Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Behavioral and corticosterone responses to carbon dioxide exposure in reptiles

  • Daniel J. D. Natusch ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Daniel J. D. Natusch, Patrick W. Aust

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    d.natusch@epicbiodiversity.com

    Affiliations Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, North Ryde, NSW, Australia, EPIC Biodiversity, Frogs Hollow, NSW, Australia

  • Patrick W. Aust ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Daniel J. D. Natusch, Patrick W. Aust

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom, Bushtick Environmental Services, Grantham, Lincolnshire, United Kingdom

  • Syarifah Khadiejah,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Wildlife and National Parks, Peninsular Malaysia, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia

  • Hartini Ithnin,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Wildlife and National Parks, Peninsular Malaysia, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia

  • Ain Isa,

    Roles Investigation, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Wildlife and National Parks, Peninsular Malaysia, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia

  • Che Ku Zamzuri,

    Roles Investigation, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Wildlife and National Parks, Peninsular Malaysia, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia

  • Andre Ganswindt,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Endocrine Research Laboratory, Mammal Research Institute, Department of Zoology and Entomology, Faculty of Natural and Agricultural Sciences, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa, Centre of Veterinary Wildlife Studies, Faculty of Veterinary Science, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, Onderstepoort, South Africa

  • Dale F. DeNardo

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, United States of America

Abstract

The use of carbon dioxide (CO2) exposure as a means of animal euthanasia has received considerable attention in mammals and birds but remains virtually untested in reptiles. We measured the behavioral responses of four squamate reptile species (Homalopsis buccata, Malayopython reticulatus, Python bivitattus, and Varanus salvator) to exposure to 99.5% CO2 for durations of 15, 30, or 90 minutes. We also examined alterations in plasma corticosterone levels of M. reticulatus and V. salvator before and after 15 minutes of CO2 exposure relative to control individuals. The four reptile taxa showed consistent behavioral responses to CO2 exposure characterized by gaping and minor movements. The time taken to lose responsiveness to stimuli and cessation of movements varied between 240–4260 seconds (4–71 minutes), with considerable intra- and inter-specific variation. Duration of CO2 exposure influenced the likelihood of recovery, which also varied among species (e.g., from 0–100% recovery after 30-min exposure). Plasma corticosterone concentrations increased after CO2 exposure in both V. salvator (18%) and M. reticulatus (14%), but only significantly in the former species. Based on our results, CO2 appears to be a mild stressor for reptiles, but the relatively minor responses to CO2 suggest it may not cause considerable distress or pain. However, our results are preliminary, and further testing is required to understand optimal CO2 delivery mechanisms and interspecific responses to CO2 exposure before endorsing this method for reptile euthanasia.

Introduction

Ensuring the humane euthanasia of animals used by humans is critically important to fulfil our ethical obligation for compassion towards other species. In addition, a painless and distress-free death can, in some contexts, result in a higher quality meat product for human consumption [1]. In pursuit of these goals, methodologies, guidelines, and regulations for humane euthanasia have been developed and implemented for animal use ranging from meat production to scientific research [2].

However, a severe taxonomic bias currently exists. Although humane treatment protocols are well established for mammals and birds, the welfare needs of reptiles and the methodologies considered humane and acceptable for euthanasia, especially in instances where human consumption of part of the carcass occurs, remain in their infancy [2]. For example, debate continues about the appropriateness of hypothermia (freezing) as an euthanasia method [35], and humane killing methods for reptiles used in the meat and skin industries were only adopted by the World Organization for Animal Health (OIE) in 2019 [see 6, 7].

Chemical agents offer an effective and humane way to euthanize reptiles, but their usefulness is sometimes limited. Access and use restrictions, and situations where large numbers of animals are slaughtered for human consumption in short periods, often prohibit their use. With the possible exception of hypothermia, all recommended non-chemical methods of reptile euthanasia involve destruction of the brain (e.g., captive bolt, pithing). However, the effectiveness of brain destruction is vulnerable to operator error and may be impractical in situations where large numbers of animals need to be killed at one time.

Carbon dioxide (CO2) is widely used as a euthanizing agent in the livestock industry and for scientific research [2, 810]. The guidelines of the American Veterinary Medical Association cite 86 studies on the effectiveness and suitability of CO2 as a humane means of euthanasia for mammals and birds [2]. Mammalian and avian responses to CO2 exposure vary considerably by species, and are dependent on CO2 concentration and delivery method [2, 810]. Mice, rats, cats, dogs, pigs, rabbits, chickens, and turkeys lose consciousness after 20–120 seconds of CO2 exposure, but may require exposures of 5–50 minutes to ensure death [2, 9, 10]. Exposure to CO2 has been shown to increase plasma corticosterone levels in rats and dogs and results in mouth gaping in mice, rats, and chickens [2, 9]. Rats and mink will actively avoid CO2 exposure if given the opportunity, but goats and chickens will not (despite the latter gaping when exposed; [2, 8]).

