Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Development of a risk classification model in early pregnancy to screen for suboptimal postnatal mother-to-infant bonding: A prospective cohort study

  • Elke Tichelman ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    e.tichelman@umcg.nl

    Affiliations Department of Midwifery Science, Amsterdam UMC, AVAG, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Department of General Practice & Elderly Care Medicine, University Medical Centre Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands

  • Jens Henrichs,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Midwifery Science, Amsterdam UMC, AVAG, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • François G. Schellevis,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of General Practice & Elderly Care Medicine, Amsterdam UMC, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, NIVEL, Netherlands Institute for Health Services Research, Utrecht, The Netherlands

  • Marjolein Y. Berger,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of General Practice & Elderly Care Medicine, University Medical Centre Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands

  • Huibert Burger

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of General Practice & Elderly Care Medicine, University Medical Centre Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands, Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam University Medical Center, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Abstract

Background

Previous studies identified demographic, reproduction-related and psychosocial correlates of suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding. Their joint informative value was still unknown. This study aimed to develop a multivariable model to screen early in pregnancy for suboptimal postnatal mother-to-infant bonding and to transform it into a risk classification model.

Methods

Prospective cohort study conducted at 116 midwifery centers between 2010–2014. 634 women reported on the Mother-to-Infant Bonding questionnaire in 2015–2016. A broad range of determinants before 13 weeks of gestation were considered. Missing data were described, analyzed and imputed by multiple imputation. Multivariable logistic regression with backward elimination was used to develop a screening model. The explained variance, the Area Under the Curve of the final model were calculated and a Hosmer and Lemeshow test performed. Finally, we designed a risk classification model.

Results

The prevalence of suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding was 11%. The estimated probability of suboptimal mother-to-infant can be calculated: P(MIBS≥4) = 1/(1+exp(-(-4.391+(parity× 0.519)+(Adult attachment avoidance score× 0.040))). The explained variance was 14% and the Area Under the Curve was 0.750 (95%CI 0.690–0.809). The Hosmer and Lemeshow test had a p-value of 0.21. This resulted in a risk classification model.

Conclusion

Parity and adult attachment avoidance were the strongest independent determinants. Higher parity and higher levels of adult attachment avoidance are associated with an increased risk of suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding. The model and risk classification model should be externally validated and optimized before use in daily practice. Future research should include an external validation study, a study into the additional value of non-included determinants and finally a study on the impact and feasibility of the screening model.

Introduction

Mother-to-infant bonding is defined as the emotional tie experienced by a mother towards her (unborn) child having the aim to protect the child [1]. The unidirectional mother-to-infant bond is not to be confused with the bidirectional infant-to-mother attachment based on which the infant uses the caregiver as secure base for exploration [2]. The development of mother-to-infant bonding already starts during pregnancy and remains stable until toddlerhood [1, 3] and has important implications. According to a recent systematic review, mother-to-infant bonding quality measured at any time point during pregnancy or postpartum is positively associated with mother-to-infant bonding quality at follow-up, either prenatally or postpartum [4].

Mother-to-infant bonding is essential for positive socio-emotional development of the child [57], and predicts child executive functioning [6]. Several explanations for these associations have been identified. Suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding is associated with more parenting stress and less sensitive and poorer maternal parenting styles and skills [8]. Less sensitive and poorer maternal parenting, in turn, has been linked to insecure attachment, depression, and anxiety in children [8]. Furthermore, women with suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding show less interest in their child’s health, and engage more frequently in negative health behaviours during pregnancy [9]. These behaviours are associated with adverse birth outcomes and with adverse long-term cognitive and socio-emotional development of the child [9].

Suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding is not uncommon. The prevalence of suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding one year postpartum varies between 5% and 11% in the general population indicating its potential to have a substantial impact on public health [10, 11]. Nevertheless, monitoring mother-to-infant bonding is presently no standard practice in pregnancy [12]. Although there are already adequate instruments to assess prenatal mother-to-infant bonding, they have not been validated before mid-pregnancy [13]. Before twenty weeks of gestation accurate assessment of mother-to-infant bonding is not possible. To target monitoring strategies and early interventions efficiently, a risk classification model will help to identify women at an early stage at risk of suboptimal postnatal mother-to-infant bonding. To our knowledge, no screening model for the general population exists. One investigation tried to establish prenatal determinants of mother-to-infant bonding in a clinical sample of 251 women enrolled at a public perinatal psychiatric service [14]. Previous studies identified demographic, reproduction-related and psychosocial correlates for impaired bonding in the general population [4]. However, their joint informative value is still unknown.

This is unfortunate as timely recognition of high risk of suboptimal bonding is an opportunity for health care professionals to provide preventive interventions at an early stage, i.e. from the beginning of pregnancy onwards. For example, midwives and gynecologists may stimulate mother-to-infant bonding by several interventions which are currently available, such as antenatal group education with attention to bonding and fetal movements, and interaction with the fetus by touching or singing [1517]. Healthcare providers could also spend more time to address transition to motherhood early in pregnancy, for example by stimulating to fantasize about the baby during ultrasound assessment [12].

