Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Infant-adult synchrony in spontaneous and nonspontaneous interactions

  • Zamara Cuadros,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Laboratorio de Lenguaje Interacción y Fenomenología, Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile

  • Esteban Hurtado,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Laboratorio de Lenguaje Interacción y Fenomenología, Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile

  • Carlos Cornejo

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    cca@uc.cl

    Affiliation Laboratorio de Lenguaje Interacción y Fenomenología, Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile

Abstract

Infant-adult synchrony has been reported through observational and experimental studies. Nevertheless, synchrony is addressed differently in both cases. While observational studies measure synchrony in spontaneous infant-adult interactions, experimental studies manipulate it, inducing nonspontaneous synchronous and asynchronous interactions. A still unsolved question is to what extent differ spontaneous synchrony from the nonspontaneous one, experimentally elicited. To address this question, we conducted a study to compare synchrony in both interactional contexts. Forty-three 14-month-old infants were randomly assigned to one of two independent groups: (1) the spontaneous interaction context, consisting of a storytime session; and (2) the nonspontaneous interaction context, where an assistant bounced the infant in synchrony with a stranger. We employed an optical motion capture system to accurately track the time and form of synchrony in both contexts. Our findings indicate that synchrony arising in spontaneous exchanges has different traits than synchrony produced in a nonspontaneous interplay. The evidence presented here offers new insights for rethinking the study of infant-adult synchrony and its consequences on child development.

Introduction

Interpersonal synchrony is defined as the spontaneous coordination of the interactants' body patterns in time and form [14]. When two or more individuals interact in social settings, they tend to coordinate at behavioral [511], physiological [1220], and linguistic levels [2126]. This phenomenon has been observed between adults chatting in an affiliative and argumentative way [2729], solving joint tasks in a competitive or cooperative setting [30, 31], playing sports games [3234], dancing [35, 36], or performing music together [37, 38]. Coordination has also been reported between pairs of preschoolers [39] and child-adult couples [40, 41] during joint drumming. Moreover, it has been described between parents and their infants in turn-taking conversation contexts [42], free play [43], and daily life routines [44, 45].

In the field of child development, the infant's ability to synchronize with their caregivers has been reported as early as the first day of life. It has been observed that newborns coordinate the movements of their limbs with adult speech [46]. Young infants can also synchronize with adults in their facial expressions [4749], gaze direction [50, 51], vocalizations [52], body orientations [47, 53], and body movements [54, 55]. These synchronization experiences have been hypothesized to be crucial for the infants' entry into the social world [47, 49, 56]. Through synch episodes, infants start to co-regulate their endogenous rhythms [57] and to adapt to interpersonal rules [58]. Synchrony also provide the foundation for the child's later capacity for cooperation [59], symbol use [60], intersubjectivity [47], speech, and language [4, 46, 61]. Thereby, the infants' ability to achieve synchrony with adults would serve co-and self-regulatory, communicative, social-emotional, and cognitive functions [56, 57, 60, 62, 63]. This statement is supported by evidence from observational as well as experimental studies on infant-adult synchrony. Nevertheless, it is relevant to note that synchrony is addressed differently in both research lines. Here we investigate whether the differences in empirical approaches point to the same type of synchrony.

Observed synchrony approach

Observational research has approached synchrony in terms of observable body patterns of parent-infant matching in mutual attention periods [56, 64]. In these works, synchrony is quantified from detailed coding and scoring techniques of spontaneous dyadic events, such as microanalysis or behavioral coding [6466]. By estimating frequencies, durations, and concurrences, observational studies evidenced that baby-mother matching exhibits coherent body patterns in timing and rhythm even as early as the first day of life [46, 54, 67, 68]. In addition to computing descriptive and probabilistic measures, contemporary studies have also calculated the degree of coherence and lead-lag structure underlying the sequences of infant-parent contingencies through time series analysis [65]. Some temporal and rhythmic characteristics of synchrony have been observed to change over the first year of life. For example, the duration, lags- and direction of influence between interactants and their functions vary over time, but not the strength of the associations between the partners’ behavior—the low but significant correlations between infant-parents behaviors range from 0.16 to 0.20 [15, 58, 69].

Mother-infant coordination has been reported to increase in length and mutuality along development. Early synchronous interactions have been characterized as shorter and less reciprocal than those observed at around 9 months [56, 62]. In the earliest interactions, mothers assume an active role in adjusting and modulating infants' biological rhythms [57, 70]. They follow the infants' responses, scaffolding their physiological regulation [4, 71]. When infants become more active interaction partners at around 3- to 9-months, the lead-follow structure grows into mutual during authentic turn-taking exchanges (i.e., infant-leads/mother-follows and mother-leads/infant-follows) [15, 57, 71].

Given that synchrony relies on each partner's contribution, its presence is not constant throughout the interpersonal interaction [49]. Recurrent but intermittent shifts from mismatched states to matching states have been reported in interactions between mothers and their sons with ages ranging from 3- to 9- month-olds [72] and 14- to 18- month-olds [54]. These matching episodes occurred during less than 40% of the entire interaction time [42, 49, 56, 73]. Furthermore, less synchronized and asynchronous exchanges have been found when one or both individuals disengage from the interaction due to changes in interactional goals [74] and the physiological and psychological functioning [13, 70, 75]. For instance, dyadic synchrony is affected by infants' physiological immaturity at birth and infant negative reactivity [76, 77]. It also is impaired by physio -and psychological dysregulation of mothers related to high cortisol levels [78], low oxytocin levels [15, 79], and mood disorders (e.g., depression and anxiety) [8083].

Observational studies suggest that synchrony in early infancy may vary according to the qualities of relationships, given the link between emotional engagement and synchrony. There were found differences in synchrony between mother-infant and father-infant [69, 71, 84]; in the same-sex infant-parent dyads in contrast to the opposite sex dyads [69, 84]; and between mothers and their twins compared to mothers and their singletons [85]. Although spontaneous interactions between infants and unfamiliar adults have been less explored, one study found that synchrony between mothers and 14- to 18-month-olds unknown infants was weaker than that between mothers and their sons [54].

As to the lead-lag structure of reciprocal relationships, it has been reported that the time lag of synch responsivity between who is leading and who follows the interaction decreases as infants develop—ranging from 3 s to 1 s, indeed reaching zero-lag [58, 69, 82, 83]. These experiences of mutual synch provide a window to shape the infant's emotional and social skills [71]. Mother-infant synchrony observed at 3 and 9 months old has been associated with secure attachment at 1 year [67, 86, 87]; self-control [73], symbolic play, and internal state talk [60] at 2 years; and empathy at 13 years old [58].

Elicited synchrony approach

The consequences of coordinated motion for child development has also been addressed by eliciting infant-adult synchrony. These works differ from observational studies in conceptualization and measurement of synchrony, dyad composition, and target age. Most of the observational studies focus on interactions between under 1-year-old infants and their mothers or fathers. Experimental works have explored the effects of infant-unknown adult synchrony beyond the first birthday when they are growing into more active social agents [88, 89]. By 12 months old, infants understand that people's actions can be goal-directed and provided them information to reach them their aims [63, 90, 91]. At 14 months old, infants also begin to cooperate with strangers [9294]

Experimental research has approached synchrony in terms of infant-unknown adult matching in shared movement experiences [9597]. In these studies, an assistant sways the infant in synchrony or asynchrony according to the interaction partner's movements. The accuracy of synchrony manipulation is almost always verified by interjudge reliability—except for one work that measured and compared the acceleration of the assistant and experimenter across the conditions by using a Nintendo Wii remotes at their waists [89]. This procedure provides infants with a particular shared movement experience in form and time. The assistant and experimenter mirror their movements, thereby synchrony acquires the same morphology across time. Given that the equal cycles of a specific action are repeated at the same time intervals, steady synch in time and rhythm occurs while the activity length [89, 97].

The coordination elicited through this procedure guarantees an experience alike to that experienced in situations of musical engagement. Similar to elicited synchrony, "musical engagement involves temporal alignment of movements to evenly spaced, predictable beats" [89]. Since the prosocial effects of musical engagement in adults are well known [98101], elicited synchrony has been used to explore the prosocial consequences of move in synch with infants who are still not good at moving to a regular beat [89]. For example, it has been found that to move steadily together with a partner guides infants' social choices at 12 months, but not at 9 months [97]. Interestingly, after being bounced to music in synchrony, 14-month-old infants enhanced helpfulness towards an unknown adult compared to those who were asynchronously bounced to the music [89, 102, 103]. The bouncing in sync improved infants' helping behavior more than asynchronous bouncing, even in the absence of music [104]. Similar results have been reported for preschoolers. Four-year-old children [59, 96] and 8–9-year-old children [105] increased their cooperation towards an unfamiliar peer following synchronous movement, compared with conditions of asynchronous movement or movement absence.

Experimental studies with adults have also reported that the underlying emotional background to social relationships precedes and proceeds to episodes of body synchronization. For example, studies with adults show that prior affective bond between two strangers impacts their subsequent bodily coordination. Thus, adults establishing positive affective ties synchronize with each other more than those who create a negative emotional bond [27, 106]. There is also evidence that following a coordinated interaction between adults, their positive feelings towards the interaction partner increase [30, 35, 107109]. It has also been reported that synchronous interaction between children improves perceived similarity and closeness towards each other [105].

