Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Neurodevelopment of HIV-exposed uninfected children in Cape Town, South Africa

  • Hlengiwe P. Madlala ,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Project administration, Writing – original draft

    hlengiwe.madlala@uct.ac.za

    Affiliations Division of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Public Health and Family Medicine, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, Western Cape, South Africa, Centre for Infectious Disease Epidemiology and Research, School of Public Health and Family Medicine, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, Western Cape, South Africa

  • Landon Myer,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Division of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Public Health and Family Medicine, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, Western Cape, South Africa, Centre for Infectious Disease Epidemiology and Research, School of Public Health and Family Medicine, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, Western Cape, South Africa

  • Thokozile R. Malaba,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Division of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Public Health and Family Medicine, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, Western Cape, South Africa, Centre for Infectious Disease Epidemiology and Research, School of Public Health and Family Medicine, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, Western Cape, South Africa

  • Marie-Louise Newell

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations School of Human Development and Health, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton, United Kingdom, School of Public Health, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa

Abstract

Background

Evidence shows that antiretroviral (ART) exposure is associated with neurodevelopmental delays in human immunodeficiency virus (HIV)-exposed uninfected (HEU) children. However, there are few insights into modifiable maternal and child factors that may play a role in improving neurodevelopment in HEU children. We used a parent-centric neurodevelopment tool, Ages & Stages Questionnaire (ASQ) to examined neurodevelopment in HEU children at 12–24 months of age, and associations with maternal and child factors.

Methods

505 HIV-infected women (initiated ART pre- or during pregnancy) with live singleton births attending primary health care were enrolled; 355 of their HEU children were assessed for neurodevelopment (gross motor, fine motor, communication, problem solving and personal-social domains) at 12–24 months using age-specific ASQ administered by a trained fieldworker. Associations with maternal and child factors were examined using logistic regression models.

Results

Among mothers (median age 30 years, IQR, 26–34), 52% initiated ART during pregnancy; the median CD4 count was 436 cells/μl (IQR, 305–604). Most delayed neurodevelopment in HEU children was in gross (9%) and fine motor (5%) functions. In adjusted models, maternal socio-economic status (aOR 0.42, 95% CI 0.24–0.76) was associated with reduced odds of delayed gross-fine motor neurodevelopment. Maternal age ≥35 years (aOR 0.22, 95% CI 0.05–0.89) and maternal body mass index (BMI) <18.5 (aOR 6.76, 95% CI 1.06–43.13) were associated with delayed communication-problem-solving-personal-social neurodevelopment. There were no differences in odds for either domain by maternal ART initiation timing.

Conclusions

Delayed neurodevelopment was detected in both gross and fine motor functions in this cohort of HEU children, with strong maternal predictors that may be explored as potentially modifiable factors associated with neurodevelopment at one to two years of age.

Introduction

Although antiretroviral therapy (ART) has been highly successful in preventing mother-to-child human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) transmission, there are more than 1 million children born annually to HIV-infected mothers with growing concerns regarding the health and neurodevelopment of HIV-exposed uninfected (HEU) children [14]. Poor early childhood neurodevelopment is linked to educational under-achievement and lifetime progression overall, contributing to high levels of inequality and poverty in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs) [5]. Regardless of HIV/ART exposure, LMICs are home to a substantial number of children who fail to reach their full development potential due to poverty and unstimulating environments [6]. This suggests that interventions targeted at improving maternal factors, including those related to home environment may make a difference in neurodevelopment outcomes of these already vulnerable children.

The first 1000 days from conception to two years of age is a critical time of substantial growth including 80% of brain development [7, 8]. This period presents a window to establish strong foundations that may improve the child’s early and late neurodevelopment outcomes, thereby positively setting the stage for success across multiple outcomes in later life. In particular, interventions that target child neurodevelopment are most effective for children when they are still young [5]. In high income countries, appropriate neurodevelopmental learning opportunities have shown significant benefits including improved cognitive function, school achievement and increased earnings [9, 10]. In sub-Saharan Africa (SSA), behavioural programs promoting child-parent/caregiver interaction and combined infant/young child feeding, improved water, sanitation and hygiene are recommended for minimizing the risk of poor child development, especially in children affected by HIV [11, 12].

Three aspects of first 1000 days crucially influencing development are nutrition and health, love and attention, play and stimulation [13]. Despite the growing number of HEU children, there are few insights on their neurodevelopment assessment using parent-centric tools which promote interaction between mother/caregiver and child, and may encourage parents to provide stimulating environments through play and learning activities to influence neurodevelopment in their kids. HEUs from SSA most commonly experience delay in motor and language scores [1, 1417], with exposure to efavirenz (EFV) regimen associated with worse delay in motor development compared to non-EFV regimen [15]. Further, earlier rather than later maternal ART initiation has been implicated in worse outcomes on HEU development [15], although this has not been confirmed in other studies which also suggested that maternal ART exposure may become less important in predicting child’s development with increasing child age [12, 18]. Therefore, further investigation of the role of ART exposure during pregnancy and timing of initiation on HEU development is needed. In addition, there is a need for identification of maternal and child factors that may be modified to improve neurodevelopment at a young age, particularly those that would enable physical and mental stimulation. In a cohort of HEU children, whose mothers initiated ART pre- or during pregnancy, we examined their neurodevelopment at 12–24 months of age using Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ), a neurodevelopmental assessment tool designed to be completed by parents/caregivers [19], and associations with maternal and child factors.