The overall aim of the current study is therefore to develop a multivariable model to screen early in pregnancy for mother-to-infant bonding postpartum. We investigated which determinants in the first trimester of pregnancy have the highest informative value for suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding up to 24 months postpartum and how to combine them into a reliable and easy to use risk classification model. Candidate determinants were derived a priori from reviews focusing on correlates of prenatal mother-to-infant bonding [1820], and from one review of correlates of prenatal and postnatal mother-to-infant bonding [4].

Methods

Study design and setting

We used data from the Pregnancy, Anxiety and Depression (PAD) Study [21]. This prospective population-based cohort study investigates symptoms of and risk factors for anxiety and depression of the mother during pregnancy and the first years postpartum.

All pregnant women in their first trimester of pregnancy visiting a total of 109 primary and nine secondary obstetric care centers in the Netherlands between 2010 and 2014 were invited to participate. Ethical Approval for the study was obtained in 2010 and additional in 2015 from the medical ethical review board of the University Medical Center Groningen (METc2009.235). Written informed consent was obtained from all women. Privacy was guaranteed in accordance with Dutch legislation.

Only a single inclusion criterion was applied, i.e. the ability to read and speak Dutch.

Because of logistic factors, we were unable to determine the number of women that had been initially invited and out of these how many participated. Therefore, no initial response rate could be calculated. In total 4157 women gave informed consent. They filled in some of the baseline assessments [21]. Follow-up measures on mother-to-infant bonding used for the present study were collected in 2015 and 2016. Hence, the eligible population for the present analysis consisted of all women with a child aged between six and 24 months (n = 1,275). Women with multiple gestation (n = 12) were excluded from the current analyses. To enhance the quality of this study, and to enhance the transparency of our approach the TRIPOD statement for reporting the development of a multivariable prediction model for Individual Prediction or Diagnosis was followed [22].

Measurements

Outcome variable.

To assess postpartum mother-to-infant bonding the Dutch version of Mother-to-Infant Bonding Scale (MIBS) was used [23]. This self-report questionnaire consists of 8 items (loving, resentful, neutral or felt nothing, joyful, dislike, protective, disappointed and aggressive) using a 4-point Likert scale ranging from “Very much” to “Not at all”. Scores on the MIBS range between 0 and 24, with high scores indicating a poor mother-to-infant bond. The MIBS is used between six and 24 months postpartum [10, 23]. The sample of women varied in terms of the postpartum month assessment time point. Therefore, we investigated with a logistic regression analyses if scores on the MIBS were stable over time. Some studies used a score of 2 or more on the MIBS to indicate poor bonding [10, 11, 24]. However, none of these studies addressed whether the cut-off of 2 was the most optimal cut-off. Neither did these studies address the effect of changing the cut-off value on test characteristics such as sensitivity and specificity or predictive values. The MIBS was the best available measure since Matsunaga et al. investigated in the most optimal cut-off score of the MIBS. A score of 4 or more on the MIBS was used to define suboptimal bonding. Matsunaga et al. investigated the most optimal cut-off score of the MIBS among 723 mothers. The optimal cut off scores were 4/5 after 1 month postpartum [25]. This was based on an optimal tradeoff between sensitivity and specificity. When it comes to defining the outcome variable for a screening model for mother-to-infant bonding based on a rating scale such as the MIBS, a sufficiently high specificity of the MIBS itself is in our view more important than a high sensitivity. This is because a high specificity is associated with relatively few false positives. Too many false positive test may lead to an overestimation of suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding, and thereby evoking unnecessary worry among mothers. Some items of the MIBS address maternal feelings towards the baby which may directly lead to child abuse, a woman who indicated to have such feelings was also captured by a score of 4 or more on the MIBS in this study. The internal consistency of the Dutch MIBS in our study was comparable to previous studies which showed acceptable reliability and validity [21].

Candidate determinants.

We applied four criteria for candidate determinants from four systematic reviews [4, 1820]: plausibility (prior probability of being informative), reliability of assessment, and the determinant distribution (i.e. prevalence sufficiently high). Furthermore, we aimed at a maximum number of determinants of 10% of the number of women with suboptimal bonding [22].

Based on these criteria we selected prenatal depressive symptoms, prenatal anxiety, social support, adult attachment anxiety, adult attachment avoidance, domestic violence in history, marital status, feelings about pregnancy, maternal age and parity [4, 1820]. Except for adult attachment style all candidate determinants were measured before 13 weeks of pregnancy.

Prenatal depressive symptomatology was measured and analyzed continuously with the Dutch version of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) [26]. Scores on this 10-item, 4-point Likert scale range from 0 to 30. Higher scores indicate more prenatal depressive symptomatology. A cut off score of ≥11 indicates a risk of minor or major depression during the first trimester of pregnancy. The EPDS has been validated for use among pregnant women [26]. The Dutch version of the EPDS has shown good internal validity with a Cronbach's alpha of 0.83 [26].

Prenatal anxiety was measured with the Dutch version of the 6-item State and Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) on a 4-point Likert scale and was analyzed as a continuous variable. Scores range from 20 to 80 and a score of 42 or higher indicates clinically significant anxiety [27]. The 6-item STAI has similar reliability as the original 20-item STAI and has been validated to use among pregnant women [27]. The Cronbach's alpha coefficient for the 6-item STAI was 0.82 [27].