The use of CO2 to euthanize reptiles has generally been discouraged by veterinary guidance, animals ethics committees, and by the OIE based on physiological considerations [2, 6, 11, 12]. The rationale implies that because reptiles have a variable metabolic rate and can potentially tolerate long periods without breathing or oxygen, they are vulnerable to the distressful effects of suffocation. However, to the best of our knowledge the argumentation against using CO2 to euthanize reptiles lacks empirical data and rests instead upon untested hypotheses and theoretical inference.

Here, we examine the efficacy of CO2 to humanely euthanize squamate reptiles (lizards and snakes). Specifically, we tested the potential value of CO2 in (1) creating a low-stress, temporary unconscious state to make physical methods of euthanasia safer and more efficient and (2) killing squamates outright. We used both behavioral responses and blood corticosterone concentrations (the primary glucocorticoid associated with stress in reptiles) to determine whether CO2 exposure provides a humane transition to unconsciousness and examined how duration of CO2 exposure influences the post-exposure duration of unconsciousness and likelihood of death.

Materials and methods

Study species and locations

Behavioral responses to CO2 exposure were examined in four species of reptile: reticulated pythons (Malayopython reticulatus); Burmese pythons (Python bivittatus); masked water snakes (Homalopsis buccata); and Asian water monitors (Varanus salvator). These species are semi-aquatic to varying degrees and wide-ranging in Southeast Asia. The two python species grow to be large (> 5 m), while masked water snakes are relatively small (< 1.2 m). Asian water monitors are the world’s second largest lizard, growing to 3 metres in length and weighing as much as 25 kg. In many instances, these species are commensal with humans and are regularly harvested and traded for their meat, skin, and medicinal value.

In May 2019, we examined responses to CO2 in these reptiles in Malaysia (2°14’N, 103°03’E) and Thailand (17°38’N, 100°07’E) at two commercial facilities producing meat for human consumption and skins for the exotic leather trade. In Malaysia, free-roaming M. reticulatus and V. salvator are legally collected from the wild by licensed hunters and brought to abattoirs for processing [13, 14]. Animals are kept alive at the facility for up to a week before being killed using a strong blow to the head followed by decapitation. No individual-based history was available for the animals used in our study, and animals were held according to standard commercial protocols (i.e., maintained individually in mesh bags with water provided intermittently). In Thailand, we examined specimens of M. reticulatus, P. bivittatus, and H. buccata. The two python species were captive-bred for commercial purposes following protocols described in Natusch and Lyons [15]. The H. buccata were wild-caught and temporarily held in large outdoor ponds with food provided. This research was undertaken with approval from the Animal Institutional Care and Use Committee of Arizona State University (protocol # 10-1689R).

Experimental design—behavioral monitoring

To assess behavioral responses of reptiles to CO2 exposure, we placed study animals individually into 100 micron 375 mm x 500 mm clear plastic bags. Very large animals were double-bagged as a precaution. CO2 was supplied via 47 litre steel cylinders containing 99.5% CO2 and fitted with single-stage CO2 regulators. A 5 mm inside diameter CO2 supply hose was placed in the bag through the opening at the top, and the bag was sealed with an elastic band to limit but not eliminate the escape of gas. Bags were gently compressed around the body of the animal prior to CO2 admission to minimize residual air pockets. This design enabled CO2 to rapidly displace the limited amount of air present in the bag and thus minimized gas equilibration time [16]. By using plastic bags instead of a rigid container, we were able to closely evaluate the animal during its exposure to CO2 (e.g., examine the animal’s righting response and its response to touch stimulation). CO2 flow was set to rapidly replace any existing air and then reduced to maintain positive CO2 pressure in the bag. For the longer exposure times, once the animal was unconscious, the flow of CO2 was stopped and the bagged was completely sealed. The process was similar for water monitors except that the bag was secured over their head rather than placing the entire body inside the bag (to minimize damage to the plastic bag by the lizard’s claws). We prevented monitors from perforating the bag during movements by gently placing a hand around the animal’s neck and preventing the forelimbs from contacting the bag. For some individuals this was not necessary and did not prevent observation of general body movements in response to CO2 exposure. For all individuals, the response of the animal to CO2 exposure was recorded via direct visual examination until the animal was removed from the bag after the duration of CO2 exposure dictated by its assigned treatment group.

For each animal, we recorded signs of consciousness and all behavioral responses to CO2, including movement, tongue flicking, and gaping. The animal’s behavior and body movements at the time of removal were recorded, as were changes in behavior over time and the eventual outcome (i.e., recovery or confirmed death). It was difficult to determine consciousness in many specimens. Although several individuals continued to respond to deep-touch stimuli (e.g., a deep pinch of the tail), a lack of righting reflex (failure to turnover when placed upside down), corneal reflex in lizards, and cessation of breathing, strongly indicated that individuals were unconscious despite exhibiting a muscular response to deep stimuli. Animals that reached a state indicative of imminent recovery of consciousness (i.e., voluntary movement often associated with tongue flicking) were euthanized using standard commercial practices (i.e., forceful blunt trauma to the dorsal surface of the head at the location of the brain case). Animals were deemed dead if no heartbeat and/or movements were detected (visually or via palpation) or by a lack of response to all stimuli (most notably a deep tail pinch) for up to one hour after removal from CO2 exposure.