Methods

The study was approved by the Faculty of Health Sciences Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Cape Town and Institutional Review Board of the University of Southampton. Written informed consent for data collection was obtained from all participants at enrolment, including consent for follow-up of children soon after delivery. Cohort details have been described elsewhere [20]. Briefly, we enrolled 552 HIV-infected, pregnant women (≥18 years) attending their first antenatal care (ANC) visit at ≤24 weeks gestational age (GA) at Gugulethu Community Health Centre (CHC). Enrolment took place between April 2015 and October 2016 and participants were prospectively followed via face-to-face study visits at the UCT-research facility located at Gugulethu CHC through May 2018. There were three antenatal (≤24, 28–32 and 34–36 weeks GA) and four postnatal (<7 days, 10 weeks, 6 and 12–24 months) study visits. The 12–24 months visit took place between March 2017 and May 2018, and ASQ assessments were also conducted at the UCT-research facility located at Gugulethu CHC. Gugulethu is a semi-urban area with a population predominantly made up of 98.8% black African ethnic group with low socioeconomic status (SES) [21, 22]. Women initiated ART pre- (n = 261) or during (n = 291) pregnancy; all were followed to 12–24 months postpartum.

Maternal socio-demographic and clinical data were collected via interviewer-administered questionnaires. SES was a composite score based on education level, employment status, type of housing, and presence of a toilet, running water, electricity, fridge, telephone and television in the house [23]; participants were of generally low SES and we categorised into tertiles corresponding to lowest, middle and highest SES group. Substance use combined use of alcohol, cigarette and drugs 30 days prior enrolment. Neonatal data including weight, length, head circumference and gender were obtained from medical records. GA at first ANC visit was measured by ultrasonography (USS) operated by an experienced sonographer. Maternal weight and height measurements were taken at first ANC visit; weight measured at first ANC visit was corrected [24] to estimate pre-pregnancy body mass index (BMI, kg/m2), which was categorised as underweight (<18.5), normal (18.5–24.9), overweight (25–29.9) or obese (≥30). Using a standardised protocol, child anthropometry (weight, length, head circumference, mid-upper arm circumference [MUAC]) was measured by a trained study nurse at all postpartum study visits. Self-reported maternal ART adherence at 12–24 months was defined as not missing taking ART medication in the past 30 days. Of the 552 women enrolled, 39 had pregnancy losses and 8 were loss-to-follow-up (LTFU) resulting in inclusion of 505 women with live births and their 355 children assessed for neurodevelopment outcomes at 12–24 months using age-specific ASQ (Fig 1). Missing categories were included in frequency tables, and in the reference category in regressions as appropriate.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Flow diagram showing participant enrolment and retention at different study visits by maternal ART initiation status.

GA—gestational age, ART—antiretroviral therapy, IUD—intrauterine death, TOP—termination of pregnancy, ANC—antenatal care, LTFU—loss to follow up, ASQ—Ages & Stages Questionnaire.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.g001

Outcome assessment

Ages and Stages Questionnaire is a global screening scale previously used in South Africa [2527], including validation in preterm and LBW children in other settings [28, 29]. The age-specific questionnaires were translated into the local language isiXhosa by our experienced translator (English-isiXhosa); this was validated by having a second independent translator (isiXhosa-English) translate the isiXhosa version back to English and the back translated English version matched with the original ASQs. Age-specific ASQ versions used in this study ranged from 11–26 months. To ensure reliability of the instrument, all assessments were done by a single trained fieldworker. With confirmation from the mother/caregiver, the assessor ensured that children were not sick, and were well-fed and rested prior to conducting the assessment. To facilitate accurate assessment, as much as possible, the mother/caregiver provided instruction to the child to ascertain their ability to perform the task. This was deemed sufficient as no task required the child to interact with peers. All communication between the fieldworker, participant and child was in local language, isiXhosa.

The ASQ screens five neurodevelopmental areas–gross motor, fine motor, communication, problem-solving and personal-social domains. Gross motor assesses use of large muscles including arms and legs while fine motor assesses coordination and movement of hands and fingers. Communication scale assesses language including what a child is able to say and what they can understand from the instructions they are given. Problem-solving domain assesses ability to solve problems through playing games and using toys; personal-social domain assesses self-help skills and interaction with parent/caregiver.

Each domain had six questions, each with a choice of three responses–‘not yet’, ‘sometimes’ and ‘yes’ corresponding to scores of 0, 5 and 10, respectively. The summary score for each of the five domains provided a total score of 0–60. Scoring was divided into three neurodevelopment categories as defined in the age-specific ASQ manual–below cutoff (delay), monitoring zone (intermediate), and above cutoff (no delay). Given the small numbers available, related domains were combined in the regression models: gross and fine motor (gross-fine motor), and communication with problem solving and personal-social (communication-problem-solving-personal-social). Detailed associations for each neurodevelopment domain are presented in supplementary material. Finally, the scores below cutoff (delay) and on monitoring zone (intermediate) were collapsed into one category of delayed neurodevelopment.

Statistical analysis

Data were analysed using STATA version 15.0 (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX, USA). Maternal and child baseline and 12–24 months characteristics were stratified by maternal ART initiation status, differences between groups were compared using Chi-Squared test for categorical variables and Wilcoxon rank-sum test for continuous variables. To assess factors associated with LTFU between enrolment in early pregnancy and 24 months postpartum, we used univariate and multivariable logistic regression models. Associations between maternal, child characteristics and neurodevelopment outcomes, were also assessed in logistic regression—with ‘no delay’ in neurodevelopment as reference category. Results are presented as unadjusted (OR) and adjusted odds ratios (aOR) with related 95% confidence intervals (CI). Model for maternal factors was adjusted for age, BMI, SES and ART initiation status; the model for child factors was adjusted for gender, size for GA, delivery GA, age and weight-for-age at assessment. Variables included in adjusted models were those significantly (p<0.05) associated with neurodevelopment in unadjusted models, or on the basis of existing literature, theoretical and conceptual reasoning.

