingent biofeedback, coherent breathing, and control condition), which did not differ significantly from each other.

Similar results are depicted for metacognitive performance (area under the type II receiver operating characteristic curve; bottom row).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248246.g003

Importantly, the change in interoceptive accuracy and metacognition from pre- to post-intervention was neither associated with performance in any training-block, nor with the training-effect from block1 to block3 (ps≥.216). None of the interoceptive awareness factors was associated with interoceptive accuracy or changes in thereof (ps≥.124). Only the subscale trusting showed a weak but significant association with metacognition before intervention (r = -.23, p = .029), but not after intervention (r = .07, p = .495).

4. Discussion

The aim of this research was to replicate the beneficial effect of a contingent biofeedback training on cardiac interoceptive skills [1]. Although we used the same biofeedback training and participants showed the expected increase of correct responses during this training, we did not observe the expected transfer-effect from the biofeedback training to the heartbeat discrimination task, which was expected to be an increase in interoceptive accuracy. This argues against the conclusion of Meyerholz et al. [1], thus suggesting that contingent cardiac biofeedback seems to not improve cardiac interoceptive accuracy (and neither metacognition).

Therefore, the findings of Meyerholz et al. [1] should be re-interpreted in terms of a within-task effect of training, which is not necessarily accompanied by an increase of cardiac interoception. As the authors discussed themselves, the direct feedback of participants’ heartbeats during the training might have changed participants’ knowledge leading to a better estimation of heart rate [40]. This interpretation is likely, since participants can achieve high scores in the tracking task without perceiving their heartbeats, but by guessing and estimating [4, 7, 8, 41]. In contrast, interoceptive accuracy derived from the heartbeat discrimination task has been considered largely independent of beliefs, knowledge, and the strategy to guess [4, 10]. The absence of an association pattern between the factors of the self-reported interoceptive awareness (MAIA) and the performance measure in the present study is in accordance with this [15, 42]. We only found one association between metacognition before intervention and the subscale trusting (out of 16; 8 subscales and two metacognition scores). Participants, who trusted their interoceptive perceptions more might be slightly more convinced to perceive (or not to perceive) their heartbeats, although their interoceptive accuracy is not different from others. This might indicate some validity of the measures. However, applying the type II receiver operating characteristic curve as a measure of metacognition has also been criticized [32]. Nevertheless, using the meta d-prime [32, 43] as an alternative index of metacognition resulted in a similar pattern of findings. All effects were non-significant (all ps≥.082).

Nevertheless, it should be emphasized that the present study does not falsify the findings of Meyerholz et al. [1], but rather complement them. Since this is a conceptual replication, the findings argue against the interpretation of an overall-effect on cardiac interoceptive accuracy [1]. If a contingent training would be effective over and above a mere training on the task, a transfer-effect should have occurred, and the training should have led to a significant increase in accuracy in the applied heartbeat discrimination task. The present null effect of the between-within interaction (f = .00) indicated no specific effect of biofeedback training. Furthermore, the observed intervention effect of d = .15 is much less consistent with the findings of Meyerholz et al. [1] as compared to the null-hypothesis. This convincingly indicates no general enhancing effects of the applied biofeedback training on cardiac sensation, which is in accordance with Phillips and colleagues [4]. These authors showed specific performance changes after (false) feedback-training in the heartbeat tracking task, while the discrimination task was unaffected. In a similar vein, Ring et al. [41] indicated that contingent and non-contingent feedback led to a comparable performance increase in the heartbeat tracking task. Until today, only Meyerholz et al. [1] reported a specific performance increase in a tracking task after a contingent training. However, the tracking task alone might not be a valid indicator for heartbeat perception, an assumption strengthened by the regular observation of a weak association between performance measures of the heartbeat tracking and the discrimination task [4, 15, 29, 40].

Similar to the feedback training, the coherent breathing condition showed no effects on interoceptive accuracy and metacognitive awareness. Probably, this training was too short or too difficult to change the blood-flow in brain areas related to interoception [17]. Nevertheless, the absence of a performance increase argues against a strong optimization effect of the current breathing intervention [18, 34] and future studies should probably apply trainings with longer durations [44] or should investigate coherent breathing as an acute strategy to modulate cardiac perception.

