Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Professionals’ attitudes towards the use of cognitive enhancers in academic settings

  • Sanyogita (Sanya) Ram ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    Sanya.ram@auckland.ac.nz

    Affiliations Centre for Medicine Use and Safety, Faculty of Pharmacy and Pharmaceutical Science, Monash University (Parkville Campus), Parkville, Australia, School of Pharmacy, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand

  • Bruce Russell,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Clinical Pharmacy, School of Pharmacy, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand

  • Carl Kirkpatrick,

    Roles Formal analysis, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Medicine Use and Safety, Faculty of Pharmacy and Pharmaceutical Science, Monash University (Parkville Campus), Parkville, Australia

  • Kay Stewart,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Medicine Use and Safety, Faculty of Pharmacy and Pharmaceutical Science, Monash University (Parkville Campus), Parkville, Australia

  • Shane Scahill,

    Roles Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Pharmacy, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand

  • Marcus Henning,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Medical and Health Sciences Education, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand

  • Louise Curley,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Pharmacy, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand

  • Safeera Hussainy

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of General Practice, School of Primary and Allied Health Care, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing and Health Sciences Monash University, Notting Hill, Victoria, Australia

Abstract

Introduction and aims

The non-medical use of prescription stimulants such as methylphenidate, dexamphetamine and modafinil is increasing in popularity within tertiary academic settings. There is a paucity of information on awareness, attitudes, and acceptability by professionals of use in this context. This study aimed to investigate professionals’ knowledge of and attitudes towards the use of cognitive enhancers (CEs) in academic settings, and their willingness to use a hypothetical CE.

Design and methods

A mail survey was sent to doctors, pharmacists, nurses, accountants and lawyers in New Zealand. These disciplines were chosen as they require professional registration to practice. The questionnaire comprised four sections: (1) demographics, (2) knowledge of CEs, (3) attitudes towards the use of CEs, and (4) willingness to use hypothetical CEs.

Results

The response rate was 34.5% (414/1200). Overall, participants strongly disagreed that it was fair to allow university students to use CEs for cognitive enhancement (Mdn = 1, IQR: 1,3), or that it is ethical for students without a prescription to use cognitive enhancers for any reason (Mdn = 1, IQR: 1,2). Professions differed in their attitudes towards whether it is ethical for students without a prescription to use CEs for any reason (p = 0.001, H 31.527).

Discussion and conclusion

Divergent views and lack of clear consensus within professions and between professionals on the use of CEs have the potential to influence both professionals and students as future professionals. These divergent views may stem from differences in the core values of self-identity as well as extrinsic factors of acceptability within the profession in balancing the elements of opportunity, fairness and authenticity in cognitive enhancement. Further research is required to inform the development of policy and guidelines that are congruent with all professions.

Introduction

The use of prescription stimulant medicines such as modafinil, amphetamines, and methylphenidate for cognitive enhancement is increasing, especially within tertiary academic environments [1, 2]. The lifetime prevalence of cognitive enhancer (CE) use amongst those students attending university has been reported to range from 1.2% to 34% [314]. This includes those students in all tertiary and college settings. CEs are used by healthy individuals in academic settings to try to improve concentration, increase alertness, stay awake longer or perform better academically. Methylphenidate is clinically indicated for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and increases synaptic levels of dopamine and noradrenaline by blocking their respective reuptake transporters and consequently acting as a central nervous system stimulant. A systematic review conducted by Repantis et al. (2010) concluded that although studies showed a positive effect of methylphenidate on the memory of healthy individuals, further studies were warranted to determine its capacity [15]. While studies have shown cognitive- enhancing effects of modafinil in sleep-deprived individuals, it is unclear to what extent this extends to non-sleep- deprived individuals [15]. While there is continued debate on the whether CEs have an effect on healthy individuals, they continue to be used by healthy individuals for cognitive enhancement [314].