Results

The median maternal age was 30 years (IQR, 26–34), 23% were nulliparous, 52% initiated ART during pregnancy, and the median CD4 count was 436 cells/μl (IQR, 305–604) (Table 1). Baseline characteristics did not differ between the 505 women with live singleton births and the 355 women whose children were assessed for neurodevelopment using ASQ at 12–24 months postpartum. Overall (n = 505), 16% children had low birth weight (LBW), 15% were small size for gestational age (SGA), and 14% were preterm (6% spontaneous preterm delivery—sPTD, 8% medically-indicated preterm delivery—MI PTD) (Table 1). Six percent of children were never breastfed; 50% breastfed for <6 months. For women whose children were assessed at 12–24 months, those who initiated ART during pregnancy were more likely younger, nulliparous and with lower CD4 counts than those who initiated ART pre-pregnancy (Table 1). The children assessed for neurodevelopment had a mean age of 14 months (SD, ±3) and median age of 12 months (IQR, 12–15). Children of mothers initiating ART during pregnancy were more likely breastfed than those of mothers initiating ART pre-pregnancy. Between birth and age 12–24 months, 13% of children had at least one hospital admission, and 52% missed at least one vaccination dose. Child characteristics at birth did not differ for the 505 liveborn and 355 children assessed for neurodevelopment at 12–24 months of age.

thumbnail
Table 1. Characteristics for women with live singleton births and their children (n = 505) and for women who had their children assessed for neurodevelopment with ASQ at 12–24 months and their children (n = 355) stratified by maternal ART initiation status.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.t001

The majority (17%) of LTFU occurred between 6 and 24 months visits. To promote retention, participants were contacted twice every 1–2 months through telephone and home visits were conducted for those unreachable over the phone. In adjusted analyses, odds of LTFU were lower for women 30–34 years old (aOR 0.46, 95% CI 0.23–0.92), overweight women (aOR 0.40, 95% CI 0.18–0.88) and those who initiated ART pre-pregnancy (aOR 0.29, 95% CI 0.17–0.51) (S2 Table). Adjusted factors non-significantly associated with increased LTFU odds included underweight BMI (aOR 3.05, 95% CI 0.81–11.44) and higher maternal SES (aOR 1.48, 95% CI 0.81–2.73); and substance use 30 days prior enrolment (OR 1.16, 95% CI 0.67–2.02) in unadjusted model.

Overall, 9% of children had delayed neurodevelopment on gross motor, 5% on fine motor, 3% on communication and problem-solving and 4% on personal-social domains (Table 2); with no substantive differences by maternal ART initiation status except for gross motor. Children of women initiating ART during pregnancy appeared less likely to have delayed neurodevelopment (combined intermediate and delay categories) than those of women initiating pre-pregnancy (13% vs 17%). Notably, delayed neurodevelopment overlapped across different domains. 42 children had delay in both fine motor and personal-social domains, 17 in fine motor, problem solving and personal-social domains and 13 in gross motor, fine motor, problem solving and personal-social domains (Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Distribution of delayed neurodevelopment on two, three and four overlapping domains.

Neurodevelopment delay overlap on two domains: Fine+PerSocial: fine motor & personal social; Fine+ProbSolv: fine motor & problem solving; Gross+ProbSolv: gross motor & problem solving; Gross+Fine: gross & fine motor; Comm+Fine: communication & fine motor; Comm+Gross: communication & gross motor. Neurodevelopment delay overlap on three domains: Fine+ProbSolv+PerSocial: fine motor & problem solving & personal social; Gross+Fine+PerSocial: gross motor & fine motor & personal social; Gross+Fine+ProbSolv: gross motor & fine motor & problem solving. Neurodevelopment delay overlap on four domains: Gross+Fine+ProbSolv+PerSocial: gross motor & fine motor & problem solving & personal social; Comm+Gross+Fine+ProbSolv: communication & gross motor & fine motor & problem solving.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.g002

thumbnail
Table 2. Frequencies of individual ASQ neurodevelopment domains stratified by maternal ART initiation status (n = 355).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.t002

Maternal factors and neurodevelopment at 12–24 months

Table 3 shows the association between maternal, child factors and delayed neurodevelopment on combined ASQ domains (results on individual domains available as S6S9 Tables). Adjusting for age, BMI, SES and ART initiation status, maternal factors associated with a (non-significant) trend towards increased odds of delayed gross-fine motor neurodevelopment included underweight BMI (aOR 2.64, 95% CI 0.51–13.71), lower SES (aOR 1.08, 95% CI 0.60–1.93) and ART initiation pre-pregnancy (aOR 1.20, 95% CI 0.72–1.97). Higher maternal SES was the only factor statistically significantly associated with reduced risk (aOR 0.42, 95% CI 0.24–0.76) of delayed gross-fine motor neurodevelopment.

thumbnail
Table 3. Associations between maternal, child factors and delayed neurodevelopment on combined ASQ domains, adjusted maternal and child factors had two separate models (n = 355).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.t003