Social support was measured and analyzed continuously with the satisfaction score of the abbreviated version of the Social Support Questionnaire (SSQ). Scores on this 6-item, 6-point Likert scale range from 6 to 36. Higher scores indicate more satisfaction with social support [28]. The satisfaction score measures the individual's degree of satisfaction with the perceived support available, and has shown good reliability and validity [28]. The Cronbach's alpha coefficient for the SSQ Satisfaction subscale was 0.94 [28].

Adult attachment was measured with the Experiences in Close Relationships questionnaire (ECR) [29]. Adult attachment style was measured during the same postpartum period as the MIBS. The ECR is a 36-item self-report measure of adult attachment containing two 18-item subscales assessing dimensions of adult attachment, Anxiety and Avoidance. These two subscales were analyzed continuously. The Anxiety subscale taps fears of abandonment and rejection. The Avoidance subscale assesses discomfort with dependence and intimate self-disclosure. Each item is rated on a 7-point scale where 1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree [29]. Higher scores indicate higher attachment related anxiety or avoidance. Adult attachment style was measured during the same postpartum period as the MIBS under the assumption that subscales anxiety and avoidance remain stable over time as well as over a two month period as across a life span [30]. We have also tested this assumption in data from the PROMISES trial [31]. The results of the test-retest reliability of the two subscales of the ECR at 13 weeks of pregnancy and at twelve months postpartum indicated consistent scores over time. The intraclass correlation coefficient for the subscale anxiety was r = 0.75 (95% CI 0.61–0.84) and for the subscale avoidance r = 0.85 (95% CI 0.78–0.90) (S1 and S2 Tables). Cronbach’s alpha for adult attachment anxiety is 0.91 and for avoidance 0.94 [29].

A history of domestic violence was measured and analyzed dichotomously and defined as being mentally or physically abused by your partner or having a conflict with your partner.

Marital status and positive feelings about pregnancy were likewise measured and analyzed dichotomously as married/not married as well as positive feeling and no positive feelings about pregnancy at approximately 13 weeks of pregnancy.

Data on maternal age and parity were extracted from medical records. The persons who extracted these data were blinded for the mother-to-infant bonding scores.

Maternal characteristics.

Maternal characteristics were measured at approximately 13 weeks of gestation and included: educational attainment level (elementary and lower tract of secondary education, higher tract of secondary education, higher vocational and university education) and ethnic background. Prematurity (yes/no) and intended breastfeeding (yes/no)were extracted from medical records. The age of the infant at the time mother-to-infant bonding was measured simultaneously with the MIBS.

Data analysis

We calculated descriptive statistics for the outcome variable, the maternal characteristics and the candidate determinants. We compared characteristics of the included women and the non-responders on the MIBS and calculated the fraction of missing data for each candidate determinant of the responders of the MIBS. We studied the missing data mechanism by predicting missingness (yes/no) of data for each of these variables from the other variables using multivariable logistic regression analyses [32]. The final imputation model included all variables used in the analyses and all variables that predicted missingness of a certain variable, or its value. Under the assumption that data was missing at random (MAR) or completely at random [32], twenty datasets were imputed by chained equations and pooled according to Rubin’s rules [32]. Because MIBS scores were available for only 50% of the participants, we refrained from their imputation. We repeated our analyses using a complete case approach as a sensitivity analysis.

We present univariable odds ratios for suboptimal postnatal mother-to-infant bonding per determinant. Subsequent logistical regression analyses included all determinant variables irrespective of their univariable association as it has been shown that univariable preselection can lead to unstable models [33]. The following steps of model development were followed in each of the imputed data sets: (1) logistic regression with backward elimination of determinants with the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) stopping rule [21]. This rule corresponds to a P-value of 0.157 for a determinant with one degree of freedom. The final model was defined as the set of determinants which were selected in at least 50% of all imputed datasets. (2) estimation of regression coefficients in the final model using logistic regression and (3) estimation of a shrinkage factor in each imputed data set. The averaged shrinkage factor over the imputed data sets was applied to the coefficients of the final model as a way of internal validation, i.e. to correct for overfitting. Shrinkage factors were estimated by bootstrapping using 250 bootstrap samples.

The following pooled performance measures of the final model were calculated: explained variance (Nagelkerke’s R2), the Hosmer and Lemeshow test, and the Area Under the Curve (AUC).

We checked if the results of our final analysis changed by adding the age of the child at assessment in the prediction model. We used Swets's criteria to assess the AUC [34]. The AUC of 0.5 to 0.6 is defined as bad performance, 0.6 to 0.7 as poor performance, 0.7 to 0.8 as satisfactory performance, 0.8 to 0.9 as good performance, and 0.9 to 1.0 as excellent performance [34].

Based on the regression coefficients of the final pooled screening model we created a risk classification model, which can be used to classify women at low, intermediate or high-risk for suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding. Low risk is defined as a estimated probability for suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding below 25%, intermediated risk between 25 and 75% and high risk as above 75%. Except for multivariable determinant selection, the level of significance was set at 0.05, two sided.

Statistical analyses were performed with SPSS Statistics 25.0 (SPSS inc. Chicago, Illinois) and with ‘RStudio’ statistical software (Version 1.1.463, R Development Core Team).

Results

The eligible population for the present analysis consisted of all women after singleton pregnancy with a child aged less than 24 months (n = 1,263). Of these, 634 responded by filling out the questionnaires, yielding a response rate of 50.2%. Fig 1 illustrates the flowchart.