Despite there being numerous studies amongst tertiary students exploring their attitudes and motivations to use CEs, there is a paucity of information on awareness and acceptability of CE use in academic settings by professionals. It is recognised that use of CEs may extend beyond student life and into the workforce [16]. A survey of surgeons at five international conferences in 2011 reported that 8.9% of surgeons had used a prescription or illicit drug as a CE at least once in their lifetime [17]. In addition, an informal, online survey conducted by Nature, ‘Look who’s doping’, received responses from 1,400 respondents from 60 countries. While specific details of the respondents’ characteristics were not supplied, it was reported that one in five respondents had used drugs for non-medical reasons to help focus, enhance concentration or memory [18].

The decision to use CEs is influenced by personal attitudes towards the use of CEs as well as the wider attitudes of social networks. Similar to substance abuse where group norms around social acceptance dictate patterns of use [19], the use of CEs is enhanced by the influence of social networks and attitudes towards use. Social networks and friends are the main sources of information on CEs [20]. Family and friends are also important sources for obtaining CEs [6, 21, 22]. Maier et al. reported that users of CEs had a smaller social network and exhibited less prosocial behavior in social interaction tasks [23]. The influence of peers and the normalisation of CE use amongst social networks may provide the impetus to use CEs [24]. Judson et al. found that attitudes towards the ethics and perceived norms of CE use predicted use [25]. Lesser concerns about adverse health effects, ethics of use and perceived control, and higher perceived positive subjective norms were also related to illicit use among non-prescription holders [25]. A survey of students enrolled within professional courses namely medicine, pharmacy, nursing, accounting and law reported that those who perceived CE use to be socially and ethically acceptable were more likely to use CEs (OR 1.56, CI 1.153–2.105, p = 0.004) [20]. Beyond use amongst students, qualitative studies have suggested that the social context within which CEs are normalised, accessed and contextualised, which is important for framing the acceptability of CEs, especially in high pressured and competitive environments [26, 27].

Understanding public attitudes towards cognitive enhancement are fundamental to the development of acceptable, reasonable and effective policy [28]. A study exploring the attitudes of parents of university students and healthcare providers reported confusion and a lack of awareness of the prevalence of CE use by students [29]. Physicians’ views about prescribing and use of CEs pivot on the delineation between the treatment of a condition compared to enhancement, suggesting a trade-off between safety, harm and benefit [3032]. Forlini and Racine (2009) reported that healthcare providers focussed their discussion of cognitive enhancement on health consequences and health risks [33]. However, there is a level of ambivalence towards what ought to be done about CE use [29]. Forlini et al. (2012) contend that professional policies may be sidestepping the values of stakeholders and call for a collaboration of professional associations with the humanities for a joint deliberation on the moral praiseworthiness of cognitive enhancement and pay closer attention to the divergence of fundamental values that has a broader impact on health and education [34].

The literature highlights the complexity of understanding the decision to use CEs; it is not a discrete and detached choice that occurs in isolation without consideration of the factors that influence the decision [20]. The biopsychosocial systems model of addiction proposes that intersecting biological, psychosocial, social and systemic properties are fundamental features of substance use [19]. It posits that knowledge of substance use occurs at the intersection of the subjective and the objective and not just as an independent reality [19]. This research focusses on the social and systems dimensions and seeks to explore the acceptability of CE use. Exploring how professionals, having undergone tertiary studies, view the use of CEs in the tertiary academic setting and whether they advocate the use of CEs, will help comprehend perspectives and endorsement of CE use.

Aim

This research aims to investigate professionals’ awareness and perceived knowledge of the use of CEs in academic settings, and their attitudes towards and willingness to use a hypothetical CE.

Methods

A mail survey was sent to a random sample of 200 professionals from each profession of general practitioners, psychiatrists, pharmacists, nurses, accountants and lawyers in New Zealand. General practitioners are medical practitioners who have specialised in the discipline of general practice, a clinical speciality orientated to primary care. These professions aligned with the academic disciplines of students we previously surveyed in a separate study [35]. These disciplines were chosen as they require registration to practice as a professional. The prevalence of CE use amongst students prompted the need to explore this phenomenon amongst practising professionals. The contact details of lawyers, accountants, nurses, general practitioners, psychiatrists and community pharmacists were obtained from either their respective registration authorities when permitted for research purposes or through publicly available online databases. Completion of the questionnaire and sending it back to the research team was deemed as consent to participate.

The questionnaire comprised four sections:

  1. demographics,
  2. knowledge of CEs,
  3. attitudes towards the use of CEs,
  4. willingness to use a hypothetical CE.