To test the effect of CO2 exposure duration on reptile responses, we first conducted a preliminary assessment using different exposure durations on five M. reticulatus (30 min, 60 min, 90 min, 120 min, or 180 min; n = 1 per duration). Based on related observations, we selected three CO2 exposure durations (15 min, 30 min, and 90 min) for the primary study. We used the results from the reticulated pythons to select exposure durations for the other species. As our results from M. reticulatus showed that 15 min was an insufficient duration, we began studies of other species with the 30 min exposure duration to minimise the number of animals used and to streamline efforts. If all specimens of the species failed to recover at this exposure duration, we assumed longer durations would achieve the same result, so did not conduct longer duration trials. This was not true for H. buccata for which we did not complete the 90 min exposure treatment due to specimen availability and logistic constraints. We measured snout-vent length (SVL; using a steel tape measure) and body mass (using a digital scale) of each specimen while unconscious or dead, and then determined sex via direct inspection of the gonads upon dissection. Sample sizes for each species and their CO2 exposure times are presented in Table 1. Air temperature was recorded to confirm constant temperatures throughout the course of study.

thumbnail
Table 1. Means, standard errors and ranges for snout-vent length (SVL) and body mass for reptile specimens used to examine behavioral responses to CO2 exposure.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240176.t001

Experimental design–sample collection for hormone monitoring

We measured the effect of the CO2 euthanasia process on circulating corticosterone by collecting blood from seven M. reticulatus and seven V. salvator before and after CO2 exposure. Specimens were brought to the National Wildlife Forensic Laboratory, Department of Wildlife and National Parks Peninsular Malaysia. Sexes and body sizes are reported in Table 2. Each animal was kept individually within a mesh bag and secured within a plastic crate at ambient temperature for two days before trials began. We collected 2 ml of blood from each individual within 90 seconds of removal from the mesh bag using a 22 gauge needle and 5 ml syringe inserted into the caudal vein at the base of the tail. The blood sample was then placed in a tube containing lithium heparin (Vacuette #454084, Greiner Bio-One, Kremsmünster, Austria). After blood collection, the same specimens were immediately exposed to CO2. A second blood sample was collected from the same specimen after 15 minutes of CO2 exposure when the animal was unconscious. We did this by amputating the lower third of the tail and collecting the blood directly into a heparinized tube. The animal was then immediately euthanized following standard methods as described above. Blood samples were placed on ice until centrifugation to separate the plasma. We stored the isolated plasma samples at -20°C until they were assayed. As confinement in the mesh bag may in itself result in elevated levels of corticosterone, we collected blood samples from several ‘control’ animals for comparison. The control water monitors (n = 3) were freshly killed wild animals harvested during a government sanctioned control program in Ladang Eng Tai, Malaysia (4°57'N 100°27'E). Animals were harvested using a 12-gauge shotgun at close range, with head shots resulting in near-instantaneous death. We collected blood from the severed tail of each animal within 90 seconds using the same method described above. Control reticulated python (n = 4) samples were obtained from captive-bred animals at a commercial reptile breeding facility outside Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia (2°56'N 101°53'E). The farm breeds high-value pythons for the pet trade, and general husbandry and welfare standards are high. Animals were selected based on size and relative docility (i.e., ease of handling), and blood samples were collected from the caudal vein within 90 seconds of removal from their enclosures using the same method described above. We recorded temperatures (27–30°C) and kept all animals at approximately the same temperature both before and after exposure to CO2. This was not possible for control specimens sampled in the wild, but plasma corticosterone levels are not highly sensitive to body temperature in reptiles [17]. We obtained all blood samples over several hours on the same day to avoid diel and seasonal variation in plasma hormone levels.

thumbnail
Table 2. Means, standard errors and ranges for snout-vent length (SVL) and body mass for reptile specimens used to examine plasma corticosterone responses to CO2 exposure.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240176.t002

Hormone analysis

Immunoreactive plasma corticosterone concentrations were determined via an enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA; ADI-900-097, Enzo Life Sciences, Farmingdale, NY) following the manufacturer’s instructions. This kit has been used in previous studies assessing plasma corticosterone concentrations in a variety of animal species, including alligators [18], birds [19], lizards [20] and turtles [21], but had not been previously documented for pythons or monitor lizards. Based on results from other species, we used a dilution ratio of 40:1. All samples were run in duplicate format on a single assay plate. Results confirmed an average difference between duplicates of less than 1.8% (mean: 1.73 ± 1.18%), and duplicate means were thus used in the analysis.