Non-responders did not substantially nor statistically significantly differ from responders on depressive symptoms, anxiety, social support, maternal age, marital status, ethnicity, breastfeeding and Apgar score after 5 minutes. However, non-responders were significantly more often multiparous women (66% versus 54%, p<0.05) (Table 1). Reasons for not responding were mainly because respondents had not actively communicated changes to their home address or email address.

thumbnail
Table 1. Characteristics of the responders and the non-responders on the MIBS and the fraction of missing data of the responders.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0241574.t001

Of the 634 included women, 339 women (53%) had a score on the Mother-to-Infant Bonding Scale less than 2, 150 women (24%) had a score of 2, 77 women (12%) had a score of 3, 36 women (6%) had a score of 4 and 32 women (5%) had a score of ≥5. The proportion of missing data of candidate determinants varied between 3% and 33%. Maternal age varied from 22 to 45 years. The results of a logistic regression analysis showed that the MIBS-scores were not significant associated with the age of the child (days postpartum) (p-value of 0.897).

Table 2 presents the univariable associations of all candidate determinants with suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding. Out of ten, six candidate determinants were significantly associated with a higher risk of suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding: adult attachment avoidance, adult attachment anxiety, prenatal depressive symptoms (measured continuously), prenatal anxiety symptoms (measured continuously), no positive feelings about pregnancy and parity. Women with a suboptimal MIBS score were more often multiparous than nulliparous women, respectively 44 (65%) versus 287 (51%). Women with a suboptimal MIBS score were not significantly different in prenatal anxiety and depression measured dichotomously (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Univariable associations in the imputed datasets between candidate determinants and suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding (MIBS ≥4).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0241574.t002

In the first step of the model development, parity and adult attachment avoidance were included in all datasets. The variable prenatal depressive symptoms was included in six of the 20 datasets. Adult attachment anxiety, social support and no positive feelings about pregnancy were included in three of the 20 datasets. Marital status and prenatal anxiety symptoms were included in two datasets.

Therefore, we included parity and adult attachment avoidance in the final model. We estimated the pooled multivariable regression coefficients in all datasets. The pooled shrinkage factor was 0.99. Table 3 describes the final model. The final formula for calculating the estimated probability of suboptimal mother-to-infant is: P(MIBS≥4) = 1/(1+exp(–(-4.3914 + (parity × 0.518698) + (Adult attachment avoidance × 0.040430863))).

thumbnail
Table 3. The full model to allow estimations for individuals (including all regression coefficients and model intercept).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0241574.t003

We checked if the results of our final analysis changed by adding the age of the child at assessment in the model. However, the age of the child did not contribute to the model.

The pooled explained variance, determined with Nagelkerke’s R2 was 14%. The Hosmer and Lemeshow test had a p-value of 0.21 which indicates a good fit. Finally, pooled AUC was 0.750 with a 95% CI of 0.690–0.809, which is classified as satisfactory performance [30].

Fig 2 presents the pooled receiver-operating characteristic curve of the final model. Table 4 presents a practical risk classification model, based on the regression coefficients of the final model, which can be used to classify women as low, intermediate or high-risk for suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding based on information on parity and scores on the Experiences in Close Relationships questionnaire which measures (among others) adult attachment avoidance. The complete case analysis showed similar results.

thumbnail
Fig 2. The pooled receiver-operating characteristic of the full model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0241574.g002

thumbnail
Table 4. Risk classification model for suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding, based on the regression coefficients of the final model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0241574.t004

Discussion

In this study we developed a multivariable model to screen early in pregnancy for mother-to-infant bonding postpartum. Parity and adult attachment avoidance were the strongest independent determinants of suboptimal postnatal mother-to-infant bonding and formed the final model.

Higher parity and higher levels of adult attachment avoidance were associated with an increased risk of suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding. The model showed satisfactory performance. Based on the results, we designed an easy to use risk classification model for detection of suboptimal bonding. This risk classification model enables health care professionals to easily classify women at risk for suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding.

Parity and adult attachment avoidance were the strongest independent determinants of suboptimal postnatal mother-to-infant bonding. The informative value of parity can be explained as follows. When a woman becomes a mother for the first time, she may have more time and attention for her pregnancy and child which may positively affect mother-to-infant bonding [31]. Some mothers having their second, third or fourth child feel concerned about bonding. They worry about whether they will have just as much love for their new arrival as they do for their older child [35]. The findings of this study were mentioned before in other studies. Parity was mentioned as a determinant in a systematic review, although mostly in combination with prenatal mother-to-infant bonding. In 7 of 17 included studies of this review it was reported, that women expecting a second or third child had lower mother-to-infant bonding. Reported correlations were mostly weak [4].

The finding that higher adult attachment avoidance is associated with an increased risk of suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding is in line with the findings of a recent cross sectional study [36]. Anxious adult attachment style was not included in our final model. Apparently, avoidant adult attachment style was a stronger determinant than anxious adult attachment style. An explanation for why adult attachment avoidance is a determinant of suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding could be that adult attachment avoidance or insecurity is considered a mediator, or partial mediator of the association between experienced childhood maltreatment and depression, anxiety or behavioural problems in the next generation [3739]. Therefore, it seems relevant to implement the identification of maternal attachment style of pregnant women into prevention or intervention strategies designed to reduce children's risk for behavioural problems. Such strategies should be combined with attachment-based interventions that are widely available and have been demonstrated to modify parenting behaviours [40].