An additional section was included for potential prescribers of CE. Questions in Sections A and B were drawn from earlier research exploring student use of and attitudes towards CEs [6]. Section C was developed from a questionnaire used to explore attitudes towards CEs [20, 36], and employing questions that were deemed relevant to seek the attitudes of professionals. Each statement was followed by a seven-point Likert-type scale, ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7).

A draft version of the questionnaire was piloted with eight participants: four doctoral candidates, two psychiatrists, an emergency consultant, and a pharmacist. Feedback received included comments on the clarity of questions, the number of questions to complete and duplication of questions in the questionnaire. The draft questionnaire was amended based on the feedback provided. The study was approved by the University of Auckland Human Ethics Committee (2015:012571) and the Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee (2016:CF15/2541–2015001029).

An abridged version of the Dillman protocol for conducting surveys was used to distribute the questionnaire and follow up participants [37]. A paper-based survey method was chosen over online mechanisms due to legal and ethical constraints in obtaining email addresses of potential participants and gaining access to participants’ electronic addresses. Following the initial mail-out, two further mail-outs were sent to non-responders at three weeks and seven weeks. If potential participants did not wish to receive follow up reminders, they were invited to send their uncompleted questionnaires back in the self-addressed envelopes. The questionnaire was not coded; however, the envelope was coded to allow tracking of responses and follow up of non-responders.

Data analysis was undertaken using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) V19 (IBM, Chicago, IL, USA) analytical software. Descriptive statistics were used to analyse participant characteristics. The median ranks in agreement and range were calculated for each attitudinal statement. Non-parametric results of the median (Mdn) and interquartile range (IQR) are reported. Kruskal-Wallis test was used to test for differences in attitudes towards the use of CEs in academic settings by the five groups of professionals for each attitudinal statement. Pairwise comparisons were used to explore the nature of any differences in attitudes found among the professionals [38].

Results

The response rate was 34.5% (414/1200). Responses were received from 24% (48/200) of Accountants, 54.5% (109/200) of pharmacists, 28% (56/200) of general practitioners, 29.5% (59/200) of nurses, 30.5% (61/200) of lawyers, and 40.5% (81/200) of psychiatrists. Just over half of the respondents were female (n = 225, 54.35%), within the 50 to 59 years age bracket (n = 133, 32.12%) and in practice for longer than 30 years (n = 109, 26.3%) (Table 1).

When participants were asked to list the CEs that they had heard about, the most commonly listed CEs were methylphenidate (n = 200, 48.3%), followed by dexamphetamine or methamphetamine (n = 131, 31.6%), modafinil (n = 83, 20%) and atomoxetine (n = 19, 4.6%).

Participants had first heard of CEs from scientific literature (23%) or the media (9.4%). Other sources included friends (4.3%), internet (3.1%), and family (1.4%). Most participants rated their knowledge of CE use in healthy individuals as either not very knowledgeable (n = 258, 62.3%), or somewhat knowledgeable (n = 93, 22.5%) (Table 1).

Overall, participants strongly disagreed that it was fair to allow university students to use CEs for cognitive enhancement (Mdn = 1, IQR: 1,3), to concentrate (Mdn = 1, IQR:1,2), to increase alertness/stay awake (Mdn = 1, IQR: 1,2) or to counteract the effects of other drugs (Mdn = 1, IQR: 1,2). Participants also strongly disagreed that it is ethical for students without a prescription to use cognitive enhancers for any reason (Mdn = 1, IQR: 1,2) and for students with a prescription to use CEs in excess or for purposes other than for which they were prescribed by a doctor (Mdn = 1, IQR: 1,2).

Participants agreed with the concern that taking medicines for cognitive enhancement, even as prescribed by a doctor, will adversely affect one’s health (Mdn = 5, IQR: 4,6). Participants indicated neutral scores on whether the use of CEs with a prescription (Mdn = 4, IQR: 2,4) or without a prescription (Mdn = 4, IQR: 3,4) is common at universities. They were also ambivalent as to whether CEs were effective for enhancement (Mdn = 4, IQR: 3,5).