The present study

Although both lines of research have successfully studied the consequences of dyadic synchronization, the type of synchrony addressed seems to be different. Observational studies measure synchrony as it spontaneously emerges during infant-parent interactions. In particular, they measure the temporal dimension of synchrony regardless of their form. On the other hand, instead of measuring synchrony, experimental studies manipulate it, inducing nonspontaneous synchronous and asynchronous interactions between infants and unfamiliar adults. Research on synchrony between adults shows a similar trend. Observational studies measure the temporal synchronization naturally emerging between pairs of conversing adults [27, 28, 108, 110, 111]. Recently, spontaneous morphological synchronization has also been described in adults. Coordination between conversing adults adopted two shapes: mirror-like and anatomical [112]. While mirror-like coordination implicates movements that reflect those of the partner-i.e., ipsilateral ones, anatomical coordination involves movements that reconstruct the partners’ body-scheme-i.e., contralateral ones [113, 114]. Conversely, experimental studies elicit synchrony in adults through joint tasks that require intentional or unintentional synchronization in time and form with a referent (e.g., a metronome, a pre-recorded video, or another adult participant). Examples of such joint tasks include finger tapping [107, 115], rocking in a rocking chair [116, 117], swinging pendulums [118120], climbing stairs [106], walking [121124] or jumping [125].

To date, no known studies have measured and differentiated the temporal and morphological dimensions of synchrony in spontaneous and nonspontaneous interactions. A key question is to what extent synchrony emerging in spontaneous interactions differs from that produced in a nonspontaneous interaction. Are the temporal and morphological dynamics of synchronous movement generated spontaneously comparable to those of being externally bounced in synchrony? To address this issue, we conducted a study to compare synchrony simultaneously in spontaneous and nonspontaneous infant-adult interactions. We measured synchrony between infants and an unfamiliar adult. We employed an optical motion capture system (henceforth: mocap) to accurately track participants' interplays. Through this device, we ensured to record, measure, and compare the participants' subtle movements rather than movements perceptible to the eye. In particular, the slight backs movements of participants were tracked while they involved in: (1) a nonspontaneous interaction context, in which an assistant bounced the infant in synchrony with a stranger; and (2) a spontaneous interaction context consisting of a storytime session, in which participants freely interact. We also asked the caregiver to report the perceived affective-state in the infant before each interactive session.

In sum, we reproduced the interaction contexts previously introduced by experimental and observational research lines but using a different method of capture and analysis in order to register the temporal and morphological differences in synchrony. Clarifying this issue is essential to a better understanding of dyadic coordination phenomena. If synchrony is different in each case, it would be suitable to introduce a denomination that corresponds to their features. Such a distinction would shed light on the complex relationship between synchronization and its consequences for social development. It would open a promising scenario to explore and discuss the specific effects of the distinct temporal and morphological characteristics of synchrony.

Materials and methods

Participants

Forty-three 14-month-old typically developing infants (22 girls; M age = 14.3 months; SD = 0.2 months) were recruited from Family Health Centers in Santiago de Chile city and day nurseries at the Chilean National Board of Kindergartens and the Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile. We select 14-month-old infants due to literature indicating that at that age, they have gained some motor independence and help strangers to reach their goals [63, 89, 90, 9294, 102104]. All infants included in the study were able to walk independently and had parents without any diagnosis of mental disorders. Seven additional infants who participated in the study were excluded from the final analyses due to equipment failure (n = 1), excessive fussiness (n = 3), and disengagement from the interaction to look for the mother or to turn away from the storyteller (n = 3). The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of Social Sciences at the Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile and the Ethical Committee of Metropolitan Health Service South-East. Informed consent was obtained from all parents.

Design

Infants were randomly assigned to one of two independent groups: the spontaneous interaction context (n = 22, 12 girls) and the nonspontaneous interaction context (n = 21, 10 girls). While the spontaneous interaction context comprised a storytime session, the nonspontaneous interaction context included an activity in which an assistant bounced the infant in synchrony with a stranger in the absence of an audible musical stimulus [104]. We did not include audible musical stimuli in each condition to control the known power of music to induce emotions, feelings, affiliation, and musical engagement [99, 101, 126128]. These variables have been reported to influence synchrony between interactants [30, 106, 108, 129]. In both conditions, synchrony was measured by means of mocap. All infants participated in a familiarization phase before synchrony was measured. In each condition infants took part in an interaction with one of two unknown female adults (M age = 21.5 years old; SD = 0.7 years old). Each storyteller interacted roughly with the same number of participants balanced by sex and condition (storyteller 1 = 22; storyteller 2 = 21). Both unknown adults were blind to the objectives of this study and were not acquainted with any participant. Based on reports of on the bidirectional relationships between synchrony and affect, we required all parents to report the perceived intensity of the infant’s affect-state before each interaction session by means of the Spanish version of the PANAS-C-SF [130].

Apparatus

The body movements of interacting pairs were recorded with a mocap consisting of 36 NaturalPoint Optitrack Prime 41 cameras and a personal computer running Motive software. The cameras were positioned close to the ceiling, surrounding a rectangular perimeter (width 3 m x depth 4 m x height 2 m) in a room. The mocap tracks the position of small infrared-reflective spherical markers. We followed the recording protocol by Cornejo, Hurtado [112] to improve participants’ comfort. Seven reflective markers were positioned on each person's body: the upper back (2x), the elbows (2x), and the head (3x). We used elastic bands to sustain the small reflective markers around the adult body. In infants, we used a comfortable sweater and a baby hat bearing the markers adhered to the key positions. Position signals were recorded at 120 Hz (sampling periods per second).

Procedure

Upon each family’s arrival at the laboratory, a female assistant executed the familiarization phase. In this phase, she followed the next sequence of actions. First, she familiarized the participants with the video and optical cameras in the room. She explained and showed the mocap functioning to parents before described the study procedure. Second, the assistant instructed in detail to parents about their role and location during the sessions. She explained to parents that they could play freely with their children during the familiarization phase, but they should avoid interrupting the interaction activity with the unknown adult. She requested the parents to remain silent and sitting behind the infants -out of their visual field-, once the familiarization phase was ended. Third, the assistant invited parents (1) to sit behind infants, who were standing up around a table, and (2) to animate them to play with toys settled on the table. The unknown adult was located on one side of the table and already had the reflective markers adhered on her body. The unknown adult was instructed to remain with a gentle attitude but without interacting directly with them. Fourth, the assistant asked the parents to dress the infant in the sweater and hat that had the markers adhered to the key positions, while he/she played. Fifth, the assistant asked parents to rate the emotional intensity perceived in the infant during their stay in the laboratory by answering the Spanish version of the PANAS-C-SF [130]. Finally, the assistant removed the toys from the table and told the parents that the interaction phase was about to begin. She reminded them to remain silent without disrupting the activity and to stay sitting on a chair behind the infants out of their line of sight.

After the familiarization phase, the infants took part in an interaction phase with one of the two unknown female adults. In the spontaneous interaction context, the infant engaged in a face-to-face storytime session with an unknown adult; both were located in front of each other, around a table separating them (see Fig 1). The adult spontaneously showed and told the Bérengère Delaporte’s short story “Where is Mom Elephant?” to the infant. The story narrates the search a baby elephant makes of his mother in the forest until he finds her. The session was recorded by mocap and video cameras and lasted on average, approximately 145 s (the duration range was from 126 s to 151 s).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Illustration of the room setting and spatial disposition of the participants in spontaneous interaction context.

36 Natural Point Prime-41 purpose-specific cameras were positioned close to the ceiling, creating a rectangular perimeter above and surrounding the participants. During the reading session, the infant and the unknown adult were located in front of each other around a square table separating them.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244138.g001

In the nonspontaneous interaction context, the infant participated in a face-to-face steady swaying session with an unknown adult. We emulated the bounce in sync without music by Cirelli, Wan [104]. Thereby, the assistant held and bounced the infant in a carrier facing the unknown adult (see Fig 2). The assistant and the unknown adult bounced gently in synchrony according to a rhythmic beep to which only they were listening via headphones. The movement session lasted 145 s and was recorded by mocap and video camera.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Depiction of the room setting and spatial disposition of the participants in nonspontaneous interaction context.

During the bouncing session, the assistant that held the infant and the unknown adult were standing in front of each other.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244138.g002

Preprocessing

We preprocessed the initial 120 s of each interaction following the recommended protocol by Cornejo, Hurtado [112]. Data preprocessing was performed manually. We used custom scripts to trajectorize the markers. Mocap data for each couple were labeled with corresponding body parts and identified the participant to which each marker belonged. Finally, we visually inspected the results. This decision of selecting 120 s of each interaction to analyze is consistent with the interaction times analyzed in synchronization manipulation studies, ranging from 40 to 144 s. After preprocessing, our data consisted of one time series for the positions of each interactant.

Analysis

To quantify bodily coordination between a child and an adult, we measured linear relationships between the motion (speeds) of both by means of Pearson correlations. Those were computed both for immediacy and for several positive and negative delays. In other words, the result was a cross-correlation curve, that displays a Pearson correlation level for each of the studied time lag values, whether the infant and the adult coordinate immediately (lag = 0), or any of the two imitates motion of the other within a short time difference (lag ≠ 0). We wanted to look for significant correlation patterns with a high statistical power, so to capture even subtle coordinations. This was relevant since the possibility of much smaller coordinations in one condition than the other was embedded in our research question: we needed to find and measure spontaneous coordinations, which could be smaller and less consistent than clearer nonspontaneous coordinations from bouncing.