In this study depressive symptoms were not included in the final model. This is in contrast with other literature which showed that postnatal depressive symptoms were consistently correlated with postnatal mother-to-infant bonding [4]. An explanation may be that adult attachment is related to postnatal depressive symptoms [41], and possibly overruled depressive symptoms in our analysis. During pregnancy adult attachment can act as a vulnerability factor for the development of depressive symptoms in the subsequent postnatal period [41]. Apparently, adult attachment appeared in our analysis to be the stronger independent determinant. It should be borne in mind here that the determinants in our risk model are not necessarily causal factors for suboptimal bonding. This is because the data analysis aimed to identify variables that jointly had high informative performance rather than to unravel the factors involved in the aetiology of bonding problems [42].

The explained variance of the screening model of suboptimal bonding in the general population of the model was relatively low (14%). However, it must be emphasized that the explained variance only concerns one aspect of performance, i.e. the overall quality of a screening model performance in a given data set [43, 44], and therefore interpretation in absolute terms must be cautious. The other two aspects of model performance are central in evaluating the performance and they are calibration and discrimination [45]. In the current study, the Hosmer and Lemeshow test had a p-value of 0.21 which indicates a good model calibration and fit. Finally, the pooled AUC was 0.750 with a 95% CI of 0.690–0.809, which is classified as satisfactory discrimination performance [34]. Mother-to-infant bonding is a complex construct determined by multiple determinants. We cannot completely rule out, that additional determinants may be involved which were not assessed in our study. Nevertheless, this study adds important knowledge to the complex construct of mother-to-infant bonding. Only a small number of candidate determinants surfaced as independent determinants. A possible explanation can be the limited statistical power associated with the relatively low number of women with suboptimal bonding in our sample. That is, 68 mothers had suboptimal bonding with an MIBS score ≥4.

Contrary to our results, Farré-Sender et al. found that emotional abuse in childhood, family psychiatric history, previous psychiatric hospitalization, and anxiety during pregnancy were the main determinants of mother-to-infant bonding. However, this was investigated in a clinical sample of 251 women enrolled at a public perinatal psychiatric service. The results showed a model explaining 10.7% of the variance. Aspects like calibration and discrimination were not reported in this study [14].

Strengths and limitations

To our knowledge, this is the first study to develop a multivariable model to screen early in pregnancy for suboptimal postnatal mother-to-infant bonding in the general population. Strengths of this study were the inclusion of a broad range of determinants all selected for analyses based on literature and the design of the study. All the three aspects of model performance, including calibration and discrimination were investigated [45]. The model showed satisfactory performance. Another strength of this study is the heterogeneity of the population caused by unselective population-based inclusion and a mixed recruitment setting of both primary and nine secondary care centers and the which likely contributes to greater generalizability [46]. The sample from primary midwifery care and secondary obstetrics care was likely to be unselected with respect to mother-to-infant bonding and the determinants.

Some limitations should be addressed regarding the interpretation of our results. First, the outcome variable was available in only 50% of the eligible population. Therefore, we decided not to impute mother-to-infant bonding scores. This rate of attrition could suggest selection bias and could decrease the generalizability of the results. Furthermore, this non-response may have resulted in bias within the determinants. Non-responders on the MIBS were more often multiparous women which may have caused an underestimation of the prevalence of suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding and of the strengths of the observed associations. However, this is unlikely to have caused spurious associations. Secondly, adult attachment was measured cross-sectionally with the outcome. Before this study, we tested the assumption that adult attachment remains stable from pregnancy to motherhood in the PROMISES trial [31]. The intraclass correlation coefficient of the subscales anxiety and avoidance are indicating respectively moderate and good test re-test reliability [47], indicating consistency over time. Our assumption that adult attachment remains stable over time has also been supported by previous literature [30]. On the other hand, the pregnancy and birth of a child can be transforming for women in many ways. Pregnancy offers a window of opportunity for future health of both the mother and the child. On average women engage more frequently in positive health behaviours during pregnancy [9]. These behaviours are associated with good birth outcomes and healthier mothers [9]. However, based on the existing literature it is still debatable whether in mothers with insecure attachment the birth of the child could transform in terms of adult attachment representations. Several studies in the adult attachment literature have demonstrated continuity of attachment organization in general [4850]. Furthermore, as far as we know, adult attachment measured by the Experiences in Close Relationships questionnaire has only once been investigated in a prospective cohort study following pregnant women [30]. The results of our study and the study of Kooiman both showed that subscales anxiety and avoidance remain stable over time. Finally, Stern et al. examined change in first-time mothers’ adult attachment style across the first 2 years of motherhood [51]. 162 economically stressed primiparous mothers completed measures of adult attachment anxiety and avoidance at five time points: when their children were 0, 6, 12, 18, and 24 months of age. Converging results of stability functions and latent growth curve models suggest that attachment styles were generally stable during the first 2 years of motherhood, even in this economically stressed sample [51]. Our results add to the growing insight in adult attachment development over time and suggest a long-term stability.