There was a large variation in responses on whether it was safe for students with a prescription to use CEs as prescribed by a doctor for cognitive enhancement (Mdn = 4, IQR: 2,5) and on whether their colleagues believed that it is okay for students to use CEs as prescribed by a doctor for cognitive enhancement (Mdn = 3, IQR: 1,4).

Professions differed in their attitudes towards whether it is ethical for students without a prescription to use CEs for any reason (p = 0.001, H = 31.527) (Table 2). Pairwise comparison with adjusted p-values showed that pharmacists (Mdn = 1, IQR: 1,1) differed in their attitudes from nurses (Mdn = 1, IQR: 1,3) accountants (Mdn 1, IQR: 1,2), and lawyers (Mdn 2, IQR: 1,3). Professions also differed on whether it is fair to use CEs in excess of what is prescribed (p = 0.008, H = 15.749), whether it is ethical for students without a prescription to use CEs to concentrate (p = 0.001, H = 27.283), or to increase alertness/stay awake (p = 0.001, H = 41.622).

thumbnail
Table 2. Attitudes towards the use of CEs in academic settings.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0241968.t002

Willingness to use a hypothetical CE

Participants were asked whether they would take a hypothetical prescription-only CE that shows proven efficacy, is approved by the regulatory authorities, and is devoid of significant side effects. As shown in Table 3, of the 398 participants who answered the question, 50% indicated that they would not use a hypothetical CE (n = 199), and only 11.6% (n = 46) said that they would. Hypothetically, the reasons they chose from a list where they could choose more than one option, included to improve concentration (n = 175), increase alertness (n = 123), alleviate pressure to perform better at work (n = 88), stay awake (n = 68), and for experimentation (n = 43). No significant differences were noted between whether participants had heard of a CE and whether they would be willing to take a hypothetical CE.

Discussion

This comparative study brings to light attitudes of professionals (pharmacists, doctors, nurses, lawyers, and accountants) towards the use of CEs in academic settings. Although participants strongly disagreed that it was fair to allow university students to use CEs for cognitive enhancement, participants were ambivalent on whether they believed that the use of CEs by students with or without a prescription for CE was common at universities. While participants disagreed that it is ethical for students without a prescription to use CEs for any reason, there was variation in responses across professions on whether it was safe for students with a prescription to use CEs as prescribed by a doctor. This may stem from divergent views on whether it is acceptable to use CEs if they are prescribed for cognitive enhancement and a level of acceptance of safety and risk if a doctor has prescribed them.

These findings are consistent with those reported by Banjo et. al. that healthcare providers posit concerns on fairness and social injustice that CE use presents. Our data, however, is inconsistent with findings by Forlini et.al. that parents and healthcare providers reported a level of ambivalence toward CE use [29]. There is agreement that it is unfair to use CEs; however, there is ambivalence on whether it is acceptable to use CEs as prescribed by a doctor and divergent views on whether it will adversely affect one’s health if used as prescribed. The acceptance of the use of CEs as prescribed may be seen as legitimising its use [39]. Students have expressed security in the safety of CEs as methylphenidate is a prescribed medication, not a street drug, and is safe because it has gone through extensive testing by pharmaceutical companies and is prescribed by medical professionals [24, 29]. Forlini et. al. contend that a multidirectional approach in interpreting ambivalence may actually reveal much deeper discomfort and moral unease with the wider social impact of CE use [29]. Peterson et al. categorised general practitioner attitudes towards the use of CEs as rejectors, navigators or acceptors, with more than half expressing comfort with optimising students capabilities [32]. Greater perceived knowledge has been related to a reduced perception of health risks with greater emphasis on alleged effectiveness and legality, rather than on risks for health [40]. This indicates divergent views and a lack of consensus on attitudes towards CEs.