In adjusted models, no factors were significantly associated with communication-problem-solving-personal-social neurodevelopment, although there was a trend towards increased odds of delayed neurodevelopment on this combined domain for underweight BMI (aOR 3.04, 95% CI 0.54–17.13) and ART initiation pre-pregnancy (aOR 1.81, 95% CI 0.97–3.38). Factors showing a trend towards decreased odds of delayed communication-problem-solving-personal-social neurodevelopment included older maternal age (aOR 0.44, 95% CI 0.14–1.37), obese BMI (aOR 0.81, 95% CI 0.37–1.74) and higher maternal SES (aOR 0.82, 95% CI 0.38–1.74) in adjusted model. In model adjusted for both maternal and child factors, older maternal age (aOR 0.22, 95% CI 0.05–0.91) significantly reduced odds of communication-problem-solving-personal-social neurodevelopment delay, while underweight BMI (aOR 6.72, 95% CI 1.05–43.00) increased the odds (S3 Table).

Child factors and neurodevelopment at 12–24 months

In a model adjusted for child gender, size for GA, delivery GA, age and weight-for-age at assessment (Table 3), there was a (non-significant) trend towards increased odds of delayed gross-fine motor neurodevelopment for sPTD (aOR 1.49, 95% CI 0.61–3.65) and MI PTD (aOR 1.18, 95% CI 0.49–2.83). Factors non-significantly associated with decreased odds of delayed gross-fine motor neurodevelopment included female gender (aOR 0.68, 95% CI 0.40–1.15) and large size-for-gestational age (LGA) (aOR 0.71, 95% CI 0.26–1.96); breastfeeding for ≥6 months (OR 0.72, 95% CI 0.45–1.17) was non-significant in unadjusted model.

In adjusted models, factors with non-significant increased odds of delayed communication-problem-solving-personal-social neurodevelopment included small size-for-gestational age (SGA) (aOR 1.45, 95% CI 0.61–3.43) and sPTD (aOR 1.58 95% CI 0.49–5.09). Although female gender (OR 0.48, 95% CI 0.25–0.93) and breastfeeding for ≥6 months (OR 0.50, 95% CI 0.26–0.97) were associated with decreased odds of delayed communication-problem-solving-personal-social neurodevelopment in unadjusted models, significance was lost in adjusted models.

Neurodevelopment of SGA children at 12–24 months

Of the 355 children assessed at 12–24 months, 16% were SGA, (18% for mothers initiating ART pre-pregnancy, 13% for those initiating ART during pregnancy) (Table 1). We analysed frequencies of delayed neurodevelopment on different ASQ domains (S4 Table) and associations with maternal factors in 56 SGA children (S5 Table). Of these 56 children, 11% had delayed neurodevelopment on gross motor, 9% on fine motor and personal-social, and 5% on communication and problem-solving (S4 Table). Although not statistically significant, children of mothers initiating ART pre-pregnancy had notably higher frequencies of delay in all domains than those of mothers initiating ART during pregnancy, similar to what was seen in the overall 355 cohort.

In unadjusted models there was a trend for underweight BMI (OR 2.29, 95% CI 0.12–43.11) and initiating ART pre-pregnancy (OR 1.35, 95% CI 0.38–4.78) to be associated with increased odds of delayed gross-fine motor neurodevelopment (S5 Table). Factors with non-significant decreased odds of delayed gross-fine motor neurodevelopment in SGA children included older maternal age (OR 0.67, 95% CI 0.11–3.90), obese BMI (OR 0.40, 95% CI 0.09–1.86), being married and living together/cohabiting (OR 0.44, 95% CI 0.02–8.25), higher SES (OR 0.71, 95% CI 0.17–2.98) and multiparity (OR 0.72, 95% CI 0.20–2.62). Except for maternal age, obese BMI and relationship status, associations with communication-problem-solving-personal-social domain combination were in the same direction as gross-fine motor domain and not substantially different to those observed in the overall cohort (n = 355).

Discussion

In HEU children of mothers who initiated ART pre- or during pregnancy, delayed neurodevelopment at age one to two years was limited, and mostly on gross or fine motor functions. Children of higher SES mothers were less likely to have delayed gross-fine motor neurodevelopment. Children breastfed for ≥6 months and children of mothers ≥35 years of age were less likely, and those of underweight BMI mothers more likely, to have delayed communication-problem-solving-personal-social neurodevelopment. This data would suggest potentially modifiable factors to improve neurodevelopment of HEU children.

Various tools are used to assess child neurodevelopment, some administered by health professionals and others by parents/caregivers. ASQ is a globally-used scale, cheap and easy to administer, and increasingly popular in LMICs [19, 30]. The parent-centric nature of ASQ makes it a convenient and appropriate tool for use in LMICs, where it is needed the most [6]. Although some studies have questioned the weak correlation between ASQ and Bayley scale for children under 13 months [30, 31], ASQ provides a critical snapshot to child’s neurodevelopment, and can identify early delays, enabling timely provision of appropriate learning activities. Research has recently validated the use of this screening tool in South Africa [25, 26] and we used this tool to assess neurodevelopment outcomes in a cohort of HEU children at age one to two years.