Data analysis

Our behavioral analysis measured the binary dependent variable of whether reptiles recovered after CO2 exposure or not. This metric was evaluated after different CO2 exposure durations for each species. For our corticosterone study we used a paired sample t-test to test for significant differences in plasma corticosterone concentrations before and after CO2 exposure. We used a one-way analysis of variance to test for differences in corticosterone level between the control animals and the pre-CO2 exposure samples from the study animals. Data were ln-transformed where needed to meet the normality and homogeneity of variance assumptions required for our parametric tests. All analyses were conducted in JMP Pro 14 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).

Results

Behavioral observations

Reticulated pythons (Malayopython reticulatus).

After exposure to CO2, reticulated pythons remained still for 60–300 secs (1–5 mins) before tongue flicking and gaping (Fig 1). These responses eventually proceeded to slow and controlled whole-body movements; at this time snakes were responsive to touch through the bag. It was difficult to determine the point at which snakes lost full consciousness. However, we suspect that snakes lost consciousness, but continued to undergo unconscious movements including a response to touch stimuli. Between 240–1380 secs (4–23 mins) after CO2 exposure the snakes ceased all movements and lost responsiveness to stimuli (Fig 1). After the cessation of movement, but sometimes before, 18 of the 30 snakes exhibited mild muscle twitching of parts of their body. This twitching was unique to the reticulated pythons.

thumbnail
Fig 1.

Variation in timing (in minutes) of key behavioural changes in (a) Malayopython reticulatus, (b) Python bivittatus, (c) Homalopsis buccata, and (d) Varanus salvator subject to carbon dioxide (CO2) exposure. Gaping: the time at which the mouth of the specimen opened. Unresponsive: the time the specimen had ceased movement and became unresponsive to stimuli. Thicker parts of the violin plots represent CO2 exposure times where the behaviour was most often observed. Note the different time scales represented on the x-axes of each panel.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240176.g001

All Malaysian reticulated pythons that were exposed to CO2 for 15 and 30 min eventually recovered (Fig 2). At the time of removal from the bag, none of these snakes had voluntary movements, but 7 of 8 snakes in the 15-min exposure group and 1 of 10 snakes in the 30-min group responded to a deep tail pinch with local movement. First voluntary movements occurred 4.9 ± 0.9 (mean ± SE) and 23.8 ± 4.7 min after removal from CO2 for the 15 min and 30-min exposure groups, respectively. In contrast, all reticulated pythons exposed to 90-min of CO2 did not recover, never having any reflex or voluntary movements (Fig 2). Reticulated pythons tested in Thailand that were exposed to CO2 for 30 min responded similarly to those in Malaysia, but one of the four snakes did not recover and, for those that did, recovery took 13.7 ± 3.7 min (42% faster than the 30-min exposure snakes in Malaysia).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Percentage of Malayopython reticulatus, Python bivittatus, Homalopsis buccata, and Varanus salvator that recovered from different durations of CO2 exposure.

X denotes treatments where no individuals recovered from CO2 exposure. Sample sizes appear above each column.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240176.g002

Burmese pythons (Python bivittatus).

Burmese pythons showed similar behavioral responses to reticulated pythons, but took slighter longer to gape and lose responsiveness to stimuli (Fig 1). Burmese pythons also did not undergo muscle twitching and late-stage non-responsive (likely unconscious) movements were greater. All 8 snakes in the 30-min group responded to a deep tail pinch upon removal from the CO2, while none of the 90-min snakes responded. Two of the 8 snakes exposed to CO2 for 30 min and all of the snakes exposed to CO2 for 90 min did not recover (Fig 2). For the six 30-min snakes that did recover, it took 17.4 ± 2.5 min until they showed their first voluntary movements.

Masked water snakes (Homalopsis buccata).

The water snakes exposed to CO2 for 30 min showed behavioral responses that were very similar to those of the Burmese pythons, with no twitching but a considerable amount of unconscious movements. Mean time of first gape was about 120 secs (range: 60–420 secs, 1–7 min) and complete loss of consciousness was 300–840 secs (5–14 mins) after the onset of exposure (Fig 1). While all eight water snakes had a tail pinch reflex upon removal from the CO2, only two of the eight snakes recovered after 10 and 20 min, respectively.

Water monitors (Varanus salvator).

The water monitors showed the least behavioral response to exposure to CO2. The lizards exhibited no tongue flicking and no muscle twitching during the 30 min exposure. All monitors gaped within 240 secs (4 mins) of the onset of CO2 exposure (Fig 1) Both conscious and unconscious movements were limited in number and intensity with the last detected movements occurring 930 ± 66 secs (range: 720–1560 seconds) after the onset of exposure (Fig 1). All monitors lacked a tail pinch reflex when removed from the CO2, and they all failed to recover (Fig 2).