There were differences amongst the professions on whether it was ethical to use CEs. Pharmacists more strongly disagreed than nurses, accountants, and lawyers that it is ethical for students without a prescription to use CEs for any reason. The social and psychosocial factors within professions may play a significant role in translating these attitudes towards CEs. For example pharmacists tend to affiliate more with the non-maleficent approach, with their primary goal being to reduce risk of harm and therefore they may consistently take a risk-averse approach [41]. Healthcare professionals differed from accountants and lawyers in their attitudes towards whether it was ethical to use CEs to increase alertness and wakefulness, with healthcare professionals having a more negative attitude. These differences may stem from personal concepts of self-identity and authenticity as well as challenges to the fundamental values held by the individual and the community [34]. These divergent views and lack of clear consensus within and between professions on the use of CEs have the potential to influence both the professionals and students as future professionals. This research provides insight into the attitudes towards the use of CEs in academic settings by professions and discourse necessary for the development of policy and guidelines that are congruent among professions.

When asked whether they would take a hypothetical prescription-only CE with proven efficacy, approved by regulatory authorities, and devoid of significant side effects, the hypothetical willingness to use CEs reported by professionals (11.6%) was higher than prevalence reported by students [35]; however, the reasons for use are similar amongst professionals and students. Most participants indicated that they would not take a hypothetical prescription CE even under these circumstances.

The hypothetical prevalence is higher than lifetime prevalence (6.6%) reported at a New Zealand tertiary institution but is within the range of 1.2% to 34% reported amongst college students [313]. Reasons for hypothetical use by professionals were consistent with students’ explanations for use, such as to improve concentration, increase alertness, stay awake longer or perform better academically. Professionals also indicated hypothetical use may occur in response to the pressure to perform at work.

Limitations

As reported earlier, the data collected may be affected by underreporting, social desirability bias or unwillingness to disclose true attitudes [42]. The low response rate (34.5%) may hinder these results from being generalisable. The comparison between hypothetical prevalence and self-reported use is difficult, as it is unclear whether self-reports of hypothetical use would eventuate in actual use and self-reported data is based on memory and willingness to disclose. Hypothetical rather than actual use was explored in this study due to the sensitivity of the topic and concern for privacy, anonymity and confidentiality of reports and may not reflect actual behaviours and attitudes towards CE use due to social desirability of responses [43].

Conclusion

This study brings to light the attitudes of professionals (pharmacists, doctors, nurses, lawyers, and accountants) towards the use of CEs in academic settings. While there is agreement that it is unfair for students to be allowed to use CEs for cognitive enhancement, there is ambivalence about the safety and use of CEs if prescribed by a doctor. Professionals differed in their attitudes towards CEs and this may stem from differences in the core values of self-identity as well as extrinsic factors of acceptability within the profession in balancing the elements of opportunity, fairness and authenticity in cognitive enhancement. Further research to explore the social and pyschosocial norms within professions, the reasons for ambivalence and the influence of the profession on endorsing or curbing the use of CEs is warranted.