In our cohort, gross and fine motor functions were domains where children most likely experienced neurodevelopment delay. Although we did not have a comparator group of HIV-unexposed children, the proportions for gross (9% vs 5%) and fine (5% vs 2%) motor function delays observed in HEU children in this study are higher than those reported in other studies for HIV-unexposed children in a similar setting in Cape Town [1, 16, 32]. Development of these functions can be stimulated by activities including sitting, standing, walking, eating, drawing and general playing [33]. In LMICs, neurodevelopment delays may be attributed to multiple risks factors regardless of ART exposure. We found that children of mothers with higher SES were less likely to experience neurodevelopment delay in these domains; higher SES may provide a healthy and stimulating home environment, with positive impact on child neurodevelopment [3437]. Other African studies also report higher maternal SES to be positively associated with child gross-fine motor neurodevelopment [38, 39], which may be partly due to educated mothers being knowledgeable about the importance of providing stimulating environment for their children, and those employed able to afford physically-stimulating learning activities. Higher maternal SES may mediate child neurodevelopment through improved child nutrition [39]. Although our results for motor function delays are comparable with other cohorts of HEU children in Cape Town, the most common delay reported in these studies is the communication domain rather than motor function [1, 16, 32]. These differences may be, in part, attributed to different assessment tools used. Overall, these results suggest that improving the factors included in SES could indirectly provide a home environment that promotes healthy growth and general play, stimulating gross and fine neurodevelopment in children, including those HEU.

Three areas in the first 1000 days critical for development are nutrition and health, love and attention, play and stimulation [13]. We found that children breastfed for ≥6 months were significantly less likely to experience delay in communication-problem-solving-personal-social combination domain. Although a previous study in our setting showed that HEU children experience neurodevelopment delays despite breastfeeding, this was particularly true for preterm children [1]. In other African settings, there is evidence of beneficial effect of breastfeeding in HEU children especially during first year of life [40, 41]. We also found that children of women who were underweight were significantly more likely to have delayed communication-problem-solving-personal-social neurodevelopment. Maternal underweight BMI is a proxy for undernutrition, which may be an indication of the child’s household environment; maternal undernutrition is negatively associated with children brain development [42]. In contrast, we found that children born from older women were significantly less likely to have delayed communication-problem-solving-personal-social neurodevelopment. Older women tend to be multiparous and we speculate that they may be more capable of providing nurturing care and interaction with their children, which could stimulate their verbal and social skills. In another study in a high HIV prevalence area, Bland et al. found that home stimulation improves executive function at 11 years of age [43], and it is possible that interventions targeting modifiable factors such as maternal SES, BMI and breastfeeding may improve nutrition, interaction and play with children at one to two years of age with longer-term impact.

Despite the undisputed success of universal ART in reducing mother to child HIV transmission, concerns have been expressed regarding ART exposure on growth and neurodevelopment of HEU children [15, 44]. We observed a strong, non-significant trend for the association between ART initiation pre-pregnancy and delayed neurodevelopment in all domain combinations. This result was unexpected given that studies elsewhere have reported similar neurodevelopment progression in HEU children as seen in their unexposed counterparts [16, 18, 45]. However, cases of poor neurodevelopment in HEU children have also been reported, with ART exposure implicated as the likely contributing factor [15, 4648]. These inconsistent findings may be attributed to the heterogeneity of regimens and ART treatment guidelines used in different studies, and over time. In our study, nearly all women were on an ART regimen of two NRTIs and EFV. Reassuringly, there is some indication that maternal ART becomes less important in predicting children’s development with increasing child age [17, 47]. Women initiating ART pre-pregnancy were significantly less likely to be LFTU, which may have biased our results, and further research remains needed.

HIV/ART has been shown to contribute to high risk of SGA and preterm children [49], we observed that women initiating ART pre-pregnancy had higher proportions of SGA children than those initiating ART during pregnancy. SGA children are likely to have delayed organ development including the brain [5052], which may have contributed to our observation of delayed communication-problem-solving-personal-social neurodevelopment, although statistical significance was not reached due to limited sample size. Some studies report absence of adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes in preterm children [53, 54]; we show a non-significant trend for children of mothers with sPTD, but not MI PTD, to be more likely to have neurodevelopment delays in this same domain combination, which is in contrast with findings elsewhere showing higher risk of neurodevelopment delay in MI PTD than sPTD children [55]. However, that cohort had a noticeable imbalance of mode of delivery of preterm children (97.2% sPTD, 2.8% MI PTD) which could explain their findings [55]. Although the distinct risks factors mediating the two types of PTD are well established, the mechanisms underlying different risks profiles for neurodevelopment outcomes at one to two years remain unclear.

Our results support the recommendation of behavioural parent/caregiver training programs for families affected by HIV aimed at stimulating early childhood development by promoting positive experiences and happy memories which may have long-lasting effects on emotional, social and behavioural domains of the brain [12, 56]. However, the findings reported should be interpreted cautiously due to limited statistical power for both overall and subset cohort of SGA children. We were unable to control for some important confounders such as mother’s mental health status, which could have been associated with the child’s neurodevelopment, and this may have resulted in overestimation of neurodevelopment delay in the regression models. In contrast, it is possible that the neurodevelopment delays reported are less than rates in the general population as vulnerable young mothers and adolescents <18 years of age, whose children could possibly face increased neurodevelopmental delay, were not included in the main cohort [20] and this study. Sample size at 12–24 months was limited by LTFU which may have contributed to non-achievement of statistical significance. However, the trends observed highlight modifiable factors that future studies should consider investigating as neurodevelopment delays may become even more apparent as children grow older. Due to small numbers, we collapsed intermediate and delayed neurodevelopment categories, and neurodevelopment delay reported may be overestimated as children on the intermediate range may have reduced associations. However, both groups of children would require increased stimulation/learning activities to improve their neurodevelopment.