Plasma corticosterone concentrations

Corticosterone concentrations for the animals that did not go through the capture and confinement associated with the trade prior to killing (i.e., ‘controls’) were significantly lower than those of the CO2-euthanized animals prior to CO2 exposure (pythons: 7.2 ± 1.3 ng/ml; F1,10 = 9.01, P = 0.015; monitors: 3.1 ± 0.7 ng/ml; F1,10 = 24.4, P < 0.001; Fig 3). Reticulated python plasma corticosterone concentrations increased by 14% after CO2 exposure, (t0 = 11.8 ± 0.9 ng/ml vs t15 = 13.2 ± 0.4 ng/ml). However, this increasing trend was not statistically significant (matched pairs t-test: t6 = 2.23, P = 0.065; Fig 3). In contrast, CO2 exposure significantly increased plasma corticosterone concentrations in water monitors (by 18%; t0 = 9.6 ± 0.9 ng/ml; t15 = 11.7 ± 0.8 ng/ml; t6 = 5.03, P = 0.02; Fig 3). Individual immunoreactive plasma corticosterone concentrations before and after CO2 exposure were significantly correlated (pythons: n = 7; r2 = 0.61; P = 0.037; lizards: n = 8; r2 = 0.77; P = 0.009).

thumbnail
Fig 3.

Mean plasma corticosterone concentrations (ng/ml) before and after 15 minutes of CO2 exposure and in control specimens (free-ranging or farmed; see text) of (a) Malayopython reticulatus and (b) Varanus salvator. Differences between corticosterone concentrations before and after CO2 exposure were not statistically significant for M. reticulatus, but were for V. salvator. Corticosterone concentrations between control specimens not subject to capture and handling are significantly lower than those captured from the wild for trade (although sample sizes were low; see text for details). Sample sizes for each group are reported directly above the x-axis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240176.g003

Discussion

Although available euthanasia methods for commercial reptile processing (e.g., brain destruction) are humane, they can be vulnerable to operator error, are aesthetically displeasing, and are inefficient for rapidly processing numerous individuals. Our study provides initial results supporting the potential for carbon dioxide asphyxiation as an effective option for euthanizing reptiles in a variety of settings. Exposure to CO2 was effective for creating a temporary unconscious state at all exposure durations that was sufficient to safely and humanely employ a physical method of euthanasia. Longer but still logistically practical exposures to CO2 were able to kill reptiles.

The different taxa in our study varied subtly in their responses to CO2 exposure, both while conscious and after losing consciousness. For example, despite the similar body size of the two python species, the CO2 exposure duration required to induce unconsciousness in P. bivittatus was greater than M. reticulatus (Fig 1). The only lizard species in our study was rapidly rendered unconscious and did not recover from CO2 exposure durations that were unable to kill most of the snakes (Fig 2). Taxonomic differences and variation in metabolic rates may both be responsible for this difference [2224]. The species we studied also differed in the effects that a given duration of CO2 exposure had once the animal was removed from CO2, including the extent of involuntary/reflex muscle activity and the likelihood of death. Unfortunately, we did not have a sufficient sample size to examine sexual differences in species’ responses to CO2 exposure. Plausibly, CO2 may affect males and females differently, especially in those species with strong sexual dimorphism. Related to this, our study was undertaken on several of the world’s largest reptiles, all of which are semi-aquatic and can remain submerged under water for considerable periods. Application of CO2 exposure to the myriad of smaller-bodied reptiles, and to strictly terrestrial species, may yield different results.

We made the assumption that because the density of CO2 is greater than air, complete (100%) CO2 saturation would occur as air was expelled from the small opening positioned at the top of the bag [25]. However, we did not directly measure the concentration of CO2 within the bag and whether the concentration was homogenous. Layering of CO2 could enable specimens to avoid exposure [2]. The variation in responses to CO2 exposure in our study may be related to minor but functionally significant difference in CO2 distribution [see 26]. In order to more broadly apply CO2 as a euthanasia method in reptiles, there needs to be a better understanding of interspecific difference among taxa as well as a delivery system with established displacement parameters and sufficient holding capacity.

Regardless of species, our behavioral observations suggest the reptiles used in our study do not suffer significant distress from CO2 exposure. Although our observational assessments were subjective, the body movements made by conscious reptiles were minor and appeared considerably less vigorous than the escape behavior displayed by these same animals when first removed from their holding bags. In the case of V. salvator, some specimens went unconscious without showing any signs of movement. Nevertheless, it is challenging to accurately determine if reptiles are indeed dead, let alone feeling pain, based solely on behavioral responses [27, 28]. For example, an active heartbeat, involuntary movements, and response to touch stimuli can continue for hours after complete destruction, pithing, and removal of the brain [Natusch unpubl. data 2020, 2]. Similarly, our data on the time reptiles take to lose responsiveness are difficult to interpret. It was often unknown if specimens were consciously responsive, or unconscious and merely exhibiting involuntary muscular reflex. Importantly, the difficulty of assuring death, and the high but less than 100% effectiveness at killing at some CO2 exposure durations, may warrant the use of a secondary method to ensure death as is commonly used for chemical-induced euthanasia of research animals [see 2].