In order to make an efficient use of data, we followed the statistical inference strategy in Cornejo, Hurtado [112], which proceeds as follows. First, a cross-correlation curve is computed for each experimental session. This yields one Pearson correlation per session, per delay time. Second, for each delay time, correlations for all sessions are pooled into a single Pearson correlation value that corresponds to that delay (see Cornejo, Hurtado [112], section “Aggregation of Cross-Correlation Curves”, for details). By correlation pooling we mean that, in the same fashion as a regular Pearson correlation, we sum up deviation products and square deviations, but those deviations are not computed as a difference from grand averages of the two variables being correlated. Instead, we allow each session to have its own pair of average levels, so deviations are taken in the context of their own session. We use this strategy because it allows us a clean handling of the distribution of Pearson correlations, while it provides it with a single standard error to test and expand, with high statistical power, and adequate handling of the high number of degrees of freedom resulting for analyzing data at a frame to frame level. Doing this instead of averaging has the advantage of producing a number that behaves as, and effectively is, a Pearson correlation value. Therefore, it can be statistically tested as such with usual techniques. Additionally, such result is backed by a number of degrees of freedom which is much bigger than the amount of sessions (as would be the case if an average was computed), because a pooled correlation obtained this way is conceptually equivalent to a single correlation from the concatenation of all recordings in the same experimental condition. Third, this aggregate correlation in a scale from -1 to 1 is transformed into a normally distributed variable by means of a Fisher transform, which consists in applying an inverted hyperbolic tangent function to the correlation value r.

(1)

This enables the following fourth step: to test whether the correlation is significantly different from zero by a simple z test (classic normal distribution) with a standard error very close to (2) where m is the number of recorded sessions and ni is the length of the i-th session in number of sampling periods [i.e. equivalent to frames in a video]. Note how standard error rapidly decreases as the number of recorded frames (of which there are several per second) increase, a fact that leads to a very efficient use of data. This approximation for the standard error, and the whole Fisher transform approach works well for high numbers of degrees of freedom, in contrast to usual t-tests for correlations which tend to increase type I error rate as degrees of freedom (i.e., sample size) increase.

As a result, even a small hypothetical study, with a sample size of 10 sessions, at 100 seconds each, with 10 samples per second, would yield a standard error of . As previously mentioned, our correlations result from pooling Pearson correlations; meaning that deviations are not computed from grand averages, but from averages in each session. This allows us to extract a single standard error figure in the same fashion as the Student t statistic pools errors from both samples, or ANOVA error mean squares pool errors from groups or subjects with different means. Since Fisher transform is practically an identity function (i.e., does not need to be applied) for correlations of small magnitude, this means that correlations of magnitude as small as |r|≈0.02 (two standard deviations) can be detected with statistical significance. For this study, with around 20 sessions in each condition, at 120 second each, with a sampling rate of 120 Hz, correlations from (two standard deviations of 0.002) can be regarded as statistically significant. It is important to note that, just like traditional t testing on correlations, and unlike other common inference tests, when the statistic under study is correlation, only the sample size and correlation value determine the statistical significance. Standard error is a function of those, and therefore does not need to be provided additionally. This is related to the fact that, even such small correlations would not survive the Pearson formula for such big numbers of samples if they were noise. In consequence, with such sample sizes, even these small correlations can result from a remarkable consistency.

Results

Fig 3 shows cross-correlation curves for the motion data captured in spontaneous and nonspontaneous interactions. We tested differences with respect to zero Pearson correlation values in cross-correlation curves. Around each cross-correlation curve was plotted a confidence interval (see the colored area surrounding the curves). The correlations are statistically significant where their confidence intervals do not touch the zero-correlation horizontal line. We used the Fisher transformation for p-values and the confidence interval computation based on a Holm-Bonferroni correction for the 41 correlation values in a cross-correlation curve to control for the family-wise error rate. The alpha level was set at 0.001.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Cross-correlation curves for spontaneous and nonspontaneous interactions.

The colored area surrounding the curve indicates the confidence interval. Negative lag times in the plot correspond to adult lagging behind the infant, i.e., the adult's reactions to the infant. Conversely, positive lag times correspond to the infant’s reactions to the unknown adult. Data have been organized so that positive correlation values indicate to mirror-like coordination between the interactants. Conversely, negative correlation values correspond to anatomical coordination between interactants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244138.g003

When comparing both conditions in Fig 3, we observed correlations with different magnitudes and morphologies. As the confidence intervals displaying around the curves do not overlap or touch the zero-correlation horizontal line, the correlations reveal statistically significant differences between the spontaneous and nonspontaneous conditions. The exceptions are a delayed coordination performed by the adult (t = -0.3 s, r = -0.000, p = 0.784) in the nonspontaneous condition and zero-lag coordination in the spontaneous condition curve (t = 0.0 s, r = -0.001, p = 0.685). Conversely, zero-lag coordination in the nonspontaneous condition is the highest correlation peak observed in Fig 3. This peak corresponds to simultaneous coordination between an adult and the infant being externally bounced in synchrony (t = 0 s, r = 0.80, p < 0.001), i.e., they move in a similar fashion and at the same time. The oscillating shape adopted by this curve is a consequence of the high consistency of the simultaneous coordination. Because of the strong general periodicity and similarity of both motion signals, the time shifting used to compute cross-correlations produces an alternation between lags at which signals are similar and lags at which they are opposite, with zero-lag corresponding to highest similarity.

Fig 4 zooms in on the cross-correlation curve for the spontaneous condition. In the positive quadrant of the upper half-plane, the curve shows a mirror-like coordination pattern between the participants. This result means that positive correlation values represent coordination where the infant tends to display a motion pattern reflecting the adult motion but later (e.g., the interactants' backs approach each other). Two mirror-like correlation peaks are evident in the curve: first at t = 0.9 s (r = 0.022, p < 0.001) and second at t = 1.5 s (r = 0.021, p < 0.001). These peaks correspond to the infant’s reactions to the adult with a 0.9 s and a 1.5 s lag, respectively. In the negative quadrant of the lower half-plane, the curve displays an anatomical coordination pattern between interactants. Negative correlation values denote that adult's movements reproduce the infant body-scheme with a lag (e.g., reverse motion between participants' backs). Two anatomical correlation peaks are evident in the curve: first at t = −0.4 s (r = -0.024, p < 0.001) and second at t = −1.6 s (r = -0.034, p < 0.001). Both peaks indicate the adult tends to imitate the infant with a 0.4 s and a 1.6 s lag, respectively.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Cross-correlation curve for spontaneous interaction.

The colored area surrounding the curve indicates the confidence interval. Negative lag times correspond to adult lagging behind the infant. Positive lag times correspond to the infant’s reactions to the unknown adult. Positive correlation values indicate to mirror-like coordination between the interactants. Negative correlation values correspond to anatomical coordination between interactants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244138.g004

Table 1 displays a comparison between cross-correlation values for the spontaneous and nonspontaneous conditions based on the times at which their main peaks occurred. A linear mixed effects model was fitted on the values of the simultaneous and delayed coordination (z scores) in both conditions, controlling for infants' sex and the unknown adult who interacted with them. The main effect of the condition still reached significance on correlation values (F(1, 39) = 382.39, p < 0.001). However, no main effects of covariates on the values of simultaneous and delayed coordination were found. There was no statistically significant effect of infants' sex (F(1, 39) = 0.55, p = .74) and the adult who interacted with them (F(1, 39) = 1.76, p = .15) on correlation values. Finally, an analysis of variance (ANOVA) on caregivers’ reports reveals that the scores of infant's affect did not differ significantly across conditions, both positive (F(1, 41) = 3.43, p = .071) and negative (F(1, 41) = 0.20, p = .66) affect.

thumbnail
Table 1. Peaks of cross-correlation values for spontaneous and nonspontaneous conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244138.t001

Discussion

Time and morphology of synchrony

The current study shows differences between synchrony accurately measured in spontaneous and nonspontaneous infant-adult interactions. Our findings indicate that synchrony emerging in spontaneous interactions differs in time and morphology from that elicited in a nonspontaneous interaction. This finding explains the ample differences in the magnitude of cross-correlation. In comparison to the induced synchrony, spontaneous coordination phenomena look very marginal; yet they are statistically significant. With the exception of synchronized bouncing in the nonspontaneous condition, our correlation values are indeed of small magnitude. This responds to the fact that coordinated motion between two people is often just a small part of total motion. Therefore, the high statistical power of our method is relevant to sensitize and test subtle coordination usually buried under bigger uncoordinated motion signals. Correlation works because it detects consistent linear relationships and is not affected too much by inconsistent (or some nonlinear) relationships. For instance, we found a couple of statistically significant correlations with values around |r|≈0.02, which corresponds to a shared variance proportion of R2 = 0.04% with respect to the whole motion of the two people under measurement. At such levels we are looking at very small coordination phenomena, hard to detect by the naked eye, both because of its little magnitude and because it can be consistently spread across considerable time within an experimental session, as opposed to easily identifiable single and clear coordination events.

Concerning time, we found higher and statistically significant zero-lag correlations in the nonspontaneous condition, compared to the spontaneous condition. This finding indicates that nonspontaneous synchrony manifests a simultaneous and temporally constant pattern of the same bounce movement throughout the 120 s interaction, hence the sinusoidal form adopted by the curve. The procedure used to synchronize participants generates this pattern. Bouncing at a constant rate produces a high correlation peak, indicating continuous and perfectly coordinated movements between partners.