Although adult attachment and mother-to-infant bonding can be seen as conceptually-close measures, they measure different constructs [1]. Mother-to-infant bonding measured the feelings and emotions a mother experiences towards her child. On the other hand the adult attachment questionnaire we used, measured adult attachment towards the partner. The anxiety subscale taps fears of abandonment and rejection, especially towards the partner. The avoidance subscale assesses discomfort with dependence and intimate self-disclosure towards the partner. Recently, other authors describe the constructs as different constructs as well [4, 52]. We cannot completely rule out that there is the slight possibility that the way the mother responded to one questionnaire affected the way she responded to the second one.

Finally, this study investigated the development and internal validation of a screening model. Yet, externally validating the final model in another population remains essential. A third limitation is that the PAD-Study is a screening study, in which there was no immediate request for help from the respondents at inclusion.

Clinical and scientific implications

Prenatal care visits by midwives and gynecologists take place from early in pregnancy onwards. However, so far mother-to-infant bonding is neglected during such visits, as only in two percent of such visits mother-to-infant bonding was addressed by primary care midwives in the Netherlands [12]. Our risk classification model enables health care professionals to identify in pregnancy women at risk for suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding. For this, they can take into account the pregnant women’s parity and adult attachment style. The model showed satisfactory performance. Therefore, we recommend to invest in an external validation and updating of the model. We also recommend to identify more possible determinants of suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding using large-scale longitudinal research. The screening model may serve as a starting point for further research and as a starting point for developing supporting material in clinical practice. Our model may also have added value to clinical practice. Whether the informative accuracy of this model in all women is adequate is also a matter of judgment and depends on available alternatives for risk stratification [46]. Currently, our model is the only screening model available for mother-to-infant bonding in the general population and may be used with the understanding that it should serve as a clinical guide rather than a prescriptive tool. Nevertheless, the impact of this model for patient relevant outcomes is not yet known. In the future, and after external validation, an impact study should, idealiter quantify the effect of using this screening model on the behaviour of health care professionals, outcomes, and costs effectiveness of care using the model compared with usual care [47]. The risk classification model can be useful to plan monitoring strategies, interventions and prevention, in order to support the development of both motherhood and parenting skills. Health care professionals can thereby help to individualize patient care plans. We advise investigating the capacity of health care professionals to systematically screen for adult attachment style in pregnancy.

Adult attachment style in this study was tapped by the 36-item questionnaire [29]. The disadvantage of this questionnaire is the length of this questionnaire. For practical use, we recommend to investigate in a shorter questionnaire measuring both adult attachment avoidance and adult attachment anxiety.

Conclusion

Higher parity and higher levels of adult attachment avoidance are independently associated with an increased risk of suboptimal mother-to-infant bonding. The developed screening model showed satisfactory performance measures. Based on the results, we designed a practical risk classification model for early detection of suboptimal bonding. The screening model should be externally validated and optimized before use in daily practice. The model may serve as a starting point for further research. Future research should include an external validation study, a study into the additional value of non-included determinants and finally a study on the impact and feasibility (such as the capacity of health care professionals to screen for adult attachment in pregnancy) of the screening model.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Results of paired sampled t-tested of the two subscales of the ECR in the PROMISES trial.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0241574.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Results of the test-retest reliability of the two subscales of the ECR in the PROMISES trial.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0241574.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. TRIPOD Checklist: Prediction model development and validation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0241574.s003

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank all participating primary and secondary obstetric care centers for the screening of participants, and all women for their participation. We also thank the International Confederation of Midwives for awarding our research proposal [53].