The most consistent behavioral response to CO2 exposure was the non-violent gaping displayed by most (90%) individuals. Gaping is common in mammals and birds subject to CO2 exposure, and in birds does not appear to be a sign of distress when exposed to CO2 [29]. It is unknown whether gaping is a sign of significant distress in reptiles. Gaping occurred within 30 seconds to 16 minutes of initiating CO2 exposure and the timing varied among taxa (Fig 1). The short duration between initial exposure and gaping, and then unconsciousness, suggests that suffocation may not be the cause of death in reptiles exposed to CO2. All species used in our study are semi-aquatic, and capable of spending significant time underwater (>20 minutes), suggesting another physiological response is taking place. Despite the lack of behavioral indicators for stress and pain, reptiles take considerably longer to lose consciousness than mammals and birds [3032]. Some consider a gentle death that takes longer is preferable to a rapid but more distressing death [26, 33]. In the context of CO2 and reptiles, further research is needed.

Our additional approach to investigate the impact of CO2 exposure in our study species, by monitoring plasma corticosterone concentrations, also suggests that reptiles experience relatively minor distress from CO2 exposure. Comparison to our control (wild or farmed) specimens suggests the relative increase in stress involved in restraint and transportation of specimens to the laboratory was greater than the distress induced by CO2 exposure [2, 34]. Brown tree snakes (Boiga irregularis) and red-sided garter snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis) captured and placed in bags for 2–4 hours increased plasma corticosterone levels by 280–1200% [35, 36], but resulted in no appreciable increase in corticosterone concentrations in bearded dragons (Pogona barbata) [37]. Several studies reveal a lack of adverse impacts of corticosterone increase on survival, feeding behavior, and reproduction [3840]. Other studies document invasive procedures (e.g., toe clipping, microchipping) inducing smaller corticosterone increases than did natural stresses experienced in the wild [27]. The relatively small increases in plasma corticosterone concentrations observed in pythons (14%) and lizards (18%) in our study may suggest that the functional relevance (distress or pain) of CO2 exposure-induced increases in corticosterone may be negligible. It is possible that the small increases in corticosterone levels we observed were related mostly to the stress caused by restraining and collecting an initial (T0) blood sample from each specimen, rather than by exposure to the CO2 itself. Alternatively, a post-CO2 exposure increase in corticosterone may have been suppressed because the recent capture, confinement, and handling had already maximized the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis response.

Intriguingly, exposure to CO2 may have additional benefits beyond the possibility of a painless death. After death, animals can have spinal cord induced muscle activity, and this can last for an extended duration in reptiles due to their tissue’s high tolerance of hypoxia. This phenomenon can lead to the impression that the animals is still alive [2], and thus has been capitalized on by activists who oppose the consumption of animals, claiming they are being processed while still alive. In addition to being aesthetically displeasing, continued muscle movements after death force staff in commercial facilities to delay the harvesting of tissues for up to two hours after death [41]. When killed via CO2 exposure, we recorded no involuntary muscle movements after the presumed point of death, including during the processing of the reptiles. The physiological cause of this lack of muscle tone is unknown but, given its functional and cosmetic advantages, warrants further investigation.

In conclusion, our study presents some of the first results on the effects of CO2 exposure in reptiles. We stress that our results are preliminary and therefore are reluctant to recommend CO2 as a humane method of reptile euthanasia at this time. Despite our results being generally positive, we identified some interspecific differences and methodological variables that may influence the effectiveness of CO2 exposure. Future studies could usefully disentangle the influence of these variables and employ alternative methods for assessing stress, pain, and death in reptiles (e.g., electroencephalography).