In conclusion, a small proportion of HEU children had delayed neurodevelopment in any of the domains assessed, which was less than expected from studies in the general South African population where there are many confounding factors that affect early child development. Maternal SES, BMI and breastfeeding are modifiable factors and could improve neurodevelopment of HEU children at one to two years of age. In line with WHO guidelines, these results suggest that nurturing care and good nutrition related to breastfeeding and healthy maternal BMI, as well as stimulation provided at home by parent/caregiver related to maternal SES may have a significant contribution in improving neurodevelopment of HEU children.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Characteristics for women who were LTFU and their children (n = 145).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.s001

(PDF)

S2 Table. Maternal factors associated with LTFU (n = 505).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.s002

(PDF)

S3 Table. Associations between maternal, child factors and delayed neurodevelopment on combined ASQ domains adjusted for maternal and child factors in one model (n = 355).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.s003

(PDF)

S4 Table. Frequencies of individual ASQ neurodevelopment domains for SGA children stratified by maternal ART initiation status (n = 56).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.s004

(PDF)

S5 Table. Unadjusted associations between maternal factors and delayed neurodevelopment on combined ASQ domains for SGA children (n = 56).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.s005

(PDF)

S6 Table. Unadjusted associations between maternal factors and delayed neurodevelopment on individual ASQ domains for SGA children (n = 56).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.s006

(PDF)

S7 Table. Unadjusted associations between maternal, child factors and delayed neurodevelopment on individual ASQ domains (n = 355).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.s007

(PDF)

S8 Table. Associations between maternal, child factors and delayed neurodevelopment on individual ASQ domains adjusted for maternal and child factors on two separate models (n = 355).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.s008

(PDF)

S9 Table. Associations between maternal, child factors and delayed neurodevelopment on individual ASQ domains adjusted for maternal and child factors in one model (n = 355).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242244.s009

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank participants, clinic staff at Gugulethu Community Health Clinic and staff members of PIMS study.