References

  1. 1. Bicking Kinsey C, Hupcey JE. State of the science of maternal–infant bonding: A principle-based concept analysis. Midwifery. 2013;29: 1314–20. pmid:23452661
  2. 2. Benoit D. Infant-parent attachment: Definition, types, antecedents, measurement and outcome. Paediatr Child Health. 2004;9: 541–45. pmid:19680481
  3. 3. de Cock ESA, Henrichs J, Vreeswijk CMJM, Maas AJ, Rijk CHAM, van Bakel HJA. Continuous feelings of love? The parental bond from pregnancy to toddlerhood. J Fam Psychol. 2016; 30:125–134. pmid:26280095
  4. 4. Tichelman E, Westerneng M, Witteveen AB, van Baar AL, van der Horst HE, de Jonge A, et al. Correlates of prenatal and postnatal mother-to-infant bonding quality: A systematic review. PLOS ONE. 2019;14: e0222998. pmid:31550274
  5. 5. Mason ZS, Briggs RD, Silver EJ. Maternal attachment feelings mediate between maternal reports of depression, infant social–emotional development, and parenting stress. J Reprod Infant Psychol. 2011;29: 382–94.
  6. 6. de Cock ESA, Henrichs J, Klimstra TA, Maas JBM, Vreeswijk CMJM, Meeus WHJ, et al. Longitudinal Associations Between Parental Bonding, Parenting Stress, and Executive Functioning in Toddlerhood. J Child Fam Stud. 2017;26:1723–33. pmid:28572718
  7. 7. Le Bas GA, Youssef GJ, Macdonald JA, Rossen L, Teague SJ, Kothe EJ, McIntosh JE, Olsson CA, Hutchinson DM. The role of antenatal and postnatal maternal bonding in infant development: A systematic review and meta–analysis. Social Development. 2020;29:3–20.
  8. 8. Schmid B, Blomeyer D, Buchmann AF, Trautmann-Villalba P, Zimmermann US, Schmidt MH, et al. Quality of early mother–child interaction associated with depressive psychopathology in the offspring: A prospective study from infancy to adulthood. J Psychiatr Res. 2011;45: 1387–94. pmid:21683371
  9. 9. Lindgren K. Relationships among maternal–fetal attachment, prenatal depression, and health practices in pregnancy. Res Nurs Health. 2001;24: 203–17. pmid:11526619
  10. 10. O’Higgins M, Roberts ISJ, Glover V, Taylor A. Mother-child bonding at 1 year; associations with symptoms of postnatal depression and bonding in the first few weeks. Arch Women’s Ment Health. 2013; 16:381–9.
  11. 11. Zanardo V, Soldera G, Volpe F, Giliberti L, Parotto M, Giustardi A, et al. Influence of elective and emergency cesarean delivery on mother emotions and bonding. Early Hum Dev. 2016;99: 17–20. pmid:27380109
  12. 12. Tichelman E, Peters LL, Oost J, Westerhout MA, Schellevis FG, Burger H, et al. Addressing transition to motherhood, guideline adherence by midwives in prenatal booking visits: Findings from video recordings. Midwifery. 2019;69: 76–83. pmid:30415104
  13. 13. van den Bergh B, Simons A. A review of scales to measure the mother-foetus relationship. J Reprod Infant Psychol. 2009;27: 114–26
  14. 14. Farré-Sender B, Torres A, Gelabert E, Andrés S, Roca A, Lashera G, Valdés M, Garcia-Esteve L. Mother-infant bonding in the postpartum period: assessment of the impact of pre-delivery factors in a clinical sample. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2017;21(3):287–297. pmid:29046965
  15. 15. Cunen NB, Jomeen J, Xuereb RB, Poat A. A narrative review of interventions addressing the parental-fetal relationship. Women and Birth. 2017;30: 141–51. pmid:27884654
  16. 16. Malm MC, Hildingsson I, Rubertsson C, Rådestad I, Lindgren H. Prenatal attachment and its association with foetal movement during pregnancy–A population based survey. Women and Birth. 2016;29: 482–86. pmid:27140328
  17. 17. Slagt-Tichelman E, Klijnstra S, Oostenbrink M, de Jonge A, Westerneng M, Peters LL. Optimale moeder-kind binding: een systematische review (Dutch for optimal mother-to-infant bonding: a systematic review of prenatal interventions). Tijdschr voor Verlos. 2019;3: 16–25.
  18. 18. Alhusen JL. A literature update on maternal-fetal attachment. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs. 2008;37: 315–28. pmid:18507602
  19. 19. Cannella BL. Maternal–fetal attachment: an integrative review. J Adv Nurs. 2005;50: 60–68. pmid:15788066
  20. 20. Yarcheski A, Mahon NE, Yarcheski TJ, Hanks MM, Cannella BL. A meta-analytic study of predictors of maternal-fetal attachment. Int J Nurs Stud. 2009;46: 708–15. pmid:19081091
  21. 21. Meijer JL, Bockting CL, Stolk RP, Kotov R, Ormel J, Burger H. Associations of life events during pregnancy with longitudinal change in symptoms of antenatal anxiety and depression. Midwifery. 2013;30: 526–31. pmid:23870748
  22. 22. Moons KG, Altman DG, Reitsma JB, loannidis JP, Macaskill P, Steyerberg EW, et al. Transparent Reporting of a multivariable prediction model for Individual Prognosis Or Diagnosis (TRIPOD): Explanation and Elaboration. Ann Intern Med. 2015;162: 1–73. pmid:25560711
  23. 23. van Bussel JCH, Spitz B, Demyttenaere K. Three self-report questionnaires of the early mother-to-infant bond: reliability and validity of the Dutch version of the MPAS, PBQ and MIBS. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2010;13: 373–84. pmid:20127128
  24. 24. Bienfait M, Maury M, Haquet A, Faillie JC, Franc N, Combes C, Daudé H, Picaud JC, Rideau A, Cambonie G. Pertinence of the self-report mother-to-infant bonding scale in the neonatal unit of a maternity ward. Early Hum Dev. 2011; 87:281–287. pmid:21316876
  25. 25. Matsunaga A, Takauma F, Tada K, Kitamura T. Discrete category of mother-to-infant bonding disorder and its identification by the Mother-to-Infant Bonding Scale: A study in Japanese mothers of a 1-month-old. Early Human Development. 2017; 111: 1–5. pmid:28525876