Supporting information

Acknowledgments

We thank Yuan Wai Lek reptile trading company, Sisatchanalai python farm, and Lim Maju Jaya Trading for providing the animals used in this study. We also thank the Malaysian Department of Wildlife and National Parks Peninsular Malaysia for providing access to their forensic laboratory and equipment. We thank anonymous reviewers for comments that improved an earlier draft of this manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Chambers PG, Grandin T. Guidelines for humane handling, transport and slaughter of livestock. Heinz G, Srisuvan T, editors. Humane Society and International 2001. [accessed on 28 May 2020]. pp. 1–17. Available from: http://www.fao.org/3/a-x6909e.pdf.
  2. 2. Leary SL, Underwood W, Anthony R, Cartner S, Corey D, Grandin T, et al. AVMA Guidelines for the Euthanasia of Animals: 2020 Edition. American Veterinary Medical Association\. Schaumburg, IL. [(accessed on 30 May 2020)]. Available from: https://www.avma.org/sites/default/files/2020-01/2020-Euthanasia-Final-1-17-20.pdf.
  3. 3. Shine R, Amiel J, Munn AJ, Stewart M, Vyssotski AL, Lesku JA. Is “cooling then freezing” a humane way to kill amphibians and reptiles? Biol Open 2015;4: 760–763. pmid:26015533
  4. 4. Shine R, Lesku JA, Lillywhite HB. Assessment of the cooling-then-freezing method for euthanasia of amphibians and reptiles. J Am Vet Med Assoc 2019;255: 48–50. pmid:31194656.
  5. 5. Lillywhite HB, Shine R, Jacobson E, DeNardo DF, Gordon MS, Navas CA, et al. Anesthesia and euthanasia of amphibians and reptiles used in scientific research: should hypothermia and freezing be prohibited? Bioscience 2017;67:53–61.
  6. 6. Swiss Expert Panel. Analysis of humane killing methods for reptiles in the skin trade. Swiss Federal Veterinary Office. 2013. [(accessed on 30 May 2020)]. Available from: https://recht.pogona.ch/data/_uploaded/file/3.0%20F%C3%BCtterung/5.2_BVET_Analysis%20on%20humane%20killing%20methods%20for%20reptiles%20in%20the%20skin%20trade%20frm%5B1%5D.pdf.
  7. 7. World Organisation for Animal Health. Terrestrial Animal Health Code, Chapter 7.14. 2019 [accessed on 29 June 2020)]. Available from: https://www.oie.int/international-standard-setting/terrestrial-code/.
  8. 8. Withrock IC. The use of carbon dioxide (CO2) as an alternative euthanasia method for goat kids. M.Sc. Thesis, Iowa State University. 2015. Available from: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/etd/14718.
  9. 9. Boivin GP, Hickman DL, Creamer-Hente MA, Pritchett-Corning KR, Bratcher NA. Review of CO2 as a euthanasia agent for laboratory rats and mice. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci. 2017;56: 491–499. pmid:28903819.
  10. 10. Çavuşoğlu E, Rault JL, Gates R, Lay DC. Behavioral Response of Weaned Pigs during Gas Euthanasia with CO2, CO2 with Butorphanol, or Nitrous Oxide. Animals 2020;10: 787. pmid:32370086
  11. 11. Close B, Banister K, Baumans V, Bernoth EM, Bromage N, Bunyan J, et al. Recommendations for euthanasia of experimental animals: Part 2. Lab anim 1997;31: 1–32. pmid:9121105.
  12. 12. Warren K. Reptile Euthanasia—No Easy Solution? Pac Conserv Biol 2014;20: 25–27.
  13. 13. Khadiejah S, Razak N, Ward-Fear G, Shine R, Natusch DJD. Asian water monitors (Varanus salvator) remain common in Peninsular Malaysia, despite intense harvesting. Wildl Res 2019;46: 265–275.
  14. 14. Natusch DJD, Lyons JA, Riyanto A, Mumpuni, Khadiejah S, Shine R. Detailed biological data are informative, but robust trends are needed for informing sustainability of wildlife harvesting: A case study of reptile offtake in Southeast Asia. Biol Conserv 2019;233: 83–92.
  15. 15. Natusch DJD, Lyons JA. Assessment of python breeding farms supplying the international high-end leather industry. A report under the ‘Python Conservation Partnership’ programme of research. Occasional Paper of the IUCN Species Survival Commission 2014;50. [(accessed on 15 May 2020)]. Available from: https://portals.iucn.org/library/sites/library/files/documents/SSC-OP-050.PDF.
  16. 16. Lasiewski RC, Acosta AL, Bern-stein MH. Evaporative water loss in birds. 1. Characteristics of the open flow method of determination, and their relation to estimates of thermoregulatory ability. Comp Biochem Physiol 1966; 19:445–457.
  17. 17. Sykes KL, Klukowski M. Effects of acute temperature change, confinement and housing on plasma corticosterone in water snakes, Nerodia sipedon (Colubridae: Natricinae). J Exp Zool 2009;311A: 172–181. pmid:19051318.
  18. 18. Finger JW Jr, Hamilton MT, Kelley MD, Stacy NI, Glenn TC, Tuberville TD. Examining the Effects of Chronic Selenium Exposure on Traditionally Used Stress Parameters in Juvenile American Alligators, Alligator mississippiensis. Arch Environ Contam Toxicol. 2019;77(1):14–21. pmid:30976886
  19. 19. Xie S, Romero LM, Htut ZW, McWhorter TJ. Stress responses to heat exposure in three species of Australian desert birds. Physiol Biochem Zool. 2017;90(3):348–358. pmid:28384428