A further limitation of the present study is that beside the heartbeat tracking task also the heartbeat discrimination task has been criticized [45]. The major weakness of this task is that individuals largely differ in the way they perceive delayed feedback as synchronous with their heartbeats or not [45]. This might explain why only one of three (to four) participants can solve the heartbeat discrimination task adequately [45, 46]. Nevertheless, Wiens et al. [47] indicted that most individuals perceive intervals of about 200ms as synchronous and shorter as well as longer intervals more likely as asynchronous [48]. The applied discrimination task was grounded on this evidence.

4.1 Conclusions

Brief interventions such as biofeedback training and coherent breathing may not strongly alter cardiac interoceptive accuracy and metacognition. Potential training effects seem to depend on the specific task applied but not the phenomenon of cardiac sensations itself. Therefore, future studies should use different tasks assessing complementary aspects of cardiac interoception simultaneously in order to investigate interoceptive accuracy and metacognition in more detail.

4.2 Registration

This study was pre-registered in the Open Science Framework (OSF, https://osf.io/9fxn6;doi:10.17605/OSF.IO/9FXN6)

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Hypotheses chart.

Distribution of likelihood ratios for observed data under an alternative hypothesis vs. under the null hypothesis. The graph is based on an adapted R script provided by Uri Simonsohn (see http://datacolada.org/appendix/78/hypchart%20post%202019%2009%2011.R). The inverse value of the presented likelihood ratio represents the ratio that data are more likely under the null hypothesis vs. under an alternative hypothesis (e.g., d = 1.21).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248246.s002

(DOCX)