References

  1. 1. le Roux S, Donald K, Brittain K, Phillips T, Zerbe A, Nguyen K, et al. Neurodevelopment of breastfed HIV-exposed uninfected and HIV-unexposed children in South Africa. AIDS. 2018;32(13):1781–91. pmid:29794831
  2. 2. McHenry MS, McAteer CI, Oyungu E, McDonald BC, Bosma CB, Mpofu PB, et al. Neurodevelopment in young children born to HIV-infected mothers: a meta-analysis. Pediatr. 2018;141(2):e20172888–902. pmid:29374109
  3. 3. Rice ML, Zeldow B, Siberry GK, Purswani M, Malee K, Hoffman HJ, et al. Evaluation of risk for late language emergence after in utero antiretroviral drug exposure in HIV-exposed uninfected infants. Pediatr Infect Dis. J 2013;32(10):e406–13. pmid:24067563
  4. 4. UNAIDS. Progress report on the global plan: towards the elimination of new HIV infections among children by 2015 and keeping their mothers alive. Geneva, Switzerland 2015. Available at: https://www.unaids.org/en/resources/documents/2015/JC2774_2015ProgressReport GlobalPlan. [Accessed 03 February 2020].
  5. 5. Baker-Henningham H, López Bóo F. Early childhood stimulation interventions in developing countries: A comprehensive literature review. Social Protection and Health Division. 2010:1–74.
  6. 6. Lu C, Black MM, Richter LM. Risk of poor development in young children in low-income and middle-income countries: an estimation and analysis at the global, regional, and country level. Lancet Glob Health. 2016;4(12):e916–e22. pmid:27717632
  7. 7. Twogood T. Early Childhood Brain Development. Healthy Childcare American Newsletter. 1999:1–80. Available at: http://ndafp.org/image/cache/18s.pdf [Accessed 05 February 2020].
  8. 8. 1000 days. Good nutrition during the first 1,000 days provides the building blocks for healthy brain development. Washington DC, USA. Available at: https://thousanddays.org/why-1000-days/building-brains/ [Accessed 05 February 2020]
  9. 9. Yoshikawa H. Long-term effects of early childhood programs on social outcomes and delinquency. Future Child. 1995;5(3):51–75. pmid:8835514
  10. 10. Heckman JJ, Masterov DV. The productivity argument for investing in young children. Appl Econ Perspect Policy. 2007;29(3):446–93.
  11. 11. Chandna J, Ntozini R, Evans C, Kandawasvika G, Chasekwa B, Majo F, et al. Effects of improved complementary feeding and improved water, sanitation and hygiene on early child development among HIV-exposed children: substudy of a cluster randomised trial in rural Zimbabwe. BMJ Glob. Health. 2020;5(1):e001718. pmid:32133164
  12. 12. Boivin MJ, Ruiseñor-Escudero H, Familiar-Lopez I. CNS impact of perinatal HIV infection and early treatment: the need for behavioral rehabilitative interventions along with medical treatment and care. Curr. HIV/AIDS Rep. 2016;13(6):318–27. pmid:27783207
  13. 13. UNICEF. First 1000 Days: the critical window to ensure that children survive and thrive. 2017. Available at: https://www.unicef.org/southafrica/SAF_brief_1000days.pdf. [Accessed 05 February 2020].
  14. 14. Ntozini R, Chandna J, Evans C, Chasekwa B, Majo FD, Kandawasvika G, et al. Early child development in children who are HIV‐exposed uninfected compared to children who are HIV‐unexposed: observational sub‐study of a cluster‐randomized trial in rural Zimbabwe. J Int AIDS Soc. 2020;23(5):e25456. pmid:32386127
  15. 15. Cassidy AR, Williams PL, Leidner J, Mayondi G, Ajibola G, Makhema J, et al. In utero efavirenz exposure and neurodevelopmental outcomes in HIV-exposed uninfected children in Botswana. Pediatr Infect Dis J. 2019;38(8):828–34. pmid:30985518
  16. 16. Springer PE, Slogrove AL, Laughton B, Bettinger JA, Saunders HH, Molteno CD, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome of HIV‐exposed but uninfected infants in the Mother and Infants Health Study, Cape Town, South Africa. Trop Med Int Health. 2018;23(1):69–78. pmid:29131457
  17. 17. Struyf T, Dube Q, Cromwell EA, Sheahan AD, Heyderman RS, Van Rie A. The effect of HIV infection and exposure on cognitive development in the first two years of life in Malawi. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2020;25:157–64. pmid:31791872
  18. 18. Van Dyke RB, Chadwick EG, Hazra R, Williams PL, Seage GR III. The PHACS SMARTT study: assessment of the safety of in utero exposure to antiretroviral drugs. Front Immunol. 2016;7:199–216. pmid:27242802
  19. 19. Singh A, Yeh CJ, Blanchard SB. Ages and Stages Questionnaire: a global screening scale. Bol Med Hosp Infant Mex. 2017;74(1):5–12. pmid:29364814
  20. 20. Malaba TR, Gray CM, Myer L, Newell M-L. Cohort Profile: Prematurity Immunology in HIV-infected Mothers and their infants Study (PIMS). MedRxiv. 2020:2020.03.18.20033654.
  21. 21. City of Cape Town. City of Cape Town—2011 census suburb Gugulethu. Stats SA 2011: 1–7 http://resource.capetown.gov.za/2011_Census_CT_Suburb_Nyanga_Profile.pdf (Accessed 27 February 2019).
  22. 22. Gadama LA. Adverse perinatal events observed in obese pregnant women in the Metro West Region. MMed Thesis, University of Cape Town 2014; 1–65.
  23. 23. Myer L, Stein DJ, Grimsrud A, Seedat S, Williams DR. Social determinants of psychological distress in a nationally-representative sample of South African adults. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66(8):1828–40. pmid:18299167
  24. 24. Santos S, Eekhout I, Voerman E, Gaillard R, Barros H, Charles M-A, et al. Gestational weight gain charts for different body mass index groups for women in Europe, North America, and Oceania. BMC Med. 2018;16(1):201–16. pmid:30396358
  25. 25. Hsiao C, Richter L, Makusha T, Matafwali B, Van Heerden A, Mabaso M. Use of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire adapted for South Africa and Zambia. Child Care Health Dev. 2017;43(1):59–66. pmid:27709653
  26. 26. van Heerden A, Hsiao C, Matafwali B, Louw J, Richter L. Support for the feasibility of the ages and stages questionnaire as a developmental screening tool: a cross-sectional study of South African and Zambian children aged 2–60 months. BMC Pediatr. 2017;17(1):55–71. pmid:28209131
  27. 27. Visser M, Nel M, Bronkhorst C, Brown L, Ezendam Z, Mackenzie K, et al. Childhood disability population-based surveillance: Assessment of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire Third Edition and Washington Group on Disability Statistics/UNICEF module on child functioning in a rural setting in South Africa. Afr J Disabil. 2016;5(1):256–64.