References

  1. 1. Maier LJ, Ferris JA, Winstock AR. Pharmacological cognitive enhancement among non-ADHD individuals—A cross-sectional study in 15 countries. International Journal of Drug Policy. 2018;58:104–12. pmid:29902691
  2. 2. Herman-Stahl MA, Krebs CP, Kroutil LA, Heller DC. Risk and protective factors for methamphetamine use and nonmedical use of prescription stimulants among young adults aged 18 to 25. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32(5):1003–15. pmid:16920275
  3. 3. Franke AG, Bonertz C, Christmann M, Huss M, Fellgiebel A, Hildt E, et al. Non-medical use of prescription stimulants and illicit use of stimulants for cognitive enhancement in pupils and students in Germany. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2011;44(02):60–6. pmid:21161883
  4. 4. DeSantis AD, Webb EM, Noar SM. Illicit use of prescription ADHD medications on a college campus: a multimethodological approach. Journal of American college health. 2008;57(3):315–24. pmid:18980888
  5. 5. McCabe SE, Teter CJ, Boyd CJ. Medical use, illicit use, and diversion of abusable prescription drugs. Journal of American college health. 2006;54(5):269–78. pmid:16539219
  6. 6. Ram S, Hussainy S, Henning M, Jensen M, Russell B. Prevalence of cognitive enhancer use among New Zealand tertiary students. Drug and Alcohol Review. 2016;35(3):345–51. pmid:26121209
  7. 7. Hall KM, Irwin MM, Bowman KA, Frankenberger W, Jewett DC. Illicit use of prescribed stimulant medication among college students. Journal of American College Health. 2005;53(4):167–74. pmid:15663065
  8. 8. Kudlow PA, Treurnicht Naylor K, Xie B, McIntyre RS. Cognitive Enhancement in Canadian Medical Students. Journal of psychoactive drugs. 2013;45(4):360–5. pmid:24377176
  9. 9. Lazuras L, Ypsilanti A, Lamprou E, Kontogiorgis C. Pharmaceutical Cognitive Enhancement in Greek University Students: Differences Between Users and Non-Users in Social Cognitive Variables, Burnout, and Engagement. Substance Use and Misuse. 2017;52(7):950–8. pmid:28426360
  10. 10. Silveira RR, Lejderman B, Ferreira PEMS, da Rocha GMP. Patterns of non-medical use of methylphenidate among 5th and 6th year students in a medical school in southern Brazil. Trends in Psychiatry and Psychotherapy. 2014;36(2):101–6. pmid:27000709
  11. 11. Teter CJ, McCabe SE, Cranford JA, Boyd CJ, Guthrie SK. Prevalence and motives for illicit use of prescription stimulants in an undergraduate student sample. Journal of American College Health. 2005;53(6):253–62. pmid:15900989
  12. 12. Wolff W, Brand R. Subjective stressors in school and their relation to neuroenhancement: A behavioral perspective on students' everyday life " doping". Substance Abuse: Treatment, Prevention, and Policy. 2013;8(1). pmid:23777577
  13. 13. Lengvenyte A, Strumila R, Grikiniene J. Use of cognitive enhancers among medical students in Lithuania. NAD Nordic Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2016;33(2):173–87.
  14. 14. Singh I, Bard I, Jackson J. Robust resilience and substantial interest: a survey of pharmacological cognitive enhancement among university students in the UK and Ireland. PLoS ONE [Electronic Resource]. 2014;9(10):e105969. pmid:25356917
  15. 15. Repantis D, Schlattmann P, Laisney O, Heuser I. Modafinil and methlyphenidate for neuroenhancement in healthy individuals: A systematic review. Pharmacological Research. 2010;62:187–206. pmid:20416377
  16. 16. Sales P, Murphy F, Murphy S, Lau N. Burning the candle at both ends: motivations for non-medical prescription stimulant use in the American workplace. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy. 2019;26(4):301–8.
  17. 17. Franke AG, Bagusat C, Dietz P, Hoffmann I, Simon P, Ulrich R, et al. Use of illicit and prescription drugs for cognitive or mood enhancement among surgeons. BMC Medicine. 2013;11(1).
  18. 18. Maher B. Poll results: look who's doping. Nature. 2008;452(7188):674–5. pmid:18401370
  19. 19. Buchman DZ, Skinner W, Illes J. Negotiating the Relationship Between Addiction, Ethics, and Brain Science. AJOB Neuroscience. 2010;1(1):36–45. pmid:20676352
  20. 20. Ram S, Hussainy S, Henning M, Stewart K, Jensen M, Russell B. Attitudes Toward Cognitive Enhancer Use Among New Zealand Tertiary Students. Substance Use & Misuse. 2017;52(11):1387–92. pmid:28429997
  21. 21. McCabe S, Teter C, Boyd C. Medical Use, Illicit Use, and Diversion of Prescription Stimulant Medication. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs. 2006b;38(1):43–56. pmid:16681175