References

  1. 1. Meyerholz L, Irzinger J, Witthöft M, Gerlach AL, Pohl A. Contingent biofeedback outperforms other methods to enhance the accuracy of cardiac interoception: A comparison of short interventions. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2019; 63:12–20. pmid:30557753.
  2. 2. Schandry R. Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiol. 1981; 18:483–8. pmid:7267933
  3. 3. Dale A, Anderson D. Information variables in voluntary control and classical conditioning of heart rate: Field dependence and heart-rate perception. Percept Mot Skills. 1978; 47:79–85. pmid:704264
  4. 4. Phillips GC, Jones GE, Rieger EJ, Snell JB. Effects of the presentation of false heart-rate feedback on the performance of two common heartbeat-detection tasks. Psychophysiology. 1999; 36:504–10. pmid:10432800.
  5. 5. Windmann S, Schonecke OW, Fröhlig G, Maldener G. Dissociating beliefs about heart rates and actual heart rates in patients with cardiac pacemakers. Psychophysiology. 1999; 36:339–42. pmid:10352557.
  6. 6. Ring C, Brener J. Heartbeat counting is unrelated to heartbeat detection: A comparison of methods to quantify interoception. Psychophysiology. 2018; 55:e13084. pmid:29633292.
  7. 7. Zamariola G, Maurage P, Luminet O, Corneille O. Interoceptive accuracy scores from the heartbeat counting task are problematic: Evidence from simple bivariate correlations. Biol Psychol. 2018; 137:12–7. pmid:29944964.
  8. 8. Desmedt O, Luminet O, Corneille O. The heartbeat counting task largely involves non-interoceptive processes: Evidence from both the original and an adapted counting task. Biol Psychol. 2018; 138:185–8. pmid:30218689.
  9. 9. Ainley V, Tsakiris M, Pollatos O, Schulz A, Herbert BM. Comment on « Zamariola et al. (2018), Interoceptive Accuracy Scores are Problematic: Evidence from Simple Bivariate Correlations”-The empirical data base, the conceptual reasoning and the analysis behind this statement are misconceived and do not support the authors’ conclusions. Biol Psychol. 2020; 152:107870. pmid:32061687.
  10. 10. Whitehead WE, Drescher VM, Heiman P, Blackwell B. Relation of heart rate control to heartbeat perception. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 1977; 2:371–92. pmid:612350
  11. 11. Couto B, Adolfi F, Sedeño L, Salles A, Canales-Johnson A, Alvarez-Abut P, et al. Disentangling interoception: insights from focal strokes affecting the perception of external and internal milieus. Front Psychol. 2015; 6:503. pmid:25983697.
  12. 12. Aspell JE, Heydrich L, Marillier G, Lavanchy T, Herbelin B, Blanke O. Turning body and self inside out: Visualized heartbeats alter bodily self-consciousness and tactile perception. Psychological Science. 2013; 24:2445–53. pmid:24104506.
  13. 13. Khalsa SS, Lapidus RC. Can interoception improve the pragmatic search for biomarkers in psychiatry. Front Psychiatry. 2016; 7:121. pmid:27504098.
  14. 14. Jones GE. Perception of visceral sensations: A review of recent findings, methodologies, and future directions. Advances in psychophysiology: A research annual, Vol.5. London, England: Jessica Kingsley Publishers; 1994. pp. 55–191.
  15. 15. Garfinkel SN, Seth AK, Barrett AB, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biological Psychol. 2015; 104:65–74. pmid:25451381.
  16. 16. Fleming SM, Weil RS, Nagy Z, Dolan RJ, Rees G. Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure. Science. 2010; 329:1541–3. pmid:20847276.
  17. 17. Vaschillo E, Vaschillo B, Buckman J, Bates M. New approach for brain stimulation. Brain Stimulation. 2019; 12:393.
  18. 18. Schwerdtfeger AR, Schwarz G, Pfurtscheller K, Thayer JF, Jarczok MN, Pfurtscheller G. Heart rate variability (HRV): From brain death to resonance breathing at 6 breaths per minute. Clin Neurophysiol. 2020; 131:676–93. pmid:31978852.
  19. 19. Lehrer P, Kaur K, Sharma A, Shah K, Huseby R, Bhavsar J, et al. Heart rate variability biofeedback improves emotional and physical health and performance: A systematic review and meta analysis. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2020; 45:109–29. pmid:32385728.
  20. 20. Lehrer PM, Gevirtz R. Heart rate variability biofeedback: how and why does it work. Front Psychol. 2014; 5:756. pmid:25101026.
  21. 21. McCraty R, Atkinson M, Tomasino D, Bradley R. The coherent heart: heart- brain interactions, psychophysiological coherence, and the emergence of system-wide order. Integral Review. 2009; 5:10–115.
  22. 22. Monti A, Porciello G, Tieri G, Aglioti SM. The “embreathment” illusion highlights the role of breathing in corporeal awareness. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2020; 123:420–7. pmid:31800367.
  23. 23. Mindfulness Gibson J., interoception, and the body: A Contemporary perspective. Front Psychol. 2019; 10:2012. pmid:31572256.
  24. 24. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res. 2007; 39:175–91. pmid:17695343