  28. 28. Schonhaut L, Armijo I, Schönstedt M, Alvarez J, Cordero M. Validity of the ages and stages questionnaires in term and preterm infants. Pediatr. 2013;131(5):e1468–e74. pmid:23629619
  29. 29. Woodward BJ, Papile L-A, Lowe JR, Laadt VL, Shaffer ML, Montman R, et al. Use of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire and Bayley Scales of Infant Development-II in neurodevelopmental follow-up of extremely low birth weight infants. J Perinatol. 2011;31(10):641–6. pmid:21311498
  30. 30. Yue A, Jiang Q, Wang B, Abbey C, Medina A, Shi Y, et al. Concurrent validity of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire and the Bayley Scales of Infant Development III in China. PloS One. 2019;14(9):e0221675–95. pmid:31487302
  31. 31. Veldhuizen S, Clinton J, Rodriguez C, Wade TJ, Cairney J. Concurrent validity of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires and Bayley Developmental Scales in a general population sample. Acad Pediatr. 2015;15(2):231–7. pmid:25224137
  32. 32. Wedderburn CJ, Yeung S, Rehman AM, Stadler JA, Nhapi RT, Barnett W, et al. Neurodevelopment of HIV-exposed uninfected children in South Africa: outcomes from an observational birth cohort study. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. 2019;3(11):803–13. pmid:31515160
  33. 33. Suggate S, Stoeger H, Pufke E. Relations between playing activities and fine motor development. Early Child Dev Care. 2017;187(8):1297–310.
  34. 34. Caçola P, Gabbard C, Santos DC, Batistela ACT. Development of the affordances in the home environment for motor development–infant scale. Pediatr Int. 2011;53(6):820–5. pmid:21507146
  35. 35. Freitas TC, Gabbard C, Caçola P, Montebelo MI, Santos DC. Family socioeconomic status and the provision of motor affordances in the home. Braz J Phys Ther. 2013;17(4):319–27. pmid:24072221
  36. 36. Piccolo LdR Arteche AX, Fonseca RP, Grassi-Oliveira R, Salles JF. Influence of family socioeconomic status on IQ, language, memory and executive functions of Brazilian children. Psychol Res Rev. 2016;29:23–33
  37. 37. Playford CJ, Dibben C, Williamson L. Socioeconomic disadvantage, fetal environment and child development: linked Scottish administrative records-based study. Int J Equity Health. 2017;16(1):203–16. pmid:29166913
  38. 38. Donald KA, Wedderburn CJ, Barnett W, Nhapi RT, Rehman AM, Stadler JA, et al. Risk and protective factors for child development: An observational South African birth cohort. PLoS Med. 2019;16(9): e1002920–40. pmid:31560687
  39. 39. Abubakar A, Van de Vijver F, Van Baar A, Mbonani L, Kalu R, Newton C, et al. Socioeconomic status, anthropometric status, and psychomotor development of Kenyan children from resource-limited settings: a path-analytic study. Early Hum Dev. 2008;84(9):613–21. pmid:18499363
  40. 40. Ásbjörnsdóttir KH, Slyker JA, Maleche-Obimbo E, Wamalwa D, Otieno P, Gichuhi CM, et al. Breastfeeding is associated with decreased risk of hospitalization among HIV-exposed, uninfected Kenyan infants. J Hum Lact. 2016;32(3):NP61–6. pmid:26423513
  41. 41. Bork KA, Cournil A, Read JS, Newell M-L, Cames C, Meda N, et al. Morbidity in relation to feeding mode in African HIV-exposed, uninfected infants during the first 6 months of life: the Kesho Bora study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;100(6):1559–68. pmid:25411291
  42. 42. Prado EL, Dewey KG. Nutrition and brain development in early life. Nutr Rev. 2014;72(4):267–84. pmid:24684384
  43. 43. Rochat TJ, Houle B, Stein A, Coovadia H, Coutsoudis A, Desmond C, et al. Exclusive breastfeeding and cognition, executive function, and behavioural disorders in primary school-aged children in rural South Africa: a cohort analysis. PLoS Med. 2016;13(6):e1002044–74. pmid:27328132
  44. 44. Mofenson LM. In-utero ART exposure and the need for pharmacovigilance. Lancet Glob Health. 2018;6(7):e716–e7. pmid:29880311
  45. 45. Chaudhury S, Williams PL, Mayondi GK, Leidner J, Holding P, Tepper V, et al. Neurodevelopment of HIV-exposed and HIV-unexposed uninfected children at 24 months. Pediatr. 2017;140(4):e20170988–1005. pmid:28912368
  46. 46. Alcaide ML, Rodriguez VJ, Abbamonte JM, Ramlagan S, Sifunda S, Weiss SM, et al. Maternal factors associated with infant neurodevelopment in HIV-exposed uninfected infants. Open Forum Infect Dis. 2019;6(10):ofz351–64 pmid:31660335
  47. 47. Boivin MJ, Maliwichi-Senganimalunje L, Ogwang LW, Kawalazira R, Sikorskii A, Familiar-Lopez I, et al. Neurodevelopmental effects of ante-partum and post-partum antiretroviral exposure in HIV-exposed and uninfected children versus HIV-unexposed and uninfected children in Uganda and Malawi: a prospective cohort study. Lancet HIV. 2019;6(8):e518–e30. pmid:31122797
  48. 48. Crowell CS, Williams P, Yildirim C, Van Dyke R, Smith R, Chadwick EG, et al., editors. LB5. Safety of in utero antiretroviral (ARV) exposure: neurologic outcomes in HIV-exposed, uninfected children. Open Forum Infect Dis; 2018: Oxford University Press.
  49. 49. Li N, Sando MM, Spiegelman D, Hertzmark E, Liu E, Sando D, et al. Antiretroviral therapy in relation to birth outcomes among HIV-infected women: a cohort study. J Infect Dis. 2016;213(7):1057–64. pmid:26265780
  50. 50. De Bie HMA, Oostrom KJ, Delemarre-Van De Waal HA. Brain development, intelligence and cognitive outcome in children born small for gestational age. Horm Res Paediatr. 2010;73(1):6–14. pmid:20190535
  51. 51. Bardin C, Piuze G, Papageorgiou A. Outcome at 5 years of age of SGA and AGA infants born less than 28 weeks of gestation. Semin Perinatol. 2004;28(4):288–294. pmid:15565789
  52. 52. Arcangeli T, Thilaganathan B, Hooper R, Khan K, Bhide A. Neurodevelopmental delay in small babies at term: a systematic review. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2012;40(3):267–75. pmid:22302630
  53. 53. Colvin M, McGuire W, Fowlie PW. Neurodevelopmental outcomes after preterm birth. BMJ. 2004;329(7479):1390–3. pmid:15591566
  54. 54. Blencowe H, Lee AC, Cousens S, Bahalim A, Narwal R, Zhong N, et al. Preterm birth-associated neurodevelopmental impairment estimates at regional and global levels for 2010. Pediatr Res. 2013;74(S1):17–34. pmid:24366461
  55. 55. Nuss EE, Spiegelman J, Turitz AL, Gyamfi-Bannerman C. Childhood neurodevelopment after spontaneous versus indicated preterm birth. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2019;100082:1–7.
  56. 56. Bonnier C. Evaluation of early stimulation programs for enhancing brain development. Acta Paediatr. 2008;97(7):853–8. pmid:18482172