  26. 26. Bergink V, Kooistra L, Lambregtse-van den Berg MP, Wijnen H, Bunevicius R, van Baar A, et al. Validation of the Edinburgh Depression Scale during pregnancy. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2011;70: 385–89. pmid:21414460
  27. 27. Marteau TM, Bekker H. The development of a six-item short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1992;31: 301–6. pmid:1393159
  28. 28. Furukawa TA, Harai H, Hirai T, Kitamura T, Takahashi K. Social support questionnaire among psychiatric patients with various diagnoses and normal controls. Social Psychiatry Epidemiology. 1999;34: 216–22. pmid:10365628
  29. 29. Brennan KA, Clark CL, Shaver PR. Self-report measurement of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J.A. Simpson & W. 1998: 46–76.
  30. 30. Kooiman CG, Klaassens ER, van Heloma Lugt JQ, Kamperman AM. Psychometrics and validity of the Dutch Experiences in Close Relationships–Revised (ECR-R) in an outpatient mental health sample. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2013;95: 217–24. pmid:23181777
  31. 31. Meijer JL, Bockting CL, Beijers C, Verbeek T, Stant AD, Ormel J, et al. PRegnancy Outcomes after a Maternity Intervention for Stressful EmotionS (PROMISES): study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. Trials. 2011;12: 157. pmid:21689394
  32. 32. White IR, Royston P, Wood AM. Multiple imputation using chained equations: issues and guidance for practice. Stat Med. 2011;30: 377–99. pmid:21225900
  33. 33. Harrell FE, Lee KL, Mark DB. Multivariable prognostic models: issues in developing models, evaluating assumptions and adequacy, and measuring and reducing errors. Stat Med. 1996;15: 361–87. pmid:8668867
  34. 34. Swets JA. Measuring the accuracy of diagnostic systems. Science. 1998;240: 1285–93.
  35. 35. Røseth I, Bongaardt R, Lyberg A, Sommerseth E, Dahl B. New mothers’ struggles to love their child. An interpretative synthesis of qualitative studies, International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-being. 2018;13: 1490621. pmid:29975176
  36. 36. Gőbel A, Barkmann C, Arck P, Hecher K, Schulte-Markwort M, Diemert A, et al. Couples’ prenatal bonding to the fetus and the association with one’s own and partner’s emotional well-being and adult romantic attachment style. Midwifery. 2019;79: 102549. pmid:31627088
  37. 37. Bifulco A, Kwon J, Jacobs C, Moran PM, Bunn A, Beer N. Adult attachment style as mediator between childhood neglect/abuse and adult depression and anxiety. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2006;41: 796–805. pmid:16871369
  38. 38. Cowan PA, Cowan CP, Pruett MK, Pruett K. Fathers’ and mothers’ attachment styles, couple conflict, parenting quality, and children’s behavior problems: an intervention test of mediation. Attachment & Human Development. 2019;11: 29–46.
  39. 39. Widom CS, Czaja SJ, Kozakowski SS, Chauhan P. Does adult attachment style mediate the relationship between childhood maltreatment and mental and physical health outcomes? Child Abuse & Neglect. 2018;76: 533–45. pmid:28522128
  40. 40. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Less is more: Meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129: 195–215. pmid:12696839
  41. 41. Aceti F, Aveni F, Baglioni V, Carluccio GM, Colosimo D, Giacchetti N, et al. Perinatal and postpartum depression: From attachment to personality. A pilot study. Journal of Psychopathology. 2012;18: 328–34.
  42. 42. van Diepen M, Ramspek CL, Jager KJ, Zoccali C, Dekker FW.Prediction versus aetiology: common pitfalls and how to avoid them. Nephrol Dial Transplant. 2017;32: ii1–5. pmid:28339854
  43. 43. Steyerberg EW. Clinical Prediction Models: A Practical Approach to Development, Validation, and Updating. New York: Springer; 2009.
  44. 44. Steyerberg EW, Vickers AJ, Cook NR. "Assessing the Performance of Prediction Models A Framework for Traditional and Novel Measures". Epidemiology. 2010; 21 (1): 128–138. pmid:20010215
  45. 45. Alba AC, Agoritsas T, Walsh M, Hanna S, Iorio A, Devereaux PJ, McGinn T, Guyatt G. Discrimination and Calibration of Clinical Prediction Models: Users' Guides to the Medical Literature. JAMA. 2017; 318(14):1377–1384. pmid:29049590
  46. 46. Moons KGM, Altman DG, Vergouwe Y, Royston P. Prognosis and prognostic research: application and impact of prognostic models in clinical practice. BMJ. 2009;338: b606. pmid:19502216
  47. 47. Portney LG, Watkins MP. Foundations of clinical research: applications to practice. Prentice Hall; New Jersey. 2000.
  48. 48. McConnell M, Moss E. Attachment across the life span: Factors that contribute to stability and change. Australian Journal of Educational & Developmental Psychology. 2011;11,60–77
  49. 49. Ng K-M, Smith SD. The Relationships Between Attachment Theory and Intergenerational Family Systems Theory. The Family Journal. 2006;14(4):430–440.
  50. 50. Obegi JH, Morrison TL, Shaver PR. Exploring intergenerational transmission of attachment style in young female adults and their mothers. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2004;21(5):625–638.
  51. 51. Stern JA, Fraley RC, Jones JD, Gross JT, Shaver PR, Cassidy J. Developmental processes across the first two years of parenthood: Stability and change in adult attachment style. Developmental Psychology, 2018; 54(5), 975–988. pmid:29355359
  52. 52. Nordahl D, Rognmo K, Bohne A, Landsem IP, Moe V, Wang CEA, Høifødt RS. Adult attachment style and maternal-infant bonding: the indirect path of parenting stress. BMC Psychol. 2020; 8(1):58. pmid:32513300
  53. 53. Duff E. International News. Midwifery. 2017;45: A1–A5.