  25. 25. Nuijten MB, van Assen MALM, Veldkamp CLS, Wicherts JM. The replication paradox: Combining studies can decrease accuracy of effect size estimates. Review of General Psychology. 2015; 19:172–82.
  26. 26. Schäfer T, Schwarz MA. The meaningfulness of effect sizes in psychological research: Differences between sub-disciplines and the impact of potential biases. Front Psychol. 2019; 10:813. pmid:31031679.
  27. 27. Mehling WE, Price C, Daubenmier JJ, Acree M, Bartmess E, Stewart A. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). PLoS ONE. 2012; 7:e48230. pmid:23133619.
  28. 28. Peirce JW. PsychoPy-Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2007; 162:8–13. pmid:17254636.
  29. 29. Michal M, Reuchlein B, Adler J, Reiner I, Beutel ME, Vögele C, et al. Striking discrepancy of anomalous body experiences with normal interoceptive accuracy in depersonalization-derealization disorder. PLoS ONE. 2014; 9:e89823. pmid:24587061.
  30. 30. Schwerdtfeger AR, Heene S, Messner E-M. Interoceptive awareness and perceived control moderate the relationship between cognitive reappraisal, self-esteem, and cardiac activity in daily life. Int J Psychophysiol. 2019; 141:84–92. pmid:30965059.
  31. 31. Kleckner IR, Wormwood JB, Simmons WK, Barrett LF, Quigley KS. Methodological recommendations for a heartbeat detection-based measure of interoceptive sensitivity. Psychophysiology. 2015; 52:1432–40. pmid:26265009.
  32. 32. Fleming SM, Lau HC. How to measure metacognition. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014; 8:443. pmid:25076880.
  33. 33. Canales-Johnson A, Silva C, Huepe D, Rivera-Rei Á, Noreika V, Garcia MdC, et al. Auditory feedback differentially modulates behavioral and neural markers of objective and subjective performance when tapping to your heartbeat. Cereb Cortex. 2015; 25:4490–503. pmid:25899708.
  34. 34. Zaccaro A, Piarulli A, Laurino M, Garbella E, Menicucci D, Neri B, et al. How breath-control can change your life: A systematic review on psycho-physiological correlates of slow breathing. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018; 12:353. pmid:30245619.
  35. 35. Bornemann B, Herbert BM, Mehling WE, Singer T. Differential changes in self-reported aspects of interoceptive awareness through 3 months of contemplative training. Front Psychol. 2014; 5:1504. pmid:25610410.
  36. 36. Laux L, Hock M, Bergner-Koether R, Hodapp V, Renner KH. Das State-Trait-Angst-Depressions-Inventar [the state-trait anxiety-depression inventory]. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2013.
  37. 37. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Paulo Alto CA: Consulting Psychologist Press; 1970.
  38. 38. Morey RD, Rouder JN. Bayes factor approaches for testing interval null hypotheses. Psychol Methods. 2011; 16:406–19. Epub 2011/07/25. pmid:21787084.
  39. 39. Bauchanan EM, Gillenwaters AM, Scofield JE, Valentine KD. MOTE: Effect size and confidence interval calculator; 2019.
  40. 40. Knoll JF, Hodapp V. A comparison between two methods for assessing heartbeat perception. Psychophysiol. 1992; 29:218–22. pmid:1635964
  41. 41. Ring C, Brener J, Knapp K, Mailloux J. Effects of heartbeat feedback on beliefs about heart rate and heartbeat counting: a cautionary tale about interoceptive awareness. Biol Psychol. 2015; 104:193–8. pmid:25553874.
  42. 42. Mehling WE, Acree M, Stewart A, Silas J, Jones A. The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness, version 2 (MAIA-2). PLoS ONE. 2018; 13:e0208034. pmid:30513087.
  43. 43. Maniscalco B, Lau H. Signal detection theory analysis of type 1 and type 2 data: Meta-d′, response-specific meta-d′, and the unequal variance SDT model. In: Fleming SM, editor. The cognitive neuroscience of metacognition. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer; 2014. pp. 25–66.
  44. 44. Tatschl JM, Hochfellner SM, Schwerdtfeger AR. Implementing mobile HRV biofeedback as adjunctive therapy during inpatient psychiatric rehabilitation facilitates recovery of depressive symptoms and enhances autonomic functioning short-term: A 1-Year Pre–Post-intervention follow-up pilot study. Front Neurosci. 2020; 14. pmid:32792897
  45. 45. Brener J, Ring C. Towards a psychophysics of interoceptive processes: The measurement of heartbeat detection. Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci. 2016; 371. pmid:28080972.
  46. 46. Brener J, Liu X, Ring C. A method of constant stimuli for examining heartbeat detection: comparison with the Brener-Kluvitse and Whitehead methods. Psychophysiology. 1993; 30:657–65. pmid:8248457.
  47. 47. Wiens S, Palmer SN. Quadratic trend analysis and heartbeat detection. Biological Psychol. 2001; 58:159–75. pmid:11600243
  48. 48. Brener J, Kluvitse C. Heartbeat detection: Judgments of the simultaneity of external stimuli and heartbeats. Psychophysiology. 1988; 25:554–61. pmid:3186884.