By contrast, in the spontaneous interaction, synchrony at zero-lag was of a low magnitude and statistically not significant. This result accords with previous findings of low magnitudes in the zero-lag correlation of participants’ movements involved in natural interactions accurately measured. For example, a statistically nonsignificant correlation was reported in a sample of 10 adult couples debating for 10 minutes on political, social, and personal topics [27]. The lack of statistical significance was attributed to the sample size. Other studies also reported low but statistically significant correlations in samples from at least 14 couples spontaneously interacting from 10 to 15 min. In particular, zero-lag correlations were reported between adults chatting [28, 108, 112, 131]. Research using behavioral coding to study infant-parent synchrony during 5 min free-play have also evidenced low but significant correlations at zero-lag [15, 58, 69].

We assume that low synchrony at zero-lag can be related in our study to the duration of the interaction. We analyzed 120 s of spontaneous interaction to match the typical duration of the synchrony manipulation, i.e., from 40 to 140 s [89, 97, 102104]. We did not increase the interaction time to avoid the risk of generating excessive fussiness in infants who were held and bounced in a carrier by an assistant. Most likely, the observation time recorded in the current study was not wide enough to observe the emergence of simultaneous coordination in the spontaneous condition. The differences found at zero-lag on spontaneous and nonspontaneous synchrony accurately measured seem to add evidence to the hypothesis that coordination in natural interactions is discontinuous temporally. Studies on interpersonal coordination in face-to-face social encounters show that synchronous co-activity emerge swiftly and for brief periods [3, 46, 54, 66, 132].

Concerning the form of spontaneous synchronization, we found that infants tend to mirror the adult’s motion, while adults coordinate with infants’ motions in an anatomical way. Similar forms of delayed coordination were reported in a study on the relationship between synchrony and trust in natural conversations [112]. In this study, participants mirrored the assistant’s movements at 1.1 s, and the assistant was anatomically coordinated with participants at 1.3 s. Accordingly, time delays between infants and the adult as well as the difference in the coordination morphology reported here can be attributed to two factors. First, the infants and the adult did not know each other, which manifests in longer time-delays than those reported in friends’ natural interactions [108, 112]. Second, the anatomical coordination from the storyteller towards participants may indicate the interference of her role limitating a fully natural involvement with the infant. There exists indeed evidence that synchrony patterns change importantly when one of the two participants play a confederate role, resulting in a disengagement from the interaction partner while attending to her role demands [35, 112]. This interpretation is supported by evidence from research on the development of imitation. Studies in this field show that preschoolers, adolescents, and adults tend to imitate in a mirror-like way rather than anatomically [113, 133, 134]. However, anatomical imitations can be related to cognitively demanding interactions [135, 136], whereas mirror-like imitations can be involved in affectively guided interactions [113, 137]. Presumably, the anatomical coordination of the adult towards infants suggests the prevalence of her role as a member of the research team. However, we do not have evidence that allows us to confirm this conjecture. Future research could include not only measures of the cognitive and affective involvement of team members interacting with participants but also other measures to ensure greater naturalness in interactions.

Synchrony in spontaneous interactions offers different information than synchrony in nonspontaneous interactions. In the nonspontaneous synchronization, the same motion is repeated in a simultaneous way. Thus, it is only possible to calculate the strength of the association between the temporally and morphologically constant movements of participants, i.e., the magnitude of correlations at zero-lag. In the spontaneous interaction, synchrony occurs with different time-lags at the millisecond scale. As a consequence, it can be computed the magnitude of correlations at different time-delays, including the zero-lag. The additional information on the temporal dimension of synchrony in spontaneous interactions allows also the estimation of the form adopted in simultaneous and delayed coordination as well as the lead-follow structure. Concerning the form of spontaneous coordination, we found that it could be anatomical or mirror-like. Regarding the lead-lag structure of infant-adult spontaneous coordination, we observed bidirectionality. While the infant tends to lead the interaction, the adult follows, and vice versa. The magnitudes of the delayed coordination we observe are low but statistically significant. Although these magnitudes are lower than those found in observational studies [15, 58, 69], they concur with reports of studies accurately measuring synchrony between chatting adults [108, 112, 131]. The coincidence between our findings and research with adults may be due to the nature of the capture and analysis methods used.

Distinguishing macro- from micro-coordinations

As a whole, our results suggest that the term ‘synchrony’ covers different interactional phenomena, observable at distinct scales. On the one hand, in nonspontaneous synchrony emerge very large coordination phenomena, easily identifiable because of its high magnitude, and temporally and morphologically simultaneous and stable pattern across the movement session. This macro-coordination emerges once interactants deliberately agree to move equal in unison by a defined time [95, 138]. Numerous studies have already informed macro-coordination phenomena between adults, children, and children-adult occurring during a variety of interpersonal activities oriented towards a common goal. For instance, there are reports of macro-coordination during events that are highly structured and require practice, such as joint music performance [40, 98, 139], rule games [30, 140, 141], sports [34, 142, 143], and dancing [36, 144, 145], among others.

On the other hand, spontaneously produced coordination can hardly be detected by the naked eye, both because of its low magnitude and because it accounts for only a fraction of the total motion of people interacting. Micro-coordination phenomena emerge swiftly, for brief periods, and discontinuously in time and morphology during spontaneous and less-structured situations that do not seem to require practice and conscious effort [1, 46, 54, 146]. The features of micro-coordination have already been reported by numerous studies on interpersonal synchrony between adults, and importantly, also on adult-infants synchrony, performed in natural and semi-natural interactional contexts [15, 46, 54, 58, 60, 69, 73, 108, 112, 131, 147].

The differences in synchronous movements between spontaneous and nonspontaneous infant-adult interactions suggest strongly that both coordinative phenomena should be conceptually distinguished. While spontaneous synchrony reveals the emergence of micro-coordination, nonspontaneous synchrony elicits macro-coordination phenomena. As a result, the evidence presented here suggest that the use of the same term (‘synchrony’) is covering differing types of coordination. We consider that macro- and micro-coordinations can be suitable terms to conceptually refer to two synchronous phenomena with different temporal and morphological scales. However, other distinctions based on their distinctive features could be useful for this purpose.

The distinction between the two types of synchrony may be useful to clarify the social effects of synchrony. Both kinds of coordination have demonstrated to be effective in influencing infants' emotional and social skills [71, 95]. For example, the self-propelled infant-parent synchrony has been shown to improve the child's development of secure attachment [67, 86, 87], self-control [73], and internal state talk [60] at 2 years. Similarly, the passively experienced synchrony with an unfamiliar adult has increased infant prosocial behavior [89, 102104]. However, we still do not know much about the prosocial effects of self-propelled synchrony, i.e., micro-coordinations. We also know too little about the emotional consequences of elicited synchrony, i.e., macro-coordinations. The distinction between micro- and macro-coordinations could be useful to investigate and discuss whether the way in which each synchrony impacts social and emotional interaction skills is the same. Thereby, an issue to solve is if each coordination is related to other variables that may precede or follow them (e.g., emotional engagement, perceived similarity, affective involvement) and how that relationship explains the social or emotional consequences. As a result, distinguish micro- from micro-coordinations could have additional implications for the methodology synchrony research, as well as for the interventions build on their results.

Limitations and future directions

Some study limitations should be mentioned that may be pondered in future researches. First, we explored synchrony from subtle torso movements intending to analyze the temporal and morphological dynamics of the same type of matching behaviors across the conditions. Previous reports indicate that this area of the body is highly sensitive to changes in interactions of participants, which makes it an objective marker of behavioral synchrony [30, 148154]. Due to our focus on the torso motion, other behavioral and biological markers sensitive to interactional synchrony were not considered. Synchrony can in fact be measured at different levels [146], such as psycho-physiological (e.g., galvanic skin response [13, 155], heartbeat rate [20, 70], and breath rate [4, 156]); neurophysiological (e.g., alpha, beta, and gamma frequency bands [157, 158]); linguistic (e.g., vocal rhythm and tone [52], utterance length [159], and speaking rate [160]); and behavioral (e.g., body limbs motion [2, 46, 119, 120], gaze direction [50, 51, 161], facial expressions of emotions [42, 84, 85] and gestures [2, 3, 162]). Future work should take into account these synchrony levels, which may require a combination of different data capture and analysis strategies and other interactional contexts.

Second, we used the storytime session and the bounce in sync as operationalizations of spontaneous and nonspontaneous interactions respectively. While the former was chosen for its naturalness, since it is an ordinary activity for children, the latter replicated previous studies [89, 102104]. Nonetheless, other feasible spontaneous and nonspontaneous contexts could have been thought. In particular, different coordination patterns are likely to be found when comparing the described nonspontaneous interaction, with other spontaneous interactional situation such as a dance session to the rhythm of the music. A dance session, although natural for children and physically more similar to the nonspontaneous bounce in sync condition, implies nevertheless music as an additional variable, which we planned to control in both conditions of this study (see Design section). Although previous research has successfully studied the effects of elicited synchrony in the presence or absence of an audible musical stimulus for interactants [89, 97, 102104], more research is needed to separately explore the role of music on the emergence of synchrony patterns during infant-adult spontaneous and nonspontaneous interactions. A still open question is to what extent vary the temporal and morphological dimension of synchrony in musical and nonmusical interactional contexts. For example, a particular unsolved question is whether the rhythmicity of repetitive behaviors in spontaneous and nonmusical interactions (e.g., joint walking or jumping) can be considered as having similar features to those underlying at spontaneous and nonspontaneous musical engagement.