  20. 20. Seddon RJ, Hews DK. Populations of the Lizard, Sceloporus occidentalis, that differ in melanization have different rates of wound healing. J Exp Zool A Ecol Genet Physiol. 2016;325(8):491–500. pmid:27597293
  21. 21. West JM, Klukowski M. Seasonal changes in baseline corticosterone, association with innate immunity, and effects of confinement in free-ranging Eastern Box Turtles, Terrapene carolina carolina. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2018;262:71–80. pmid:29548757
  22. 22. Chappell MA, Ellis TM. Resting metabolic rates in boid snakes: allometric relationships and temperature effects. J Comp Physiol B 1987;157: 227–235. pmid:3106432.
  23. 23. Thompson G, Heger N, Heger T, Withers P. Standard metabolic rate of the largest Australian lizard, Varanus giganteus. Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol 1995;111: 603–608.
  24. 24. Hopkins W, Roe J, Philippi T, Congdon J. Standard and digestive metabolism in the banded water snake, Nerodia fasciata fasciata. Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol 2004;137: 141–149. pmid:14720599.
  25. 25. Murray D. (2009). CO2 euthanasia methods for neonatal piglets. Allen D. Lemon Swine Conference; 2009 University of Minnesota: Minneapolis, MN United States of America. p. 114–116. [(accessed on 15 May 2020)]. Available from: http://hdl.handle.net/11299/139762.
  26. 26. Coenen AM, Drinkenburg WH, Hoenderken R, van Luijtelaar EL. Carbon dioxide euthanasia in rats: oxygen supplementation minimizes signs of agitation and asphyxia. Lab Anim 1995;29: 262–268. pmid:7564209.
  27. 27. Langkilde T, Shine R. How much stress do researchers inflict on their study animals? A case study using a scincid lizard, Eulamprus heatwolei. J Exp Biol 2006;209:1035– 1043. pmid:16513929.
  28. 28. Eatwell K. Options for analgesia and anaesthesia in reptiles. In Pract 2010;32: 306–311.
  29. 29. McKeegan DEF, McIntyre J, Demmers TGM, Wathes CM, Bryan Jones R. Behavioural responses of broiler chickens during acute exposure to gaseous stimulation. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2006;99: 271–286.
  30. 30. Hewett TA, Kovacs MS, Artwohl JE, Bennett BT. A comparison of euthanasia methods in rats, using carbon dioxide in prefilled and fixed flow-rate filled chambers. Lab Anim Sci 1993;43: 579–582. pmid:8158983.
  31. 31. Shea SA, Harty HR, Banzett RB. Self-control of level of mechanical ventilation to minimize CO2-induced air hunger. Respir Physiol 1996;103: 113–125. pmid:8833543.
  32. 32. Reed B, Varon J, Chait BT, Kreek MJ. Carbon dioxide-induced anaesthesia result in a rapid increase in plasma levels of vasopressin. Endocrinology 2009;150: 2934–2939. pmid:19213839
  33. 33. Hawkins P, Playle L, Golledge H, Leach M, Banzett R, Coenen A, et al. Newcastle Consensus Meeting on Carbon Dioxide Euthanasia of Laboratory Animals; Newcastle upon Tyne, UK. 27–28 February 2006; [(accessed on 28 May 2020)]. pp. 1–17. Available from: www.nc3rs.org.uk/downloaddoc.asp?id=416&page=292&skin=0.
  34. 34. Sharp J, Azar T, Lawson D. Comparison of carbon dioxide, argon, and nitrogen for inducing unconsciousness or euthanasia of rats. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 2006;45: 21–25. pmid:16542038.
  35. 35. Moore IT, Lemaster MP, Mason RT. Behavioural and hormonal responses to capture stress in the male red-sided garter snake, Thamnophis sirtalis parietalis. Anim Behav 2000;59: 529e534. pmid:10715174.
  36. 36. Mathies T, Felix TA, Lance VA. Effects of trapping and subsequent short-term confinement stress on plasma corticosterone in the brown treesnake, Boiga irregularis on Guam. Gen Comp Endocrinol 2001;124(1):106–114. pmid:11703076.
  37. 37. Cree A, Amey AP, Whittier JM. Lack of consistent hormonal responses to capture during the breeding season of the bearded dragon, Pogona barbata. Comp Biochem Physiol 2000;126: 275–285. pmid:16153645.
  38. 38. Cote J, Clobert J, Meylan S, Fitze P. Experimental enhancement of corticosterone levels positively affects subsequent male survival. Horm Behav 2006;49: 320–327. pmid:16153645.
  39. 39. Thaker M, Vanak AT, Lima SL, Hews DK. Stress and aversive learning in a wild vertebrate: the role of corticosterone in mediating escape from a novel stressor. Am Nat 2010;175: 50–60. pmid:19922261.
  40. 40. Dupoué A, Angelier F, Brischoux F, DeNardo DF, Trouvé C, Parenteau C, et al. Water deprivation increases maternal corticosterone levels and enhances offspring growth in the snake, Vipera aspis. J Exp Biol 2016;219: 658–667. pmid:26747902.
  41. 41. Aust PW, Webb GJW, DeNardo DF, Natusch DJD. Welfare Principles for Snakes and Monitor Lizards in the Southeast Asian Skin Trade–a guide for stakeholders. Swiss Federal Veterinary Office, Switzerland; Southeast Asian Reptile Conservation Alliance, France 2019.