  22. 22. McNiel AD, Muzzin KB, DeWald JP, McCann AL, Schneiderman ED, Scofield J, et al. The nonmedical use of prescription stimulants among dental and dental hygiene students. Journal of Dental Education. 2011;75(3):365–76. pmid:21368261
  23. 23. Maier LJ, Wunderli MD, Vonmoos M, Römmelt AT, Baumgartner MR, Seifritz E, et al. Pharmacological Cognitive Enhancement in Healthy Individuals: A Compensation for Cognitive Deficits or a Question of Personality? PLOS ONE. 2015;10(6):e0129805. pmid:26107846
  24. 24. Desantis AD, Hane AC. "Adderall is Definitely Not a Drug": Justifications for the Illegal Use of ADHD Stimulants. Substance Use & Misuse. 2010;45(1–2):31–46.
  25. 25. Judson R, Langdon SW. Illicit use of prescription stimulants among college students: Prescription status, motives, theory of planned behaviour, knowledge and self-diagnostic tendencies. Psychology, Health & Medicine. 2008;14(1):97–104.
  26. 26. Coveney C, Williams SJ, Gabe J. Enhancement imaginaries: exploring public understandings of pharmaceutical cognitive enhancing drugs. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy. 2019;26(4):319–28.
  27. 27. Sattler S, Sauer C, Mehlkop G, Graeff P. The Rationale for Consuming Cognitive Enhancement Drugs in University Students and Teachers. PLOS ONE. 2013;8(7):e68821. pmid:23874778
  28. 28. Fitz NS, Nadler R, Manogaran P, Chong EWJ, Reiner PB. Public Attitudes Toward Cognitive Enhancement. Neuroethics. 2014;7(2):173–88.
  29. 29. Forlini C, Racine E. Stakeholder perspectives and reactions to "academic" cognitive enhancement: Unsuspected meaning of ambivalence and analogies. Public understanding of science. 2012;21(5):606–25. pmid:23823168
  30. 30. Banjo OC, Nadler R, Reiner PB. Physician Attitudes towards Pharmacological Cognitive Enhancement: Safety Concerns Are Paramount. PLOS ONE. 2010;5(12):e14322. pmid:21179461
  31. 31. Bergström LS, Lynöe N. Enhancing concentration, mood and memory in healthy individuals: An empirical study of attitudes among general practitioners and the general population. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health. 2008;36(5):532–7. pmid:18635734
  32. 32. Petersen MA, Lyngsø-Dahl Ølgaard LJ, Nørgaard LS. Contextualizing study drugs—An exploratory study of perceptions and practices among study counselors, general practitioners, psychiatrists and from student polls. Research in Social and Administrative Pharmacy. 2019;15(10):1204–11. pmid:30448283
  33. 33. Forlini C, Racine E. Autonomy and Coercion in Academic “Cognitive Enhancement” Using Methylphenidate: Perspectives of Key Stakeholders. Neuroethics. 2009;2(3):163–77.
  34. 34. Forlini C, Racine E. Added Stakeholders, Added Value(s) to the Cognitive Enhancement Debate: Are Academic Discourse and Professional Policies Sidestepping Values of Stakeholders? AJOB Primary Research. 2012;3(1):33–47.
  35. 35. Ram SS, Hussainy S, Henning M, Jensen M, Russell B. Prevalence of cognitive enhancer use among New Zealand tertiary students. Drug and alcohol review. 2015. pmid:26121209
  36. 36. Judson R, Langdon SW. Illicit use of prescription stimulants among college students: prescription status, motives, theory of planned behaviour, knowledge and self-diagnostic tendencies. Psychology, Health & Medicine. 2009;14(1):97–104. pmid:19085316
  37. 37. D D. Mail and internet surveys. New York: John Wiley and Sons; 2000.
  38. 38. Field AP. Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. 5th edition. ed. Field AP, editor: London Thousand Oaks, California: SAGE Publications. 2018 ©2018; 2018.
  39. 39. Petersen MA, Nørgaard LS, Traulsen JM. Going to the doctor with enhancement in mind–An ethnographic study of university students’ use of prescription stimulants and their moral ambivalence. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy. 2015;22(3):201–7.
  40. 40. Mayor E, Daehne M, Bianchi R. How perceived substance characteristics affect ethical judgement towards cognitive enhancement. PLOS ONE. 2019;14(3):e0213619. pmid:30870469
  41. 41. Duffull SB, Wright DFB, Marra CA, Anakin MG. A philosophical framework for pharmacy in the 21st century guided by ethical principles. Research in Social and Administrative Pharmacy. 2018;14(3):309–16. pmid:28473230
  42. 42. Ram S, Russell BR, Stewart K, Kirkpatrick C, Henning M, Scahill S, et al. Psychiatrists’ attitudes towards and willingness to prescribe cognitive enhancers in academic settings. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy. 2020:1–8.
  43. 43. Davis CG, Thake J, Vilhena N. Social desirability biases in self-reported alcohol consumption and harms. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35(4):302–11. pmid:19932936