Third, although spontaneous interaction consisted of a naturalistic book-reading session, it is still an activity framed in an experiment and conducted in an unknown place for the participants. We did not measure synchrony in the participants' houses, given the mobility restrictions of the mocap and the need for control. However, the proposed spontaneous situation offered a setting as close as possible to a natural interaction between an infant and an adult who reads the story. In addition to microanalysis, other recent technologies (e.g., Motion Energy Analysis [110], Frame Differencing Method [27], Facial Thermal Imprints [14]) can be used in future studies to capture synchrony in more natural activities and situations accurately.

Fourth, infants interacted with an unknown female in both conditions. Therefore, caution is needed before generalizing our findings to other relationships, specially the infant-parent ones. As we reviewed previously, synchrony is very sensitive to the psycho-physiological characteristics of each partner, as well as to the affective qualities and interactive modalities that are built during daily interactions with others. Findings even suggest differences in synchrony between mother-infant and father-infant interactions [69, 71, 84]. After the first birthday, infants and their parents would have had enough time to practice and adjust their physiological and behavioral rhythms [57]. Indeed, they would have had to become intimately familiar with each other's gestures and behavioral rhythms [54]. Furthermore, because of the affective reciprocity and the particularities shaping each close relationship, infants' motivation to interact with their acquaintances may vary [42] and, of course, differ from the motivation to interact with an unknown woman whom she/he had just met. For these reasons, a highly plausible hypothesis is that infant-unknown adult synchrony will be slighter than that between mothers and their sons. Despite this comparison exceeds the scope of this article, we point out the convenience of address this issue in future studies.

Finally, we found that infants’ affective states preceding the interaction with the assistant did not influence the synchrony levels during the spontaneous and nonspontaneous conditions. Nonetheless, we did not measure whether affect levels changed across the conditions. Previous studies have reported that factors related to affiliation influences synchrony and vice versa. For example, people who do not like each other tend to be less in sync [106, 163] than people who like each other [108, 121]. Research also show increases in variables related to affect and affiliation between adults following a nonspontaneous synchronization, positive affect [30], social bonding [35], and trust [109]. Synchronous interactions between preschoolers improve perceived similarity and closeness towards each other [105]. Thus, questions remain open for future research: Do infants’ affects change after a spontaneous or a nonspontaneous interaction with an unknown adult? Are there differences in affect changes between spontaneous and nonspontaneous interactions? Do changes in participants’ affects influence the synchrony levels? Future research has the challenge of finding new ways to objectively measure affect-states of overcoming the widely known problems of social desirability inherent to self-reports and the reliability of observational measures. Novel ways of accurately measuring affect could favor the study of the possible relationships between synchrony and emerging affect in interactions between infants and adults.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Sofía Castillo and Catalina Rozic for helped us with data recollection, and Ricardo Morales and Zhen Tham for technical assistance.

References

  1. 1. Bernieri FJ, Rosenthal R. Interpersonal coordination: behavioral matching and interactional synchrony. In: Feldman RS, Rimé B, editors. Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1991. p. 401–32.
  2. 2. Condon WS, Ogston WD. Sound film analysis of normal and pathological behavior patterns. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1966;143(4):338–47. pmid:5958766
  3. 3. Kendon A. Movement coordination in social interaction: some examples described. Acta Psychologica. 1970;32:101–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(70)90094-6 pmid:5444439
  4. 4. McFarland DH, Fortin AJ, Polka L. Physiological measures of mother-infant interactional synchrony. Developmental psychobiology. 2020;62(1):50. pmid:31493313
  5. 5. Del-Monte J, Capdevielle D, Varlet M, Marin L, Schmidt RC, Salesse RN, et al. Social motor coordination in unaffected relatives of schizophrenia patients: A potential intermediate phenotype. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2013; 7:137. pmid:24106467; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3788352
  6. 6. Doi H, Kato M, Nishitani S, Shinohara K. Development of synchrony between activity patterns of mother–infant pair from 4 to 18 months after birth. The Journal of Physiological Sciences. 2011;61(3):211–6. pmid:21424393
  7. 7. Llobera J, Charbonnier C, Chagué S, Preissmann D, Antonietti JP, Ansermet F, et al. The subjective sensation of synchrony: An experimental study. PLoS one. 2016;11(2): e0147008. pmid:26870943; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4752214
  8. 8. Marmelat V, Delignières D. Strong anticipation: complexity matching in interpersonal coordination. Experimental Brain Research. 2012;222(1):137–48. pmid:22865163
  9. 9. Ouwehand PW, Peper CE. Does interpersonal movement synchronization differ from synchronization with a moving object? Neuroscience letters. 2015;606:177–81. pmid:26327144
  10. 10. Preissmann D, Charbonnier C, Chagué S, Antonietti J-P, Llobera J, Ansermet F, et al. A Motion Capture Study to Measure the Feeling of Synchrony in Romantic Couples and in Professional Musicians. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7:1673. pmid:27833580; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5082227
  11. 11. Varlet M, Marin L, Capdevielle D, Del-Monte J, Schmidt R, Salesse R, et al. Difficulty leading interpersonal coordination: Towards an embodied signature of social anxiety disorder. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014; 8:29. pmid:24567707; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3915144
  12. 12. Atzil S, Hendler T, Feldman R. The brain basis of social synchrony. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2014;9(8):1193–202. pmid:24056729; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4127029
  13. 13. Busuito A, Quigley KM, Moore GA, Voegtline KM, Dipietro JA. In Sync: Physiological Correlates of Behavioral Synchrony in Infants and Mothers. Developmental Psychology. 2019;55(5):1034–45. pmid:30742469; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6465151
  14. 14. Ebisch SJ, Aureli T, Bafunno D, Cardone D, Romani GL, Merla A. Mother and child in synchrony: Thermal facial imprints of autonomic contagion. Biological Psychology. 2012;89(1):123–9. pmid:22001267
  15. 15. Feldman R, Gordon I, Zagoory-Sharon O. Maternal and paternal plasma, salivary, and urinary oxytocin and parent-infant synchrony: considering stress and affiliation components of human bonding. Developmental science. 2011;14(4):752. pmid:21676095
  16. 16. Thomas KA, Burr RL, Spieker S, Lee J, Chen J. Mother-infant circadian rhythm: development of individual patterns and dyadic synchrony. Early human development. 2014;90(12):885. pmid:25463836; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4312214
  17. 17. Tsai S-Y, Barnard KE, Lentz MJ, Thomas KA. Mother-Infant Activity Synchrony as a Correlate of the Emergence of Circadian Rhythm. Biological Research For Nursing. 2011;13(1):80–8. pmid:20798158
  18. 18. Gottman JM, Levenson RW. A valid procedure for obtaining self-report of affect in marital interaction. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1985;53(2):151–60. pmid:3998244
  19. 19. Konvalinka I, Roepstorff A. The two-brain approach: how can mutually interacting brains teach us something about social interaction? Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2012;6:215. pmid:22837744; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3402900
  20. 20. Mitkidis P, McGraw JJ, Roepstorff A, Wallot S. Building trust: Heart rate synchrony and arousal during joint action increased by public goods game. Physiology & Behavior. 2015;149:101–6. pmid:26037635
  21. 21. Abney DH, Warlaumont AS, Oller DK, Wallot S, Kello CT. Multiple Coordination Patterns in Infant and Adult Vocalizations. Infancy. 2017;22(4):514–39. pmid:29375276; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5785941
  22. 22. Harder S, Lange T, Hansen GF, Væver M, Køppe S. A Longitudinal Study of Coordination in Mother–Infant Vocal Interaction From Age 4 to 10 Months. Developmental Psychology. 2015;51(12):1778–90. pmid:26501726
  23. 23. Van Puyvelde M, Loots G, Vinck B, De Coster L, Matthijs L, Mouvet K, et al. The Interplay Between Tonal Synchrony and Social Engagement in Mother-Infant Interaction. Infancy. 2013;18(5):849. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12007
  24. 24. Van Puyvelde M, Vanfleteren P, Loots G, Deschuyffeleer S, Vinck B, Jacquet W, et al. Tonal synchrony in mother–infant interaction based on harmonic and pentatonic series. Infant Behavior and Development. 2010;33(4):387–400. pmid:20478620
  25. 25. Branigan HP, Pickering MJ, Cleland AA. Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue. Cognition. 2000;75(2):B13–25. pmid:10771277
  26. 26. Pardo JS. On phonetic convergence during conversational interaction. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2006;119(4):2382–93. pmid:16642851
  27. 27. Paxton A, Dale R. Argument disrupts interpersonal synchrony. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2013;66(11):2092–102. pmid:24303888
  28. 28. Paxton A, Dale R. Interpersonal Movement Synchrony Responds to High- and Low-Level Conversational Constraints. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017;8:1135. pmid:28804466; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5532444.
  29. 29. Hammal Z, Cohn JF, George DT. Interpersonal Coordination of HeadMotion in Distressed Couples. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing. 2014;5(2):155–67. pmid:26167256; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4495975
  30. 30. Tschacher W, Rees GM, Ramseyer F. Nonverbal synchrony and affect in dyadic interactions. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014; 5(1323). pmid:25505435; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4241744
  31. 31. Valdesolo P, Ouyang J, DeSteno D. The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2010;46(4):693–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03. 004
  32. 32. Araújo D, Davids K. Team Synergies in Sport: Theory and Measures. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7(1449). pmid:27708609; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5030782
  33. 33. Rodrigues M, Passos P. Patterns of Interpersonal Coordination in Rugby Union: Analysis of Collective Behaviours in a Match Situation. Advances in Physical Education. 2013;3:209–14. https://doi.org/10.4236/ape.2013.34034
  34. 34. Yamamoto Y, Okumura M, Yokoyama K, editors. Interpersonal coordination in competitive sports contexts: martial arts. In: Passos P, Davids K, Chow JY, editors. Interpersonal Coordination and Performance in Social Systems. New York: Routledge; 2016. p. 179–94.
  35. 35. Tarr B, Launay J, Dunbar RIM. Silent disco: dancing in synchrony leads to elevated pain thresholds and social closeness. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2016;37(5):343–9. pmid:27540276; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4985033
  36. 36. Ellamil M, Berson J, Wong J, Buckley L, Margulies DS. One in the Dance: Musical Correlates of Group Synchrony in a Real-World Club Environment. PLoS one. 2016;11(10):e0164783. pmid:27764167; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5072606
  37. 37. Jakubowski K, Eerola T, Alborno P, Volpe G, Camurri A, Clayton M. Extracting Coarse Body Movements from Video in Music Performance: A Comparison of Automated Computer Vision Techniques with Motion Capture Data. Frontiers in Digital Humanities. 2017;4(9). https://doi.org/10.3389/fdigh.2017.00009
  38. 38. Ragert M, Schroeder T, Keller PE. Knowing too little or too much: The effects of familiarity with a co-performer’s part on interpersonal coordination in musical ensembles. Frontiers in Psychology. 2013;4(368). pmid:23805116; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3691551
  39. 39. Endedijk HM, Ramenzoni VCO, Cox RFA, Cillessen AHN, Bekkering H, Hunnius S. Development of interpersonal coordination between peers during a drumming task. Developmental Psychology. 2015;51(5):714–21. pmid:25775110
  40. 40. Kirschner S, Ilari B. Joint Drumming in Brazilian and German Preschool Children: Cultural Differences in Rhythmic Entrainment, but No Prosocial Effects. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2014;45(1):137–66. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022113493139
  41. 41. Kirschner S, Tomasello M. Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2010;31(5):354–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004
  42. 42. Kokkinaki TS, Vasdekis VGS, Koufaki ZE, Trevarthen CB. Coordination of Emotions in Mother–Infant Dialogues. Infant and Child Development. 2017;26(2):e1973. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1973
  43. 43. Hoch J, Ossmy O, Cole W, Hasan S, Adolph K. “Dancing” together: Infant-mother locomotor synchrony. PsyArXiv. 2020;1–44. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/swr2y
  44. 44. Costantini C, Akehurst L, Reddy V, Fasulo A. Synchrony, Co-Eating and Communication During Complementary Feeding in Early Infancy. Infancy. 2018;23(2):288–304. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12220
  45. 45. Reyna BA, Brown LF, Pickler RH, Myers BJ, Younger JB. Mother–infant synchrony during infant feeding. Infant Behavior and Development. 2012;35(4):669–77. pmid:22982266
  46. 46. Condon WS, Sander LW. Synchrony Demonstrated between Movements of the Neonate and Adult Speech. Child Development. 1974;45(2):456–62. https://doi.org/10.2307/1127968 pmid:4837718
  47. 47. Trevarthen C. Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In: B M., editor. Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication Cambridge: University Press; 1979. p. 321–47.
  48. 48. Beebe B, Gerstman L, Carson B, Dolins M, Zigman A, Rosensweig H, et al. Rhythmic communication in the mother–infant dyad. In: Davis M, editor. Interaction rhythms: Periodicity in communicative behavior New York: Human Sciences Press; 1982. p. 77–100.
  49. 49. Tronick EZ. Emotions and emotional communication in infants. Am Psychol. 1989;44(2):112–9. pmid:2653124
  50. 50. Harel H, Gordon I, Geva R, Feldman R. Gaze Behaviors of Preterm and Full-Term Infants in Nonsocial and Social Contexts of Increasing Dynamics: Visual Recognition, Attention Regulation, and Gaze Synchrony. Infancy. 2011;16(1):69. pmid:32693482
  51. 51. Farran DC, Kasari C. A longitudinal analysis of the development of synchrony in mutual gaze in mother-child dyads. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 1990;11(4):419–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/0193-3973(90)90018-F
  52. 52. Papoušek M, Papoušek H. Musical elements in the infant's vocalization: Their significance for communication, cognition, and creativity. In: Lipsitt LP, Rovee-Collier CK, editors. Advances in Infancy Research. 1. Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1981. p. 163–224.
  53. 53. Collis GM. Describing the structure of social interaction in infancy. In: Bullowa M, editor. Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication New York: Cambridge University Press; 1979. p. 89–110.
  54. 54. Bernieri FJ, Reznick JS, Rosenthal R. Synchrony, pseudosynchrony, and dissynchrony: Measuring the entrainment process in mother-infant interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54(2):243–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.2.243
  55. 55. Brazelton TB. Four Early Stages in the Development of Mother-Infant Interaction. In: Kobayashi N, Brazelton TB, editors. The growing child in family and society: An interdisciplinary study in parent–infant bonding. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press; 1984. p. 19–34.
  56. 56. Harrist AW, Waugh RM. Dyadic synchrony: Its structure and function in children’s development. Developmental Review. 2002;22(4):555–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0273-2297(02)00500-2
  57. 57. Feldman R. From biological rhythms to social rhythms: Physiological precursors of mother-infant synchrony. Dev Psychol. 2006;42(1):175–88. pmid:16420127
  58. 58. Feldman R. Mother-infant synchrony and the development of moral orientation in childhood and adolescence: Direct and indirect mechanisms of developmental continuity. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2007;77(4):582–97. pmid:18194038
  59. 59. Rabinowitch TC, Meltzoff AN. Synchronized movement experience enhances peer cooperation in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2017;160:21–32. pmid:28412556
  60. 60. Feldman R, Greenbaum CW. Affect Regulation and Synchrony in Mother-Infant Play as Precursors to the Development of Symbolic Competence. Infant Mental Health Journal. 1997;18(1):4–23. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199721)18:1<4::AID-IMHJ2>3.0.CO;2-R
  61. 61. LaFrance M. Review of children and communication: verbal and nonverbal language development. J. Commun. 1977;27:213–4.
  62. 62. Reyna BA, Pickler RH. Mother-Infant Synchrony. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, & Neonatal Nursing. 2009;38(4):470–7. pmid:19614883
  63. 63. Fawcett C, Tunçgenç B. Infants’ use of movement synchrony to infer social affiliation in others. Journal of experimental child psychology. 2017;160(160):127–36. pmid:28427721
  64. 64. Delaherche E, Chetouani M, Mahdhaoui A, Saint-Georges C, Viaux S, Cohen D. Interpersonal Synchrony: A Survey of Evaluation Methods across Disciplines. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing. 2012;3(3):349–65. https://doi.org/10.1109/T-AFFC.2012.12
  65. 65. Leclère C, Viaux S, Avril M, Achard C, Chetouani M, Missonnier S, et al. Why Synchrony Matters during Mother-Child Interactions: A Systematic Review. PLoS One. 2014;9(12):e113571. pmid:25469637; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4254467
  66. 66. Cuadros Z, Hurtado E, Cornejo C. Measuring Dynamics of Infant-Adult Synchrony Through Mocap. Frontiers in Psychology. 2019;10(2839). pmid:31920869; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6930835
  67. 67. Isabella RA, Belsky J, Von Eye A. Origins of Infant–Mother Attachment: An Examination of Interactional Synchrony During the Infant's First Year. Developmental Psychology. 1989;25(1):12–21. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.1.12
  68. 68. Karger RH. Synchrony in mother-infant interactions. Child development. 1979;50(3):882. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128959 pmid:498860
  69. 69. Feldman R. Infant–mother and infant–father synchrony: The coregulation of positive arousal. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2003;24(1):1–23. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.10041
  70. 70. Feldman R, Magori-Cohen R, Galili G, Singer M, Louzoun Y. Mother and infant coordinate heart rhythms through episodes of interaction synchrony. Infant Behavior and Development. 2011;34(4):569–77. pmid:21767879
  71. 71. Feldman R. Parent–Infant Synchrony: Biological Foundations and Developmental Outcomes. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16(6):340–5. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00532.x
  72. 72. Tronick EZ, Cohn JF. Infant-Mother Face-to-Face Interaction: Age and Gender Differences in Coordination and the Occurrence of Miscoordination. Child Development. 1989;60(1):85–92. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131074 pmid:2702877
  73. 73. Feldman R, Greenbaum CW, Yirmiya N. Mother–Infant Affect Synchrony as an Antecedent of the Emergence of Self-Control. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35(1):223–31. pmid:9923477
  74. 74. Ruvolo P, Messinger D, Movellan J. Infants Time Their Smiles to Make Their Moms Smile. PLoS one. 2015;10(9):e0136492. pmid:26398187; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4580458
  75. 75. Waters SF, Mendes WB. Physiological and Relational Predictors of Mother-Infant Behavioral Coordination. Adaptive human behavior and physiology. 2016;2(4):298. pmid:29104853; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5665409
  76. 76. Skuban EM, Shaw DS, Gardner F, Supplee LH, Nichols SR. The correlates of dyadic synchrony in high-risk, low-income toddler boys. Infant Behavior and Development. 2006;29(3):423–34. pmid:17138295
  77. 77. Pratt M, Singer M, Kanat-Maymon Y, Feldman R. Infant negative reactivity defines the effects of parent-child synchrony on physiological and behavioral regulation of social stress. Development and psychopathology. 2015;27(4):1191. pmid:26439070
  78. 78. Tarullo AR, St. John AM, Meyer JS. Chronic stress in the mother-infant dyad: Maternal hair cortisol, infant salivary cortisol and interactional synchrony. Infant Behavior and Development. 2017;47:92–102. pmid:28391126; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5493894
  79. 79. Gordon I, Zagoory-Sharon O, Leckman JF, Feldman R. Oxytocin and the Development of Parenting in Humans. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;68(4):377–82. pmid:20359699; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3943240
  80. 80. Lotzin A, Schiborr J, Barkmann C, Romer G, Ramsauer B. Maternal emotion dysregulation is related to heightened mother-infant synchrony of facial affect. Development and psychopathology. 2016;28(2):327–39. pmid:26040307
  81. 81. Granat A, Gadassi R, Gilboa-Schechtman E, Feldman R. Maternal depression and anxiety, social synchrony, and infant regulation of negative and positive emotions. Emotion. 2017;17(1):11–27. pmid:27441576
  82. 82. Field T, Healy B, LeBlanc WG. Sharing and synchrony of behavior states and heart rate in nondepressed versus depressed mother-infant interactions. Infant Behavior and Development. 1989;12(3):357–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(89)90044-1
  83. 83. Field T, Healy B, Goldstein S, Guthertz M. Behavior-State Matching and Synchrony in Mother-Infant Interactions of Nondepressed versus Depressed Dyads. Developmental Psychology. 1990;26:7–14.
  84. 84. Kokkinaki T, Vasdekis VGS. Comparing emotional coordination in early spontaneous mother–infant and father–infant interactions. European Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2015;12(1):1–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2014.950220
  85. 85. Kokkinaki T, Markodimitraki M. Spontaneous emotional coordination of first-born dizygotic twins and singletons with their mothers in early infancy. European Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2019;16(3):268–89. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2017.1375909
  86. 86. Isabella RA, Belsky J. Interactional Synchrony and the Origins of Infant-Mother Attachment: A Replication Study. Child Development. 1991;62(2):373–84. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131010 pmid:2055128
  87. 87. Lundy BL. Father–and mother–infant face-to-face interactions: Differences in mind-related comments and infant attachment? Infant Behavior and Development. 2003;26(2):200–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(03)00017-1
  88. 88. Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2005;28(5):675–91. pmid:16262930
  89. 89. Cirelli LK, Einarson KM, Trainor LJ. Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Developmental Science. 2014;17(6):1003–11. pmid:25513669
  90. 90. Fawcett C, Liszkowski U. Infants Anticipate Others' Social Preferences. Infant and Child Development. 2012;21(3):239–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.014
  91. 91. Liszkowski U, Carpenter M, Tomasello M. Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition. 2008;108(3):732–9. pmid:18721918
  92. 92. Warneken F, Hare B, Melis AP, Hanus D, Tomasello M. Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children. PLoS Biology. 2007;5:1414–20. pmid:17594177
  93. 93. Warneken F, Tomasello M. Altruistic Helping in Human Infants and Young Chimpanzees. Science. 2006;311(5765):1301–3. pmid:16513986
  94. 94. Warneken F, Tomasello M. Helping and Cooperation at 14 Months of Age. Infancy. 2007;11(3):271–94. https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000701310389
  95. 95. Cirelli LK. How interpersonal synchrony facilitates early prosocial behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2018;20:35–9. pmid:28830004
  96. 96. Rabinowitch TC, Meltzoff AN. Joint Rhythmic Movement Increases 4-Year-Old Children’s Prosocial Sharing and Fairness Toward Peers. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017;8(1005). pmid:28694786; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5483466
  97. 97. Tunçgenç B, Cohen E, Fawcett C. Rock With Me: The Role of Movement Synchrony in Infants' Social and Nonsocial Choices. Child Development. 2015;86(3):976–84. pmid:25702860
  98. 98. Ilari B. Rhythmic Engagement With Music in Early Childhood: A Replication and Extension. Journal of Research in Music Education. 2015;62(4):332–43.
  99. 99. Trainor LJ, Cirelli L. Rhythm and interpersonal synchrony in early social development. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2015;1337:45–52. pmid:25773616; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2851927
  100. 100. Cross I. Music and communication in music psychology. Psychology of Music. 2014;42(6):809–19. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735614543968
  101. 101. Dissanayake E. Ritual and ritualization: Musical means of conveying and shaping emotion in humans and other animals. In: Brown S, Voglsten U, editors. Music and manipulation: On the social uses and social control of music. New York: Berghahn Books; 2006. p. 31–56.
  102. 102. Cirelli LK, Wan SJ, Trainor LJ. Fourteen-month-old infants use interpersonal synchrony as a cue to direct helpfulness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2014;369(1658):1–8. pmid:25385778 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4240967
  103. 103. Cirelli LK, Wan SJ, Trainor LJ. Social Effects of Movement Synchrony: Increased Infant Helpfulness only Transfers to Affiliates of Synchronously Moving Partners. Infancy. 2016;21(6):807–21. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12140
  104. 104. Cirelli LK, Wan S, Spinelli C, Trainor L. Effects of Interpersonal Movement Synchrony on Infant Helping Behaviors: Is Music Necessary? Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal. 2017;34:319–26. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2017.34.3.319
  105. 105. Rabinowitch TC, Knafo-Noam A. Synchronous rhythmic interaction enhances children's perceived similarity and closeness towards each other. PloS one. 2015;10(4):e0120878. pmid:25853859; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4390221
  106. 106. Miles LK, Griffiths JL, Richardson MJ, Macrae CN. Too late to coordinate: Contextual influences on behavioral synchrony. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2010;40(1):52–60. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.721
  107. 107. Hove MJ, Risen JL. It's All in the Timing: Interpersonal Synchrony Increases Affiliation. Social Cognition. 2009;27(6):949–60. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949
  108. 108. Latif N, Barbosa AV, Vatiokiotis-Bateson E, Castelhano MS, Munhall KG. Movement Coordination during Conversation. PLoS one. 2014;9(8):e105036. pmid:25119189 PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4132081
  109. 109. Launay J, Dean R, Bailes F. Synchronising movements with the sounds of a virtual partner enhances partner likeability. International Quarterly of Cognitive Science. 2014;15(4):491–501. pmid:24805849
  110. 110. Ramseyer F, Tschacher W. Nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: Coordinated body movement reflects relationship quality and outcome. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2011;79(3):284–95. pmid:21639608
  111. 111. Tsuchiya A, Ora H, Hao Q, Ono Y, Sato H, Kameda K, et al. Body Movement Synchrony Predicts Degrees of Information Exchange in a Natural Conversation. Frontiers in psychology. 2020;11(817). pmid:32411064; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7201108
  112. 112. Cornejo C, Hurtado E, Cuadros Z, Torres-Araneda A, Paredes J, Olivares H, et al. Dynamics of Simultaneous and Imitative Bodily Coordination in Trust and Distrust. Frontiers in Psychology. 2018;9(1546). pmid:30210391; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6121516
  113. 113. Dunphy-Lelii S. Initial investigations into preschoolers' mirror-style versus transposed bodily imitation. Canadian journal of experimental psychology. 2014;68(1):38–45. pmid:24219247
  114. 114. Chiavarino C. Imitazione speculare e imitazione anatomica: un’ipotesi unificata. Giornale Italiano di Psicol. 2012;36:73–87.
  115. 115. Oullier O, de Guzman GC, Jantzen KJ, Lagarde J, Kelso JAS. Social coordination dynamics: measuring human bonding. Soc Neurosci. 2008;3(2):178–92. pmid:18552971; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC215619
  116. 116. Richardson MJ, Marsh KL, Isenhower RW, Goodman JRL, Schmidt RC. Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Human Movement Science. 2007;26(6):867–91. pmid:17765345
  117. 117. Demos AP, Chaffin R, Begosh KT, Daniels JR, Marsh KL. Rocking to the beat: Effects of music and partner's movements on spontaneous interpersonal coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2012;141(1):49–53. pmid:21668129
  118. 118. Amazeen PG, Schmidt RC, Turvey MT. Frequency detuning of the phase entrainment dynamics of visually coupled rhythmic movements. Biological cybernetics. 1995;72(6):511–8. pmid:7612722
  119. 119. Coey C, Varlet M, Schmidt RC, Richardson MJ. Effects of movement stability and congruency on the emergence of spontaneous interpersonal coordination. Experimental Brain Research. 2011;211(3):483–93. pmid:21526336
  120. 120. Schmidt RC, Bienvenu M, Fitzpatrick PA, Amazeen PG. A comparison of intra- and interpersonal interlimb coordination: Coordination breakdowns and coupling strength. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1998;24(3):884–900. pmid:9627423
  121. 121. Cheng M, Kato M, Saunders JA, Tseng C-h. Paired walkers with better first impression synchronize better. PLoS one. 2020;15(2):e0227880. pmid:32084136; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7034894
  122. 122. Nessler JA, Gilliland SJ. Interpersonal synchronization during side by side treadmill walking is influenced by leg length differential and altered sensory feedback. Hum Mov Sci. 2009;28(6):772–85. pmid:19796834
  123. 123. van Ulzen NR, Lamoth CJ, Daffertshofer A, Semin GR, Beek PJ. Characteristics of instructed and uninstructed interpersonal coordination while walking side-by-side. Neuroscience letters. 2008;432(2):88–93. pmid:18242846
  124. 124. Zivotofsky AZ, Hausdorff JM. The sensory feedback mechanisms enabling couples to walk synchronously: An initial investigation. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation. 2007;4(1):28. pmid:17686150; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1973071
  125. 125. Vesper C, van der Wel RP, Knoblich G, Sebanz N. Are you ready to jump? Predictive mechanisms in interpersonal coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2013;39(1):48–61. pmid:22564161
  126. 126. Trost W, Vuilleumier P. Rhythmic entrainment as a mechanism for emotion induction by music. In: Cochrane T, Fantini B, Scherer K, editors. The Emotional Power of Music: Multidisciplinary perspectives on musical arousal, expression, and social control. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013. p. 213–225.
  127. 127. Krueger J. Empathy, enaction, and shared musical experience. Evidence from infant cognition. In: Cochrane T, Fantini B, Scherer K, editors. The Emotional Power of Music: Multidisciplinary perspectives on musical arousal, expression, and social control. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013. p. 177–196.
  128. 128. Chanda ML, Levitin DJ. The neurochemistry of music. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17(4):179–93. pmid:23541122
  129. 129. Marzoli D, Palumbo R, Di Domenico A, Penolazzi B, Garganese P, Tommasi L. The Relation between Self-Reported Empathy and Motor Identification with Imagined Agents. PLoS one. 2011;6(1):e14595. pmid:21298089; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3027625
  130. 130. Sanmartín R, Vicent M, Gonzálvez C, Inglés CJ, Díaz-Herrero Á, Granados L, et al. Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Short Form: Factorial Invariance and Optimistic and Pessimistic Affective Profiles in Spanish Children. Frontiers in Psychology. 2018;9(392). pmid:29628906; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5876292
  131. 131. Boker SM, Xu M, Rotondo JL, King K. Windowed cross-correlation and peak picking for the analysis of variability in the association between behavioral time series. Psychological methods. 2002;7(3):338–55. pmid:12243305
  132. 132. Cornejo C, Cuadros Z, Morales R. Neurobiological approaches to interpersonal coordination: Achievements and pitfalls. In: Ibáñez AM, García AM, Sedeño L, editors. Neuroscience and Social Science: The Missing Link: Springer, Cham; 2017. p. 69–94.
  133. 133. Ubaldi S, Barchiesi G, Cattaneo L. Bottom-up and top-down visuomotor responses to action observation. Cerebral cortex. 2015;25(4):1032–41. pmid:24132640
  134. 134. Wapner S, Cirillo L. Imitation of a model's hand movements: age changes in transposition of left-right relations. Child Dev. 1968;39(3):887–94. https://doi.org/10.2307/1126991 pmid:5687333
  135. 135. Gleissner B, Meltzoff A, Bekkering H. Children's coding of human action: cognitive factors influencing imitation in 3-year-olds. Dev Sci. 2000;3(4):405–14. pmid:25147476; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4137778
  136. 136. Pierpaoli C, Ferrante L, Manzoni T, Fabri M. Anatomical or mirror mode imitation? A behavioral approach. Archives italiennes de biologie. 2014;152(1):20–31. https://doi.org/10.4449/AIB.V152I1.1526 pmid:25181594
  137. 137. Avikainen S, Wohlschlager A, Liuhanen S, Hanninen R, Hari R. Impaired mirror-image imitation in Asperger and high-functioning autistic subjects. Current biology. 2003;13(4):339–41. pmid:12593801
  138. 138. Richardson MJ, Marsh KL, Isenhower RW, Goodman JR, Schmidt RC. Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Hum Mov Sci. 2007;26(6):867–91. pmid:17765345
  139. 139. Kirschner S, Tomasello M. Joint drumming: Social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. Journal of experimental child psychology. 2009;102(3):299–314. pmid:18789454
  140. 140. Wiltermuth SS, Heath C. Synchrony and Cooperation. Psychological Science. 2009;20(1):1–5. pmid:19152536
  141. 141. Valdesolo P, DeSteno D. Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion. 2011;11(2):262–6. pmid:21500895
  142. 142. RodrÌgues M, Passos P. Patterns of Interpersonal Coordination in Rugby Union: Analysis of Collective Behaviours in a Match Situation. Advances in Physical Education. 2013;03:209–14. https://doi.org/10.4236/ape.2013.34034
  143. 143. Seifert L, Lardy J, Bourbousson J, Adé D, Nordez A, Thouvarecq R, et al. Interpersonal Coordination and Individual Organization Combined with Shared Phenomenological Experience in Rowing Performance: Two Case Studies. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017;8(75). pmid:28194127; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5278567
  144. 144. Burger B, Thompson MR, Luck G, Saarikallio SH, Toiviainen P. Hunting for the beat in the body: on period and phase locking in music-induced movement. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8(903). pmid:25426051; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4224089
  145. 145. Toiviainen P, Luck G, Thompson MR. Embodied Meter: Hierarchical Eigenmodes in Music-Induced Movement. Music Perception. 2010;28(1):59–70. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2010.28.1.59
  146. 146. Cornejo C, Cuadros Z, Morales R, Paredes J. Interpersonal Coordination: Methods, Achievements, and Challenges. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017; 8(1685). pmid:29021769; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5623900
  147. 147. Duran ND, Fusaroli R. Conversing with a devil’s advocate: Interpersonal coordination in deception and disagreement. PLoS one. 2017;12(6):e0178140. pmid:28574996; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5456047
  148. 148. Kupper Z, Ramseyer F, Hoffmann H, Kalbermatten S, Tschacher W. Video-based quantification of body movement during social interaction indicates the severity of negative symptoms in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2010;121(1–3):90–100. pmid:20434313
  149. 149. Ramseyer F, Tschacher W. Nonverbal synchrony of head- and body-movement in psychotherapy: different signals have different associations with outcome. Frontiers in psychology. 2014;5(979). pmid:25249994; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4155778
  150. 150. Dittmann AT. The role of body movement in communication. In: Siegman AW, Feldstein S, editors. Nonverbal Behavior and Communication Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1987. p. 37–64.
  151. 151. Noel JP, Niear MAD, Lazzara NS, Wallace MT. Uncoupling Between Multisensory Temporal Function and Nonverbal Turn-Taking in Autism Spectrum Disorder. IEEE Transactions on Cognitive and Developmental Systems. 2018;10(4):973–82. https://doi.org/10.1109/TCDS.2017.2778141
  152. 152. Clayton M, Jakubowski K, Eerola T. Interpersonal entrainment in Indian instrumental music performance: Synchronization and movement coordination relate to tempo, dynamics, metrical and cadential structure. Musicae Scientiae. 2019;23(3):304–31. https://doi.org/10.1177/1029864919844809
  153. 153. Davis TJ, Brooks TR, Dixon JA. Multi-scale interactions in interpersonal coordination. Journal of Sport and Health Science. 2016;5(1):25–34. pmid:30356924; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6191966
  154. 154. Athreya DN, Riley MA, Davis TJ. Visual influences on postural and manual interpersonal coordination during a joint precision task. Experimental Brain Research. 2014;232(9):2741–51. pmid:24770859
  155. 155. Chatel-Goldman J, Congedo M, Jutten C, Schwartz J-L. Touch increases autonomic coupling between romantic partners. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;8(95). pmid:24734009; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3973922
  156. 156. McFarland DH. Respiratory markers of conversational interaction. Journal of speech, language, and hearing research. 2001;44(1):128–43. pmid:11218097
  157. 157. Astolfi L, Toppi J, De Vico Fallani F, Vecchiato G, Salinari S, Mattia D, et al. Neuroelectrical Hyperscanning Measures Simultaneous Brain Activity in Humans. Brain Topography. 2010;23(3):243–56. pmid:20480221
  158. 158. Konvalinka I, Bauer M, Stahlhut C, Hansen LK, Roepstorff A, Frith CD. Frontal alpha oscillations distinguish leaders from followers: Multivariate decoding of mutually interacting brains. NeuroImage. 2014;94:79–88. pmid:24631790
  159. 159. Bilous FR, Krauss RM. Dominance and accommodation in the conversational behaviours of same- and mixed-gender dyads. Language & Communication. 1988;8(3):183–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/0271-5309(88)90016-X
  160. 160. Street RL. Speech convergence and speech evaluation in fact-finding interviews. Human Communication Research. 1984;11(2):139–69. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1984.tb00043.x
  161. 161. Lotzin A, Romer G, Schiborr J, Noga B, Schulte-Markwort M, Ramsauer B. Gaze Synchrony between Mothers with Mood Disorders and Their Infants: Maternal Emotion Dysregulation Matters. PLoS one. 2015;10(12):e0144417. pmid:26657941; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4681006
  162. 162. Holler J, Wilkin K. Co-Speech Gesture Mimicry in the Process of Collaborative Referring During Face-to-Face Dialogue. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 2011;35(2):133–53. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-011-0105-6
  163. 163. Zhao Z, Salesse RN, Gueugnon M, Schmidt RC, Marin L, Bardy BG. Moving attractive virtual agent improves interpersonal coordination stability. Human Movement Science. 2015;41:240–54. pmid:25854798